The Science and Folklore of Traumatic Amnesia

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

R E V IE W A ND C O M M E N T AR I E S

The Science and Folklore of Traumatic Some clinicians believe that the more traumatic an
Amnesia experience is, the more likely some victims will be
unable to remember it. As Brown, Scheflin, and
Richard J. McNally, Harvard University
Hammond (1998) asserted,

Some clinical theorists believe that certain experiences


when emotional material reaches the point of being
traumatic in intensity—something that cannot be
are so overwhelmingly traumatic that many victims
replicated in artificial laboratories—in a certain sub-
dissociate their memory for the experience (Gleaves,
population of individuals, material that is too intense
Smith, Butler, & Spiegel, this issue). Unfortunately,
may not be able to be consciously processed and so
clinicians who endorse this hypothesis often exhibit
may become unconscious and amnesic. (p. 97)
confusion about the very studies they cite in support of
it. For example, they often misinterpret everyday forget- That is, some victims are unable—not merely un-
fulness that develops after a trauma with an inability to
willing—to remember their traumatic experience, pre-
cisely because it was so emotionally overwhelming.
remember the trauma itself; they confuse organic amne-
Moreover, these clinicians believe that dissociated
sia with traumatic amnesia; they confuse psychogenic
trauma must be remembered for healing to occur
amnesia (massive non-organic retrograde amnesia cou-
(e.g., Olio, 1989), and that special techniques are
pled with loss of personal identity) with (alleged) inability
sometimes needed to help patients recover the traumatic
to remember a traumatic event; and they confuse not memories they are otherwise unable to recall:
thinking about something (e.g., sexual abuse) for a long
period of time with an inability to remember it (i.e.,
Because some victims of sexual abuse will repress their
memories by dissociating them from consciousness,
amnesia). The purpose of this commentary is to dispel
hypnosis can be very valuable in retrieving these
some of this confusion.
memories. Indeed, for some victims, hypnosis may
Key words: amnesia, dissociation, repression, sexual
provide the only avenue to the repressed memories.
abuse, trauma, posttraumatic stress disorder. [Clin Psy-
(Brown et al., 1998, p. 647)
chol Sci Prac 11: 29–33, 2004]
Citing Brown, Scheflin, and Whitfield’s (1999)
review article, Gleaves, Smith, Butler, and Spiegel (this
issue) appear to endorse this traumatic-amnesia hypoth-
esis. Unfortunately, clinicians often exhibit confusion
Address correspondence to Richard J. McNally, Department about the studies they cite in support of this notion. The
of Psychology, Harvard University, 1230 William James purpose of my commentary is to dispel some of this
Hall, 33 Kirkland Street, Cambridge, MA 02138. E-mail: confusion. (For extensive critiques, see McNally, 2003b,
rjm@wjh.harvard.edu. pp. 186–228; Piper, Pope, & Borowiecki, 2000.)

DOI: 10.1093/clipsy/bph056
Ó 2004 AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGICAL ASSOCIATION D12 29
CO N FU S IN G E V ER YD AY F O R G ET FU L NE S S W IT H personal identity; and supposedly remits gradually,
T R A U MAT I C AM NE S I A often during ‘‘recovered memory therapy.’’
After having been exposed to trauma, some people
report difficulty concentrating and remembering things C O NF USIN G IN COMP L ET E E NC ODIN G WI T H
in everyday life (e.g., Wilkinson, 1983). Unfortunately, T R A U MA T I C AM NE S I A
some theorists misconstrue these reports as evidence for The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
traumatic amnesia (e.g., Brown et al., 1999). In reality, (4th ed.; DSM-IV) lists ‘‘inability to recall an important
this form of memory impairment refers to ordinary aspect of the trauma’’ (American Psychiatric Associa-
absentmindedness that develops following the trauma; it tion, 1994, p. 428) as a symptom of posttraumatic stress
does not refer to difficulty remembering the trauma disorder (PTSD). Unfortunately, the meaning of this
itself. Everyday forgetfulness that develops after a trauma symptom is ambiguous. Because the mind does not
must not be confused with amnesia for the trauma. operate like a video recorder, not every aspect of
a traumatic experience will get encoded into memory in
CO N FU S IN G O R G A NI C AM NES I A W IT H the first place. Incomplete encoding must not be
T R A U MAT I C AM NE S I A confused with an inability (amnesia) to remember (cf.
Some clinicians mistakenly cite cases of organic amnesia Gleaves et al., this issue). For example, people robbed at
as relevant to traumatic amnesia. For example, accord- gunpoint sometimes fail to encode the face of their
ing to Brown et al. (1998), ‘‘Dollinger (1985) found that assailant, often because their attention is captured by the
two of the 38 children studied after watching lightning assailant’s weapon. A later inability to recall what the
strike and kill a playmate had no memory of the event’’ assailant looked like would not constitute amnesia
(pp. 609–610). Brown et al., however, forgot to because the person failed to attend and encode every
mention that both amnesic children had themselves aspect of the experience (e.g., the assailant’s face).
been hit by side flashes from the main lightning bolt,
knocked unconscious, and nearly killed. Amnesia C O N F U S I N G NO N D I S CL O S U R E W I T H
resulting from direct physical insult to the brain must T R A U MA T I C AM NE S I A
not be confused with amnesia arising from psychic When queried by survey interviewers, some known
causes. abuse victims fail to mention their abuse (e.g., Widom &
Morris, 1997). But a failure to disclose must not be
CON FU S IN G P SY CHOGEN IC AMN ESIA WI T H equated with an inability to remember (amnesia). For
T R A U MAT I C AM NE S I A example, one research team recontacted nondisclosing
Psychogenic amnesia is a rare syndrome characterized by respondents and learned that all of them had re-
sudden, massive retrograde memory loss that cannot be membered their abuse, but had been previously un-
attributed to physical insult to the brain (Kihlstrom & willing to discuss it with the survey interviewer
Schacter, 2000). The syndrome sometimes occurs (Femina, Yeager, & Lewis, 1990). Therefore, one cannot
following exposure to a stressful event, but these events assume that failure to disclose means inability to
are often quite ordinary and nontraumatic (e.g., remember.
romantic disappointment).
Psychogenic amnesia must not be confused with C O NF USIN G NOT THIN KI N G A B O U T S O M E T H I N G
traumatic amnesia (McNally, 2003b, pp. 186–189). F O R A L ON G T I ME W I T H T R A U MAT I C AM NE S I A
Classic psychogenic amnesia begins suddenly, often Some researchers have reported that nearly 60% of adult
after a nontraumatic stressor; involves massive retro- patients who report having been sexually abused as
grade memory loss, including loss of personal identity; children answer affirmatively when asked whether there
seldom lasts for more than a few weeks; and usually had ever been ‘‘a time when you could not remember’’
remits suddenly. Alleged traumatic amnesia has an the abuse (Briere & Conte, 1993, p. 24). Such findings
uncertain mode of onset; involves selective forgetting of have been interpreted as evidence for ‘‘sexual abuse-
specific traumatic events; does not involve loss of related repression’’ (Briere & Conte, 1993, p. 26). An

CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE  V11 N1, SPRING 2004 30


affirmative reply to this question implies that the amnesia for their abuse, thanks to postulated inhibitory
respondent had spent a period of time unsuccessfully mechanisms (e.g., repression, dissociative encoding),
trying to recall his or her childhood sexual abuse. But if then therapists who believe recollecting such memories
a person has dissociated all memories of abuse, on what is vital to healing will likely engage in ‘‘recovered
basis would he or she attempt to recall them in the first memory’’ procedures to help patients overcome these
place? Most probably, respondents interpret such inhibitory barriers. Indeed, the entire recovered mem-
questions as meaning, ‘‘Has there ever been a time when ory enterprise is based on the assumption that patients
you did not think about your abuse?’’ But not thinking are unable to recall their traumatic memories—not that
about one’s trauma is not the same thing as being unable patients have simply failed to think about these early
to remember it. And inability to remember is what experiences in many years.
defines amnesia. Assuming Briere and Conte’s respon-
dents had been abused, it is likely that the memories D EB UN KI NG FR E UDI AN F O L KL O R E
would have come immediately to mind had someone Alluding briefly to Freud’s (1896/1962) abandonment
posed this question during the period of alleged of his seduction theory of hysteria, Gleaves et al. do
‘‘amnesia.’’ Moreover, as Schooler, Bendiksen, and not mention the fascinating historical scholarship
Ambadar (1997) discovered, some individuals, who concerning this topic (for a review, see McNally,
believe that they had not thought about such experiences 2003b, pp. 159–171). Scholars have debunked three
in many years, are surprised when others tell them that urban legends about this episode (e.g., Cioffi, 1998;
they had, in fact, remembered and discussed the Esterson, 2001, 2002). Contrary to folklore, (a) Freud’s
experiences during the period of alleged amnesia. That hysteria patients did not tell him that they had been
is, some people forget having previously recalled these sexually molested by their fathers; (b) Freud’s medical
experiences. colleagues were not shocked by Freud’s apparent
Moreover, as we have learned in our research (for discovery of widespread incest among the respectable
a review, see McNally, 2003a), the typical participant Viennese bourgeoisie; and (c) Freud did not retract his
who reports having remembered a long-forgotten ‘‘scandalous’’ seduction theory to restore his reputation
episode of early sexual abuse did not experience the among his colleagues.
abuse as traumatic when it occurred (McNally, Clancy,
Barrett, & Parker, in press). These individuals usually E XPERI MEN T AL COGN ITIV E R E S EARC H
remember it as being confusing, upsetting, or disgusting, Given their commendable interest in applying cognitive
but not terrifying. Often failing to understand it as psychology methods to the study of traumatized people
sexual abuse, they simply did not think about it until (not merely college students), I was surprised that
reminded of it many years later. Obviously, these cases Gleaves et al. (this issue) scarcely mentioned the large
do not count as traumatic amnesia. If something was not literature on this topic. Ever since the 1980s, clinical
experienced as traumatic when it occurred, there is no researchers have used laboratory methods to test
reason to believe that putative dissociative mechanisms hypotheses about cognitive abnormalities in patients
would come into play to render the experience with PTSD (e.g., McNally et al., 1987; McNally,
inaccessible to awareness. Metzger, Lasko, Clancy, & Pitman, 1998; for reviews,
The distinction between not thinking about some- see Buckley, Blanchard, & Neill, 2000; McNally, 1998).
thing for a long time versus being unable to remember it My colleagues and I have extended this line of research
is not a semantic quibble. In fact, it has enormous clinical to people reporting ‘‘repressed and recovered’’ memo-
implications. For example, if patients have merely not ries of childhood sexual abuse (e.g., Clancy, McNally, &
thought about their abuse in many years (but the Schacter, 1999; McNally, Clancy, Schacter, & Pitman,
memories are not blocked by active inhibitory forces), 2000; McNally, Clancy, & Schacter, 2001) and of
then a simple question or two during an assessment abduction by space aliens (Clancy, McNally, Schacter,
interview will prompt recollection of the long-forgotten Lenzenweger, & Pitman, 2002). For example, contrary
memories. On the other hand, if patients have genuine to the traumatic (dissociative) amnesia hypothesis, adults

COMMENTARIES ON GLEAVES ET AL. 31


whose posttraumatic stress disorder is linked to early Femina, D. D., Yeager, C. A., & Lewis, D. O. (1990). Child
childhood sexual abuse actually experience impairments abuse: Adolescent records vs. adult recall. Child Abuse and
in forgetting trauma-relevant cues in the laboratory Neglect, 14, 227–231.
(McNally et al., 1998)—findings directly contrary to the Freud, S. (1962). The aetiology of hysteria. In J. Strachey (Ed.
notion that these individuals ought to exhibit a superior and Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psychological
ability to forget material related to abuse. works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 3, pp. 191–221). London:
Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1896).
Gleaves, D. H., Smith, S. M., Butler, L. D., & Spiegel,
AC KN OWLEDGM ENT D. (2004). False and recovered memories in the labora-
The author’s research is supported by NIMH grant tory and clinic: A review of experimental and clinical
#MH61268. I thank John F. Kihlstrom for his comments on evidence. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11, xxx–
an earlier draft of this article. xxx.
Kihlstrom, J. F., & Schacter, D. L. (2000). Functional amnesia.
In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychol-
REFERENCES ogy (2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 409–427. Amsterdam: Elsevier
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and Science.
statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, McNally, R. J. (1998). Experimental approaches to cognitive
DC: Author. abnormality in posttraumatic stress disorder. Clinical
Briere, J., & Conte, J. (1993). Self-reported amnesia for abuse Psychology Review, 18, 971–982.
in adults molested as children. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 6, McNally, R. J. (2003a). Recovering memories of trauma: A
21–31. view from the laboratory. Current Directions in Psychological
Brown, D., Scheflin, A. W., & Hammond, D. C. (1998). Science, 12, 32–35.
Memory, trauma treatment, and the law. New York: Norton. McNally, R. J. (2003b). Remembering trauma. Cambridge, MA:
Brown, D., Scheflin, A. W., & Whitfield, C. L. (1999). Belknap Press/Harvard University Press.
Recovered memories: The current weight of the evidence McNally, R. J., Clancy, S. A., Barrett, H. M., & Parker, H. A.
in science and in the courts. Journal of Psychiatry and Law, (in press). Inhibiting retrieval of trauma cues in adults reporting
27, 5–156. histories of childhood sexual abuse.
Buckley, T. C., Blanchard, E. B., & Neill, W. T. (2000). McNally, R. J., Clancy, S. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2001).
Information processing and PTSD: A review of the Directed forgetting of trauma cues in adults reporting
literature. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 1041–1065. repressed or recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse.
Cioffi, F. (1998). Freud and the question of pseudoscience. Chicago: Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 151–156.
Open Court. McNally, R. J., Clancy, S. A., Schacter, D. L., & Pitman,
Clancy, S. A., McNally, R. J., & Schacter, D. L. (1999). Effects
R. K. (2000). Cognitive processing of trauma cues in adults
of guided imagery on memory distortion in women
reporting repressed, recovered, or continuous memories of
reporting recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse.
childhood sexual abuse. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109,
Journal of Traumatic Stress, 12, 559–569.
355–359.
Clancy, S. A., McNally, R. J., Schacter, D. L., Lenzenweger,
McNally, R. J., Luedke, D. L., Besyner, J. K., Peterson, R. A.,
M. F., & Pitman, R. K. (2002). Memory distortion in
Bohm, K., & Lips, O. J. (1987). Sensitivity to stress-relevant
people reporting abduction by aliens. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 111, 455–461. stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Anxiety
Dollinger, S. J. (1985). Lightning-strike disaster among Disorders, 1, 105–116.
children. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 58, 375–383. McNally, R. J., Metzger, L. J., Lasko, N. B., Clancy, S. A., &
Esterson, A. (2001). The mythologizing of psychoanalytic Pitman, R. K. (1998). Directed forgetting of trauma cues in
history: Deception and self-deception in Freud’s accounts adult survivors of childhood sexual abuse with and without
of the seduction theory. History of Psychiatry, 12, 329– posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
352. ogy, 107, 596–601.
Esterson, A. (2002). The myth of Freud’s ostracism by the Olio, K. A. (1989). Memory retrieval in the treatment of adult
medical community in 1896–1905: Jeffrey Masson’s assault survivors of sexual abuse. Transactional Analysis Journal, 19,
on truth. History of Psychology, 5, 115–134. 93–100.

CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE  V11 N1, SPRING 2004 32


Piper, A., Jr., Pope, H. G., Jr., & Borowiecki, J. J., III. (2000). Widom, C. S., & Morris, S. (1997). Accuracy of adult rec-
Custer’s last stand: Brown, Scheflin, and Whitfield’s latest ollections of childhood victimization: Part 2. Childhood
attempt to salvage ‘‘dissociative amnesia.’’ Journal of sexual abuse. Psychological Assessment, 9, 34–46.
Psychiatry and Law, 28, 149–213. Wilkinson, C. B. (1983). Aftermath of a disaster: The collapse
Schooler, J. W., Bendiksen, M., & Ambadar, Z. (1997). of the Hyatt Regency Hotel skywalks. American Journal of
Taking the middle line: Can we accommodate both fabri- Psychiatry, 140, 1134–1139.
cated and recovered memories of sexual abuse? In M. A.
Conway (Ed.), Recovered memories and false memories (pp.
251–292). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Received June 3, 2003; accepted June 23, 2003.

COMMENTARIES ON GLEAVES ET AL. 33

You might also like