Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

Psychiatry Research 250 (2017) 141–145

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Psychiatry Research
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychres

Patient characteristics associated with aggression in mental health units MARK


a,c a,c a,b,c b,c,⁎
Tegan Podubinski , Stuart Lee , Yitzchak Hollander , Michael Daffern
a
Monash Alfred Psychiatry Research Centre, the Alfred and Monash University Central Clinical School, Melbourne, Australia
b
Swinburne University of Technology, Melbourne, Australia
c
Victorian Institute of Forensic Mental Health, Melbourne, Australia

A R T I C L E I N F O A BS T RAC T

Keywords: Aggression in mental health units is a significant and pervasive problem. However, the characteristics of
Hostile-dominance patients associated with increased aggression propensity remain unclear and there are few attempts to expand
Personality understanding of these characteristics by drawing upon contemporary aggression theory. This study assessed
Psychiatric the influence of interpersonal (hostile-dominance) and personality (psychopathy), General Aggression Model-
Aggression
specified (aggressive script rehearsal, attitudes towards violence, and trait anger), and clinical (psychiatric
symptoms) factors on aggression during psychiatric hospitalization in 200 inpatients (132 men and 68 women;
19–64 years, M=38.32 years, S.D.=11.13 years). Patient characteristics were assessed on admission using
structured interviews and self-report psychological tests. Patients’ files were reviewed and nurses were
interviewed after patients were discharged to establish whether patients were aggressive during their hospital
stay. Results of univariate analyses showed that higher levels of interpersonal hostile-dominance, psychopathy
and aggressive script rehearsal, positive attitudes towards violence, trait anger, and disorganized and excited
type psychiatric symptoms all predicted aggression. In the final multivariable logistic regression model, only
hostile-dominance remained as a significant predictor of aggressive behavior. This important personality
characteristic should be considered in violence risk assessments and aggression prevention strategies.

1. Introduction create an internal state which affects decision-making processes that


determine aggressive action. According to the GAM, habitual aggres-
Aggression in mental health units erodes the therapeutic environ- sion results from the acquisition of aggression-related cognitions
ment, causes psychological and physical harms to patients and staff, including aggression-related behavioral scripts (i.e. scripts denoting
and ultimately impairs care (Daffern and Howells, 2002). Research into how a person should interact with their environment), attitudes (i.e. a
aggression within mental health units typically neglects theoretical person's beliefs about the acceptability of aggression), and their related
perspectives, distal, environmental and interactional causes in favor of affective states (i.e. anger (Anderson and Bushman, 2002)).
proximal psychiatric symptoms, consistent with an internal model of Interpersonal hostile-dominance (Dolan and Blackburn, 2006) and
inpatient aggression (Nijman et al., 1999). In this regard extant psychopathy (Hare, 2003) have also been associated with aggressive
research reveals a small but significant association between some behavior in inpatient settings (Daffern et al., 2010). Where psycho-
psychiatric symptoms and aggression; active positive symptoms of pathy is thought to comprise persistent behavioral deviancy accom-
psychosis including delusions, conceptual disorganisation, hallucina- panied by emotional-interpersonal detachment (Patrick et al., 2009),
tions and paranoia appear to have the strongest association with interpersonal hostile-dominance describes a pattern of relating to
aggression (Swanson et al., 2006; Douglas et al., 2009). Few models others in a manner that is antagonistic and domineering.
of inpatient aggression incorporating personal, interpersonal and The current study examined the impact of interpersonal (e.g.,
environmental factors have been developed (for exception see Nijman hostile-dominance), personality (psychopathy), GAM-specified (ag-
et al. (1999), Duxbury and Whittington (2005). gressive script rehearsal, attitudes towards violence, and trait anger),
Contemporary aggression theories such as the General Aggression and clinical (psychiatric symptoms) factors on aggression during
Model (GAM; Anderson and Bushman, 2002) consider aggression to be psychiatric hospitalization. It was hypothesized that (1) the interper-
the product of multiple interacting factors; accordingly, distal (i.e. sonal and personality, GAM-specified, and clinical factors would all
personality characteristics) and situational factors (i.e. provocation) significantly predict inpatient aggressive behavior, and that (2) the


Correspondence to : Centre for Forensic Behavioural Science, 505 Hoddle Street, Clifton Hill, Victoria 3068, Australia.
E-mail address: mdaffern@swin.edu.au (M. Daffern).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.01.078
Received 17 May 2016; Received in revised form 8 November 2016; Accepted 29 January 2017
Available online 31 January 2017
0165-1781/ © 2017 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
T. Podubinski et al. Psychiatry Research 250 (2017) 141–145

addition of interpersonal variables would improve the prediction of sures beliefs supportive of aggression (e.g., “It's all right to fight
aggression beyond clinical, personality and GAM-specified variables. someone if they stole from you”); respondents indicate whether they
‘agree’ or ‘disagree’ with each item. The MCAA: ATV scale demon-
2. Methods strates good internal consistency with a Cronbach's alpha coefficient of
0.80 in samples of incarcerated offenders (Mills et al., 2002).
2.1. Participants The State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory-2 (Spielberger, 1999)
Trait Anger scale (STAXI-2: TA) measures the disposition to perceive a
Participants were 200 patients admitted to two acute units at the wide range of situations as annoying or frustrating, and the tendency to
Alfred Hospital Inpatient Psychiatry Unit, Melbourne, Australia, respond to these situations with anger (Spielberger, 1999). Good
between 12th of January 2012 and 10th of October 2012. The sample internal consistency for the STAXI-2: TA is reported in both non-
included 132 men (M=38.12 years, S.D.=11.14 years) and 68 women clinical adults (Cronbach's alpha coefficient of 0.84–0.86) and psy-
(M=38.69 years, S.D.=11.20 years) with an age range of 19–64 years chiatric patients (Cronbach's alpha coefficient of 0.87; Spielberger,
(M=38.32 years, S.D.=11.13 years). The average length of hospital stay 1999).
for participants was approximately two weeks (M=14.59 days, The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS; Kay et al.,
S.D.=15.96 days). 1987) assesses the presence and severity of positive and negative
The most common primary diagnosis (recorded on the day of the symptoms of schizophrenia, as well as general psychopathology. For
interview from case notes entered by the treating psychiatrist) was the purpose of the current research, PANSS items were grouped into
schizophrenia or another psychotic illness (55.5%), followed by uni- five subscales (Negative, Positive, Disorganized, Excited, and
polar depressive episode/disorder (11.5%), bipolar disorder or a manic Emotional Distress), in accordance with Kelly, White, Compton, and
episode (8.5%), borderline personality disorder (7.0%), alcohol or other Harvey (2013). Only the PANSS Positive, Disorganized, and Excited
substance induced disorders/related issues (6.5%), and acute stress subscales were used, as aggressive behavior is most often associated
reaction (4.5%). At the time of the interview, 4.0% of participants had with the symptoms incorporated in these subscales (Douglas et al.,
no diagnoses, and 2.5% had other diagnoses (e.g. cluster “personality 2009; Bowers et al., 2011).
traits”). During the study period, 746 patients were admitted to the The Overt Aggression Scale (OAS; Silver and Yudofsky, 1987) was
Alfred Psychiatry inpatient units and 200 (27%) chose to participate. used to measure aggressive behavior (physical aggression towards
An audit of all patients admitted to the Alfred Psychiatry Inpatient others, verbal aggression towards others, physical aggression against
Units was conducted in 2010 (see Lee et al., 2013, for details); the objects, and aggression towards the self) during each participant's
current sample can reasonably be considered representative of the hospitalization. The current research utilized a dichotomous OAS
Alfred Psychiatry inpatient population. score; aggressive behavior during admission was scored as 1 (present)
or 0 (absent), with a score in any category of the OAS giving an overall
2.2. Setting score of 1. The OAS was scored using two data sources: (1) review of
case files for the period of hospital stay; and, (2) an interview with each
Alfred Psychiatry is the main provider of public mental health patient's primary nurse following the patient's discharge from hospital.
services to people living in the inner southeast suburbs of Melbourne,
Australia. A hospital-based acute psychiatric response is provided to 2.4. Procedure
adult patients via two 28-bed units. Each unit offers care in low-
dependency (requiring less intensive observation) and high depen- This prospective study involved a semi-structured interview with
dency (for patients at higher risk of harm to self or others) environ- participants and their completion of self-report psychological tests
ments. within five days of admission to the Alfred Psychiatry inpatient units.
The semi-structured interview incorporated the PANSS, STAXI-2: TA,
2.3. Materials MCAA: ATV, and SIV. Following the interview, demographic data (sex,
age, date of birth, diagnosis, and date of admission) was collected/
The Impact Message Inventory-Circumplex (IMI-C; Kiesler and confirmed through review of the patient's medical records; IMI-C was
Schmidt, 2006) yields eight interpersonal scales (Dominant, Hostile- scored following the interview. The PCL: SV was completed based on
Dominant, Hostile, Hostile-Submissive, Submissive, Friendly- the review of each patient's medical file and information collected
Submissive, Friendly, and Friendly-Dominant) although in this study during the semi-structured interview, while the IMI-C was completed
only interpersonal hostile-dominance was evaluated since this has an based on the researcher's impressions during the semi-structured
established relationship with aggression in psychiatric units (Daffern interview. The date of discharge was noted and the OAS completed
et al., 2010). Internal consistency for the Hostile-Dominance scale following each participant's discharge.
ranges from 0.69 to 0.96, with a median Cronbach alpha coefficient of
0.81 (Kiesler and Auerbach, 2004). 2.5. Data analysis
The Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version (PCL: SV; Hart
et al., 1995) assesses psychopathic traits. The PCL: SV demonstrates Raw data consisted of total scores for IMI-C Hostile-Dominance
good internal consistency, with a weighted mean Cronbach's alpha and PCL: SV, answers to the ‘frequency’ item on the SIV, total scores
across 11 studies of 0.84 for the total scale (0.81 for F1 and 0.75 for F2; for the MCAA: ATV, STAXI-2: TA, and three PANSS subscales
Hart et al., 1995). (Positive, Disorganized, and Excited), and the dichotomous OAS score
The Schedule of Imagined Violence (SIV; Grisso et al., 2000) (aggression present or absent). The hypotheses were addressed using
screens for aggressive scripts through participants’ self-report. In the descriptive statistics, and univariate and multivariate parametric tests.
current research the frequency of script rehearsal was of primary Univariate logistic regression was used to assess the unadjusted
interest and was ascertained by asking the question “How often do you relationship between the dichotomous OAS score and IMI-C Hostile-
have thoughts about hurting or injuring other people?” There were Dominance, PCL: SV, SIV, MCAA: ATV, STAXI-2: TA, and PANSS
eight possible response options: 0= never, 1= once every few years, 2= Positive, Disorganized, and Excited. Hierarchical multivariable logistic
several times a year, 3= several times a month, 4= once a week, 5= regression was then conducted to assess the ability of IMI-C Hostile-
several times a week, 6= once a day, and 7= several times a day. Dominance and PCL: SV to predict the dichotomous OAS score, after
The Measures of Criminal Attitudes and Associates (Mills and controlling for the influence of PANSS Positive, Disorganized, and
Kroner, 2001) Attitudes Towards Violence scale (MCAA: ATV) mea- Excited, SIV, MCAA: ATV, and STAXI-2: TA. The significance thresh-

142
T. Podubinski et al. Psychiatry Research 250 (2017) 141–145

old was set at alpha=0.05. Table 2


The target sample size for this study was 200 participants. This was Mean (SD) participant scores for total sample, patients who engaged in aggressive
behavior, and patients who did not engage in aggressive behavior for IMI-C HD, PCL: SV,
based on recruitment feasibility considerations and expected propor-
SIV, MCAA: ATV, STAXI-2: TA, and PANSS Positive, Disorganized, and Excited.
tion of the sample that would engage in some form of aggressive
behavior during the admission. Given the frequency with which Total Sample Aggressive Non-Aggressive
aggressive behavior is observed, approximately 40% of included (n=200) (n=70) (n=130)
participants were expected to display some form of aggressive behavior
IMI-C HD 10.09 (3.37) 11.86 (4.21) 9.13 (2.32)
during their admission. In accordance with the recommended events PCL: SV 1.74 (2.27) 2.66 (2.85) 1.24 (1.71)
per variable ratio of 10 proposed by Peduzzi et al. (1996), this would SIV 1.66 (2.17) 2.24 (2.50) 1.34 (1.91)
enable robust logistic regression modelling with up to 8 covariates with MCAA: ATV 2.75 (2.84) 3.49 (3.18) 2.35 (2.56)
a sample size of 200 participants. STAXI−2: TA 18.15 (6.28) 19.79 (6.67) 17.27 (5.89)
PANSS Positive 12.58 (5.77) 13.64 (5.65) 12.01 (5.77)
PANSS Disorganized 14.92 (4.81) 16.27 (4.94) 14.19 (4.59)
2.6. Ethics PANSS Excited 5.65 (2.35) 6.26 (2.89) 5.32 (1.94)

This research received ethical approval from the Monash University


group mean=18.77, S.D.=21.17, Non-Aggressive group mean =12.33,
Human Research Ethics Committee and the Alfred Human Research
S.D=11.77, p=0.006).
Ethics committee. One researcher with experience and training in the
Mean (SD) IMI-C Hostile-Dominance, PCL: SV, SIV, MCAA: ATV,
administration of all measures recruited all participants and adminis-
STAXI-2: TA, and PANSS Positive, Disorganized, and Excited scores
tered all measures. Only patients deemed well enough to give informed
for the total sample and patients who engaged in or did not engage in
consent by their treating doctor were approached to participate. At this
any aggressive behavior are displayed in Table 2.
time the purpose and nature of the study was explained and patients
To provide an overall measure of the relationship between the risk
who expressed an interest in the study were given an information sheet
of any aggression occurring and each interpersonal and personality,
and, if willing to participate, they signed a consent form, after which
GAM-specified, and clinical variable, results of the univariate logistic
the semi-structured interview commenced.
regression analyses are presented in Table 3. Increased levels of IMI-C
Hostile-dominance, PCL: SV, SIV, MCAA: ATV, STAXI-2: TA, and
3. Results PANSS Disorganized and Excited were all significant univariate pre-
dictors of any aggression occurring during hospitalization.
All data was examined for accuracy, missing values, and outliers. A Prior to conducting hierarchical multivariable logistic regression,
random check of 50 participants’ entered data showed data entry to be the standardized residuals were examined so as to isolate points for
accurate. There were no missing values. Four participants were which the model fitted poorly. Ninety-five percent of cases had values
identified as having univariate outlier responses, as indicated by z- within ± 1.96, 99.5% of cases had values within ± 2.58, and no cases
scores greater than 3.29. One participant was an outlier on the STAXI- had values greater than ± 3. To assess the influence of individual cases
2: TA and three participants were outliers on the PCL: SV. Given the the values of Cook's Distance, DFBeta for the constant, and Leverage
large sample size, the decision was made to retain these participants in were examined. All values were less than 1, suggesting no influential
the analysis. cases. No Tolerance value was less than 0.1, indicating no multi-
The frequencies and percentages of participants who engaged in collinearity issues. Assumptions for the conduct of logistic regression
aggressive behavior during their hospitalization are displayed in were therefore met.
Table 1. A total of 70 (35%) patients were aggressive, however, for Results of the hierarchical multivariable logistic regression analyses
most patients this consisted of verbal aggression or physical aggression are presented in Table 4.
against objects. Nineteen (9.5%) patients engaged in physical aggres- At Step 1, the clinical and GAM-specified variables (PANSS
sion targeting staff and/or co-patients. Positive, Disorganized, and Excited, SIV, MCAA: ATV, and STAXI-2:
Prior to assessing whether interpersonal and personality, GAM- TA) significantly predicted aggression risk, (p=0.003), explaining
specified, and clinical factors predicted any aggressive incident, ag- between 9.5% (Cox and Snell R2) and 13.1% (Nagelkerke R2) of the
gressive (n=70) and non-aggressive participants (n=130) were com- variance in aggression and correctly classifying 65.5% of cases. At Step
pared with regard to demographic variables and length of hospital stay. 1 only PANSS Disorganized made a unique statistically significant
Neither age (Aggressive group mean=37.51, S.D.=11.30, Non- contribution to the prediction of aggression, with an odds ratio of 1.09,
Aggressive group mean=38.75, S.D.=11.06, p=0.46), days assessed p=0.020. The addition of the interpersonal (IMI-C Hostile-dominance)
after LDU admission (Aggressive group mean =2.90, S.D.=1.42, Non- and personality (PCL: SV) variables to the clinical and GAM-specified
Aggressive group mean=2.64, S.D.=1.40, p=0.21) or sex (Aggressive variables significantly improved the prediction of any aggression (p <
group % males=68.6%, Non-Aggressive group % males=64.6%,
p=0.57) differed between groups. Patients displaying aggression ex-
perienced significantly longer hospital lengths of stay (Aggressive Table 3
Univariate logistic regression relationships between interpersonal and personality, GAM-
Table 1 specified, and clinical variables and the occurrence of any aggressive incident (n=200).
Proportion of participants engaging in aggressive behavior during their hospital stay
(n=200). B (SE) p-value Odds 95% CI for Odds
Ratio Ratio
Frequency (%)
IMI-C HD 0.26 (0.05) < 0.001 1.29 1.17–1.43
Any Aggressive Incident 70 (35.0%) PCL: SV 0.30 (0.08) < 0.001 1.35 1.16–1.58
Any Physical Aggressive Incident Against Staff or Patients 19 (9.5%) SIV 0.19 (0.07) 0.006 1.21 1.06–1.38
Any Physical Aggressive Incident Against Staff 13 (6.5%) MCAA: ATV 0.14 (0.05) 0.008 1.15 1.04–1.27
Any Physical Aggressive Incident Against Patients 12 (6.0%) STAXI−2: TA 0.06 (0.02) 0.008 1.07 1.02–1.12
Any Verbal Aggressive Incident Against Staff or Patients 67 (33.5%) PANSS Positive 0.05 (0.03) 0.06 1.05 1.00–1.10
Any Verbal Aggressive Incident Against Staff 59 (29.5%) PANSS Disorganized 0.09 (0.03) 0.004 1.09 1.03–1.16
Any Verbal Aggressive Incident Against Patients 48 (24.0%) PANSS Excited 0.17 (0.06) 0.008 1.18 1.04–1.34
Any Physical Aggressive Incident Against Objects 32 (16.0%)
B=Unstandardized regression coefficient.

143
T. Podubinski et al. Psychiatry Research 250 (2017) 141–145

Table 4 with regard to how staff interactions with patients high in interpersonal
Hierarchical logistic regression analysis assessing the contribution of interpersonal and hostile-dominance may unintentionally frustrate patient motives and
personality factors over and above clinical and GAM-specified factors to the occurrence
escalate conflict, may also assist in averting aggressive incidents.
of any aggressive incidents (n=200).
According to Interpersonal Theory, a person's interpersonal style can
B (SE) p-value Odds 95% CI for Odds be conjectured as the balance between the dimensions of Communion
Ratio Ratio and Agency. Communion, which ranges from friendliness to hostility,
refers to connection with others, while Agency, ranging from submis-
Step 1a
PANSS Positive −0.01 0.71 0.99 0.93–1.05 sion to dominance, emphasizes influence, control, or mastery over the
(0.03) self, other people and the environment (Kiesler, 1996; Horowitz et al.,
PANSS Disorganized 0.09 (0.04) 0.02 1.09 1.01–1.17 2006).
PANSS Excited 0.09 (0.07) 0.20 1.09 0.95–1.25 Balanced and productive interpersonal interactions occur when the
SIV1 0.12 (0.09) 0.19 1.12 0.94–1.34
agentic and communal needs of both persons are met during the
MCAA: ATV 0.03 (0.07) 0.64 1.03 0.90–1.18
STAXI−2: TA 0.03 (0.03) 0.38 1.03 0.97–1.10 interaction. For this to occur, corresponding responses are invited on
Constant −3.07 < 0.001 0.05 the Communion dimension (i.e. friendliness invites friendliness) and
(0.79) reciprocal responses are invited on the Agency dimension (i.e. dom-
Step 2b
inance invites submission; Pincus and Ansell, 2013). This is described
PANSS Positive −0.04 0.25 0.96 0.90–1.03
(0.04) as the principle of Complementarity. During inpatient psychiatric
PANSS Disorganized 0.05 (0.04) 0.24 1.05 0.97–1.13 treatment, interpersonal interactions are often Acomplementary, that
PANSS Excited −0.17 0.08 0.84 0.69–1.02 is, correspondence occurs on the Communion dimension or reciprocity
(0.10) occurs on the Agency dimension but never both at once, or
SIV1 0.05 (0.10) 0.59 1.05 0.87–1.27
Anticomplementary, that is, neither correspondence on Communion
MCAA: ATV 0.04 (0.07) 0.56 1.04 0.90–1.21
STAXI−2: TA 0.02 (0.03) 0.58 1.02 0.95–1.09 or reciprocity on Agency is exhibited. For example, a patient who has a
IMI-C HD 0.29 (0.09) 0.001 1.34 1.13–1.59 persistent need for dominance may respond to staff requests with
PCL: SV 0.06 (0.11) 0.59 1.06 0.86–1.31 behavior that attempts to control the situation. Rather than submit to
Constant −3.52 < 0.001 0.03
this behavior, staff will typically respond in a non-reciprocal manner
(0.91)
(i.e. assertiveness) in an attempt to regain control. This may lead to a
a
Step 1: χ2 change (6) =20.05, p=0.003. disruption in the interpersonal relationship, where the patient is
b
Step 2: χ2 change (2) =17.27, p < 0.001. increasingly motivated to command the situation and does so through
aggressive action.
0.001). The model as a whole explained between 17.0% (Cox and Snell Patients with elevated levels of hostile-dominance often approach
R2) and 23.4% (Nagelkerke R2) of the variance in aggression, and interpersonal situations with a hostile attitude, and the view that the
correctly classified 72.5% of cases. For the whole model, only IMI-C world is an unreceptive and unfriendly place; domineering behavior
Hostile-dominance made a significant unique contribution to the may be used to protect self-interests. Over time, interpersonal inter-
prediction of aggression, with an odds ratio of 1.34, p=0.001. actions that affirm these expectations may strengthen the motivation to
act in a hostile and dominant manner, leading to interpersonal
behavior that is maladjusted. Psychiatric patients with high levels of
4. Discussion hostile-dominance may engage in intense and rigid hostile and
dominant behaviors; they may lack the flexibility to adapt their
At the univariate level, interpersonal hostile-dominance, psycho- behavior to the particular demands of the inpatient routine. When
pathy, aggressive script rehearsal, attitudes towards violence, trait demands are made of these patients (e.g. adhere with prescribed
anger, and PANSS Disorganized and Excited symptoms all predicted medication), they may feel as though they are being forced into
aggressive behavior. Positive symptoms did not independently predict submission, which may in turn be viewed as threatening. As the
aggression. Multivariable analysis showed that interpersonal hostile- individual is unsettled by the frustrating interaction, aggression may
dominance predicted aggressive behavior independent of clinical and be used to restore dominance and protect oneself. Thus, it is important
GAM-specified factors. Furthermore, the addition of personality vari- for staff to acknowledge and consider the impact their interpersonal
ables significantly improved the prediction of inpatient aggressive behavior has on patients, as well as the patient's particular interperso-
behavior, although only hostile-dominance remained as a significant nal priorities and sensitivities, and how these may influence their
unique predictor of aggression risk. This result highlights the impor- reactions, particularly in situations that are deemed directive and
tance of interpersonal hostile-dominance for understanding inpatient controlling by patients. Interpersonal Theory provides a framework for
aggressive behavior. understanding how patients’ interpersonal sensitivities and propensi-
Interpersonal hostile-dominance is an important internal risk ties may trigger aggression within the inpatient setting, particularly
factor that is associated with psychopathy, the tendency to rehearse with regard to acts of aggression towards staff that follow their
aggressive scripts and psychiatric symptomatology (Podubinski et al., demands for activity or denials of patients’ requests (Daffern et al.,
2014). It contributes to the development of aggression-related cogni- 2007). Nijman and colleagues (1999) and Duxbury and Whittington
tions and a tendency towards negative affect. Results of the current (2005) have noted how problematic staff-patient communications may
study suggest that hostile-dominance should be a focus of violence risk precede aggression, and Gudjonsson et al. (2004) have suggested that
assessments on admission to hospital and furthermore, treatment mental health professionals are increasingly forced to act in controlling
targeted at reducing elevated levels of hostile-dominance may prove ways because of institutional pressures.
beneficial to the prevention of aggressive behavior, both in hospital and
in the community post-discharge (Daffern et al., 2013). Furthermore, 4.1. Limitations
since hostile-dominance has been implicated in impaired therapeutic
alliance and treatment adherence (Cookson et al., 2012), adjusting The interpretation of these findings should be considered in light of
treatment to the interpersonal style of patients, may help reduce the fact that aggressive behavior was measured as a combination of
aggressiveness as well as enhance therapeutic alliance (Birtchnell, verbal aggression against staff, verbal aggression against patients,
2002). physical aggression against staff, physical aggression against patients,
Increasing staff awareness of Interpersonal Theory, particularly and physical aggression against objects. Attempts were made to

144
T. Podubinski et al. Psychiatry Research 250 (2017) 141–145

conduct separate analyses with the different types of aggressive and functional assessment approaches. Aggress. Violent Behav. Rev. J. 7, 477–497.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1359-1789(01)00073-8.
behavior, and in particular physical aggression. However, the low base Daffern, M., Howells, K., Ogloff, J.R.P., 2007. What's the point? Towards a methodology
rate of physical aggression made statistical analysis untenable. Further, for assessing the function of psychiatric inpatient aggression. Behav. Res. Ther. 45,
the observed proportion of 35% of the sample that were aggressive 101–111.
Daffern, M., Thomas, S., Ferguson, M., et al., 2010. The impact of psychiatric symptoms,
meant that the event per variable ratio=8.75 was somewhat lower than interpersonal style, and coercion on aggression during psychiatric hospitalization.
recommended by Peduzzi et al. (1996). It was determined that Psychiatry.: Interpers. Biol. Process. 73, 365–381. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/
inclusion of the 8 covariates (all displaying univariate associations psyc.2010.73.4.365.
Daffern, M., Thomas, S., Lee, S., et al., 2013. The impact of treatment on hostile-
with aggression of at least p < 0.10) within the final multivariable dominance in forensic psychiatric inpatients: relationships between change in
model was necessary to provide a more complete analysis of factors hostile-dominance and recidivism following release from custody. J. Forensic
contributing to aggression occurrence. Therefore some caution should Psychiatry Psychol. 24, 675–687. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
14789949.2013.834069.
be used when interpreting the stability of calculated model coefficients.
Dolan, M., Blackburn, R., 2006. Interpersonal factors as predictors of disciplinary
Finally, consideration of the findings in light of the low rate of infractions in incarcerated personality disordered offenders. Personal. Individ.
psychopathy in the patient sample studied is also important. The mean Differ. 40, 897–907. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.10.003.
total score on the PCL: SV was only 1.74 (S.D. =2.27) out of a possible Douglas, K.S., Guy, L.S., Hart, S.D., 2009. Psychosis as a risk factor for violence to others:
a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 135, 679–706. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0016311.
24. Even the maximum score (15) was below the suggested diagnostic Duxbury, J., Whittington, R., 2005. Causes and management of patient aggression and
cutoff ( > 18; Hart et al., 1995). Thus, these results may not generalise violence: staff and patient perspectives. J. Adv. Nurs. 50, 469–478. http://
to patients higher in psychopathy. Additionally, although the final dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2648.2005.03426.x.
Grisso, T., Davis, J., Vesselinov, R., et al., 2000. Violent thoughts and violent behavior
model explained between 17.0% and 23.4% of the variance in aggres- following hospitalization for mental disorder. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 68, 388–398.
sion, there is still considerable unexplained additional variance. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-006X.68.3.388.
Gudjonsson, G.H., Rabe-Hesketh, S., Szmukler, G., 2004. Management of psychiatric in-
patient violence: patient ethnicity and use of medication, restraint and seclusion. Br.
4.2. Conclusion J. Psychiatry 184, 258–262.
Hare, R.D., 2003. The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised 2nd ed.. Multi-Health
This study found that interpersonal hostile-dominance, psychopa- Systems, Toronto, ON.
Hart, S.D., Cox, D.N., Hare, R.D., 1995. The Hare Psychopathy Checklist: Screening
thy, aggressive script rehearsal, positive attitudes towards violence,
Version. Multi-Health Systems, Toronto, ON.
trait anger, and PANSS Disorganized and Excited psychiatric symp- Horowitz, L.M., Wilson, K.R., Turan, B., et al., 2006. How interpersonal motives clarify
toms all predicted aggressive behavior in psychiatric inpatients. Only the meaning of interpersonal behavior: a revised circumplex model. Personal. Soc.
Psychol. Rev. 10, 67–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_4.
interpersonal hostile-dominance predicted aggression at the multi-
Kay, S.R., Fiszbein, A., Opler, L.A., 1987. The positive and negative syndrome scale
variable level, highlighting the critical role of hostile-dominance in (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 13, 261–276. http://dx.doi.org/
inpatient aggression. Accordingly, the assessment of hostile-dominance 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261.
should be a focus of initial violence risk assessments on admission to Kiesler, D.J., 1996. Contemporary Interpersonal Theory and Research: Personality,
Psychopathology and Psychotherapy. Wiley, New York.
hospital; more effective treatment outcomes, and reductions in aggres- Kiesler, D.J., Auerbach, S.M., 2004. Psychometric Characteristics of the Impact Message
sive behavior may be achieved by developing interventions targeting Inventory-Octant Version (IMI-C): An update. Available at: 〈http://sitarsociety.
reductions in hostile-dominance and modifying interactional styles weebly.com/uploads/1/0/4/0/10405979/imi-cpsychometricdata2004.pdf〉
(accessed 2014 July 2014).
when treating patients with elevated levels of hostile-dominance. Kiesler, D.J., Schmidt, J.A., 2006. Manual for the Impact Message Inventory-Circumplex
(IMI-C). Mind Garden, Menlo Park, CA.
Funding Lee, S., Hollander, Y., Scarff, L., Dube, R., Keppich-Arnold, S., Stafrace, S., 2013.
Demonstrating the impact and model of care of a Statewide psychiatric intensive care
service. Australas. Psychiatry 21, 466–471. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in 1039856213497525.
the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors. Mills, J.F., Kroner, D.G., 2001. Measures of Criminal Attitudes and Associates (MCAA).
Unpublished instrument and user guide.
Mills, J.F., Kroner, D.G., Forth, A.E., 2002. Measures of criminal attitudes and associates
Declaration of Conflicting Interests (MCAA): Development, factor structure, reliability and validity. Assessment 9,
240–253. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073191102009003003.
Nijman, H.L.I., à Campo, J.M.L.G., Ravelli, D.P., Merckelbach, H.L.G.J., 1999. A
The Authors declare that there is no conflict of interest.
tentative model of aggression on in-patient psychiatric wards. Psychiatr. Serv. 50,
832–834.
Acknowledgements Patrick, C.J., Fowles, D.C., Krueger, R.F., 2009. Triarchic conceptualization of
psychopathy: developmental origins of disinhibition, boldness, and meanness. Dev.
Psychopathol. 21, 913–938. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579409000492.
None. Peduzzi, P., Concato, J., Kemper, E., Holford, T.R., Feinstein, A.R., 1996. A simulation
study of the number of events per variable in logistic regression analysis. J. Clin.
References Epidemiol. 49, 1373–1379.
Podubinski, T., Lee, S., Hollander, Y., Daffern, M., 2014. Characteristics of interpersonal
hostile-dominance in psychiatric inpatients. Psychiatry.: Interpers. Biol. Process. 77,
Anderson, C.A., Bushman, B.J., 2002. Human aggression. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 27–51 275–288.
. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135231. Pincus, A.L., Ansell, E.B., 2013. Interpersonal Theory of Personality. In: Suls, J., Tennen,
Birtchnell, J., 2002. Relating in Psychotherapy: The Application of a New Theory. H. (Eds.), Handbook of psychology Vol 5: Personality and social psychology2nd ed.
Brunner-Routledge, Hove. Wiley, New Jersey, 141–159.
Bowers, L., Stewart, D., Papadopoulos, C., et al., 2011. Inpatient Violence and Silver, J.M., Yudofsky, S.C., 1987. Documentation of aggression in the assessment of the
Aggression: A Literature Review. Report from the Conflict and Containment violent patient. Psychiatr. Ann. 17, 375–384.
Reduction Research Programme. Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, Spielberger, C.D., 1999. State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory–2 (STAXI-2):
London. Professional manual. Psychological Assessment Resources, Odessa, FL.
Cookson, A., Daffern, M., Foley, F., 2012. Relationship between aggression, interpersonal Swanson, J.W., Swartz, M.S., Van Dorn, R.A., et al., 2006. A national study of violent
style and therapeutic alliance during short-term psychiatric hospitalization. Int. J. behavior in persons with schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 63, 490–499. http://
Ment. Health Nurs. 21, 20–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1447- dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.63.5.490.
0349.2011.00764.x.
Daffern, M., Howells, K., 2002. Psychiatric inpatient aggression: a review of structural

145

You might also like