Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 116

Autobiographical Memory Functioning Among Abused, Neglected and

Nonmaltreated Children:

The Overgeneral Memory Effect

by

Kristin Valentino

Submitted in Partial Fulfillment

O f the

Requirements for the Degree

Doctor of Philosophy

Supervised by

Professor Sheree L. Toth

Department of Clinical and Social Sciences in Psychology


The College
Arts and Sciences

University of Rochester
Rochester, NY

2007

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
UMI N um ber: 3279157

INFORMATION TO USERS

The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy
submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and
photographs, print bleed-through, substandard margins, and improper
alignment can adversely affect reproduction.
In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript
and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized
copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion.

UMI
UMI Microform 3279157
Copyright 2007 by ProQuest Information and Learning Company.
All rights reserved. This microform edition is protected against
unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code.

ProQuest Information and Learning Company


300 North Zeeb Road
P.O. Box 1346
Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Curriculum Vitae

The author was bom in Manhasset, New York on December 8, 1980. She

attended Georgetown University from 1998 to 2002, where she was awarded Phi Beta

Kappa honors as well as the Sebastian Brennikmeijer Medal for excellence in

psychology. She graduated Magna Cum Laude with a Bachelor of Arts degree in

2002. She came to the University of Rochester in the Fall of 2002 and began

graduate studies in Clinical Psychology. She pursued her research in Psychology

under the direction of Dante Cicchetti, Ph.D. and received the Master of Arts degree

from the University of Rochester in 2005. In December 2005 she received the Alfred

Baldwin Award for Excellence in Research from the Department of Clinical & Social

Sciences in Psychology. She has pursued her dissertation research under the

guidance of Sheree Toth, Ph.D. at Mt. Hope Family Center in the Department of

Clinical & Social Sciences in Psychology at the University of Rochester.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Acknowledgements

This research was supported by grants from the Office of Child Abuse and

Neglect to Dante Cicchetti, and the Spunk Fund, Inc. to Dante Cicchetti and Sheree

Toth.

I wish to thank Dante Cicchetti and Sheree Toth for their invaluable guidance

as advisors to this work. I forever will be grateful for their unmatched dedication to

my mentoring, both in challenging me to grow as a scientist and in providing me with

support to do so. I also express gratitude to Patrick Davies and Wendy Nilsen for

their thoughtful and insightful feedback as members of my dissertation committee. In

addition, I thank Fred Rogosch for generously sharing his expertise and enthusiasm

with me, Jody Todd Manly for her assistance with maltreatment classification, and

Janice Lomibao and Meghan Mitchell for their devoted coding. I give special thanks

to my parents, for giving me the courage to pursue my highest aspirations, and to my

husband, for helping me to enjoy every step of the way. Finally, I thank the all of my

colleagues at Mt Hope Family Center, and all of the children and families who

participated in this research.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Abstract

Research on the relation of trauma to memory has yet to determine how the

experience of trauma affects memory, if at all. Despite a proliferation of research

focusing on recall of traumatic experiences, much less attention has been paid to the

examination o f memory processes for nontraumatic stimuli with traumatized

individuals and to the conduct of research with maltreated children. Therefore, the

current investigation addresses significant gaps in the literature to ascertain whether

there are differences in the form and content of autobiographical memory recall as a

function of maltreatment, and to examine what roles self-system functioning and

psychopathology play in autobiographical memory processes among maltreated

children. Autobiographical memory for positive and negative nontraumatic events

was evaluated among abused, neglected, and nonmaltreated school-aged children

using the Autobiographical Memory Test (AMT; Williams & Broadbent, 1986). In

Part I of this investigation, a comprehensive analysis of children’s autobiographical

memory performance was conducted; this included assessment of children’s ability to

recall specific autobiographical memories, the number of prompts elicited from the

interviewer, children’s response latency and the length of their memories. Positive

and negative self- and maternal- representations contained within children’s memory

narratives were also examined. Part II focused on overgeneral memory, a pattern of

memory retrieval characterized by difficulty in recollecting specific autobiographical

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
V

memories, and evaluated children’s self-representations as well as children’s

depressive and dissociative symptomatology in relation to children’s memory.

Results revealed that abused children recalled more overgeneral memories and

elicited more prompts from the interviewer to retrieve specific memories than did the

neglected and the nonmaltreated children. Abused children’s memory narratives also

contained more negative self-representations than did those of the nonmaltreated

children. Negative self-representations were significantly related to overgeneral

memory, but did not mediate the relation between abuse and overgeneral memory.

Abused children demonstrated more depressive and dissociative symptoms than did

the nonmaltreated children; however, depressive and dissociative symptoms did not

mediate the relation of abuse to overgeneral memory. The meaning of these findings

for models of memory and for the development of overgenerality is emphasized.

Moreover, the clinical implications of the current research are discussed.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table of Contents

Introduction 1
Chapter 1 Background & Significance 3
1.1 Memory & Trauma 3
1.2 Development of Autobiographical Memory 11
1.3 Self Development and Maltreatment 14
1.4 Depression and Autobiographical Memory 17
1.5 Dissociation and Autobiographical Memory 20
Chapter 2 Hypotheses 22
Chapter 3 Research Designand Methods 25
3.1 Participants 25
3.2 Maltreatment Determinations 27
3.3 Procedure 29
3.4 Measures 30
3.5 Coding 33
Chapter 4 Results 36
4.1 P arti 36
4.2 Part II 42
Chapter 5 Discussion 46
5.1 Aspects of Autobiographical Memory 46
5.1.1 Overgeneral Memory 46
5.1.2 Interviewer Prompts 51
5.1.3 Response Latency 52
5.1.4 Length o f M em ory 53

5.1.5 Gender Effects 55


5.1.6 V alence Effects 55
5.1.7. Age Effects 58

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
5.2 Content of Memories: Self & Maternal 60
Representations
5.3 Relationship Effects: Controllingness 63
& Warmth
5.4 Mediation of Overgeneral Memory 65
5.4.1 Affective Representations 65
5.4.2 Depression & Dissociation 68
5.5 Limitations and Future Research 70
5.6 Clinical Implications 73
References 77
Appendix 107

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
List of Tables

Table 1 Demographic Characteristic Means and Standard 99


Deviations for Abused, Neglected and Nonmaltreated
Children

Table 2 Reliability Analyses 100

Table 3 Control/Opposition Scale 101

Table 4 Global Warmth/Friendliness of Interaction 102

Table 5 Correlation Matrix: Gender & Cognitive Performance 103


with Dependent Variables

Table 6 Means and Standard Deviations by Maltreatment Subtype 104


Group, Controlling for Gender and PPVT-III Standard
Score

Table 7 Mediation of the Relation of Abuse to Overgeneral 105


Memory: Negative Self- Representations

Table 8 Mediation of the Relation of Abuse to Overgeneral 106


Memory: Depressive Symptoms

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
1

Introduction

Fueled by a lack of consensus on how the experience of trauma may exert its

effect on memory, if at all, researchers continue to debate about the relation of trauma

to memory processes (Howe, Cicchetti, Toth & Cerrito, 2004; Howe, Toth, &

Cicchetti, 2006). Some contend that traumatic experiences, as distinctive and

emotionally salient events, enhance memory for the trauma (e.g., Howe, Courage &

Peterson, 1994,1995; Cicchetti & Toth, 1998). Others highlight evidence of brain

structure alteration in response to prolonged stress (e.g., Bremner & Narayan, 1998;

DeBellis, Keshavan, Casey, Clark, Giedd, Boring, et al., 1999; Sapolsky, 1992) and

impaired memory performance among adults who were traumatized as children (e.g.,

Henderson, Hargreaves, Gregory & Williams, 2002) to suggest that trauma may

adversely affect memory. Clearly, this is a critical area o f research because of the

immense clinical and forensic implications that would be associated with evidence of

memory deficiencies among maltreated children.

Most research to date has focused on recall of the traumatic event, whereas

less attention has been paid to the examination of memory processes for nontraumatic

stimuli. Furthermore, little is known about basic memory functioning among

maltreated children (Eisen, Qin, Goodman & Davis, 2002; Howe et al., 2004).

Although a small body o f literature is developing with regard to semantic memory for

neutral stim uli, and neuropsychological functioning am ong maltreated children, far

less research exists on episodic, autobiographical memory for nontraumatic events.

Furthermore, the limited research that has been conducted on maltreated children’s

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
2

autobiographical memory has failed to differentiate maltreatment effects from

psychopathology such as depression (Johnson, Greenhoot, Glisky & McCloskey,

2005).

In this investigation, we sought to address significant gaps in the literature on

the relation between trauma and autobiographical memory among maltreated

children. First, the literature on the association between trauma and memory are

reviewed, contrasting the adult literature with findings from research on children.

This review examines both direct and indirect effects of trauma on memory. Next, an

overview of the development of autobiographical memory is presented. After

highlighting the influence of self- development upon autobiographical memory, the

literature on self- development among maltreated children is reviewed. This section

underscores maltreated children’s increased risk for the development of self- related

psychopathologies, such as depression and dissociation. Finally, the literature on the

relation of depression and of dissociation to autobiographical memory is presented.

The goals of the present investigation are:

(1) To ascertain whether there are differences in the form and content

of autobiographical memory recall as a function of maltreatment;

(2) To examine what roles self-system functioning and

psychopathology play in autobiographical memory processes

among maltreated children.

These specific aims are addressed through the conduct of an investigation

with school-aged maltreated and nonmaltreated children. The first part of the study

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
3

provides a comprehensive assessment of autobiographical memory processes among

abused, neglected and nonmaltreated children. The second part of the study focuses

on one aspect o f autobiographical memory, the overgeneral memory effect, and

examines the relation of self- representations, as well as dissociative and depressive

symptomatology, to this phenomenon.

Chapter 1 Background and Significance

1.1 Memory and Trauma

In order to clarify the relationship between trauma and memory, researchers

have operationalized trauma and stress in various ways. Across studies, the defined

trauma has ranged from laboratory-controlled stressful experiences with

nonmaltreated children at one extreme, to research among children with

maltreatment- related PTSD at the other. Utilizing this first approach of assessing

memory for analog traumas, evidence supports the notion that increased levels of

stress generally lead to better memory for the event (for review see Howe 1998,

2000). Research findings have converged across various analog stressors such as

expected medical procedures (e.g., Brack, Ceci, Francouer, & Barr, 1995; Goodman,

Hirschman, Hepps & Rudy, 1991; Goodman & Quas, 1997), and unanticipated

emergency room visits (e.g., Howe et al., 1994,1995), to demonstrate that stress has a

positive impact on children’s ability to recall the central elements of the event.

In contrast, some researchers argue that stress impairs children’s memories

and increases children’s susceptibility to suggestion leading to recall of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
4

misinformation (e.g., Bugental, Blue, Cortez, Fleck, & Rodriquez, 1992; Peters,

1987; but see Merritt, Omstein & Spicker, 1994). However, closer examination of

these studies reveals that reports of memory deficiencies are limited to the recall of

information that is peripheral to the central event. This pattern of findings is

consistent with Christianson’s (1992) theory that during stressful events, children’s

attention becomes hyper-focused on the main stressor to the detriment of attention to

more peripheral information. Therefore, memory for the traumatic event may be

facilitated, whereas memory for peripheral information may be impeded.

(Christianson, 1992; Goodman et al., 1991.)

However, despite the high level of emotionality and stress that may

accompany recollections of unanticipated emergency room procedures (Howe, et al.,

1994, 1995) or hospital experiences that involve genital touching (e.g., Goodman &

Quas, 1997), it is unclear whether or not the intensity of these emotions is comparable

to that which accompanies the experience of physical or sexual abuse, or the

experience of chronic neglect. Thus, although these analog-trauma studies provide

important information regarding the relationship between stress and memory, further

research is needed among populations of traumatized children.

Unfortunately, little research exists on how maltreatment affects memory,

particularly among children (Eisen et al., 2002). In fact, much of our knowledge is

derived from studies of memory functioning among adults who report abuse histories,

where findings on memory performance have been inconsistent. On the one hand,

evidence that maltreated adults experience hyperarousal that may generalize beyond

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
5

stressful situations (Carlson, Furby, Armstrong & Shales, 1997) provides support for

the notion that maltreatment may lead to enhancement in memory performance. On

the other, examinations o f autobiographical memory and general cognitive processing

among adult women suggest that the experience of child maltreatment is associated

with poorer memory for autobiographical events (Henderson et al., 2002). In addition

to the problematic applicability of research conducted among adults to children, these

studies’ reliance on retrospective reports of abuse weakens the strength of their

conclusions.

One major obstacle to conducting research on memory for trauma is the

inherent difficulty in verifying the accuracy of memory reports of trauma, especially

when they are retrospectively recalled years after the traumatic event occurred. In

response to this problem, some recent studies have emerged in which children who

recalled recent traumatic experiences were then tracked longitudinally to assess for

memory o f the trauma at a later point in time (Alexander, Quas, Goodman, Ghetti,

Edelstein, Redlich, et al., 2005; Eisen, Goodman, Qin & Davis, 2005; Greenhoot,

McCloskey & Glisky, 2005). For example, in a longitudinal study o f long-term

memory for child sexual abuse (defined as memory for abuse that occurred at least 12

years ago), CSA victims who had more Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD)

symptoms remembered the abuse particularly well, even when reporting on abuse that

occurred up to 21 years earlier (Alexander et al., 2005). Another longitudinal follow

up study of children who experienced an invasive hospital procedure revealed that

being a victim o f childhood sexual abuse (CSA) is associated with better memory for

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
6

the event (Eisen et al., 2005). In sum, research with maltreated children has not

supported deficiencies in memory performance for recall of the traumatic event.

Despite the lack of an association between maltreatment and memory deficits

uncovered by the literature reviewed thus far, it should be noted that this research has

all focused on memory for the traumatic event itself. However, there are reasons to

suspect that maltreatment might have a more generalized impact on basic memory

functioning. For example, child maltreatment may impact memory processes

because o f physiological changes to brain regions related to memory processes (e.g.,

Bremner, Krystal, Southwick & Chamey, 1995; Cicchetti & Rogosch, 2001a, 2001b).

Until recently, investigations on basic memory processes (i.e., encoding,

storage and retrieval of neutral, nontraumatic stimuli) were virtually nonexistent. In

fact, to date very few investigations of basic memory among maltreated and

nonmaltreated children have been conducted (Howe et al., 2004; Porter, Lawson &

Bigler, 2005). In a foundational study, Howe and colleagues (2004) utilized Deese-

Roediger-McDermott (DRM) lists to assess true and false memory processes among

children 5-12 years o f age. Recall and recognition measures revealed no differences

between maltreated and nonmaltreated children’s semantic memory functioning for

neutral stimuli. Similarly, a recent examination o f the neurobehavioral sequelae of

child sexual abuse found no significant differences in memory function between

sexually abused and nonabused children (Porter et al., 2005), despite elevations in

psychopathology and diminished performance on tests of attention and executive

function for the abused group. Research conducted with children who had

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
7

maltreatment-related PTSD is consistent with these findings, such that maltreated

children with PTSD performed more poorly on measures of attention and abstract

reasoning/executive function, whereas memory problems were not identified (Beers

& De Beilis, 2002, but see Moradi, Taghavi, Doost, Yule, & Dalgliesh, 1999).

However, a trend in the data of Beers and DeBellis suggests that maltreated children

with PTSD may have difficulties in long-term memory for verbal information.

Because this investigation lacks a control group of maltreated children who did not

manifest symptoms o f PTSD, it is unclear whether any of these effects can be

attributed to maltreatment, PTSD, or both.

Although the conduct of research on memory for neutral stimuli has been a

necessary first step towards a comprehensive analysis of basic memory functioning

among maltreated children, there is a missing link in the memory literature between

semantic memory for neutral information and autobiographical memories recalled in

response to emotion-evoking prompts. Specifically, it remains unclear how

emotionally valenced and nontraumatic autobiographical memory functions among

maltreated children. It is critical to develop a better understanding of

autobiographical memory processes in maltreated children, especially in light of

findings that have revealed that measures of nontraumatic autobiographical memory

are positively related to memory for the trauma (Greenhoot et al., 2005).

Autobiographical m em ory research am ong adults w ho report childhood

trauma suggests that childhood maltreatment may be associated with difficulty in

recollecting specific autobiographical memories in response to cue words, a

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
8

phenomenon known as the overgeneral memory effect (Brittlebank, Scott, Williams

& Ferrier, 1993; Henderson et al., 2002; Kuyken & Brewin, 1995). Moreover, some

studies have demonstrated that child trauma is associated with difficulty in specific

memory retrieval in adulthood over and above the effects of depression (de Decker,

Hermans, Raes, & Eelen, 2003; Henderson et al., 2002; Kuyken & Brewin, 1995). In

contrast, other studies have demonstrated a null effect of maltreatment on

autobiographical memory retrieval (Orbach, Lamb, Sternberg, Williams, & Dawud-

Noursi, 2001; Wessel, Meeren, Peeters, Amtz, & Merchelbach, 2001). One limitation

of the aforementioned research is its reliance on retrospective reporting, or self-

reporting of child abuse in the absence of alternate sources of information to

corroborate these experiences. Thus, in order to better understand the impact of

childhood trauma on autobiographical memory, research conducted among samples

of maltreated children is necessary.

Researchers have just begun to investigate autobiographical memory

functioning for non-trauma related stimuli with maltreated children and adolescents

(e.g., deDecker et al., 2003; Johnson, et al., 2005). For example Johnson and

colleagues (2005) examined the relative effects of self-reported childhood sexual

abuse, current exposure to family violence, and depressive symptomatology on

autobiographical memories among adolescents. Results indicated that recent family

violence w as associated with overgeneral m em ories, shorter m em ories and lower

rates of negative memories, while sexual abuse in childhood was related to the need

for an increased number o f prompts to elicit memories. Overall, overgeneral memory

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
9

problems were more consistently related to current circumstances (e.g., domestic

violence) than to self- reported childhood abuse history. Closer examination of

adolescents’ use of internal state language during autobiographical recall indicated

that adolescents with child abuse histories used fewer emotion terms than did any

other teens, but only for memories related to conflict or punishment (Greenhoot,

Johnson, & McClosky, in press). Taken in tandem, these two studies suggest that

sexually abused children and adolescents display an avoidance of negative emotion in

autobiographical memory recall. Depression in this sample was also related to

overgeneral autobiographical memory recall; however, it was unclear whether

depression was a result, cause, or correlate of this pattern of memory recall.

DeDecker and colleagues (2003) also investigated the relationship between

autobiographical memory specificity and trauma among adolescents. Results showed

that higher levels of trauma were associated with reduced autobiographical memory

specificity. Because this study was conducted among adolescent inpatients and relied

on self-reported accounts of trauma, it again remains uncertain how psychopathology

relates to autobiographical memory. In summarizing the research on

autobiographical memory among maltreated children, it is unclear how basic memory

processes, self- system processes (e.g., processes related to the development of a

coherent self) and/or psychopathology might account for overgeneral memory effects.

The literature review ed so far coheres to suggest that maltreatment m ay not

have a direct effect on children’s memory. It is possible, however, that some kinds of

psychopathology and coping that are associated with experiences of maltreatment

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
10

may be related to alterations in memory processes (Goodman, 2005). For example,

some theorists contend that maltreatment may lead to poor memory as a result of

stress and dissociation (e.g., Putnam, 1997). This has been supported by research

among adults that has demonstrated a positive association between dissociation and

suggestibility (e.g., Hyman & Billings, 1998). In addition, several studies have

demonstrated a link between depression and difficulty in the retrieval of specific

autobiographical memories (e.g., Williams & Broadbent, 1986; Williams & Scott,

1988) (see sections on dissociation and depression for further elaboration). Elevated

rates of depressive symptomatology have been consistently found among maltreated

children in comparison to nonmaltreated children (Kim & Cicchetti, 2004; Sternberg,

Lamb, Greenbaum, Cicchetti, Dawud, Cortes et al., 1993; Toth & Cicchetti, 1996;

Toth, Manly, & Cicchetti 1992). Therefore, maltreatment may have an indirect effect

on autobiographical memory processes via dissociation and/or depression.

Overall, research on the relation between trauma and memory to date has not

consistently supported deficiencies in memory performance among maltreated

children, despite contrary evidence among adults. However, the majority of studies

with children have focused on memory for the traumatic event rather than on basic

memory performance for nontraumatic stimuli. In particular, there is a paucity of

research on autobiographical memory processes among maltreated children.

Although the literature does not tend to support a direct relationship betw een

maltreatment and memory performance, maltreated children are at increased risk for

psychopathology, such as depression, as well as for the development of dissociation,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
11

each of which has been associated with alterations in memory function. In this

investigation, several aspects of autobiographical memory performance, including

measures of form and content, will be analyzed among abused, neglected and

nonmaltreated children. In particular, this investigation will focus on examining

overgenerality in autobiographical memory retrieval and the mechanisms that might

underlie this phenomenon. Children’s self-representations as well as depressive and

dissociative symptomatology will be assessed and examined in relation to

maltreatment in predicting autobiographical memory performance.

1.2 Development o f Autobiographical Memory

Autobiographical memory is defined as an explicit memory o f an event that

occurred in a specific time and place in one’s personal past (Nelson & Fivush, 2004).

Several component factors have been associated with the emergence and development

of autobiographical memory, including basic memory systems and representation of

the self, as well as the acquisition of complex spoken or signed language, narrative

comprehension and production, temporal understanding, style of parent talk, and

overall scaffolding of narratives. A key development is the emergence of the self,

which can be used as a schema to interpret and organize incoming information

(Greenwald & Banji, 1989; Klein & Kihlstrom, 1986). The ability to have a sense of

recollection of the self at a particular time in the past distinguishes autobiographical

m emory from other memory processes such as semantic memory recall (Siegel,

2001 ).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
12

During the second year of life, children develop a sense of the physical world,

time and sequence, and o f the self, all of which form the foundation of

autobiographical memory (Wheeler, Stuss, & Tulving, 1997). Autobiographical

memory begins as early as 18-24 months of age (see Howe, 2000, 2004a; Howe &

Courage, 1993, 1997; Howe, Courage & Edison, 2003), when children acquire a

sense of self that organizes memories as autobiographical. Once children can use the

self to organize events, autobiographical memory emerges and children’s memories

of events that happened to them become more stable and durable (Howe & Courage,

1993,1997).

Beyond its influence on the encoding and organization of memory, recent

models of autobiographical memory emphasize the self as central to the process of

retrieval. For example, Conway and Pleydell-Pearce’s self- memory model (2000)

highlights the relationship between an individual’s sense of self and autobiographical

memory, particularly as it relates to the guidance of goal-directed activity. In this

model, autobiographical memories are stored in three hierarchical levels of

representation: event specific memories, general event descriptions, and lifetime

periods. Consistent with earlier models of autobiographical memory (e.g., Burgess &

Shallice, 1996), retrieval is conceptualized as a top down process whereby general

descriptions are activated early and become index points for the retrieval of the

specific event. C onw ay and Pleydell-Pearce (2000) add that retrieval requires

supervisory executive processes to progressively refine the pattern of activation and

to inhibit irrelevant information. In addition, this model emphasizes that many of the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
13

intermediate descriptions used to aid in the retrieval of autobiographical memory are

conceptually based self-representations, including personal semantic memories, self-

referential attributes and/or abstracted representations of generic aspects of the self

and experience (Conway et al., 2004).

Although this predominance of conceptual self-relevant information in the

early stages o f memory search typically facilitates autobiographical memory retrieval,

there is some evidence that individuals who possess highly activated or elaborated

repertoires of emotion-related self-representations, and individuals prone to

rumination (repetitive and passive thinking about one’s problems and the possible

causes and consequences of these problems; Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991) are at risk for

overgeneral autobiographical memory retrieval (Williams, Crane, Hermans, Raes,

Watkins, & Dalgliesh, 2007). For example, Williams and colleagues (2007) posit

that individuals who are depressed represent a group in whom emotion-related

conceptual self-representations are in a heightened state of activation. Rather than

aiding the memory search, this may result in individuals becoming “captured” at the

level of negative self-representation, resulting in difficulties in progressing to the

retrieval of specific autobiographical memories. Because children who have been

maltreated are at risk for a number of impairments in self-system functioning,

including the development o f negative self- representations and depression, the

centrality o f self-representations to the retrieval o f autobiographical m em ory is

particularly relevant to our understanding of this process among maltreated children.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
14

1.3 Self Development and Maltreatment

Deviations in normative self- system development and functioning are among

the most well documented sequelae of child maltreatment (see Cicchetti & Valentino,

2006 for review). Developmentally, the earliest evidence for self-system deviation

among maltreated children emerges in response to viewing the self at 18 months of

age. Despite the fact that maltreatment does not appear to impede the timing or

emergence o f visual self-recognition, maltreated toddlers are more likely to show

negative emotion when viewing the self than are nonmaltreated toddlers (Schneider-

Rosen & Cicchetti, 1991). Therefore, despite evidence that the timing or emergence

o f a cognitive self may be highly canalized (see Howe, Toth, & Cicchetti, 2006 for

review), environmental factors are influential in shaping the development of the self.

Moving into late toddlerhood, the ability to talk about the self, label emotions

and discuss feelings o f self and other emerges, allowing for observations of more

overt expressions o f self- representation. Self-other differentiation can be reflected in

children’s self-descriptions, use of personal pronouns and active agency during

symbolic play (Beeghly & Cicchetti, 1994; Kagan, 1981). During observations of

mother-child play, delays in maltreated children’s self- systems have been noted

through the analysis o f verbal communicative abilities and internal state language.

Specifically, maltreated children talk less about themselves and about their internal

states than do nonmaltreated children, show less differentiation in their attributional

focus, and are more context -bound in their use of internal state language (Beeghly &

Cicchetti, 1994; Coster, Beeghly, Gersten & Cicchetti, 1989).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
15

Examination o f maltreated preschool children’s narratives reveals that

maltreated preschoolers have more negative representations of the self and of their

caregivers than do nonmaltreated preschoolers (Toth, Cicchetti, Macfie & Emde,

1997; Toth, Cicchetti, Macfie, Maughan & Vanmeenan, 2000). Specifically, the

narratives o f children who had been physically abused demonstrated the most

negative maternal representations and high levels of negative self- representations. In

contrast children who had been neglected evidenced the lowest levels of positive self-

representations (Toth et al., 1997). Indeed, maltreated children may not have

received the parental support necessary to develop a sense of the self as worthy,

leading to a sense of inner badness (Harter, 1998). In addition to high levels of

negative representations of the self and of parents, maltreated preschool children also

display evidence o f grandiose self- representations (Toth et al., 2000). Moreover,

Toth and colleagues found that maltreated preschool children portrayed more

grandiose self- representations over a one- year interval, whereas nonmaltreated

children portrayed fewer.

Research conducted with school age maltreated children provides further

evidence for self- system deviation. Maltreated children are consistently rated by

teachers as having low self- esteem, and as having less positive self- concepts than do

nonmaltreated children (Bolger, Patterson, & Kupersmidt, 1998; Cicchetti &

R ogosch, 1997; Egeland, Sroufe & Erickson, 1983). There also is evidence that

younger maltreated children overestimate their own sense of competence and peer

acceptance (Black, Dubowitz, & Harrington, 1994; Vondra et al., 1989) and have

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
16

inflated levels of perceived self- efficacy in conflictual peer interactions compared to

young nonmaltreated children (Kim & Cicchetti, 2003). Both of these patterns of

behavior are consistent with the juxtaposition of negative and grandiose self­

representations noted in maltreated preschool children’s narratives (Toth et al., 2000).

Some have suggested that younger maltreated children may be engaging in defensive

processing to increase their sense of personal control and competence (Vondra et al.,

1989). However, as children get older, this overly positive sense of self is no longer

present; instead, maltreated children perceive themselves as being less competent

than do nonmaltreated children (Kim & Cicchetti, 2003; Vondra, Barnett & Cicchetti,

1989). Corroboration of children’s self- evaluations with teachers’ ratings reveals

that children’s perceptions may in fact become more accurate and realistic as they

grow older and view themselves in a more negative light. In addition, prospective

longitudinal research with maltreated children demonstrates that abused and

neglected children are at elevated risk for the development of major depressive

disorders in young adulthood (Widom, DuMont & Czaja, 2007.)

Finally, the development of more complex disturbances in self- functioning

may emerge as maltreated children enter adolescence. For example, research

supports a connection between affective splitting of self- representations and chronic

sexual abuse. Furthermore, the risk for the development of dissociation is high

among maltreated children (M acfie, Cicchetti & Toth, 2001a, 2001b; O gawa, Sroufe,

Weinfield, Carlson, & Egeland, 1997, Putnam, 2000).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
17

Given the extensive theoretical and empirical support for self- system

impairments among maltreated children, and the critical role that the self plays in the

organization o f memory, it is important to assess how the experience of maltreatment

might alter autobiographical memory for nontraumatic information. In particular, the

affective valence o f maltreated children’s self- representations may influence the

encoding and retrieval of positive and negative life events. Furthermore, child

maltreatment increases the risk for the development o f depression and dissociation,

each of which are associated with alterations in autobiographical memory.

1.4 Depression and Autobiographical Memory

Over the past several decades, research with adults has supported a clear

association between depression and overgeneral memory, defined as difficulty in

retrieving specific autobiographical memories (e.g., Brewin, Watson, McCarthy,

Hyman, & Dayson, 1998; Brittlebank et al., 1993; Burnside, Startup, Rollinson, &

Hill, 2004; Wessel et al., 2001; Williams & Broadbent, 1986; Williams & Scott

1988). In this literature, autobiographical memories are typically assessed through a

test where participants are asked to generate/recall specific personal memories in

response to cue words. When compared to control participants, depressed or suicidal

adults consistently demonstrate a tendency to recall overgeneral memories, that is,

memories referring to repeated events (e.g., every Sunday when I go to church...”),

or referring to events that lasted for a longer period o f tim e (e.g., the tim e I lived in

London), rather than recalling specific episodes in their personal past.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
18

Depressed or suicidal adults also demonstrate a slower response latency in

comparison to nondepressed adults, which further reflects difficulty in the retrieval of

specific memories (Brittlebank et al., 1993; Williams & Broadbent, 1986).

Additionally, differences in autobiographical memory retrieval in response to positive

and negative cue words have been noted among depressed adults. For example,

Williams & Scott (1988) found that depressed patients, unlike controls, took longer to

respond to the positive cue words than they did to the negative cues. Depressed

patients were also less specific in their memories, particularly in response to positive

cues.

The overgeneral memory effect has been noted among depressed adolescents

as well (Orbach et al., 2001; Park, Goodyear & Teasdale, 2002). For example Park

and colleagues (2002) reported that adolescents with clinical depression were more

likely to recall categoric, rather than specific memories. Moreover, categoric

responses to negative cue words were positively correlated with measures of

depression severity.

Beyond the association between depression and difficulty in retrieving

specific autobiographical memories, overgeneral memory has significant clinical

implications. Specifically, there is empirical evidence that demonstrates the extent to

which an individual recalls overgeneral autobiographical memories, particularly in

response to positive cues, is predictive o f the severity o f future depressive episodes

(Brittlebank et al., 1993). Notably, neither the level of dysfunctional attitudes nor the

severity of depressive symptoms at baseline was predictive of later depression in this

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
19

investigation. Moreover, the predictive quality of overgeneral memory to future

depression has been replicated several times among depressed samples. For example

Dalgleish, Spinks, Yiend, & Kuyken (2001) found that the number o f general

memories generated in response to positive cue words predicted the severity of

depression at followup. Similarly, Peeters, Wessel, Merchelbach & Boon-Vermeenen

(2001) demonstrated that the number of specific memories recalled in response to

negative cues predicts the severity of depression at follow-up assessment. In addition

to the relationship between overgeneral memory and clinical depression, overgeneral

memory also has been associated with subsequent increases in subclinical depressive

symptomatology (Gibbs & Rude, 2004). Furthermore, overgeneral memory has been

associated with impaired problem solving (e.g. Goddard, Dritschel & Burton, 1996;

1997, Raes, Hermans, Williams, Demyttenaere, et al., 2005) such that specificity of

memory moderates the effect o f negative mood disturbance on problem-solving

performance (Williams, Bamhofer, Crane, & Beck, 2005).

It should be noted that several studies of the overgeneral memory effect and

depression have been conducted among depressed individuals who have experienced

trauma. Some studies have shown that childhood trauma is related to difficulty in

retrieving specific autobiographical memories, even after controlling for levels of

depression (e.g., deDecker et al., 2003; Henderson et al., 2002; Kuyken & Brewin,

1995). H ow ever, other studies have failed to find a direct association betw een

childhood trauma and overgeneral autobiographical memory (Orbach et al., 2001;

Wessel et al., 2001) and have suggested that the effect o f child trauma on

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
20

autobiographical memory may be mediated by depression. Perhaps contributing to

the lack of consistency among studies, all but one of these investigations (Orbach et

al., 2001) were conducted among adults and relied on retrospective reports of child

abuse to assess trauma. This raises questions about the accuracy of their reports in

the absence of additional, more objective measures of trauma (Widom, 1989).

Nonetheless, the current literature supports a clear association between depression

and difficulty in retrieving specific autobiographical memories; it remains unclear,

however, how, if at all, child maltreatment is associated with the development of

overgeneral memories.

1.5 Dissociation and Autobiographical Memory

Dissociation may be conceptualized as the opposite of an integrated self

(Ogawa et al., 1997), and refers to a disruption in the normal integration of memories,

perception and identity (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Dissociation has

been described as both a failure of information processing in the face of trauma

(Bower & Sivers, 1998) and as a psychological mechanism that aids an individual’s

ability to cope with extreme stressors (see Cordon, Pipe, Say fan, Melinder &

Goodman, 2004 for review). In addition, dissociation has been conceptualized as a

combination o f both failed information processing and defensive processes (Liotti,

1999), as dissociation may become an automatic and unconscious response to stress

that m ay generalize over tim e (Post, W eiss, Li, Smith, Zhang, X ing, et al., 1998).

With respect to memory, some individuals experience temporary dissociative

states during and immediately after the occurrence of a traumatic event (e.g., Dancu,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
21

Riggs, Hearst-Ikeda, Shoyer & Foa, 1996), which may lead to isolated memories for

the event. If these memories are not integrated into the self, then the memory for the

trauma will be less accessible to conscious recollection (Marmar, Weiss, Metzler &

Delucchi, 1996). In adults, a number of memory impairments have been linked with

dissociation, including episodes of time loss, recall delays for abuse-related

information, total memory loss for traumatic events, and gaps in autobiographical

memory (Goodman, Bottoms, Rudy, Davis, & Schwartz-Kenny, 2001; Henderson et

al., 2002; Putnam, 1997, 2000).

Despite research indicating a connection between dissociation and poor

memory for the trauma among adults, research among children has failed to support

such an association. For example, studies of maltreated children have shown that

higher dissociation scores are related to more detailed memories of abuse (Eisen et

al., 2002), as well as to the expression of more negative emotion when asked about

the abuse (Sayfan, Mitchell, Goodman, Eisen, Qin, & Davis, 2002). However, as Foa

and Riggs (1994) point out, there may be important differences between the manner

in which maltreated children remember trauma-related and nontrauma-related

information (see also Toth & Cicchetti, 1998). Thus, it is important to assess the role

of dissociation in maltreated children’s memories for non-trauma related stimuli.

Moreover, according to Howe and Courage (1997), when the self becomes a

viable cognitive entity w ith recognizable features, the encoding o f such features into a

functional memory trace becomes possible. However, events can remain

depersonalized if features of the self are not sampled and encoded. Therefore,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
22

memory may be affected if the self is not well integrated, without a coherent set of

features describing the self. This possibility is particularly relevant to child

maltreatment, where the risk for the development of dissociation is high (Macfie et

al., 2001a, 2001b; Ogawa, et al., 1997; Putnam, 2000).

Chapter 2 Hypotheses

Based upon the literature reviewed and in order to address significant gaps in

our extant knowledge o f autobiographical memory functioning among maltreated

children, this investigation aimed to examine the following:

P a r ti

(1) To provide a comprehensive analysis of abused and neglected children’s

autobiographical memories in comparison to nonmaltreated children, the

present study examined additional elements of autobiographical memory

responses, including latency to respond, number o f prompts (for specific

memories) elicited from the experimenter, and the length of memories

generated. This investigation extends the extant literature by focusing on

additional subgroups of maltreated children (physical abuse and neglect), and

by including more rigorous methods for assessing child maltreatment

experiences. It was expected that abused children in particular would (a)

demonstrate more overgeneral m em ories, (b) need increased prompting for

specific memories, (c) take longer to verbally respond, and (d) provide less

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
23

detailed (shorter) autobiographical memory responses in comparison to

nonmaltreated children.

(2) To extend prior research by including children of a younger age range (7-13),

this investigation is able to address developmental influences upon

autobiographical memory. We anticipated that the older children would

demonstrate more overgeneral memories than will the younger children

(Johnson et al., 2005; Rubin 1986).

(3) To examine the content o f children’s autobiographical memories this

investigation assessed the affective valence of children’s self- and caregiver-

representations. We expected that the memories of abused children would

contain the most negative self- representations, whereas the memories of

neglected children would contain the least positive self- representations. A

similar pattern was expected for caregiver-representations.

(4) To determine whether children’s levels of controllingness and relationships

with the examiner differ as a function of maltreatment experiences. In light

of evidence that maltreated children are more controlling and have less

positive relationships with the examiner during similar paradigms in which

children are asked to narrate responses (e.g., Toth, et al., 1997), we expected

that both the abused and neglected children would demonstrate more

controlling behaviors and would exhibit less positive relationships with the

examiner.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
24

Part II

(5) To evaluate the role of self- representations in children’s memory, self-

representations were evaluated as a mediator of the relationship between

trauma and overgeneral autobiographical memory. Because we expected that

abused children would evince higher rates of negative self- representations

than will nonmaltreated children (Hypothesis 3), it was predicted that

negative self-representations would mediate the relationship between abuse

and overgeneral autobiographical memory. Given our focus on clarifying the

relationship between trauma and memory, this analysis was conducted

between the abused and nonmaltreated groups only.

(6) To address the role of psychopathology in the relation between trauma and

autobiographical memory, depressive and dissociative symptomatology were

assessed between abused and nonmaltreated children. It was expected that

the abused children would evince higher rates of depressive and dissociative

symptoms than would the nonmaltreated children.

(7) In light of normative decreases in dissociative symptoms and increases in

depressive symptoms during the transition from middle to late childhood

(Angold & Costello, 1993; Putnam, 1996; Putnam, Helmers & Trickett.,

1993), it was predicted that younger children would demonstrate more

dissociative and less depressive sym ptom atology than would older children

(8) To evaluate the role of depression and dissociation in children’s memory,

depression and dissociation were evaluated as mediators of the relation

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
25

between maltreatment and overgeneral autobiographical memory. It was

expected that depression and dissociation would mediate the effect of

maltreatment on memory, and that this effect would differ by age.

Specifically, we expected that dissociative symptoms would mediate the

relation between maltreatment and overgeneral memory for the younger

children, whereas depressive symptoms would mediate this relation for the

older children.

Chapter 3 Research Design and Methods

3.1 Participants

Participants in this study consisted of 185 children (70 maltreated and 115

nonmaltreated) between 7 and 13 years of age (average age = 10.61, SD = 1.55).

Children attended a research program in upstate New York that was designed to

provide children from low socioeconomic status families a recreational experience in

which maltreated and nonmaltreated children’s behavior could be observed in an

ecologically valid context (see Cicchetti & Manly, 1990, for a description of the camp

context).

The maltreated children who participated in this investigation were recruited

from those reported to the local Department of Human Services (DHS) because of

concerns associated w ith child maltreatment. Maltreatment status was determined via

examination of Child Protective Service (CPS) and Preventive Services records at

DHS, for which parents provided consent. Families’ maltreatment history also was

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
26

investigated through maternal interviews, during which research assistants inquired

about a history o f abusive and/or neglectful acts. Utilizing the Maltreatment

Classification System (Barnett, Manly & Cicchetti, 1993), the presence or absence of

four subtypes of maltreatment was determined for each child.

Maltreated children were assigned a primary subtype classification based

upon a hierarchy that assesses the degree to which a particular experience of

maltreatment violates social norms (Manly, Cicchetti & Barnett, 1994). Primary

subtype classifications, in descending order of deviation from societal norms, are

sexual abuse, physical abuse, neglect and emotional maltreatment (see later section

on maltreatment classification for a detailed description). These primary subtype

effects were further conceptualized as acts of commission (e.g., sexual abuse and

physical abuse) versus acts of omission (e.g. neglect), based on the knowledge that

these experiences may exert differential impacts on children’s development (Shields

& Cicchetti, 1998, 2001; Smetana, Toth, Cicchetti, Bruce, Kane & Daddis, 1999). In

this sample, co-occurring subtypes were present in 65% of the maltreated children.

Furthermore, the decision to classify children into commission/abused and

omission/neglected groups allows for the examination of memory processes among

neglected children, a population o f maltreated children that are often ignored in the

literature (Hildyard & Wolfe, 2002), especially with regard to memory. Thus, any

child who experienced physical or sexual abuse, regardless of concomitant neglect,

was classified into the abused group (N = 36), whereas the remaining maltreated

children who experienced neglect (N = 34) were placed in the neglected group.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
27

Children who experienced emotional maltreatment, but not any other form of

maltreatment (N = 7) were dropped from further analyses. Although the commission

of emotional maltreatment is associated with several maladaptive developmental

sequelae for children, this experience substantially differs from the commission of

other forms of child maltreatment that involve physical trauma; therefore, we chose to

limit the abuse group to children who experienced physical or sexual abuse.

For demographic comparability to the maltreatment sample, low- SES

nonmaltreated families were recruited from families receiving public assistance in the

form of Temporary Assistance to Needy Families (TANF). All families also

participated in maternal interviews in order to inquire about a history of maltreatment

(Cicchetti, Toth & Manly, 2003). Parental informed consent and child assent were

obtained for participation in research and parental permission allowed researchers to

examine CPS records. Nonmaltreatment status was confirmed by an absence of CPS

or Preventive Services records for the family and via maternal interview.

3.2 Maltreatment Determinations: Maltreatment Classification System (MCS; Barnett

et al., 1993)

Dimensions of family maltreatment were determined by trained coders using

the MCS (Barnett et al., 1993). Rather than relying solely on child protective agency

designation, multi-informant reports of the children’s experiences, as documented in

child protective and preventive records, were coded. U tilizing the w ell- validated

MCS (e.g., Bolger et al., 1998; Dubowitz, Pitts, Litrownik, Cox, Runyan & Black,

2005; English, Bangdiwala, & Runyan, 2005; Manly, 2005; Manly et al., 1994),

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
28

coders classified children’s maltreatment experiences according to subtype(s). The

MCS contains operational definitions of each subtype and these definitions were

utilized to determine the presence or absence of each subtype; multiple subtypes

could be coded for each report.

Maltreatment subtype categories include sexual abuse, physical abuse,

physical neglect, and emotional maltreatment. The presence of sexual abuse is coded

when any sexual contact or attempted sexual conduct occurred between the child and

an adult. These behaviors range from exposure to inappropriate sexual activities to

forced intercourse. Physical abuse is determined by injuries that had been inflicted

upon a child by nonaccidental means. These incidents could range from excessive

corporal punishment leading to bruising, to permanently disfiguring injuries.

Physical neglect is coded “when the primary caregiver failed to meet a child’s needs

for food, clothing, shelter, medical, dental, or mental health care, education, hygiene,

or physical safety” (Manly, Kim, Rogosch, & Cicchetti, 2001). Typical neglect

incidents included leaving young children at home alone, maintaining unsanitary

living conditions, and providing inadequate nourishment. Emotional maltreatment is

coded for chronic or extreme neglect or disregard of children’s emotional needs. This

includes the need for acceptance and self-esteem, psychological security, and age-

appropriate autonomy. Examples of emotionally maltreating events included serious

threats to injure a child, exposure to violent acts among fam ily m em bers, and

abandonment by primary caregivers. (See Barnett et al., 1993 for a detailed

description of the nosological system used to code incidents of maltreatment).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
29

Coding of DHS records for maltreatment subtype was conducted by trained research

assistants and Ph.D. level psychologists, among whom adequate reliability was

established (weighted k = .78- .94).

Abusing, neglecting and nonmaltreating families did not differ on a number of

important demographic characteristics (see Table 1). The three groups were

equivalent with respect to child age, F(2, 182) = .47, n.s., child gender, x (2) = 4.5,

n.s., and ethnic composition x2 (2)= 4.52, n.s.; with 69.4% of the abused group,

79.4% of the neglected group, and 85.2% of the nonmaltreated group represented

ethnic minorities. In addition, the groups were comparable on family marital status,

X2 (2) = 2.32, n.s., and socioeconomic status, x2 (2) = 3.19, with 86.1% of the abused

group, 68.8% of the neglected group and 73.5% of the nonmaltreated group falling

within the lowest two social strata (Hollingshead, 1975). In contrast, significant

differences emerged among groups regarding cognitive performance as assessed by

the Peabody Picture Verbal Test (PPVT-R and PPVT-III; Dunn & Dunn, 1981;

1997), F(2, 182) = 8.98,/? < .001. Bonferroni corrected post-hoc comparisons

revealed that the abused children (M = 86.78, SD = 10.2) performed significantly

lower than both the neglected children (M = 93.85, SD = 10.6) and the nonmaltreated

children (M = 96.52, SD = 12.9). Cognitive differences were controlled for in all

subsequent analyses.

3.3 P r o c e d u r e

Data for the current investigation were collected during a 6 week- long

recreational program where research was conducted. Children were assigned to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
30

same-age, same-sex groups of peers and were supervised by adult counselors who

were unaware of the children’s maltreatment status and of the research hypotheses.

Children attended the summer camp for one of the six weeks, and were observed for

approximately 35 total hours during the week by trained research assistants who were

unaware of group status. During the program, children were interviewed by trained

research staff, who were also unaware of child maltreatment status and study

hypotheses, in order to obtain assessments relevant to memory and self­

representation. Child interviews took place in a quiet, private context so that children

would feel comfortable discussing their memories honestly. Extra care was taken to

ensure that the child was familiar and comfortable with the interviewer. Additionally,

the counselors completed assessment instruments at the conclusion of their time with

the children.

3.4 Measures

Demographics Interview (Carlson & Cicchetti, 1979)

This interview assesses marital status, parental occupations and income, level

of education, and family composition. It was completed by the primary caregivers

(generally the mother) o f all children. Based upon this interview, the Hollingshead

Four Factor index (Hollingshead, 1975) of socioeconomic status can be derived.

Autobiographical Memory Test (AMT; Williams & Broadbent, 1986)

The Autobiographical M em ory Test (AM T) consists o f 10 em otional cue

words, five positive and five negative. The cues are simultaneously presented orally

and visually in a fixed order, alternating positive and negative cues (happy, sorry,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
31

safe, angry, interested, clumsy, successful, hurt, surprised and lonely). Children are

asked to generate a specific memory in response to each cue word (e.g. “ tell me about

a time when you felt ).” Children are given 1 minute in each case to retrieve a

specific personal memory in response to a cue word. If the child responds with a

memory that is not specific, then he/she is prompted to do so (e.g., “Can you think o f

a specific time or event when you f e lt . ? Tell me about it). This prompting can be

repeated if the children’s responses remain inappropriately general. Specific,

standard prompts were also provided to clarify the meaning o f some of the cue words

in cases where children did not understand the cue word. If the child did not retrieve

a specific memory within 60 seconds, then the experimenter proceeded to the next

item. The AMT was audiotaped, and all verbatim responses were transcribed.

Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-R, PPVT-III; Dunn & Dunn, 1981,

1997)

Because the recall and narration of autobiographical memories may be related

to general intelligence and language abilities, a cognitive assessment was

administered to all participants. The PPVT-R and the PPVT-III are individually

administered, multiple- choice tests designed to assess the receptive vocabulary skills

of persons aged 2.5 through 90+ years. Both versions of this measure correlate

highly with several measures of intelligence: the correlation of PPVT-R and WISC-R

= .70 (Dunn & Dunn, 1997) and the correlation o f PPVT-III and W ISC-III ranges

from .82 to .92. Different versions of the test were administered because study

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
32

participants were drawn from two separate time points. All PPVT-R scores were

transformed into equivalent PPVT-III standard scores for analyses.

Child Depression Inventory (CDI; Kovacs, 1981)

The CDI contains 27 items that assess the cognitive, affective and behavioral

sequelae of depression. Scores on the CDI may range from 0 to 54, with higher

scores indicating more severe depressive symptomatology. Typically, scores greater

than 12 on the CDI are conceptualized as reflecting mild depression, whereas scores

of 19 or greater signify clinical levels of depression (Smucker, Criaghead, Wilcoxan

Craighead, & Green, 1986). The CDI has acceptable reliability and validity (Kovacs,

1992).

Teacher’s Report Form o f the Child Behavior Checklist (TRF; Achenbach,

1991); Dissociation Subscale (Ogawa et al., 1997)

Counselors, who were unaware of children’s maltreatment status or of the

study hypotheses, used the TRF to rate children’s behaviors after approximately 35

hours of observation. The TRF is a well-validated instrument that includes 118 items

rated for frequency of occurrence. Ogawa and colleagues (1997) constructed a scale

to assess dissociative symptoms in childhood and adolescence based on 12 TRF

items, similar to those assessed by Putnam’s Child Dissociative Checklist (Putnam et

al., 1993; see Appendix). Individual child scores for dissociative symptomatology

reflect the sum o f the average o f three counselor’s scores for each o f the 12 items.

Adequate reliability among coders was established. Interrater reliabilities (intraclass

correlations) ranged from .66 to .84.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
33

3.5 Coding

Participants’ responses to the AMT were audiotaped and subsequently

transcribed. The memory narratives were then coded for the number of overgeneral

and specific memories, the number of interviewer prompts requited to elicit each

memory, the latency o f first response per cue word, and the length of the narratives.

In addition, the content o f children’s memory narratives was coded for the number of

positive and negative child and maternal representations by cue word. Finally, coders

rated the controllingness of the child, and the global warmth/friendliness of the

relationship between the child and interviewer. Two research assistants who were

unaware of group status or study hypotheses completed the coding. For reliability,

each coded 10% o f the others’ work (totaling 20%). Intraclass correlations ranged

from .71 to .99 (M = .88, see Table 2). Details of the coding procedures are described

in the following sections and are adapted from the coding schemes of Johnson and

colleagues (2005) and Williams & Broadbent (1986).

Overgeneral and Specific Memories

All memories were coded as either specific or overgeneral. Overgeneral

memories were defined as “memories that did not contain at least one specific detail

that identifies an event as a distinct episode” (Johnson et al., 2005). The total number

of overgeneral and specific memories in response to positive and negative cue words

were calculated. The total number o f nonresponses to positive and negative cues

were also recorded.

Prompts

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
34

Prompts are defined as statements by the interviewer that request a more

specific memory (e.g., “Can you think o f one time in particular when that

happened?”), or additional information (e.g., “How old were you?”). The total

number of prompts was calculated in response to positive and negative cue words.

Latency to Respond

The latency to response was assessed by timing the interval between the

interviewer’s verbal presentation of the cue word and the participant’s first word of

response. Participant verbalizations such as “ummm” or “hmm” were not evaluated as

the first word of response, and timing continued until the first word of memory recall.

Memory Length

Memory length was measured using a raw word count procedure (Dickinson

& Poole, 2000), in which all words in a narrative were counted, except for off-topic

non-memory statements (e.g. “I ’m tired”). Anticipating that some parts of the

narratives may be muffled or unintelligible, coders added 1 to the raw count when

they judged that one or two words were missing. If it seemed that more than two

words were missing from the transcript, then the memory was coded as unintelligible

and not used in the raw count analyses.

Child and Maternal Representations (Adapted from the MacArthur narrative

coding manual- Rochester Revision, MNCM-RR; Robinson, Mantz-Simmons,

Macfie, & MacArthur Narrative Working Group, 2000)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
35

The presence/absence o f positive and negative representations of self(child)

and of mother was recorded for each cue word. The total number of positive and

negative representations was then summed for positive and negative cue words.

Positive self- representations include instances where the child describes the

self as displaying or receiving affection (engages in hugs, kisses or praise), displaying

empathy/helping (exhibits an understanding of the thoughts or feelings of another, or

helps another), compliance (follows rules), and affiliation (engages in a positive

activity with another). Negative self-representations involve instances of aggression

(behaving in hostile, destructive behaviors, either verbally or physically),

noncompliance (ignores rules or does something in opposition to the rule), and shame

(exhibits signs o f embarrassment or makes self-reproaching statements). Positive

maternal representations include behaviors that are protective, caretaking,

affectionate, and helpful. Negative maternal behaviors were coded for behaviors that

are harsh, rejecting, or ineffectual.

Child Control/Opposition (Adapted from the MacArthur narrative coding

manual- Rochester Revision, MNCM-RR; Robinson et al., 2000)

After listening to the entire AMT, coders rated participants’ controlling or

oppositional behaviors on a 12-point Likert-type scale. At the low end of the scale

scores of 1-3 were rated for no controlling or oppositional behavior. For example, a

score of 3 would be given to a child who may be initially mildly resistant but

becomes truly engaged with the task. At the high end of the scale, scores of 10-12

were rated if the child is openly hostile, rude or provocative in behavior, criticizing of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
36

the task, and refusing to co-operate such that the task is not completed, despite

attempts on the examiner’s part to redirect the child to the task (see Table 3)

Engagement with Interviewer/Global Warmth or Friendliness o f Child

(Adapted from the MacArthur narrative coding manual- Rochester Revision,

MNCM-RR; Robinson et al., 2000)

This scale is designed to provide a “macro” impression of the child’s

friendliness, warmth and engagement with the examiner. Immediately following the

memory prompts, and the controllingness rating, the coder rated the quality of the

interaction between the examiner and the child on a 12-point Likert-type scale. This

includes the degree to which a connection was present between examiner and child,

as well as the degree to which the examiner and child liked each other, and had a

sense of comfort and positive relating. It is less a measure of the child’s behavior or

engagement with the task than it is the overall level of positive feelings experienced

with the child in the session. The rating ranges from a conflictual, uncomfortable

interaction, through more neutral relating, to a feeling of warm connectedness (see

Table 4).

Chapter 4 Results

4.1 Part I

First a correlational analysis was conducted on all dependent variables with

child cognitive performance (PPVT-III scores) and child gender (Table 5). Gender

was included in this analysis because there is evidence that females have more

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
37

accessible and extensive autobiographical memories than do males (Buckner &

Fivush, 1998; Johnson et al., 2005). Children’s PPVT-III standard scores had a

significant inverse relationship with the number of positive overgeneral memories (r

= -.31,/? < .001) and negative overgeneral memories (r = -.17, p < .05) recalled as

well as with the number o f prompts elicited for positive (r = -.26, p < .001) and

negative (r = -.18,/? < .05) cue words. In addition, children’s cognitive performance

was positively related to children’s warmth in their interactions with the interviewer

(r = .24, p < .01). Gender was significantly associated with the length of children’s

memories such that females provided longer memory narratives to both positive (r = -

.19,/? <.01) and negative word cues (r = -.28,/? < .001). Male gender was associated

with increased overgenerality (r = .15,/? < .05). Additionally, female gender was

associated with higher ratings of warmth (r = .22, p < .01) and lower ratings of

controllingness (r = -.28, p < .01). Because child cognitive performance and gender

were significantly related to several of the dependent variables of interest, child

cognitive performance and gender were used as covariates in order to minimize their

role in subsequent analyses.

Given the normative developmental changes in self- development and

memory that are associated with the transition from middle to late childhood (e.g.,

Harter, 2006; Howe et al., 2004; Greenhoot et al., 2005; Rudolph, Hammen and

Burge, 1995) age group w as evaluated as an independent variable in memory

analyses. Age grouping follows Hammen & Zupan (1984), and Rudolph, Hammen

and Burge (1995), such that children ages 7-9 were compared to children ages 10-14.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
38

The 10-year cut-point allowed the sample to be divided somewhat evenly, and it also

roughly marks the end of concrete-operational thinking (Piaget, 1962). In addition,

from a conceptual, self-development perspective, this age grouping is consistent with

that of Vondra and colleagues (1989), where maltreated children demonstrated a shift

in self- perceptions at approximately the age of 9-10 years.

To evaluate the first set of hypotheses (la-d) concerning autobiographical

memory, the design utilized a series of 2(age group: 7-9 years vs 10-13 years) x

3(status: abused vs. neglected vs. nonmaltreated) x 2(valence of cue: positive vs.

negative) repeated measures analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) on the number of

overgeneral memories, the number of prompts, latency to respond, and length of

memories. The repeated measure was valence of cue type (positive vs. negative).

Based upon the prior correlational analysis, PPVT-III scores and gender were entered

into the four analyses as covariates (Table 6).

The results of the first repeated measures ANCOVA on the number of

overgeneral memories demonstrated no significant within-measures effects of

valence. However, the between subjects analysis revealed a significant effect of

subtype, F(2, 177) = 4.62 , p < .05, Partial r\ = .05. Consistent with Hypothesis 2,

there also was a significant main effect of age group, F (l, 177) = 8.26, p < .01,

Partial r\2 = .05; however, the direction of effect was opposite to that which was

predicted, such that younger children demonstrated more overgeneral memories (M—

2.8, SD = 1.2) than did the older children (M = 2.3, SD - 1.3). The interaction of

subtype by age was nonsignificant, F(2, 177)= 1.00, n.s. Gender and PPVT-III scores

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
39

were each significant covariates, F( 1,177) = 3.98,p <.05 and F (l, 177) = 12.68, p <

.001, respectively. Subsequent pairwise comparisons, Bonferroni corrected, to

investigate the significant subtype main effect revealed that the abused children

demonstrated more overgeneral memories (M = 3.00, SD = 1.2) than did the

neglected children (M = 2.26, SD =1.1 ,p <.01) and the nonmaltreated children (M =

2.39, SD = 1.2, p < .01), thus confirming Hypothesis la.

A similar pattern of results emerged from the ANCOVA regarding the number

of prompts elicited by the children during the memory task. No significant within

subjects effects of valence were detected. However, consistent with the prior

analyses, the main effect of subtype was significant, F{2,177) = 5.50, p < .01, Partial
y < t
r| = .06. Confirming Hypothesis lb, Bonferroni- corrected pairwise comparisons,

revealed that abused children elicited more prompts (M = 7.41, SD = 3.5) than did the

neglected children (M= 5.35, SD = 3.4, p = .01) and the nonmaltreated children ( M -

5.15, SD = 3.5, p < .001). The main effect of age group F (l, 177) = 5.24, p < .05,

Partial r\ = .03, was also significant, such that younger children elicited more

prompts (M = 6.64, SD = 3.8) than did the older children (M = 5.30, SD = 4.0).

Next, an ANCOVA on latency to respond was conducted. The within subjects

comparisons of valence were nonsignificant, F (l, 177) = .006, n.s., as were all

between subjects effects. Thus, no differences emerged among subtypes, F(2, 177) =

.59, n.s., or between age groups, F( 1, 177) = .02, n.s., regarding the amount of time

children took to respond to each memory cue, disconfirming Hypothesis lc.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
40

Finally, an ANCOVA on the length of children’s memories was performed.

Unlike the prior three analyses, a within subjects trend for the effect of valence

emerged, F ( 1, 177) = 2.98, p - .086, Partial r\ = .017, such that children’s memory

narratives in response to negative cue words (M = 25.10, SD = 16.9) were

significantly longer than were children’s responses to positive cues (M = 21.09, SD =

14.14). The interaction of valence by subtype and age group were each

nonsignificant. Between subjects analyses also revealed no significant main effects

for subtype F(2, 177) = .53, n.s., age group, F (\, 177) = 1.11, n.s., or their interaction,

F(2,177) = .63, n.s, disconfirming Hypothesis Id.

Next, to evaluate differences among abused, neglected and nonmaltreated

children with respect to their self- and maternal- representations, as well as their

controllingness and relationship with the examiner, a multivariate analysis of

covariance (MANCOVA) was conducted. Specifically, a MANCOVA was run on

positive and negative self- representations, positive and negative matemal-

representations, controllingness and relationship with the examiner with maltreatment

subtype (abuse, neglect, nonmaltreatment) and age group (younger and older) as the

between subjects factors. Consistent with our prior set of analyses, child gender and

PPVT-III scores were entered as covariates. A significant multivariate effect of

subtype emerged, F ( 12, 346) = 2.81,/? < .01, Partial r\2 = .09. The main effect of age

group w as nonsignificant, F (6, 172) = .84, n.s., as w as the interaction o f age group

and subtype, F(12, 346) = .66, n.s.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
41

To further evaluate the significant multivariate effect of subtype, univariate

analyses revealed a significant main effect of subtype for negative child

representations, F{2,177) = 7.10,/? = .001, Partial r\ = .074, controllingness, F (2,

177) = 3.15,/? <.05, Partial r|2 = .034, and warmth, F( 1, 177) = 5.46,/? < .01, Partial

r) = .058 . Subsequent pairwise comparisons, Bonferroni corrected, revealed that

abused children’s memories (M = 1.79, SD = 1.1) contained more negative self­

representations than did those of the nonmaltreated children (M = .98, SD = 1.1,/?<

.01). The neglected children’s memories (M - 1.36, SD = 1.1) did not significantly

differ from either the abused or the nonmaltreated children’s memories regarding

negative self- representations. These findings are partially consistent with Hypothesis

3 where it was predicted that abused children would have more negative

representations than both the neglected and nonmaltreated children. Predictions

among maltreatment subtypes regarding differences in maternal positive

representations, F (l, 177) = .23, n.s., and maternal negative representations, F (l, 177)

= 1.27, n.s., were not supported as (Table 6).

Regarding controllingness, the abused children were rated higher (M = 3.84,

SD = 2.1) than were the neglected children (M = 2.60, SD = 2.1, p < .05). Abused

children not significantly differ from the nonmaltreated children (M = 3.12, SD = 2.1)

on controllingness. Concerning warmth, the neglected children were rated higher on

warmth ( M — 9.17, S D = 2.4) than both the abused children ( M = 7.20, S D = 2.5 , p <

.05) and the nonmaltreated children (M = 7.67, SD = 2.5,/? < .01). This finding was

unanticipated, as Hypothesis 4 predicted that abused and neglected children would be

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
42

rated lower on warmth and higher on controllingness than would nonmaltreated

children (Table 6).

4.2 Part II

Analysis for Part II o f this study was conducted with a subset of 93 children

between the ages of 7 and 13 years for whom the self- report measures necessary for

this analysis were available. In addition, this sample included children from the

abused (N = 27) and nonmaltreated (N = 65) groups only because of our goal of

understanding the effects of physical trauma upon autobiographical memory.

Unlike Part I o f the investigation which examined several aspects of

children’s autobiographical memory recall, Part II focused on one particular aspect of

children’s memory (overgeneral memory) in relation to self-representations as well as

depressive and dissociative symptomatology between abused and nonmaltreated

children. Children’s scores on the Children’s Depression Index (CDI) were used to

assess depressive symptomatology and scores on the Dissociation Scale, derived from

a subset of responses on the Child Behavior Checklist, were used to assess

dissociative symptoms. A square-root transformation was performed on both CDI

and Dissociation Scale scores to correct for skewness of the distribution.

First, children’s self-representations were evaluated as potential mediators of

the relationship between abuse and overgeneral memory (Hypothesis 5). Because the

affective valence o f children’s overgeneral m em ories w as not a significant within

subjects factor (as demonstrated in Part I) the current regression analysis was

performed on the total number of overgeneral memories children produced, rather

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
43

than via separate parallel regressions on positive and negative overgeneral memories.

As established by the earlier MANOVA, children’s negative self-representations were

significantly related to maltreatment subtype. Specifically, a history of abuse

accounted for a significant proportion of variance in the amount of negative self­

representations contained within children’s memory narratives, F (1, 86) = 7.00,/? =

. 01 .

Next, the relationship between children’s negative self-representations and

total overgeneral memory was assessed via regression. Negative self-representations

accounted for a significant portion of variance in predicting overgeneral memory,

AR2 = .076, F( 1, 83) = 6.81,/? = .01. Higher levels of negative self-representation

were positively associated with overgeneral memory, p = .28.

To complete the mediation analyses, a hierarchical regression was conducted

to determine the role o f negative self- representations in the relationship between

abuse and overgeneral memory (Table 7). Gender, age, and cognitive test scores

were entered into the regression analysis as covariates at the first step, AR2 = .22, F(3,

81) = 7.61,/? < .001. Next, maltreatment subtype (abused vs. nonmaltreated) was

entered into the model, accounting for a significant portion of variance, AR = .057,

F (l, 80) = 6.26,/? < .05. At the third step, children’s negative self- representations

were added to the model; however, negative self-representations did not account for a
• • 'y •
significant amount o f variance, AR = .023, F (1, 79) = 2.60, n.s. Finally, the

interaction term between subtype and negative self- representations was entered into

the model in the final step, accounting for a nonsignificant change in variance.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
44

Although negative self-representations did not account for a significant proportion of

variance in predicting overgeneral memory, the strength o f the relationship between

abuse and overgeneral memory decreased in the presence of this variable, Ap = -.066.

Nonetheless, the findings o f current regression demonstrate that negative self­

representations do not predict overgeneral memory when controlling for abuse.

Therefore, children’s negative self- representations did not mediate the pathway

between abuse and overgeneral memory, discontinuing Hypothesis 5.

To evaluate Hypothesis 6 regarding differences between abused and

nonmaltreated children with respect to their depressive and dissociative

symptomatology, a MANCOVA was conducted on square-root transformed CDI

scores and Dissociation Scale scores with maltreatment subtype

(abuse/nonmaltreatment) as the between subjects factor. Because no significant

interaction effects of age group with maltreatment subtype emerged in the prior

analyses, child age was entered as a covariate rather than as a between subjects factor.

A significant multivariate effect of subtype emerged, F(2, 88) = 3.62, p < .05.

Univariate tests revealed a significant effect o f subtype for square-root transformed

depressive symptoms, F (l, 89) = 3.86, p < .05 such that abused children

demonstrated more depressive symptoms ( M - 2.14, SD = 1.2) than did the

nonmaltreated children (M = 2.14, SD = 1.4). Also consistent with Hypothesis 6,

there was a significant effect o f subtype for square-root transformed dissociative

symptomatology, F (l, 89) = 5.59, p < .05, such that abused children demonstrated

more dissociative symptoms (M = 1.69, SD = .7) than did the nonmaltreated children

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
45

(M = 1.22, S D - .8). Age group was not a significant covariate, F(2, 88) = .25, n.s.;

therefore, predictions regarding differences in depressive and dissociative

symptomatology (Hypothesis 7) were not supported.

Next, to evaluate Hypothesis 8 regarding the association between depressive

and dissociative symptomatology in relation to overgeneral memory, a regression

analysis was conducted. At the first step of analysis, transformed CDI scores and

Dissociation scale scores were entered into the model. Together, CDI and

Dissociation scores accounted for a significant portion of variance in predicting

overgeneral memory, AR2 = .085, F(2, 90) = 4.18, p < .05. Individually, depressive

symptomatology accounted for a significant portion of variance, p = .22, F (1,90) =

4.32, p < .05, whereas Dissociative scores did not, P = .13, F{ 1, 90) = 1.52, n.s.. At

the next step, the interaction o f CDI and Dissociation scale scores was entered into

the regression, accounting for a nonsignificant portion of variance AR2 = .002, F (l,

89) = .22, n.s.

Next, depressive symptomatology was evaluated as a mediator of the

relationship between maltreatment and overgeneral autobiographical memory.

Dissociative symptomatology could not be evaluated as a mediator as predicted

because the prior analysis failed to demonstrate a significant relationship between

dissociative symptoms and overgeneral autobiographical memory.

The role o f depression as a possible mediator o f overgeneral autobiographical

memory was evaluated via hierarchical regression analysis (Table 8). Gender, age,

and cognitive test scores were entered into the regression analysis as covariates at the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
46

first step, AR2 = .22, F(3, 86) = 8.13, p < .01. Next, maltreatment subtype (abused vs.

nonmaltreated) was entered into the model, accounting for a significant portion of

variance, AR2= .07, F (l, 85) = 8.59,p < .01. At the third step, children’s CDI scores

were added to the model, accounting for a nonsignificant amount of variance, AR2 =

.01, F (1, 84) = 1.28, n.s. Finally, the interaction terms between subtype and

depressive symptoms were entered into the model in the final steps, also accounting

for a nonsignificant change in variance. The addition of depressive symptoms to the

model did not significantly decrease the strength of the relation of abuse to

overgeneral memory (Ap = .002), therefore, depressive symptomatology did not

mediate the relationship between trauma and overgeneral memory as predicted in

Hypothesis 8.

Chapter 5 Discussion

5.1 Aspects o f Autobiographical Memory

5.1.1 Overgeneral Memory

This investigation addresses several significant gaps in the literature on the

relation between trauma and autobiographical memory by providing a comprehensive

analysis o f the form and content of children’s autobiographical memory recall.

Additionally, the examination of children’s self-system functioning through the

analysis o f self- representations and psychopathology informs theoretical accounts o f

the mechanisms which underlie the overgeneral memory effect.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
47

A central finding of this research is that abused children demonstrated more

overgeneral memory than did nonmaltreated children. Establishing that children who

have been abused experience difficulty in retrieving specific personal memories adds

to the extant literature on the association between trauma and overgeneral

autobiographical memory. Despite the plethora of studies that have linked

overgeneral autobiographical memories to trauma among adults (see Williams et al.,

2007 for review), this investigation provides the first evidence of overgeneral

memory as a function of trauma among a sample of children with documented

histories o f abuse.

Prior to the current investigation, only two other studies had examined the

role of trauma in autobiographical memory processes among samples of children or

adolescents (deDecker et al., 2003; Johnson et al., 2005). Consistent with the relation

between abuse and overgeneral memory identified in this investigation, DeDecker

and colleagues (2003) found that higher levels of childhood trauma were associated

with reduced autobiographical memory specificity among a sample of adolescents in

an inpatient setting; however, this investigation relied on adolescents’ retrospective

reports of abuse. Therefore childhood abuse determination was not independent of

the participants’ current recall abilities or of their willingness to disclose trauma.

Moreover, the effect of participants’ current psychopathology on memory retrieval

could not be differentiated from the effects o f self-reported abuse.

The study conducted by Johnson and colleagues (2005) represented an

advance in the literature by assessing overgeneral autobiographical memory in a

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
48

nonclinical sample of children and adolescents and by avoiding retrospective

reporting o f childhood abuse; however, abuse was assessed via child self-report and

was limited to reports of sexual abuse. Therefore, children’s willingness to report

abuse experiences remained a potential confound. In contrast to the findings of the

current investigation, Johnson and colleagues found no association between

children’s self-reported abuse histories and overgeneral memory. Rather, recent

family violence was associated with overgeneral memories and lower rates of specific

memories. In interpreting these findings, it is difficult to disentangle the impact of

abuse on autobiographical memory apart from participants’ current mental health

conditions, such as depression, which was also prevalent in their sample. Moreover,

family violence was operationalized as the total frequency of child-reported exposure

to mother-directed violence as well as exposure to violence towards the child him or

herself, thus preventing the authors from differentiating the effects of domestic

violence from that o f child physical abuse on children’s memories.

Therefore, this investigation is the first to demonstrate an association between

child abuse and overgeneral autobiographical memory among a sample of children

for whom multiple sources of information regarding maltreatment experiences were

available and utilized to inform maltreatment classification. Corroboration of

maltreatment experiences independent of self- report is particularly critical for

investigations of the impact of trauma on memory, given the inherent flaw in relying

on participants’ memories to both identify abuse and provide evidence of memory

deficiency.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
49

The finding that children who have been abused demonstrate overgenerality in

their autobiographical memory recall is important for informing theoretical

perspectives on the development of overgeneral memory. Perhaps the most widely

stated account o f overgeneral memory is that of Williams (1996), who proffered that

children who experience early trauma/adversity adopt a generic style in retrieving

autobiographical memories as an affect-regulating strategy. According to this

functional-avoidance theory, being less specific reduces the risk of confrontation with

painful memories, i.e., trauma. Labeling the process mnemonic interlock, Williams

asserted that memory retrieval is truncated at an intermediate stage of retrieval and

the tendency to stop the memory search is reinforced (negatively) by the avoidance of

negative affect attached to painful memories. These intermediate memories are

closely related to categoric labels or self-descriptors. Because overgenerality in

autobiographical memory recall was first noted among adults with depression

(Williams, 1996; Williams & Broadbent, 1986, Williams & Scott, 1988), Williams’

theory o f overgenerality included a relative focus on linking overgeneral memory to

depression. According to his theory, persistent use of functional-avoidance strategy,

with one overgeneral categoric memory leading to another, is a ruminative, self­

focused processing style. This cognitive style is thought to increase an individual’s

vulnerability to depression because an inability to recall the details of past

experiences impairs problem solving ability and the use of an overgeneral retrieval

style will result in an endless stream of attempts to think through such problems.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
50

Several aspects of Wiliams’ (1996) theory have been supported with empirical

evidence, including the relation o f overgenerality to rumination (Nolen-Hoeksema,

1991; Raes et al, 2005) and problem solving ability (e.g., Goddard et al., 1996, 1997)

among depressed samples. However, one significant gap in the literature supporting

Williams’ theory has been the lack of evidence of overgenerality among abused

children, despite the centrality of early trauma to his model. Instead, empirical work

demonstrating a link between trauma and overgeneral memory has mainly relied upon

retrospective reporting of childhood abuse experiences among adults. Therefore,

evidence that children who have been abused demonstrate more overgeneral memory

than do nonmaltreated children supports Williams’ theoretical perspective and

provides much needed evidence that such a pattern of memory retrieval also is

associated with memory overgenerality in childhood.

Furthermore, the demonstration of overgeneral autobiographical memory

among a sample of maltreated children adds to the literature regarding the impact of

trauma on memory for nontraumatic stimuli in children. As reviewed in the

introduction, extant research examining basic memory processes (i.e., the encoding,

storage and retrieval of neutral, nontraumatic stimuli) among maltreated children is in

its infancy, whereas studies that have focused on traumatic memories have

burgeoned. For the most part, foundational work on basic memory processes

suggests that maltreatment does not have an impact on children’s mem ory. For

example, maltreatment does not appear to have an effect on true and false recall for

neutral stimuli, nor does it affect children’s memory for positive and negative words

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
51

when encoded under self-referent instructions (Howe et al., 2004; Valentino et al., in

press). However, one recent investigation suggests that under certain conditions,

children who have been abused demonstrate poorer semantic recall; under matemal-

referent encoding conditions abused children recall fewer positive and negative words

than do nonmaltreated children (Valentino et al., under review). Thus, the current

research provides a second example of alterations in memory performance among

children who have experienced abuse. In addition, this investigation augments the

literature on nontraumatic semantic memory through the comprehensive assessment

of nontraumatic autobiographical memory processes.

5.1.2 Interviewer Prompts

Beyond the frequency with which children retrieve overgeneral

autobiographical memories, this investigation examined several additional aspects of

autobiographical recall. For example, analysis of the number of interviewer prompts

to encourage retrieval of specific memories during the Autobiographical Memory

Test (AMT) revealed that abused children elicited more prompts from the interviewer

than did the nonmaltreated and the neglected children. This finding is consistent with

prior research conducted by Johnson and colleagues (2005), who found that teens

with abuse histories received more prompting by the interviewer than did teens

without an abuse history. Notably, this was the only memory variable significantly

related to childhood trauma in their study. It is logical that those who are having

difficulty recalling specific memories will elicit more prompts from the interviewer

because the interviewer is instructed to prompt if the child initially responds to a cue

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
52

word with an overgeneral memory. Given our finding that the abused children

provided more overgeneral responses than did the nonmaltreated children, it is

understandable that these children would also receive more interviewer prompts

overall.

5.1.3 Response Latency

Abused, neglected and nonmaltreated children evidenced no differences in

their response latencies, thus disconfirming our hypothesis that abused children

would require more time to generate memories following cue word presentation.

Assuming that abused children would have difficulty retrieving specific

autobiographical memories, we additionally anticipated that abused children would

spend more time engaged in memory search for a specific memory before responding

with one that is overgeneral. However, the findings here suggest that although

abused children demonstrate more overgenerality in their memory recall, they do not

utilize more time for their memory search. Although this lack o f differences was

unexpected, it is consistent with the findings of the only other investigation to

measure response latencies during the AMT among abused children (Johnson et al.,

2005). In fact, all of the evidence supporting increased response latency during

autobiographical memory retrieval has been conducted among adults with depression

(Brittlebank et al., 1993; Williams & Broadbent, 1986). Therefore it is possible that

increased response latency is indicative o f the psychom otor retardation that is

associated with depression and is not characteristic of overgeneral memory retrieval.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
53

5.1.4 Length o f Memory

Abused, neglected and nonmaltreated children also demonstrated comparable

length in their memory narratives. Given our prediction that abused children would

be more overgeneral in their autobiographical memory responses, we also

hypothesized that abused children would be less detailed in their memory narratives,

resulting in shorter memories. However, abused, neglected and nonmaltreated

children did not exhibit differences in the length of memory narratives generated

during the AMT, which fails to support our hypothesis. Only one previous study has

connected length of memory to exposure to current family violence (Johnson et al.,

2005); however, it is noteworthy that a history of self-reported child maltreatment

was not associated with memory length in that same sample.

One possible explanation for the lack of differences in memory length is that

the average length of narrative for participants in this investigation was rather short

(M = 11.63, SD = 7.8). Therefore, floor effects could have precluded the detection of

differences as a function of maltreatment. Nonetheless, the lack of differences in

memory length suggests that the abused, neglected and nonmaltreated children did

not differ in expressive language abilities or verbal fluency. Although it would have

been ideal to include a direct measure of language abilities so that any differences

could be controlled for statistically, the findings regarding length of memory suggest

that verbal fluency is not a significant confound.

Despite a lack o f differences in memory length as a function of maltreatment

experience, differences were revealed as a function of the valence of cue type.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
54

Specifically, children generated longer memories in response to negative cues than to

positive cues. This finding is consistent with the literature on autobiographical

memory and reminiscing among typical and atypical samples (Fivush, Hazzard,

Sales, Sarfatti & Brown, 2003; Sales, Fivush, & Peterson, 2003). For example, in a

study of children aged 5-12 living in violent neighborhoods, children talked about

their thoughts and emotions more when recalling negative than positive events

(Fivush et al, 2003). Whereas positive experiences can generally assimilate into self­

representations with ease, negative experiences potentially threaten one’s sense of

personal competence. Thus, in order for children to understand the personal meaning

of negative experiences, and integrate them into a coherent autobiography, a more in

depth discussion of the causes and consequences of emotion may be needed to

mitigate against feelings of vulnerability (Bird & Reese, 2006). Often, parent-child

conversations serve as a medium through which children can begin to understand the

personal meaning o f past experiences. At least among normative samples, there is

evidence that parents are more elaborative when discussing a negative event,

compared to a positive event, and are more likely to include an explanation for the

emotion in the negative event (Sales et al., 2003). Although not directly assessed in

this investigation, it is possible that the children’s memory narratives were longer in

response to negative cues than to positive cues due to a similar desire to reconcile

negative experiences w ith their sense o f self.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
55

5.1.5 Gender effects

Associations between gender and aspects of autobiographical memory

processes also replicated some well-established patterns in the memory literature. For

example, females had longer memory narratives than did males in response to both

positive and negative cue words. In addition, males demonstrated more overgeneral

memory overall. This is consistent with a number of studies that cohere to suggest

that females have more extensive and accessible autobiographical memories than do

males during childhood and adulthood (Buckner & Fivush, 1998; Davis, 1999). Most

explanations of the difference in autobiographical memory between males and

females focus on gender differences in the socialization of autobiographical memory.

For example, there is evidence that mothers reminisce differently with female

compared to male children in that they are more elaborative with their daughters

(Reese, Haden & Fivush, 1996). Moreover, there is a large body of literature

demonstrating a connection between maternal elaborative reminiscing and their

children’s developing autobiographical memory skills (see Fivush, Haden & Reese,

2006 for review).

5.1.6 Valence effects

In contrast to a significant effect of valence on the length of memories

generated, valence differences were not found for overgenerality, the number of

prompts elicited from the interviewer, or response latency during the AM T. The lack

of differences in the number of overgeneral memories between those that were

generated in response to positive and negative cues is consistent with some, but not

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
56

all, of the extant literature. A recent synthesis of the research on the overgeneral

memory phenomenon by Williams and colleagues (2007), reviewed 12 studies that

examined the relation of trauma to overgeneral memories, including adult and child

samples. O f these studies, two reported that overgenerality in response to negative

cues (but not positive cues) was associated with trauma (Dalgleish, Tchanturia,

Serpell, Hems, Yiend, DeSilva, et al, 2003; Peeters, Wessel, Merchelbach, & Boon-

Vermeenan, 2002), and one reported the opposite pattern, such that negative cues

elicited more specific memories (Burnside et al., 2004). In contrast, nine studies

reported that trauma history is associated with overgenerality in response to both

valences, a finding that is consistent with the current results.

Examination of the research on overgeneral memory recall among samples of

individuals with depression reveals a somewhat different pattern of results. Although

the predominant finding is that overgenerality is found in response to both positive

and negative cues, several studies of adults with depression indicate that positive cues

elicit more overgeneral memories than do negative cues (e.g., Dalgleish et al., 2001;

Williams & Broadbent, 1986; Williams & Dritchel, 1988; Williams & Scott, 1988).

Thus, the major difference between studies among depressed samples and samples of

those with a trauma history is that none of the trauma studies show significantly

increased overgenerality for positive cues. Because depressed mood is associated

with reduced availability of positive m nem onic material (Eich, 1995; W iliam s, Watts,

MacLeod, & Matthews, 1997), it is possible that increased overgenerality for positive

cues might be explained by mood congruency effects. Nonetheless, it is important to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
57

highlight that the two aforementioned studies, which identify a valence effect in the

relation of trauma to overgeneral memory, were conducted among clinical samples

with comorbid psychopathology (eating disorders in the Dalgleish et al., 2003 study

and depression in the Peeters et al., 2002 study).

Across the body of literature regarding overgeneral autobiographical memory,

the lack of a consistent valence effect is problematic for Williams’ functional-

avoidance theory. If autobiographical memory search is truncated at a categorical

level of memory in order to avoid the recall of aversive experiences and their

associated emotions, then one would expect overgenerality to occur more frequently

in response to negative cues then to positive cues. Thus, it seems that the notion of

functional-avoidance alone cannot explain all of the data (Williams et al., 2007).

Consistent with advancements in models of autobiographical memory, which

now include the role o f central executive control in the activation and inhibition of

memory components, and emphasize the role of the self (e.g., Conway & Pleydell-

Pearce, 2000; Howe & Courage, 1997), revised models of overgeneral

autobiographical memory recognize that overgenerality may represent a truncated

hierarchical search that is exacerbated by conceptual structures (of the self). These

impairments are further compounded by reduced processing resources and the

continuing impact of early trauma and adversity on the self-memory system

(W illiam s et al., 2007). A s explained in the follow ing sections, central executive

activation and inhibition, and self- representations play critical roles in the

interpretation o f the findings of this investigation.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
58

5.1.7 Age effects

Age was also related to autobiographical memory performance; however, the

direction of this effect was opposite than that which was predicted. In particular

younger children (aged 7-9) produced more overgeneral memories than did older

children (aged 10-14). Drawing from the developmental and cognitive literature on

the emergence o f autobiographical memory, there are several potential explanations

for the higher rates o f overgeneral memory among the younger in comparison to the

older children. For example, several theories of autobiographical memory

development place the self as central to the encoding, storage and retrieval of

autobiographical memories (e.g., Conway& Pleydell-Pearce, 2000; Howe &

Courage, 1997). Therefore, until the self becomes a viable entity with developed

features, the encoding o f memories that are attached to the self (autobiographical

memories), is difficult. However, as the self becomes more developed and the

number of features associated with the self multiplies, the likelihood that these self­

features will be activated becomes greater, allowing event memories to be encoded as

part of one’s personal past (Howe & Courage, 1997). Therefore, increased

overgenerality among younger children may be accounted for by a relatively

restricted set o f self-features from which to sample in comparison to older children.

Alternately, Conway & Pleydell-Pearce emphasize the importance of

executive control during autobiographical m em ory search in their self-m em ory m odel

(Conway & Pleydell-Pearce; 2000). In this model, supervisory processes are

necessary for generative retrieval by establishing intermediate search descriptions to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
59

direct the memory search towards activation of appropriate event specific knowledge

and to inhibit irrelevant autobiographical information. Considering that inhibitory

control is a skill that is developed throughout childhood, increased overgenerality in

autobiographical memory among younger children might be related to difficulty in

inhibiting information that is activated at the intermediate description level of

memory search.

If overgenerality is related to inhibitory control, then levels of overgenerality

should correlate with performance on executive tasks that provide a measure of errors

due to interference from irrelevant information that should have been inhibited.

Recent work by Dalgliesh and colleagues (Dalgliesh, Williams, Golden, Barnard, Au-

Yeung, et al, in press) supports such a hypothesis by demonstrating that overgeneral

memory correlated with a task in which irrelevant material needed to be inhibited (the

emotional stroop test) and not with a task that merely required sufficient executive

capacity overall (a test of explicit memory).

In contrast, Johnson and colleagues (2005) demonstrated that age was

positively related to overgeneral memories in their sample of 12 to 18 year old

adolescents. However, a methodological difference between the administration of the

AMT in the Johnson study and the protocol utilized in this investigation renders a

comparison o f the two investigations difficult. Specifically, Johnson and colleagues

asked their adolescent participants to recall “childhood m em ories” (defined as prior to

age 9), whereas children in the current investigation were not restricted in the time

period from which they could recount memories. Therefore, the difficulty older teens

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
60

in the Johnson study experienced in retrieving specific memories might be accounted

for by memory decay and reduced accessibility of memories that typically may occur

overtime (e.g., Rubin, 1986).

In sum, the findings of this investigation regarding the relation of age to

overgeneral memory revealed that younger children display more overgeneral

memory than do older children. Although this pattern of results is inconsistent with

prior work among adolescents, overgeneral autobiographical memory among younger

children may be accounted for by developmental processes related to the self and

inhibitory control.

5.2 Content o f Memories: Self and Maternal Representations

The content o f the memory narratives children produced during the AMT

were coded for positive and negative self- representations as well as for positive and

negative maternal representations. This in itself represents advancement in the use of

the AMT, as the vast majority of prior research with this paradigm has reduced

autobiographical memory performance to a single number: the total number of

overgeneral memories produced upon initial response to each cue word. In addition

to providing a richer account of children’s autobiographical memory, the analysis of

children’s representations allowed us to better address the role of the self in

overgeneral memory.

A s predicted, abused, neglected and nonmaltreated children differed with

respect to self- representations. Specifically, abused and neglected children’s

narratives contained more negative self- representations than did the narratives of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
61

nonmaltreated children. Furthermore, abused children demonstrated more negative

self- representations than did the neglected children, confirming our hypothesis that

abused children would have the most negative self-representations. The presence of

elevated rates of negative self- representation among children who have been abused

is consistent with several prior studies of self- system functioning among maltreated

children. For example, analysis of maltreated preschool children’s narratives in

response to the MacArthur Story Stem Battery revealed that the narratives of children

who had been physically abused contained high levels of negative self-

representations (Toth et al., 1997). Research among school-aged children reveals that

maltreated children have lower self-esteem, and have less positive self- concepts than

do nonmaltreated children (Bolger et al., 1998; Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1997; Egeland,

et al., 1983; Kim & Cicchetti, 2003, 2004). Taken together, the current finding that

abused children’s autobiographical memories contain more negative self-

representations than do the narratives of neglected and nonmaltreated children confers

the presence of self-system deviation among physically abused children in particular.

The presence o f positive and negative maternal- representations also was

examined in this investigation. Unlike our findings regarding self-representations,

there were no differences in positive or negative matemal-representations among

abused, neglected and nonmaltreated children. This lack of findings might be

attributable to floor effects given the low base rates o f the appearance o f matem al-

representations within children’s autobiographical memory narratives. On average,

children within all three subgroups of maltreatment history included positive or

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
62

negative maternal- representations in one of ten memory narratives. Thus the low

frequency and lack o f variance in children’s positive and negative matemal-

representations precluded our ability to detect differences as a function of

maltreatment history. Because the current investigation is the first to examine

representations in the memories produced during the AMT, normative data on the

frequency with which children include matemal-representations in their

autobiographical memory narratives is not available. To better understand the

impoverished rates of maternal- representations contained within the memories of

children in this investigation, it will be important for future research to determine

normative rates among a group of typically developing children.

Additionally, the role of maternal- representations in children’s

autobiographical memory is worth further investigation because prior research with

maltreated children suggested an association between the activation o f children’s

maternal- representations and their semantic recall abilities (Valentino et al., under

review). Specifically, abused children demonstrated poorer recall during an

incidental recall task where children encoded adjectives under maternal referent or

nonmatemal referent conditions. In contrast, research with a similar memory

paradigm in a different sample revealed that abused children demonstrated

comparable memory performance during a recall task that encoded words under self­

referent or nonself-referent conditions (Valentino et al., in press). Therefore, it is

possible that the activation of maternal schema might have a unique impact on

memory encoding and retrieval.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
63

J.3 Relationship Effects: Controllingness and Warmth

Because the majority o f prior research on the overgeneral memory effect has

relied on retrospective reporting of abuse, adults’ willingness (or lack thereof) to

disclose information and share memories may have influenced earlier research

findings. The current investigation addresses this issue in a number of ways. First, by

independently coding maltreatment records for incidences of abuse and neglect, we

were able to corroborate information gained via maternal interview regarding

children’s maltreatment histories. In addition, we included measures of child

controllingness and the quality of the child- interviewer relationship to determine

whether relationship/interaction variables might impact memory findings. For

example a child who is rated high on controllingness might refuse to answer the

questions, or attempt to alter the course of the paradigm (see Table 3 for operational

definitions). Thus, we wanted to ensure that overgeneral memories were not

accounted for by differences between abused, neglected, and nonmaltreated

children’s interaction abilities and styles.

Despite our hypothesis that abused children would be more controlling than

the nonmaltreated children during the administration of the AMT, no such differences

emerged. Theoretically, this controlling style is thought to emerge from disorganized

attachment relationships, characterizing interactions where the child utilizes

controlling behaviors as a coping strategy when attachment concerns are aroused

(Carlson, 1998; Hennighausen & Lyons-Ruth, 2005). Given the very high prevalence

of disorganized attachment relationships among children with abuse histories (up to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
64

80-90%; Bamett, Ganiban & Cicchetti, 1999), we predicted that children who were

abused would be more controlling with the interviewer during the AMT, a task that

could activate attachment organization via recollection o f aversive memories. Prior

research among maltreated children has found abused children to be more controlling

during narrative paradigms that are similar to that used in this investigation (Toth et

al., 1997). However, consistent with the principles of multifinality (Cicchetti &

Rogosch, 1996), not all children with atypical attachment organizations develop a

controlling interaction style. Nonetheless, it is possible that children’s participation

in the AMT did not lead to sufficient emotional-arousal for activation of children’s

attachment systems, in response to which controlling behaviors might emerge. In

fact, one important difference between the story-stem narrative paradigm utilized in

the study by Toth and colleagues (1997) and the AMT, is that the story-stems are

structured scenarios created to elicit emotions (e.g., injury) whereas the AMT is more

open-ended. Alternately, abused children may have regulated their emotional

response and reduced their arousal to normative levels via their use of overgeneral

memory recall as a functional avoidance strategy.

In addition to controllingness, we included a measure of the warmth of the

relationship between child and interviewer throughout the AMT paradigm. Felt

warmth during the AMT is likely to have an impact on the degree to which children

are comfortable sharing private m emories. Therefore, it w ould be important to

control for potential differences in warmth as a function of maltreatment subtype

experiences. We predicted that interactions between the interviewer and abused

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
65

children would be less warm than interactions with nonmaltreated children; however

the findings did not support this hypothesis. Instead, an unanticipated pattern

emerged such that neglected children’s interactions with the interviewer were rated as

higher in warmth than were those of the abused and nonmaltreated children, who did

not differ from one another. In retrospect, these findings are consistent with what one

could expect from neglected children, who are likely to crave the positive attention

from adults that they lack in their caregiving relationships. Additionally, increased

warmth among neglected children’s interactions with the interviewer may be related

to a caretaking behavior style that is common among children who displayed

disorganization in their attachment relationships, in which children become overly

attuned to the needs o f others during interactions to elicit a positive response

(Hennighausen & Lyons-Ruth, 2005). Even so, given the overall pattern of findings

regarding relationship variables, with no differences in controllingness or warmth

between the abused and nonmaltreated children and only evidence of increased

warmth among neglected children, it was not necessary to control for these relational

variables in subsequent analyses.

5.4 Mediation o f Overgeneral Memory

5.4.1 Affective Representations

Whereas the first part of this investigation provided a comprehensive review

o f several aspects o f abused, neglected and nonmaltreated children’s autobiographical

memory recall, including aspects of both the form and content of memory, Part II was

more narrowly focused on identifying mediators of the pathway between abuse and

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
66

overgeneral memory. This analysis is informative for the understanding o f memory

processes among maltreated children, as well as for theories regarding the emergence

of overgeneral memory.

First, children’s self-representations were evaluated as potential mediators of

the relationship between trauma and overgeneral memory. Because a significant

relationship between abuse history and child representations emerged for negative

self-representations, and not for positive self-representations, only negative

representations were evaluated as a mediator. Regression analyses revealed that

negative representations did not mediate the relationship between abuse and

overgeneral memory. Although negative self-representations were significantly

related to overgeneral memory, negative self- representations did not independently

account for a significant proportion of variance in predicting overgeneral memory

when controlling for the effect of abuse. However, the strength of the relationship

between abuse and overgeneral memory decreased in the presence of negative self­

representations.

Although negative self- representations do not mediate the relationship

between abuse and overgeneral memory, the finding that negative self representations

are related to overgeneral memory has important implications for theories of

autobiographical memory, as well as for models of overgeneral memory

development. In particular, our findings corroborate the notion that conceptual self-

representations play a significant role in autobiographical memory, as theorized in

modem models o f autobiographical memory (e.g., Conway & Pleydell-Pearce; 2000;

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
67

Howe & Courage, 1993, 1997). Moreover, these findings support Williams and

colleagues’ (2007) updated model of overgeneral memory, CaR-FA-X. In this model,

elements of Conway & Pleydell-Pearce’s self-memory theory are integrated with

William’s (1996) functional avoidance (FA) theory by adding that capture and

rumination (CaR) and impaired executive control (X) might also contribute to

overgeneral memory. In particular, Williams asserts that individuals with activated

negative self- representations may become captured in their memory search at the

level of conceptual self- representations, resulting in overgeneral memory. Noting

that these conceptual representations are essentially the same as the negative self­

schemas described by traditional cognitive theorists of depression (Beck et al., 1979),

Williams and colleagues point to evidence of overgeneral memory among individuals

with depression to support a capture mechanism. However, no investigations to date

have directly examined the relationship between negative self- representation and

overgeneral memory. Therefore, the current investigation adds to the literature by

providing empirical support for the association of negative self- representations to

overgeneral memory.

Nonetheless, it is important to acknowledge that despite the significant

relationship between negative self-representations and overgeneral memory, negative

self-representations were not a significant predictor of overgeneral memory when

controlling for abuse in the current sample. Although the strength o f the relationship

between abuse and overgeneral memory decreased when negative self-representations

were entered into the regression model, it seems that experience of abuse is uniquely

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
68

related to overgeneral memory. Moreover, the data suggests that the relationship

between negative self-representations and overgeneral memory may be accounted for

through the strong relation of child abuse to negative self-representations. Because

high levels of negative self-representations were closely associated with abuse in the

current sample, it was difficult to ascertain the relation of negative self­

representations to overgeneral memory independent of child abuse. It will be

necessary for future research to continue to investigate the role of negative self-

representations in the development of overgeneral memory independently among

samples of maltreated and nonmaltreated children.

5.4.2 Depression and Dissociation

Depressive and dissociative symptoms were also examined as potential

mediators of the relationship between abuse and overgeneral autobiographical

memory. First, to establish the conditions necessary for mediation, depressive and

dissociative symptoms were evaluated as a function of subtype, revealing a

significant multivariate effect. As predicted, abused children demonstrated more

depressive and dissociative symptomatology than did the nonmaltreated children; this

pattern of results is consistent with several other investigations that have identified

internalizing symptomatology and dissociation among the sequelae of child

maltreatment (e.g., Kim & Cicchetti, 2004; Macfie et al., 2001a; Ogawa et al., 1997;

Putnam, 2000; Toth & Cicchetti, 1996; Toth, M anly & Cicchetti, 1992). Contrary to

our hypotheses, age was not significantly related to depressive or dissociative

symptoms. This finding conflicts with evidence of age-related increases in

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
69

depression and decreases in dissociation over time (Angold & Costello, 1993;

Putnam, 1996; Putnam et al., 1993). However, it should be noted that the

symptomatology demonstrated by most children in the current investigation was not

in a clinically-significant range, whereas evidence of age related changes in

symptoms have focused on clinical populations.

Next, the relation of depressive and dissociative symptoms to overgeneral

memory was assessed. In partial support of our hypotheses, depression was

significantly related to overgeneral memory, thus confirming a well-established

pattern in the literature (see Williams et al., 2007 for review). However, dissociative

symptoms were not significantly related to overgeneral memory recall as predicted.

Nonetheless, this investigation adds to our understanding of overgeneral memory by

being the first to examine whether dissociation might be a mechanism related to

overgeneral memory recall. Given the centrality of the self to autobiographical

memory processes, in addition to evidence of overgenerality among individuals with

PTSD (McNally et al., 1995), where the incidence of dissociation is high (e.g., Briere,

Scott, & Weathers, 2005; Ehlers, 2006) it seemed possible that dissociation might

contribute to overgeneral memory as a functional avoidance mechanism. However,

the findings of the current research do not support such an account. In contrast,

additional investigations of overgeneral memory among adults with PTSD suggest the

extent to w hich people experience intrusive m em ories o f negative events predicts

difficulty in retrieving specific personal memories (McNally et al., 1995). Taken

together with our findings regarding increased overgenerality among those with

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
70

negative self-representations, it appears that heightened accessibility of, or

preoccupation with, negative representations (of specific events or of the self) hinders

the ability to recall specific personal memories.

Finally, depressive symptoms were evaluated in the relationship between

abuse and overgeneral memory. Dissociative symptoms were not considered further

because of their lack of relationship to overgenerality. Contrary to our hypothesis,

hierarchical regression did not support a significant indirect pathway between abuse

and overgeneral memory via depressive symptoms. Therefore this investigation adds

clarity to our understanding of the mechanisms that underlie overgeneral memory by

demonstrating that trauma is associated with overgeneral memory, independent of

depression. This finding corroborates several other investigations that have linked

childhood trauma to overgeneral memory, even after controlling for levels of

depression (DeDecker et al., 2003; Henderson et al., 2002; Kuyken & Brewin, 1995).

However, all prior work relied on retrospective report or self-report to identify

childhood trauma among depressed samples. Therefore, the current research

validates the association between abuse in childhood and overgeneral memory, which

cannot be accounted for by depression, with a sample of children for whom

maltreatment experiences were determined independently of children’s memory and

willingness to disclose abuse.

5.5 L im ita tio n s a n d F u tu re D ire c tio n s

There are limitations to this investigation that should be addressed. For

example, although a general measure of language abilities that is highly correlated

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
71

with IQ was included and controlled for throughout our analyses, the lack of an

additional measure of memory performance or executive control precludes our ability

to differentiate overgeneral memory processes from broader deficits in general

memory performance. However, several studies have illustrated that

autobiographical memory deficits linked to trauma and depression are not accounted

for by measures of working memory (deDecker et al., 2003), general episodic

memory (e.g., immediate or delayed story recall, deDecker et al., 2003), semantic

memory (Raes, Hermans, Williams, Demytetenaere et al., 2006) or intrusive thoughts

(Henderson et al., 2002, Kuyken & Brewin, 1995). Moreover, several studies that

have controlled for additional cognitive abilities such as semantic processing speed,

semantic fluency, and IQ (Park et al., 2002; Williams & Broadbent, 1986; Williams

& Scott, 1988) found that group differences in overgeneral memory remain even

when matching for these variables. In tandem with research that supports a lack of

alterations in basic memory functioning among maltreated children (e.g., Howe et al.,

2004; Valentino et al., in press), it seems unlikely that differences in generalized

memory ability have confounded the overgeneral memory findings of the current

investigation.

Also, the current research was unable to evaluate the role of matemal-

representations in children’s memory. Unfortunately, children in this sample rarely

included their mothers in the m em ory narratives they generated during the AM T, thus

preventing analysis o f positive and negative maternal- representations in relation to

children’s overgeneral memory. The lack of maternal- references in children’s

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
72

spontaneous autobiographical memories suggests in itself that matemal-

representations may not play an important role in autobiographical memory.

However, an alternative explanation is that children’s memories did not often include

their mothers as an additional avoidance strategy to reduce the risk of experiencing

negative affect. Together with evidence that abused children display poor semantic

recall performance in comparison to nonmaltreated children when words are encoded

under matemal-referent conditions (Valentino et al., 2007), but demonstrate

comparable recall to nonmaltreated children under self-referent conditions (Valentino

et al., in press), further investigation of the role of maternal- representations in abused

children’s memory is warranted.

In addition, it should be highlighted that the data presented in this

investigation are not longitudinal in nature, thus preventing the establishment of

temporal precedence that is ideal for mediational analyses. In particular, we assumed

that child maltreatment preceded the development of negative self- representations,

when we examined negative self-representation as a potential mediator of the

relationship between abuse and overgeneral memory. Certainly, several prior

investigations have demonstrated that negative self- representations are among the

developmental sequelae o f child maltreatment, and of physical abuse in particular

(e.g. Toth et al., 1997). Nonetheless, longitudinal research will be necessary to

determine whether negative self-representations are a mediator or moderator in the

relationship between child abuse and overgeneral autobiographical memory recall.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
73

Longitudinal research would also allow us to evaluate the transactional

relationship of depressive and dissociative symptoms to overgeneral memory recall

over time. The findings of the current investigation demonstrate that depressive

symptoms do not mediate the relationship between abuse and overgeneral memory.

However, it should be noted that most of the depressive symptomatology

demonstrated by the children in this sample was in the subclinical range; therefore, it

remains unclear how clinical depression might play a role in overgeneral memory

recall. Given the high rate of negative self-representations among the abused children

in this sample, there is reason to anticipate the development of clinically significant

levels of depression, particularly as these children reach adolescence, where the

incidence o f depression is high (e.g., Lewinsohn, Rohde, & Seeley, 1998). Thus,

prospective longitudinal research would inform our understanding of the role of

depression in overgeneral memory, and of overgenerality as a potential marker of

vulnerability to the development of depression.

5.6 Clinical Implications

The main findings o f this investigation, which demonstrate that overgeneral

autobiographical recall is more frequent among abused children in comparison to

nonmaltreated children possess important clinical implications. The finding that child

abuse is associated with overgeneral memory is particularly significant, considering

evidence that overgeneral m em ory is itself associated with several important aspects

of psychological functioning. For example, quite a few studies have demonstrated

that overgenerality is predictive of delayed recovery from episodes of affective

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
74

disorders (Brittlebank et al., 1993; Dalgliesh et al., 2003; Harvey, Bryant, & Dang,

1998; Peeters et al., 2002). Moreover, overgeneral memory is clinically significant

because memory remains overgeneral in those with a history of emotional disorder,

even if they are not currently in an episode (e.g., Mackinger, Pachinger, Liebetseder

& Faracek, 2000; Williams & Dritchel, 1998). Therefore, overgeneral memory might

serve as a marker for future vulnerability for depression. In fact, there is evidence

that overgeneral memory when one is not depressed predicts later symptomatology

(e.g., Gibbs & Rude, 2004; Mackinger et al., 2000). However, no investigations to

date have prospectively examined overgenerality in childhood in relation to later

emotional disturbance. Thus, a longitudinal follow-up of the current sample could

address whether overgenerality is a risk factor associated with the development of

depression.

Theoretically, overgenerality is thought to increase an individual’s

vulnerability to depression because an inability to recall the details of past

experiences impairs problem solving ability. In fact, lack of memory specificity is

related to deficits in interpersonal problem solving and poorer outcomes for

individuals in therapy (Brittlebank et al., 1993; Evans, Williams, O’Loughlin, &

Howells, 1992; Sidley, Whitaker, Calam, & Wells, 1997; Williams & Dritschel,

1988), as well as problems in imagining future events, such that overgenerality for

past events predicts overgenerality in specifying future events (W illiam s, Ellis, Tyers,

Healy, Rose, & MacLeod, 1996). Therefore, interventions that target memory

specificity for day-to-day experiences have been proposed to improve outcomes

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
75

(Johnson et al., 2005). For example Serrano, Latorre, Gatz, and Rodriguez (2004)

examined the impact o f a 4-week intervention designed to increase specificity of

positive memories in depressed elderly patients. Results revealed that overgenerality

was reduced following intervention, as were symptoms of hopelessness and

depression. Thus, it seems that overgeneral memory is an important factor in

maintaining depression and its modification can positively impact its course.

Finally, it is important to discuss the implications of the current investigation

for the forensic arena. Research on children’s memories often effects decisions

regarding the viability o f child testimony in the courtroom as well as whether officials

are confident in relying on child report to substantiate abuse and neglect (Toth &

Valentino, in press). However, it is essential to acknowledge that studies that assess

children’s memory and suggestibility for mundane events may have little applicability

to our understanding of children’s reporting of significant, often traumatic, events in

their lives. In fact, because forensic interviews are mainly interested in the recall of

central details regarding salient emotional experiences, empirical evidence supports

the accuracy o f traumatized children’s memory reports (e.g., Alexander et al., 2005,

Howe, Courage & Peterson, 1994,1995). Therefore, the finding of the current

investigation, that abused children have more difficulty retrieving specific

autobiographical memories than do nonmaltreated children in response to non­

trauma-related prompts, should not be generalized to abused children’s ability to

accurately recall traumatic memories. That being said, law officials should be

sensitive to impairments in abused children's ability to recall specific memories and

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
76

should be careful to not misinterpret abused children’s recollection of overgeneral or

vague memories as indicative of a lack of cooperation to disclose personal

experiences.

In summary, this investigation addresses significant gaps in the extant

literature on the relation of trauma to memory among maltreated children by

providing a comprehensive assessment of abused, neglected and nonmaltreated

children’s autobiographical memories for non-trauma related experiences. The

findings revealed that abused children, but not neglected children, demonstrated

difficulty in retrieving specific autobiographical memories compared to the

nonmaltreated children. The examination of additional mechanisms that may

contribute to overgeneral autobiographical memory among abused children

demonstrated that negative self-representations, as well as depressive and dissociative

symptomatology, did not mediate the relation of abuse to overgeneral memory.

These findings substantiate theories that have emphasized the role of early trauma in

the development of overgeneral memory by providing the first evidence of

overgeneral memory among a sample of maltreated children. It will be important for

future research with maltreated children to further investigate the affect of children’s

internal representations on the development of adaptive functioning, and for clinical

interventions to target the reduction of negative self-representations in particular.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
77

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Integrative guide fo r the 1991 CBCL/4-18, YSR, and TRF

profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont.

Alexander, K., Quas, J. A., Goodman, G. S., Ghetti, S., Edelstein, R., Redlich, A., et

al. (2005). Traumatic impact predicts long-term memory of documented child

sexual abuse. Psychological Science, 16, 33-40.

Angold, A., & Costello, E.J. (1993). Depressive comorbidity in children and

adolescents: Empirical, theoretical, and methodological issues. American

Journal o f Psychiatry, 150(12), 1779-1791.

American PsychiatricAssociation. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual o f

mental disorders, 4th edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric

Association.

Barnett, D., Ganiban, J., & Cicchetti, D. (1999). Maltreatment, negative expressivity,

and the development of Type D attachments from 12- to 24-months of age.

Society fo r Research in Child Development Monograph, 64, 97-118.

Barnett, D., Manly, J. T., & Cicchetti, D. (1993). Defining child maltreatment: The

interface between policy and research. In D. Cicchetti & S. L. Toth (Eds.),

Child abuse, child development, and social policy (pp. 7-73). Norwood, NJ:

Ablex.

Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). C o g n itive th e ra p y o f

depression. New York: Guilford Press.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
78

Beeghly, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1994). Child maltreatment, attachment, and the self

system: Emergence of an internal state lexicon in toddlers at high social risk.

Development and Psychopathology, 6, 5-30.

Beers, S. R., & DeBellis, M. D. (2002). Neuropsychological function in children with

maltreatment-related posttraumatic stress disorder. American Journal o f

Psychiatry, 159(3), 483-486.

Bird, A., & Reese, E. (2006). Emotional reminiscing and the development of an

autobiographical self. Developmental Psychology, 42, 613-626.

Black, M., Dubowitz, H., & Harrington, D. (1994). Sexual abuse: Developmental

differences in children's behavior and self-perception. Child Abuse & Neglect,

18, 85-95.

Bolger, K. E., Patterson, C. J., & Kuperschmidt, J. B. (1998). Peer relationships and

self-esteem among chidlren who have been maltreated. Child Development,

69, 1171-1197.

Bower, G. H., & Sivers, H. (1998). Cognitive impact of traumatic events.

Development and Psychopathology, 10, 625-654.

Bremner, J. D., Krystal, J. H., Southwick, S. M., & Chamey, D. S. (1995). Functional

neuroanatomical correlates of the effects of stress on memory. Journal o f

Traumatic Stress, 8, 527-553.

Bremner, J. D ., & Narayan, M . (1998). The effects o f stress on m em ory and the

hippocampus throughout the life cycle: Implications for childhood

development and aging. Development and Psychopathology, 10, 871-885.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
79

Brewin, C. R., Watson, M., McCarthy, S., Hyman, P., & Dayson, D. (1998). Intrusive

memories and depression in cancer patients. Behavior Research and Therapy,

36, 1131-1142.

Briere, J., Scott, C., & Weathers, F. (2005). Peritraumatic and persistent dissociation

in the presumed etiology of PTSD. American Journal o f Psychiatry, 162,

2295-23012.

Brittlebank, A. D., Scott, J., Williams, J. M. G., & Ferrier, I. N. (1996).

Autobiographical memory in depression: State or trait marker? British Journal

o f Psychiatry, 1 6 2 , 118-121.

Brack, M., Ceci, S. J., Francoeur, E., & Barr, R. (1995). "I hardly cried when I got

my shot!": Influencing children's reports about a visit to a pediatrician. Child

Development, 66, 193-208.

Buckner, J. P., & Fivush, R. (1998). Gender and self in children's autobiographical

narratives. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 1 2 , 407-429.

Bugental, D., Blue, J., Cortez, V., Fleck, K., & Rodriguez, A. (1992). Influences of

witnessed affect on information processing in children. Child Development,

63, 774-786.

Burgess, P. W., & Shallice, T. (1996). Confabulation and the control of recollection.

Memory, 4, 359-411.

Burnside, E., Startup, M ., R ollinson, L., & Hill, J. (2004). The role of overgeneral

autobiographical memory in the development of adult depression following

childhood trauma. British Journal o f Clinical Psychology, 43, 365-376.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
80

Carlson, E. A. (1998). A prospective longitudinal study of attachment

disorganziation/disorientation. Child Development, 69(4), 1107-1128.

Carlson, E. B., Furby, L., Armstrong, J., & Shales, J. (1997). A conceptual framework

for the long-term psychological effects of severe and chronic childhood abuse.

Child Maltreatment, 2, 272-295.

Carlson, V., & Cicchetti, D. (1979). Demographics Interview. Cambridge, MA:

Harvard University.

Christianson, S. A. (1992). Emotional stress and eyewitness memory: A critical

review. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 284-309.

Cicchetti, D., & Manly, J. T. (1990). A personal perspective on conducting research

with maltreating families: Problems and solutions. In G. Brody & I. Sigel

(Eds.), Methods o f family research: Families at risk (Vol. 2, pp. 87-133).

Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifmality and multifmality in

developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 597-

600.

Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1997). The role of self-organization in the promotion

of resilience in maltreated children. Development and Psychopathology, 9,

799-817.

Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2001a). Diverse patterns of neuroendocrine activity

in maltreated children. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 677-694.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
81

Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2001b). The impact of child maltreatment and

psychopathology upon neuroendocrine functioning. Development and

Psychopathology, 13, 783-804.

Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S.L. (1998). The development of depression in children and

adolescents. American Psychologist, 53(2), 221-241.

Cicchetti, D., Toth, S. L., & Manly, J. T. (2003). Maternal Maltreatment

/nteraew.Unpublished manuscript, Rochester, NY.

Cicchetti, D., & Valentino, K. (2006). An ecological transactional perspective on

child malteatment: Failure of the average expectable environment and its

influence upon child development. In D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen (Eds.),

Developmental Psychopathology (2nd ed.): Risk Disorder, and Adaptation

(Vol. 3, pp. 129-201). New York: Wiley.

Conway, M.A. (2004). Autobiographical memory and self. Australian Journal o f

Psychology, 56, 171-171.

Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The contraction of

autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review,

107, 261-288.

Cordon, I. M., Pipe, M., Sayfan, L., Melinder, A., & Goodman, G. S. (2004).

Memory for traumatic experiences in early childhood. Developmental Review,

24 , 101-132.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
82

Coster, W.J., Beeghly, M., Gersten, M.S., & Cicchetti, D. (1989). Communicative

functioning in maltreated toddlers. Developmental Psychology, 35(6), 1020-

1029.

Dalgliesh, T., Spinks, H., Yiend, J., & Kuyken, W. (2001). Autobiographical memory

style in seasonal affective disorder and its reltionship to future symptom

remission. Journal o f Abnormal Psychology, 110, 335-340.

Dalgliesh, T., Tchanturia, K., Serpell, L., Hems, S., Yiend, J., De Silva, P., et al.

(2003). Self-reported parental abuse relates to autobiographical memory style

in patients with eating disorders. Emotion, 3 , 211-222.

Dalgliesh, T., Williams, J. M. G., Golden, A. M. J., Barnard, P. J., Au-Yeung, C., &

al., e. (in press). Reduced specificity of autobiographical memory and

depression: The role of executive processes. Journal o f Experimental

Psychology: General.

Dancu, C. V., Riggs, D. S., Hearst-Ikeda, D., Shoyer, B. G., & Foa, E. B. (1996).

Dissociative experiences and posttraumatic stress disorder among female

victims o f criminal assault and rape. Journal o f Traumatic Stress, 9, 253-261.

Davis, P. J. (1999). Gender differences in autobiographical memories for childhood

emotional experiences. Journal o f Personality and Social Psychology, 76,

498-510.

D eB ellis, M. D ., Keshavan, M. S., Casey, B. J., Clark, D. B., Giedd, J., Boring, A.

M., et al. (1999). Developmental traumatology: Biological stress systems and

brain development in maltreated children with PTSD part II: The relationship

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
83

between characteristics of trauma and psychiatric symptoms and adverse brain

development in maltreated children and adolescents with PTSD. Biological

Psychiatry, 45, 1271-1284.

deDecker, A., Hermans, D., Raes, F., & Eelen, P. (2003). Autobiographical memory

specificity and trauma in inpatient adolescents. Journal o f Clinical Child and

Adolscent Psychology, 32, 22-31.

Dickinson, J J., & Poole, D.A. (2000). Efficient coding o f eyewitness narratives: A

comparison of syntactic unit and word count procedures. Behavior Research

Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 32 (4), 537-545.

Dubowitz, H., Pitts, S. C., Litrownik, A. J., Cox, C. E., Runyan, D. K., & Black, M.

(2005). Defining child neglect based upon child protective services data. Child

Abuse & Neglect, 29(5), 493-511.

Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody picture vocabulary test-revised. Circle

Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody picture vocabulary test (3rd ed.). Circle

Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

Egeland, B., Sroufe, L. A., & Erickson, M. F. (1983). Developmental consequence of

different patterns of maltreatment. Child Abuse and Neglect, 7, 459-469.

Ehlers, A. (2006). More evidence for the role of persistent dissociation in PTSD.

A m erica n J o u rn a l o f P syc h ia try, 1 6 3 (6 ), 1112-1112.

Eich, E. (1995). Searching for mood-dependent memory. Psychological Science, 6,

67-75.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
84

Eisen, M. L., Goodman, G. S., Qin, J., & Davis, S. L. (2005). Maltreated children's

memory: Accuracy, suggestibility, and psychopathology. Manuscript

submitted fo r publication.

Eisen, M. L., Qin, J., Goodman, G. S., & Davis, S. L. (2002). Memory and

suggestibility in maltreated children: Age, stress arousal, dissociation and

psychopathology. Journal o f Experimental Child Psychology, 83, 167-212.

English, D. E., Bangdiwala, S. I., & Runyan, D. K. (2005). The dimensions of child

maltreatment: introduction. Child Abuse & Neglect, 29(5), 441-460.

Evans, J., Williams, J. M. G., O'Loughlin, S., & Howells, K. (1992).

Autobiographical memory and problem-solving strategies of parasuicide

patients. Psychological Medicine, 22, 399-405.

Fivush, R., Haden, C. A., & Reese, E. (2006). Elaborating on elaborations: Role of

maternal reminscing style in cognitive and socioemotional development.

Child Development, 77(6), 1568-1588.

Fivush, R., Hazzard, A., Sales, J.M., Sarfati, D., & Brown, T. (2003). Creating

coherence out of chaos? Children’s narratives of emotionally positive and

negative events. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17(1), 1-19.

Foa, E. B., & Riggs, D. S. (1994). Post-traumatic stress disorder and rape. In R. S.

Pynoos (Ed.), Post-traumatic stress disorder: A clinical review (pp. 207-224).

Baltim ore, MD: Plenum Press.

Gibbs, B. R., & Rude, S. F. (2004). Overgeneral autobiographical memory as

depression vulnerability. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 25(4), 511-526.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
85

Goddard, L., Dritschel, B. H., & Burton, A. (1996). Role of autobiographical memory

in social problem solving and depression. Journal o f Abnormal Psychology,

105, 609-616.

Goddard, L., Dritschel, B. H., & Burton, A. (1997). Social problem solving and

autobiographical memory in non-clinical depression. British Journal o f

Clinical Psychology, 36, 449-451.

Goodman, G. S. (2005). Wailing babies in her wake. American Psychologist, 872-

881.

Goodman, G. S., Bottoms, B. L., Rudy, L., Davis, S. L., & Schwartz-Kenny, B. M.

(2001). Effects of past abuse experiences on children's eyewitness memory.

Law and Human Behavior, 25, 269-298.

Goodman, G. S., Hirschman, J., Hepps, D., & Rudy, L. (1991). Children's memory

for stressful events. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 37, 109-158.

Goodman, G. S., & Quas, J. A. (1997). Trauma and memory: Individual differences

in children's recounting of a stressful experience. In N. L. Stein, A. Omstein,

B. Tversky & C. J. Brainerd (Eds.), Memory fo r everyday and emotional

events. Mahwah: NJ: Erlbaum.

Greenhoot, A. F., Johnson, R., & McCloskey, L. (in press). Internal states language in

the childhood recollections of adolescents with and without abuse histories.

J o u rn a l o f C ogn ition a n d D evelo p m en t.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
86

Greenhoot, A. F., McCloskey, L., & Glisky, E. (2005). A longitudinal study of

adolescents' recollections of family violence. Applied Cognitive Psychology,

19, 719-743.

Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. (1989). The self as a memory system: Powerful but

ordinary. Journal o f Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 41-54.

Hammen, C., & Zupan, B. (1984). Self-schemas, depression, and the processing of

personal information in children. Journal o f Experimental Child Psychology,

37, 598-608.

Harter, S. (1998). The development of self-representations. In W. Damon & N.

Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook o f child psychology: Social, emotional, and

personality development (5th ed., Vol. 4, pp. 553-617). New York: Wiley.

Harvey, A. G., Bryant, R. A., & Dang, S. T. (1998). Autobiographical memory in

acute stress disorder. Journal o f Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 500-

506.

Henderson, D., Hargreaves, I., Gregory, S., & Williams, J. M. G. (2002).

Autobiographical memory and emotion in a nonclinical sample of women

with and without a reported history of childhood sexual abuse. British Journal

o f Clinical Psychology, 41, 129-141.

Hennighausen, K., & Lyons-Ruth, K. (2005). Disorganization o f attachment

s tr a te g ie s in infan cy a n d ch ildh ood. Retrieved Februrary, 2007, from

http://www.excellence-earlychildhood.ca/documents/Hennighausen-

LyonsRuthANGxp.pdf

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
87

Hildyard, K. L., & Wolfe, D. A. (2002). Child neglect: Developmental issues and

outcomes. Child Abuse and Neglect, 26, 679-695.

Hollingshead, A. (1975). Four-factor index o f social status. Unpublished manuscript,

Yale University.

Howe, M. L. (1998). Individual differences in factors that modulate storage and

retrieval of traumatic memories. Development and Psychopathology, 10, 681-

698.

Howe, M. L. (2000). The fate o f early memories: Developmental science and the

retention o f childhood experiences. Washington, D.C.: American

Psychological Association.

Howe, M. L., Cicchetti, D., Toth, S. L., & Cerrito, B. M. (2004). True and false

memories in maltreated children. Child Development, 75(5), 1402-1417.

Howe, M. L., & Courage, M. L. (1993). On resolving the enigma o f infantile

amnesia. Psychological Bulletin, 305-326.

Howe, M. L., & Courage, M. L. (1997). The emergence and early development of

autobiographical memory. Psychological Review, 104(3), 499-523.

Howe, M. L., Courage, M. L., & Edison, S. (2003). When autobiographical memory

begins. Developmental Review, 23, 471-494.

Howe, M. L., Courage, M. L., & Peterson, C. (1994). How can I remember when "I"

wasn't there: Long-term retention o f traumatic experiences and the emergence

of the cogntive self. Consciousness and Cognition, 3, 327-355.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
88

Howe, M. L., Courage, M. L., & Peterson, C. (1995). Intrusions in preschooler's

recall o f traumatic childhood events. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, ISO-

134.

Howe, M. L., Toth, S. L., & Cicchetti, D. (2006). Memory and Developmental

Psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental

Psychopathology (2nd Edition), Volume 1: Theory and Method (pp. 629-655).

New York: Wiley.

Hyman, I. E., & Billings, F. G. (1998). Individual differences and the creation of false

memories. Memory, 6, 1-20.

Johnson, R. J., Greenhoot, A. F., Glisky, E., & McCloskey, L. (2005). The relations

among abuse, depression and adolescents' autobiographical memory. Journal

o f Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 34(2), 235-247.

Kagan, J. (1981). The second year: The emergence o f self-awareness. Cambridge,

MA: Harvard University Press.

Kim, J., & Cicchetti, D. (2004). A process model of mother-child relatedness and

psychological adjustment among maltreated and nonmaltreated children: The

role of self-esteem and social competence. Journal o f Abnormal Child

Psychology, 32, 341-354.

Kim, J. E., & Cicchetti, D. (2003). Social self-efficacy and behavior problems in

maltreated and nonmaltreated children. Journal o f Clinical Child and

Adolescent Psychology, 32, 106-117.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
89

Klien, S. B., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1986). Elaboration, organization, and self-reference

effects in memory. Journal o f Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 26-38.

Kovacs, M. (1981). Rating scales to assess depression in school-aged children. Acta

Paedopsychiatrica, 46, 305-315.

Kovacs, M. (1992). Children’s Depression Inventory: Manual. New York: Multi-

Health Systems.

Kuyken, W., & Brewin, C. R. (1995). Autobiographical memory functioning in

depression and reports of early abuse. Journal o f Abnormal Psychology, 104,

585-591.

Lewinsohn, P. M., Rhode, P., & Seeley, J. R. (1998). Major depressive disorder in

older adolescents: prevalence, risk factors, and clinical implications. Clinical

Psychology Review, 18, 765-794.

Liotti, G. (1999). Disorganization of attachment as a model for understanding

dissociative psychopathology. In J. Solomon & C. George (Eds.), Attachment

Disorganization (pp. 291-317). London: Guilford Press.

Macfie, J., Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2001a). Dissociation in maltreated versus

nonmaltreated preschool-aged children. Child Abuse and Neglect, 25, 1253-

1267.

Macfie, J., Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2001b). The development o f dissociation in

maltreated preschool-aged children. D e v e lo p m e n t a n d P sy c h o p a th o lo g y , 13,

233-254.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
90

Mackinger, H. F., Pachinger, M. M., Leibetseder, M. M., & Fartacek, R. R. (2000).

Autobiographical memories in women remitted from major depression.

Journal o f Abnormal Psychology, 109, 331-334.

Manly, J. T. (2005). Advances in research definitions of child maltreatment. Child

Abuse & Neglect, 29(5), 425-439.

Manly, J. T., Cicchetti, D., & Barnett, D. (1994). The impact of subtype, frequency,

chronicity, and severity of child maltreatment on social competence and

behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 121-143.

Manly, J. T., Kim, J. E., Rogosch, F. A., & Cicchetti, D. (2001). Dimensions of child

maltreatment and children's adjustment: Contributions of developmental

timing and subtype. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 759-782.

Mannar, C. R., Weiss, D. S., & Metzler, T. (1996). Peritraumatic dissociation and

posttraumatic stress disorder. In J. D. Bremner & C. R. Mannar (Eds.),

Trauma, memory, and dissociation (pp. 229-252). Washington, D.C.:

American Psychiatric Press.

McNally, R. J., Lasko, N. B., Macklin, M. L., & Pitman, R. K. (1995).

Autobiographical memory disturbance in combat-related posttraumatic stress

disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 619-630.

Merritt, K. A., Omstein, P. A., & Spicker, B. (1994). Children's memory for a salient

m edical procedure: Implications for testim ony. P e d ia tr ic s, 9 4 , 17-23.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
91

Moradi, A. R., Taghavi, M. R., Doost, H. T. N., Yule, W., & Dalgliesh, T. (1999).

Performance o f children and adolscents with PTSD on the Stroop colour-

naming task. Psychological Medicine, 29(415-419).

Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2004). The emergence of autobiographical memory: A

social cultural developmental theory. Psychological Review, 111(2), 486-511.

Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration

of depressive episodes. Journal o f Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569-582.

Ogawa, J. R., Sroufe, L. A., Weinfield, N. S., Carlson, E. A., & Egeland, B. (1997).

Development and the fragmented self: A longitudinal study of dissociative

symptomatology in a normative sample. Development and Psychopathology,

9, 855-879.

Orbach, Y., Lamb, M. E., Sternberg, K. J., Williams, J. M. G., & Dawud-Noursi, S.

(2001). The effect o f being a victim or witness of family violence on the

retrieval o f autobiographical memories. CMld Abuse and Neglect, 2 5 , 1427-

1437.

Park, R. J., Goodyear, I. M., & Teasdale, J. D. (2002). Categoric overgeneral

autobiographical memory in adolescent major depression. Psychological

Medicine, 32, 267-276.

Peeters, F., Wessel, I., Merchelbach, H., & Boon-Vermeeren, M. (2001).

Autobiographical memory and the course of depressive disorder, submitted fo r

publication.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
92

Peeters, F., Wessel, I., Merchelbach, H., & Boon-Vermeeren, M. (2002).

Autobiographical memory specificity and the course of major depressive

disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 43, 344-350.

Peters, D. P. (1987). The impact of naturally occuring stress on children's memory. In

S. J. Ceci, M. P. Toglia & D. F. Ross (Eds.), Children's eyewitness memory

(pp. 122-141). New York: Springer-Verlag.

Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.

Porter, C., Lawson, J. S., & Bigler, E. D. (2005). Neurobehavioral sequelae of child

sexual abuse. Child Neuropsychology, 11(2), 203-220.

Post, R. M., Weiss, S. R. B., Li, H., Smith, M. A., Zhang, L. X., Xing, G., et al.

(1998). Neural plasticity and emotional memory. Development and

Psychopathology, 10(4), 829-855.

Putnam, F. W. (1996). Development of dissociative disorders. In D. Cicchetti & D.

Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Risk, disorder, and

adaptation (Vol. 2, pp. 581-608). New York: Wiley.

Putnam, F. (1997). Dissociation in children and adolescents: A developmental

perspective. New York: Guilford.

Putnam, F. W. (2000). Dissociative disorders. In A. Sameroff, M. Lewis & S. Miller

(Eds.), Handbook o f developmental psychopathology (Vol. 771, pp. 708-715).

N ew York: K luw er Academ ic/Plenum Publishers.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
93

Putnam, F.W., Helmers, K., & Trickett, P.K. (1993). Development, reliability and

validity o f a child dissociation scale. Child Abuse and Neglect, 17(6), 731-

741.

Raes, F., Hermans, D., Williams, J. M. G., Demyttenaere, K., Sabbe, B., Pieters, G.,

& Eelen, P. (2005). Reduced specificity of autobiographical memories: A

mediator between rumination and ineffective problem solving in major

depression? Journal of Affective Disorders, 87, 331-335.

Raes, F., Hermans, D., Williams, J. M. G., & Eelen, P. (2006). Reduced

autobiographical memory specificity and affect regulation. Cognition &

Emotion, 20, 402-429.

Reese, E., Haden, C. A., & Fivush, R. (1996). Mothers, fathers, daughters, sons:

Gender differences in autobiographical reminiscing. Research on Language

and Social Interaction, 29(1), 27-56.

Robinson, J., Mantz-Simmons, L., Macfie, J., & The MacArthur Narrative Working

Group (2000). The narrative coding manual- Rochester Revision.

Unpublished manuscript.

Rubin, D. C. (1986). Autobiographical memory. New York: Cambridge University

Press.

Rudolph, K. D., Hammen, C., & Burge, D. (1995). Cognitive representations of self,

family, and peers in school-age children: Links with social com petence and

sociometric status. Child Development, 66(5), 1385-1402.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
94

Sales, J. M., Fivush, R., & Peterson, C. (2003). Parental reminscing about positive

and negative events. Journal o f Cognition and Development, 4, 185-209.

Sapolsky, R. M. (1992). Stress, the aging brain, and the mechanisms o f neuron death.

Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Sayfan, L., Mitchell, E., Goodman, G. S., Eisen, M. L., Qin, J., & Davis, S. L. (2002).

Children's emotional reaction when they disclose abuse, unpublished

maunscript.

Schneider-Rosen, K., & Cicchetti, D. (1991). Early self-knowledge and emotional

development: Visual self-recognition and affective reactions to mirror self-

image in maltreated and nonmaltreated toddlers. Developmental Psychology,

27, 481-488.

Serrano, J. P., Latorre, J. M., Gatz, M., & Rodriguez, J. M. (2004). Life review

therapy using autobiographical retrieval practice for older adults with

depressive symptomatology. Psychology and Aging, 19, 272-277.

Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1998). Reactive aggression among maltreated children:

The contributions o f attention and emotion dysregulation. Journal o f Clinical

Child Psychology, 27, 381-395.

Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (2001). Parental maltreatment and emotion dysregulation

as risk factors for bullying and victimization in middle childhood. Journal o f

Clinical Child Psychology, 30, 349-363.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
95

Sidley, G. L., Whitaker, K., Calam, R., & Wells, A. (1997). The relationship between

problem-solving and autobiographical memory in parasuicide patients.

Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 25, 195-202.

Siegel, D. J. (2001). Memory: An overview, with emphasis on developmental,

interpersonal, and neurobiological aspects. Journal o f the American Academy

o f Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(9), 997-1011.

Smetana, J. G., Toth, S. L., Cicchetti, D., Bruce, J., Kane, P., & Daddis, C. (1999).

Maltreated and nonmaltreated preschoolers' conceptions of hypothetical and

actual moral trangressions. Developmental Psychology, 35, 269-281.

Smucker, M. R., Craighead, W. E., Wilcoxan Craighead, L., & Green, B. J. (1986).

Normative and reliability data for the Children's Depression Inventory.

Journal o f Abnormal Child Psychology, 14, 25-39.

Sternberg, K., Lamb, M., Greenbaum, C., Cicchetti, D., Dawud, S., Cortes, R., et al.

(1993). Effects o f domestic violence on children's behavior problems and

depression. Developmental Psychology, 29, 44-52.

Toth, S. L., & Cicchetti, D. (1996). Patterns of relatedness and depressive

symptomatology in maltreated children. Journal o f Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 64, 32-41.

Toth S.L., & Cicchetti, D. (1998). Remembering, forgetting, and the effects of

trauma on memory: A developm ental psychopathology perspective.

Development & Psychopathology, 10(4), 589-605.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
96

Toth, S. L., Cicchetti, D., Macfie, J., & Emde, R. N. (1997). Representations of self

and other in the narratives of neglected, physically abused, and sexually

abused preschoolers. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 781-796.

Toth, S. L., Cicchetti, D., Macfie, J., Maughan, A., & VanMeenan, K. (2000).

Narrative representations of caregivers and self in maltreated preschoolers.

Attachment and Human Development, 2, 271-305.

Toth, S. L., Manly, J. T., & Cicchetti, D. (1992). Child maltreatment and vulnerability

to depression. Development and Psychopathology, 4, 97-112.

Valentino, K., Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Toth, S. L. (in press). True and false

memory and dissociation among maltreated children: The role of self-schema.

Development and Psychopathology.

Valentino, K., Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Toth, S. L. (2007). Memory, maternal

representations and internalizing symptomatology among abused, neglected

and nonmaltreated children. Manuscript submitted for publication.

Vondra, J., Barnett, D., & Cicchetti, D. (1989). Perceived and actual competence

among maltreated and comparison school children. Development and

Psychopathology, 7, 237-255.

Wessel, I., Meeren, M., Peeters, F., Amtz, A., & Merchelbach, H. (2001). Correlates

of autobiographical memory specificity: The role of depression, anxiety and

childhood trauma. B e h a v io r R esea rch a n d T herapy, 3 9 , 409-421.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
97

Wheeler, M. A., Stuss, D. T., & Tulving, E. (1997). Toward a theory o f episodic

memory: the frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness. Psychological

Bulletin, 121, 331-354.

Widom, C. S. (1989). Child abuse, neglect, and adult behavior: Research design and

findings on criminality, violence, and child abuse. American Journal o f

Orthopsychiatry, 59, 355-367.

Widom, C.S., DuMont, K., Czaja, S. (2007). A prospective investigation of major

depressive disorder and comorbidity in abused and neglected children grown

up. Archives o f General Psychiatry, 64(1), 49-56.

Williams, J. M. G. (1996). Depression and the specificity of autobiographical

memory. In D. C. Rubin (Ed.), Remembering our past: Studies in

autobiographical memory (pp. 244-267). Cambridge, England: Cambridge

University Press.

Williams, J. M. G., & Broadbent, K. (1986). Autobiographical memory in suicide

attempters. Journal o f Abnormal Psychology, 95, 144-149.

Williams, J.M.G., Bamhofer, T., Crane, C., & Beck, A.T. (2005). Problem solving

deteriorates following mood challenge in formerly depressed patients with a

history of suicidal ideation. Journal o f Abnormal Psychology, 114(3), 421-

431.

W illiam s, J. M. G., Crane, C., Hermans, D ., Raes, F., Watkins, E., & D algliesh, T.

(2007). Autobiographical memory specificity and emotional disorder.

Psychological Bulletin, 733(1), 122-148.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
98

Williams, J. M. G., & Dritschel, B. H. (1988). Emotional disturbance and the

specificity of autobiographical memory. Cognition & Emotion, 2, 221-234.

Williams, J. M. G., Ellis, N. C., Tyers, C., Healy, H., Rose, G., & MacLeod, A. K.

(1996). The specificity of autobiographical memory and imageability of the

future. Memory & Cognition, 24, 116-125.

Williams, J. M. G., & Scott, J. (1988). Autobiographical memory in depression.

Psychological Medicine, 18, 689-695.

Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1997). Cognitive

psychology and emotional disorders (2nd ed.). Chichester, England: Wiley.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
99

Table 1

Demographic Characteristic Means and Standard Deviations for Abused, Neglected


and Nonmaltreated Children

Abused Neglected Non-Maltreated


(N= 361 (N= 341 (N= 115)

Mean Child Age


Years 10.69(1.6) 10.78(1.7) 10.51(1.5)

Child Gender
Male 66.7% 55.9% 47%
Female 33.3% 44.1% 53%

Family Marital Status


Single 61.1% 78.1% 67.3%

Ethnicity
Minority 69.4% 79.4% 85.2%

SES(Hollingshead)
Lowest two social strata 86.1% 68.8% 73.5%

Cognitive Performance**
PPVT-III(standard score) 86.78(10.2)a 93.84(10.6)b 96.52(12.9)b

a Indicates significant group differences, see text for explanation;


*p < .05* (2-tailed), **p <.01

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
100

Table 2
Reliability Analyses

Variable Aloha
Positive Overgeneral Memories .86
Negative Overgeneral Memories .83
Positive Prompts .88
Negative Prompts .92
Positive Response Latency .98
Negative Response Latency .99
Positive Memory Length .99
Negative Memory Length .99
Positive Child Representations .81
Negative Child Representations .74
Positive Maternal Representations .83
Negative Maternal Representations .89
Controllingness .91
Warmth .71

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
101

Table 3

Control/Opposition Scale
(Adapted from the MacArthur narrative coding manual- Rochester Revision, MNCM-
RR; Robinson, Mantz-Simmons, Macfie, & MacArthur Narrative Working Group,
2000 )
Rating

(1-3) No controlling or opposition behavior, child shows appropriate


engagement. At the lower end, the child is co-operative, engaged
throughout; at the higher end of the band, child may be initially mildly
resistant but becomes truly engaged with the task

(4-6) Child is resistant, requires some prompting to complete the task, may
express dislike for the prompts, completes them in a minimal or
avoidant way but without changing the essential nature of the task or
threatening refusal in any way. At the low end is the child who
resists more passively or quietly, gives minimal responses in order to
hurry up and finish so he/she can move on to something else; at the
high end of the band is the child who resists but without much
prompting finishes the task

(7-9) Child’s resistance to the task requires much prompting and


encouraging from the examiner in order to complete it, however the
child does complete the task. The child attempts to take over, control
the task and stories, may change essential nature of the task by
attempting to introduce new materials not on the table or changing the
prompts. At the low end, the child is quite resistant or controlling but
there is no risk that the task will not be completed in some form; at the
high end o f the band the child barely finishes the task

(10-12) Child is openly hostile, rude or provocative in behavior, criticizing of


the task, and refusing to co-operate such that the task is not
completed, despite attempts on the examiner’s part to redirect the
child to the task.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
102

Table 4

Global Warmth/Friendliness of the Interaction


(Adapted from the MacArthur narrative coding manual- Rochester Revision, MNCM-
RR; Robinson, Mantz-Simmons, Macfie, & MacArthur Narrative Working Group,
2000)

Rating

(1-3) Cold, unfriendly relating between child and examiner such that they do
not like each other

(4-9) Neutral relating, the child and examiner could “take or leave” each
other

(10-12) Warm, positive relating between the child and examiner, a significant
connection and engagement is noted

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

103

Table 5
Correlation Matrix: Gender and Cognitive Performance with Dependent Variables

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16
1. Gender -.0 2 .15* .13 .13 .14 -.0 1 -.07 -.19** -.28** -.14 -.07 -.14 -.08 .23** -.28**

00
*
2. PPVT-III -.31** -.17* -.26** -.08 -.06 .04 .01 -.14 -.08 .04 -.08 -.34** .24**

1
3. PGeneral 44 ** .34** .32** .1 2 .1 2 -.41** . 36** - 1 9 ** .05 -.1 2 -.0 1 .40** -.33**
4. NGeneral .32** .51** .1 1 .1 2 -.19* -.18* -.14 .15* -.09 -.07 34 ** -.27**

5 .PPrompt .6 8 ** -.04 .05 .08 .09 -.01 .2 0 ** -.09 .04 .46** 19**

6 . NPrompt .05 .1 2 .1 0 .15* -.05 .28** -.07 -.0 2 .39** -.1 2

- 2 2 ** -.18* ** i **

*
*
On
29

00
7. PLatency .04 -.05 -.15* .07 . 2

n
©
3 Q * *
. NLatency -.13 -.15*

0
1

1
8 -.0 1 -.1 0 .0 1

O
9. PLength 83 ** 28** .16* .41** .24** -.37** .46**
10. NLength .29** .2 1 ** .36** 34 ** _ 2 0 ** 41**

11. Pchild -.03 .2 1 ** .1 0 -.19* .23**


12. Nchild -.03 .07 -.0 2 .1 0

13. PMom -.2 2 ** .2 1 **


14. NMom -.1 1 .09
15. Control -. 6 6 **
16. Warmth

(Note: PGeneral = Overgeneral memory to positive cues; NGeneral = Overgeneral memory to negative cues; PPrompt = number o f prompts to positive cues;
NPrompt = number o f prompts to negative cues; PLatency = average response latency to positive cues; NLatency = average response latency to negative cues;
PLength = average length o f memory to positive cues; NLength = average length of memory to negative cues; Pchild = number of positive self-representations;
Nchild = number of negative self-representations; PMom - number o f positive maternal representations; NMom = number o f negative maternal representations,
Control = Controllingness rating’ Warmth = Warmth of interaction rating); *p < .05* (2-tailed), **p <.01
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

104

Table 6
Means and Standard Deviations by Maltreatment Subtype Group, Controlling for Gender and PPVT-III standard score

Dependent Variable Abused Neglected Nonmaltreated F p Partial Eta2

Overgeneral Memory** 3.00(1. 2)a 2.26(1. l)b 2.39(1.2)b 4.62 .011 .051
Number o f Prompts** 7.41(3.5)a 5.35(3.4)b 5.15(3.5)b 5.50 .005 .059
Response Latency 2.93(2.6) 3.43(2.6) 3.53(2.6) 0.59 .55 .007
Length of Memory 23.36(15.3) 25.56(14.7) 22.89(15.3) 0.53 .59 .006
Positive Self- Representations 1.17(1.1) 1.40(1.1) 1.04(1.1) 0.86 .47 .010
Negative Self- Representations** 1.75(1.l ) a 1.41(1.1) .097(1. l ) b 7.10 .001 .074
Positive Matemal-Representations 0.73(.79) 0.58(.79) 0.65(.93) 0.23 .79 .003
Negative Maternal- Representations 0.29(.76) 0.62(1.0) 0.42(.78) 1.27 .29 .014
Controllingness 3.28(2.0) 2.39(1.9) 3.1(2.0) 3.15 .046 .034
Warmth** 7.20(2.5)b 9.17(2.4)a 7.67(2.5)b 5.47 .005 .058
aE--------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Indicates significant group differences, see text for explanation;
*p < .05* (2-tailed), **p <.01
105

Table 7
Mediation of the Relation of Abuse to Overgeneral Memory: Negative Self-
Representations

Step P F AR2 Adjusted R2 sr2

1 7.61** .22 .191


Gender .18 .03
PPVT-III -.33 11**
Age -.27 07**
2 7.64** .06 .24
Gender .15 .02
PPVT-III -.27 07**
Age -.29 .08**
Abuse .25 .06*
3 6.75** 023 .255
Gender .15 .02
PPVT-III -.24 .05*
Age -.29 08**
Abuse .21 .04*
NSelfRep .16 .02
Note: PPVT-III = PPVT-III standard score; NSelfRep = Negative Self-
Representations
*p < .05* (2-tailed), **p <.01

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
106

Table 8
Mediation of the Relation of Abuse to Overgeneral Memory: Depressive Symptoms

Step P F AR2 Adjusted R2 sr2

1 8.11** .22 .19


Gender .15 .02
PPVT-III -.36 .13**
Age -.24 .05*
2 8.77** .07 .26
Gender .12 .01
PPVT-III -.28 .07**
Age -.27 Q7**
Abuse .28 .07*
3 7.30** .01 .26
Gender .12 .01
PPVT-III -.25 .05*
Age -.26 .06**
Abuse .27 .06**
CDI .11 .01
Note: PPVT-III = PPVT-III standard score; *p < .05* (2-tailed), **p <01

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
107

Appendix

Child Behavior Checklist Items for the Dissociation Scale (Ogawa et al., 1997)

1. Acts too young for his/her age


8. Can’t concentrate, can’t pay attention for long
13. Confused or seems to be in a fog
17. Day-dreams or gets lost in his/her thoughts
18. Deliberately harms self or attempts suicide
36. Gets hurt a lot, accident-prone
40. Hears sounds or voices that aren’t there
76. Explosive and unpredictable behavior
78. Inattentive, easily distracted
80. Stares blankly
87. Sudden changes in mood or feelings
91. Talks about killing self

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

You might also like