Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/306394998

History of Psychology [From "Encyclopedia of Critical Psychology"]

Chapter · January 2014


DOI: 10.1007/978-1-4614-5583-7_137

CITATIONS READS

14 30,703

1 author:

Adrian Brock
University of Cape Town
76 PUBLICATIONS   413 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

The Psychodynamics of Race View project

The Indigenous Turn in Psychology View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Adrian Brock on 23 August 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


H 872 History of Psychology

Psychology portrays the sexes (pp. 97–125). Winston, A. S. (2004). Defining difference: Race and
New York: Columbia University Press. racism in the history of psychology. Washington, DC:
Morawski, J. S. (1985). The measurement of masculinity American Psychological Association.
and femininity: Engendering categorical realities. Young, R. M. (1966). Scholarship and the history of the
Journal of Personality, 53, 196–223. behavioural sciences. History of Science, 5, 1–51.
Morawski, J. S. (2005). Reflexivity and the psychologist.
History of the Human Sciences, 18, 77–105.
O’Connell, A. N., & Russo, N. F. (Eds.). (1990). Women in
Online Resources
Bibliography: Race and racism by Thomas Teo. http://
psychology: A bio-bibliographic sourcebook.
ahp.apps01.yorku.ca/?p¼922
Westport, CT: Greenwood.
Kurt Danziger by Adrian Brock. http://www.kurtdanziger.
Pickren, W. E. (2010). Hybridizing, transforming,
com/default.htm
indigenizing: Psychological knowledge as mélange.
Power and subjectivity: Critical history and psychology
Boletı́n de la Sociedad de Historia de la Psicologı́a,
by Nikolas Rose. http://www.academyanalyticarts.
44, 6–12.
org/rose1.htm
Richards, G. (1997). ‘Race’, racism and psychology:
Psychology’s Feminist Voices by Alexandra Rutherford.
Towards a reflexive history. London: Routledge.
www.feministvoices.com
Rose, N. (1990). Governing the soul: The shaping of the
private self. London: Routledge.
Rose, N. (1996). Power and subjectivity: Critical history
and psychology. In C. F. Graumann & K. J. Gergen
(Eds.), Historical dimensions of psychological
discourse (pp. 103–124). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
History of Psychology
University Press.
Rutherford, A. (2002). Back to the classroom. . .and Adrian C. Brock
beyond: Comment on Bhatt and Tonks. Bulletin for School of Psychology, University College
the History and Philosophy of Psychology, 14, 17–19.
Dublin, Belfield, Dublin 4, Ireland
Samelson, F. (1974). History, origin myth, and ideology:
“Discovery” of social psychology. Journal for the
Theory of Social Behaviour, 4, 217–231.
Scarborough, E., & Furumoto, L. (1987). Untold lives: Introduction
The first generation of American women psychologists.
New York: Columbia University Press.
Shields, S. A. (1975). Functionalism, Darwinism, and the History of psychology is an unusual field. We can
psychology of women. American Psychologist, broadly categorize the various specialties within
30, 739–754. psychology in terms of those that are thought to
Smith, R. (2005). Does reflexivity separate the human
be central or core and those that are relatively
sciences from the natural sciences? History of the
Human Sciences, 18, 1–25. peripheral. The former might include neuropsy-
Smith, R. (2007). Being human: Historical knowledge and chology, cognitive psychology, social psychol-
the creation of human nature. New York: Columbia ogy, and developmental psychology. Some
University Press.
examples of the latter are cultural or cross-
Swartz, S. (1995). The black insane in the cape,
1891–1920. Journal of Southern African Studies, cultural psychology, community psychology,
21, 399–415. theoretical/philosophical psychology, and critical
Teo, T. (2005). The critique of psychology from Kant to psychology. History of psychology does not fall
postcolonial theory. New York: Springer.
neatly into either of these categories. The subject
Tucker, W. H. (1994). The science and politics of racial
research. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press. is widely taught in psychology departments, and
Van Ommen, C., & Painter, D. (Eds.). (2008). Interiors: there is a substantial market for its textbooks.
A history of psychology in South Africa. Leicester, UK: History of psychology is also a formal require-
BPS Books.
ment in degrees that are approved by professional
Vaughn-Blount, K., Rutherford, A., Baker, D., & Johnson, D.
(2009). History’s mysteries, demystified: Becoming organizations like the American Psychological
a psychologist-historian. American Journal of Association and the British Psychological Soci-
Psychology, 122, 117–129. ety, though neither of these organizations
Watson, R. I. (1975). The history of psychology as
requires that it be offered as a separate course.
a specialty: A personal view of its first 15 years.
Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 11, The requirement can be met by including histor-
5–14. ical material in other courses.
History of Psychology 873 H
In spite of this situation, history of psychology nineteenth century (Lapointe, 1968). To consider
is not usually recognized as an area of speciali- the work of earlier writers as “psychology” is to
zation or research. There is only one university in commit what many historians consider to be the
the whole of North America which offers sin of “presentism,” which consists of projecting
a postgraduate specialization in history of psy- the views of the present onto the past. Like the
chology and that is York University in Toronto, anthropologists who study people in remote cor-
Canada. There was until recently a smaller pro- ners of the world, historians study the past
gram in the area at the University of New Hamp- because it is different, and we will often miss
shire in the United States, but it has since been the subtle differences that exist between the pre-
discontinued. Given that it is not usually consid- sent and the past if we insist on viewing the past
ered to be as an area of specialization or research, through the categories of the present.
there are few academic positions in the field, and Most historians of psychology are of the view
most of the courses on the history of psychology that psychology is a product of the second half of
are taught by specialists in other areas of psychol- the nineteenth century on the grounds that this is
ogy. Only a small minority of the psychologists when a specialist area of knowledge that went
who specialize in the history of their field did under the name began to emerge. Thus, they H
their postgraduate training in this area. The have come back to the view that psychology has
majority were originally specialists in another a short history and a long past, albeit for different
area and developed an interest in history of psy- reasons than the one that is usually associated
chology later in their careers, often after getting with that view. The traditional explanation is
academic tenure, and are usually self-taught. that psychology emerged in the nineteenth cen-
tury because that was at that time when the field
distanced itself from philosophy and became an
Definition empirical science.
In recent years, the view that psychology first
The term “history of psychology” requires little appeared in the nineteenth century has been chal-
explanation. It is the story of psychology from its lenged, though it has been challenged on the
earliest beginnings to the present day. Some grounds that it dates back to the sixteenth century
debate has taken place over when the story (Vidal, 2011). Few people nowadays would
begins. In spite of the well-known adage attrib- maintain that it can be traced back to the ancient
uted to Ebbinghaus that “psychology has a short Greeks. That was more about conferring legiti-
history and a long past,” textbook writers have macy on the field by providing it with
usually started the story with the ancient Greeks. a distinguished ancestry, and Aristotle is one of
A well-known example is the textbook by Robert the most distinguished ancestors that any field
Watson titled The Great Psychologists: From can have.
Aristotle to Freud (1963). In the 1980s, two Brit-
ish historians of psychology took issue with this
view (Richards, 1987; Smith, 1988). They argued Keywords
that it was inappropriate to consider the work of
someone like Aristotle as “psychology” since History; historical; presentism; celebratory; crit-
there was no special area of knowledge that ical; revisionism; progress
went under that name at the time that he lived.
The word itself is of relatively recent origin. The
earliest surviving manuscripts that contain it were History
written in the sixteenth century, but the term did
not become popular in Germany and France until The situation that was outlined up the start of this
the eighteenth century, and it was not adopted by piece where history of psychology is widely
the English language until well into the taught in psychology departments without being
H 874 History of Psychology

regarded as an area of specialization or research number of historians of science had moved


can be traced historically. Textbooks on the his- away from the traditional emphasis of their dis-
tory of psychology are almost as old is the disci- cipline on the natural sciences and had begun to
pline itself (e.g., Baldwin, 1913; Dessoir, 1912; take an interest in what are variously known as
Klemm, 1914). This situation might seem strange the behavioral, social, and human sciences.
but the field had historical antecedents in subjects Around the same time, an interdisciplinary orga-
like philosophy, education, medicine, and biol- nization, the International Society for History of
ogy. This is what Ebbinghaus considered to be its the Behavioral and Social Sciences, known as
long past, and the earliest textbooks were devoted Cheiron for short, was established. Another
to examining that past. The appearance of several important forum for research is journals and
American textbooks on the history of psychology a new journal, Journal of the History of the
in the 1920s would suggest that the field was Behavioral Sciences, was also established.
already being widely taught. The most famous A great deal of historical research involves the
of these textbooks is E. G. Boring’s A History of use of archival material, and yet another impor-
Experimental Psychology whose first edition was tant development around this time was the estab-
published in 1929 (Boring, 1929). Boring had lishment of the Archives for the History of
been a student of Edward B. Titchener at Cornell American Psychology at the University of
University, and he mentions in the preface that Akron in Ohio. Finally, a graduate program in
much of the book is based on Titchener’s lectures history and theory of psychology was established
on the history of psychology. Although Boring’s at the University of New Hampshire. As men-
textbook came to be regarded as the authoritative tioned earlier, this program no longer exists but
work on the history of psychology, it was only all of the other institutions that were established
one of three textbooks that were published in in the 1960s have continued to the present day.
1929. The others were by Gardner Murphy and It is important to avoid a triumphalist account
Walter Pillsbury (Murphy, 1929; Pillsbury, of these developments. As noted earlier, the num-
1929). This situation continued after World War ber of psychologists who consider history of psy-
II as new editions of the textbooks by Murphy and chology to be their main area of specialization
Boring were produced (Boring, 1950; Murphy, and research is very small, and it continues to be
1949). It should be noted that none of these widely regarded as an exclusively pedagogical
authors considered history of psychology to be field. Textbooks are generally the only kind of
their main area of specialization. Boring, for literature in this field to which many psycholo-
example, had over 500 publications in experi- gists are exposed, and some do not have even that
mental psychology and tended to do his historical degree of exposure.
work during his summer vacations (Young,
1966).
This situation began to change during the Critical Debates
1960s. Within the space of a few years, most of
the institutions that are associated with history of One of the consequences of these developments
psychology in the United States were established was that the kind of history that was being written
as the field was turned from a purely pedagogical began to change. This was to be expected in some
subject to an area of specialization and research. respects. In 1966, the historian of science, Robert
Conferences are a common way of presenting the Young, published a devastating critique of the
results of research, and the changes included field. His main complaint was its lack of profes-
the establishment of a new division devoted to sionalism and the absence of the scholarly stan-
history of psychology within the American Psy- dards that it brings (Young, 1966). Most
chological Association. This was Division 26. psychologists who publish on the history of psy-
Psychologists were not the only academics who chology were (and are) self-taught. Many of them
were taking an interest in this field. A small were not aware of the most basic standards of
History of Psychology 875 H
scholarship that exist among historians, such as and it found plenty to criticize. Scientific racism,
the need to consult primary sources, preferably in the traditional views of women that it endorsed,
the language in which they were written, instead and the view that homosexuality is a mental ill-
of relying on secondary accounts. This situation ness are some of the more obvious examples.
is less of a problem nowadays, largely due to the Given that the traditional accounts of the history
interaction of psychologists and historians in of psychology offered a different view of
organizations like Cheiron, but it was a major psychology’s past, it took issue with these
problem prior to the 1970s. accounts as well, and it generally did so on the
It was largely due to this situation that a great basis of a more scholarly approach involving the
deal of revisionist history began to appear. One use of primary sources.
of the authoritative sources on the history of The rise of critical history coincided with what
social psychology up to the point had been Gor- was widely considered to be psychology’s cen-
don Allport’s historical introduction to the tennial in 1979. It was largely due to the influence
Handbook of Social Psychology. It had origi- of Boring’s textbook that this view came about.
nally appeared in 1954 and was reprinted in the Boring had famously traced the history of psy-
second edition of 1968 (Allport, 1954, 1968). In chology back to the establishment of Wilhelm H
a famous paper of 1974, Franz Samelson took Wundt’s laboratory for experimental psychology
issue with Allport’s claim that Comte had “dis- at the University of Leipzig in 1879. Although
covered” social psychology and dismissed it as this account is still widely believed, no serious
“an origin myth” (Samelson, 1974). Yet another historian of psychology would endorse it. The
example of the genre was a paper by Benjamin history of psychology cannot be traced back to
Harris in the American Psychologist in which he a single person, a single place, or a single date. It
took issue with the standard accounts of J. B. was a gradual process that occurred in several
Watson’s work with Little Albert (Harris, 1979). countries over a long period of time. In spite of
The problem was always the same: successive this, the American Psychological Association
writers had relied on secondary sources without declared 1979 to be psychology’s centennial,
bothering to look at the original work. The result and the International Congress of Psychology,
was similar to the old children’s party game which takes place every 4 years, was held in
where a message is passed on along the line of Leipzig to mark the occasion.
people and is totally unrecognizable at the end. The widespread international interest in
Harris’ work did not go without criticism and, in Wundt that accompanied these events provided
his reply, he made a distinction that was to an opportunity for historians of psychology to
become popular in subsequent years. The dis- gain a readership for their work that was unusual
tinction was that of “ceremonial” and “critical” and possibly unique. Most of the accounts of
history. Traditional history of psychology had Wundt that were published around this time
been the former and he was producing the latter were revisionist in their aims. A central figure in
(Harris, 1980). these developments was Kurt Danziger (e.g.,
Although the terms “celebratory” and “criti- Danziger, 1979, 1980). In an interview that
cal” were more popular, the distinction remained. I conducted with him in 1994, he compared his
Traditional history of psychology was concerned experience to that of a subject in an Asch confor-
with celebrating the achievements of the past, and mity experiment in that the views he was reading
these were often considered to be the achieve- in Wundt’s original works bore little or no rela-
ments of “Great Men.” It also tended to embody tionship to the views that had been traditionally
the assumption of progress and, in doing so, attributed to him by Boring and others. At first, he
helped to reinforce the status quo. Critical history wondered if he was misunderstanding things, but
took a different approach. Rather than celebrating it eventually became clear that there really was
the achievements of the past, it took a critical a discrepancy between the primary and the sec-
stance towards much of psychology’s history ondary sources (Brock, 1995).
H 876 History of Psychology

Critical history continued to be popular Future Directions


throughout the 1980s, and several discussions of
what it involved were published during this Many historians of psychology have been
decade (e.g., Danziger, 1984; Woodward, concerned with what they see as a decline in the
1980). The changes that had taken place were field, and some have implicated the rise of critical
famously summarized by Laurel Furumoto in an history of psychology in this perceived decline
invited address that she gave to the American (e.g., Bhatt & Tonks, 2002; Lovett, 2006;
Psychological Association with the title “The Rappard, 1997). The argument runs along the
New History of Psychology” (Furumoto, 1989). lines of the expression “biting the hand that feeds
you.” It states that psychologists are unlikely to
continue supporting research and courses on the
International Relevance history of psychology if it only results in criticism
of the field. This argument, which is sometimes
The developments that took place in the United supported by anecdotes, assumes a degree of
States during the 1960s were replicated in Canada familiarity with contemporary historical scholar-
and Europe in the 1980s. It was during this ship among specialists in other areas of psychol-
decade that the professional organizations for ogy that may be unwarranted.
psychologists in Canada, the United Kingdom, The problems run much deeper than that. Psy-
and Germany established branches devoted to chology has found a niche for itself in society by
history of psychology. Their Spanish counterpart claiming to be useful in areas like medicine,
came slightly later in 1991. It was also during the education, industry, law enforcement, and the
1980s that the graduate programs in history and military. It is largely due to this perceived useful-
theory of psychology at York University in ness that psychologists are able to obtain financial
Canada and the University of Groningen in the support for their research from external sources,
Netherlands were established. Other important and this type of funding has become increasingly
developments during this decade include the important as governments have become less will-
establishment of a European equivalent of ing to support their universities with money
Cheiron, which was originally called “Cheiron- raised through taxation. History of psychology
Europe” but which is now known as “ESHHS” is not likely to succeed in such an environment
(European Society for the History of the Human since it is not usually considered to have any
Sciences), and the British-based journal, History practical uses.
of the Human Sciences. Yet another aspect of the situation is that psy-
Although there is not the same degree of orga- chologists have usually sought legitimacy for
nization in other countries, historians of psychol- their field by claiming that it is a science and
ogy can be found all over the world. history does not conform to this self-image. Psy-
Unsurprisingly, they tend to be more common chology is a microcosm of a wider situation
in countries like Argentina, Brazil, South Africa, where the natural sciences are well supported,
Japan, and Australia where psychology itself is while the humanities are relatively neglected
well established. The growth of psychology out- (e.g., Nussbaum, 2010). There is consequently
side its traditional heartland of North America a noticeable trend in the United States for history
and Europe is likely to continue, and this will of psychology to be pursued by professional his-
inevitably have an impact on its historiography. torians rather than psychologists. While there are
Indeed there is a current trend to move the history no universities where the psychology department
of psychology away from its exclusive focus on has a graduate program in history of psychology,
North America and Europe in order to include there are several universities where it can be
places like Asia, Africa, and Latin America (e.g., pursued within history of science.
Baker, 2011; Brock, 2006). This trend is likely to This development is hardly surprising since
continue in future years. history of psychology, as the name implies, is
History of Psychology 877 H
history, not psychology. By making common ground with history or philosophy is like marry-
cause with historians of science, psychologists ing into a lower social class, and it often happens
who specialize in history of psychology are in such cases that the family of the partner from
becoming increasingly isolated from their field. the higher social class will boycott the wedding.
One way of taking history into the heart of the
discipline is through historical psychology which
as the name implies is psychology, not history. References
Historical psychology is concerned not with the
theories and practices of psychologists but with Allport, G. W. (1954). The historical background of mod-
ern social psychology. In G. Lindzey (Ed.), Handbook
historical changes in what psychology considers
of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 3–56). Reading, MA:
to be its subject matter. A recent example of this Addison-Wesley.
kind of work is Kurt Danziger’s book on the Allport, G. W. (1968). The historical background of mod-
history of memory, Marking the Mind (Danziger, ern social psychology. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson
(Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 1,
2008). It is only partly about the history of psy-
pp. 1–80). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
chology since much of the book is concerned with Baldwin, J. M. (1913). A history of psychology: A sketch
historical periods when the modern discipline of and an interpretation (2 vols.). New York: Putnam. H
psychology did not exist. Danziger has also been Baker, D. B. (Ed.). (2011). Oxford handbook of the history
of psychology. New York: Oxford University Press.
explicit in his desire to see the growth of histor-
Barbu, Z. (1960). Problems of historical psychology. Lon-
ical psychology (e.g., Danziger, 2003). Ulti- don: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
mately, history is about historicization and Bhatt, G., & Tonks, R. (2002). What lies in the future of
psychologists who do historical research are the teaching of history of psychology? History and
Philosophy of Psychology Bulletin, 14(1), 2–9.
missing a golden opportunity to historicize the
Boring, E. G. (1929). A history of experimental psychol-
subject matter of their field. ogy. New York: Appleton Century.
Historical psychology is well established in Boring, E. G. (1950). A history of experimental psychol-
Continental Europe, but it is virtually unknown ogy (2nd ed.). New York: Appleton Century Crofts.
Brock, A. C. (1995). An interview with Kurt Danziger.
in the English-speaking world, and the small
History and Philosophy of Psychology Bulletin, 7,
amount of literature on this subject in English 10–22.
has been largely forgotten (e.g., Barbu, 1960; Brock, A. C. (Ed.). (2006). Internationalizing the history
van den Berg, 1961). A notable exception is of psychology. New York: New York University Press.
Danziger, K. (1979). The positivist repudiation of Wundt.
Kenneth Gergen’s paper, “Social Psychology as
Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 15,
History” (1973), which is one of the most widely 205–230.
cited papers in the history of psychology. It led to Danziger, K. (1980). Wundt’s psychological experiment
a great deal of discussion but very little research. in the light of his philosophy of science. Psychological
Research, 42, 109–122.
A subsequent volume on historical social psy-
Danziger, K. (1984). Towards a conceptual framework for
chology that Gergen coedited with his wife has a critical history of psychology. In H. Carpintero &
disappeared from the radar as well (Gergen & J. M. Peiro (Eds.), Essays in honour of J. Brozˇek
Gergen, 1984). (pp. 99–107). Valencia, Spain: Monografı́as de la
Revista de Historia de la Psicologı́a.
No one should be under any illusion about
Danziger, K. (2003). Prospects for a historical psychol-
how difficult it will be to promote this field. The ogy. History and Philosophy of Psychology Bulletin,
comments made earlier about the support for the 15(2), 4–10.
natural sciences and relative neglect of the Danziger, K. (2008). Marking the mind: A history of
memory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
humanities apply equally here as well. Psycholo-
Press.
gists have common interests with many subjects Dessoir, M. (1912). Outlines of a history of psychology.
but these common interests are differentially pur- New York: Macmillan.
sued. Exploring its common ground with neurol- Furumoto, L. (1989). The new history of psychology. The
G. Stanley Hall Lecture Series, 9, 9–34.
ogy or computer science is like marrying into
Gergen, K. J. (1973). Social psychology as history. Jour-
a higher social class, something that is almost nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 26,
universally approved. Exploring its common 309–320.
H 878 Holocaust

Gergen, K. J., & Gergen, M. M. (Eds.). (1984). Historical http://www.yorku.ca/gradpsyc/field5/moreinfo.html : Main-


social psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. tained by the Graduate Program in History & Theory of
Harris, B. (1979). Whatever happened to little Albert? Psychology at York University in Toronto, Canada.
American Psychologist, 34, 151–160. http://www.eshhs.eu/wordpress-3.3.1/wordpress/?page_
Harris, B. (1980). Ceremonial versus critical history of id¼2 : Maintained by ESHHS (European Society for
psychology. American Psychologist, 35, 218–219. the History of the Human Sciences).
Klemm, G. O. (1914). A history of psychology. New York:
Scribner.
Lapointe, F. H. (1968). Origin and evolution of the term
“psychology”. American Psychologist, 25, 640–646.
Lovett, B. J. (2006). The new history of psychology: Holocaust
A review and critique. History of Psychology, 9,
17–37.
Murphy, G. (1929). Historical introduction to modern Amy Fisher-Smith1 and Charles R. Sullivan2
1
psychology. New York: Harcourt Brace. Department of Psychology, University of
Murphy, G. (1949). Historical introduction to modern Dallas, Irving, TX, USA
psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Harcourt Brace. 2
Department of History, University of Dallas,
Nussbaum, M. C. (2010). Not for profit: Why democracy
needs the humanities. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Uni- Irving, TX, USA
versity Press.
Pillsbury, W. B. (1929). The history of psychology. New
York: Norton. Introduction
Richards, G. (1987). Of what is the history of psychology
a history? British Journal for the History of Science,
20, 201–211. Has the Holocaust left an indelible mark on con-
Samelson, F. (1974). History, origin myth and ideology: temporary consciousness? Or do we remain to be
‘Discovery’ of social psychology. Journal for the The- convinced of its significance? Is the Holocaust
ory of Social Behavior, 13, 217–231.
Smith, R. (1988). Does the history of psychology have unique? Or is it better understood within the
a subject? History of the Human Sciences, 1, 147–177. broader historical context of genocide? While
van den Berg, J. H. (1961). The changing nature of man. scholars across multiple disciplines have
New York: Norton. described the Holocaust as one of the central
Van Rappard, J. F. (1997). History of psychology turned
inside(r) out: A comment on Danziger. Theory & Psy- events in modernity, representing a “rupture and
chology, 7, 101–105. break with tradition” (Bernstein, 2002, p. 4) – an
Vidal, F. (2011). The sciences of the soul: The early event that challenges us to rethink our rendering
modern origins of psychology. Chicago: University of and understanding of the parameters of being
Chicago Press.
Watson, R. I. (1963). The great psychologists: From Aris- human – many educators have simultaneously
totle to Freud. Philadelphia: Lippincott. noted that we currently suffer from Holocaust
Woodward, W. R. (1980). Toward a critical historiogra- fatigue or overexposure to the educational and
phy of psychology. In J. Brožek & L. J. Pongratz moral lessons of the Holocaust such that the orig-
(Eds.), Historiography of modern psychology
(pp. 29–67). Toronto, ON: Hogrefe. inal rupture has lost its salience and force
Young, R. M. (1966). Scholarship and the history of the (Schweber, 2006). Hence, for contemporary cul-
behavioral sciences. History of Science, 2, 1–41. ture, the Holocaust may exist in a kind of paradox
or dialectic, wherein its significance is both
Online Resources simultaneously recognized and rejected.
Several professional organizations and university depart- For some, the Holocaust challenges our
ments have a “resources” or “links” page which leads preconceived assumptions of human responsibil-
to a large number of websites. Instead of giving these
websites individually, I would advise the reader to
ity, the structures of society and civilization, and
consult these pages. Some of the best are: even the nature of evil (Bernstein, 2002). The
http://historyofpsych.org/historyresources.html : Maintained Holocaust constitutes what Giorgio Agamben
by the Society for History of Psychology, aka American (2002) calls a “state of exception.” It raises both
Psychological Association Division 26.
https://www.uakron.edu/cheiron/links/ : Maintained by
the question of how extreme circumstances
Cheiron (International Society for History of the impact individuals and the question of how indi-
Behavioral and Social Sciences). viduals respond to these “limit” experiences. The

View publication stats

You might also like