Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 20

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/298794680

Collaborative Strategic Reading for Students With Learning


Disabilities in Upper Elementary Classrooms

Article  in  Exceptional Children · March 2016


DOI: 10.1177/0014402915625067

CITATIONS READS

35 3,129

6 authors, including:

Alison Boardman Sharon Vaughn


University of Colorado Boulder University of Texas at Austin
29 PUBLICATIONS   1,209 CITATIONS    371 PUBLICATIONS   18,539 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Pamela Buckley Colleen Reutebuch


University of Colorado Boulder University of Texas at Austin
20 PUBLICATIONS   135 CITATIONS    22 PUBLICATIONS   793 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Reading Enhancements for Students with Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Reading Comprehension Intervention View project

Collaborative Strategic Reading View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Colleen Reutebuch on 16 May 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


625067
research-article2016
ECXXXX10.1177/0014402915625067Exceptional ChildrenBoardman et al.

Original Research
Exceptional Children
1­–19
Collaborative Strategic Reading for © 2016 The Author(s)
DOI: 10.1177/0014402915625067
Students With Learning Disabilities ec.sagepub.com

in Upper Elementary Classrooms

Alison G. Boardman1, Sharon Vaughn2, Pamela Buckley1,


Colleen Reutebuch2, Gregory Roberts2, and Janette Klingner1

Abstract
Sixty fourth- and fifth-grade general education teachers were randomly assigned to teach
Collaborative Strategic Reading (CSR; Klingner, Vaughn, Boardman, & Swanson, 2012), a set
of reading comprehension strategies, or to a business-as-usual comparison group. Results
demonstrate that students with learning disabilities (LD) who received CSR instruction in their
general education classrooms—approximately two times each week over a 14-week period—
made significantly greater gains in reading comprehension than students with LD in comparison
classrooms (g = .52). Teachers in CSR classrooms were also more likely to provide feedback to
students and to use collaborative grouping structures.

Reading to understand, to learn, and to stay Further, initiatives such as the Common
connected via technology are essential life Core State Standards (CCSS; National Gover-
skills. Yet for many students with learning dis- nors Association Center for Best Practices,
abilities (LD), understanding text remains an Council of Chief State School Officers, 2010)
elusive goal. Increasingly, general education call for increasing rigor with a particular
teachers are responsible for improving read- emphasis on engaging students with challeng-
ing comprehension for all learners—including ing expository text. Instructional models sup-
those with disabilities. In fact, more than two porting struggling readers in general education
thirds of students with LD spend most of their classrooms are needed to help students
day in general education classrooms (National increase their reading comprehension out-
Center for Education Statistics, 2014), comes and facilitate access to high-level texts.
whereas only a decade ago less than 50% of The current study examined the influence of
students with LD were included to the same using a set of reading comprehension strate-
extent. In addition, the Individuals With Dis- gies, Collaborative Strategic Reading (CSR;
abilities Education Act (2006) recommends Klingner, Vaughn, Boardman, & Swanson,
that students with disabilities are educated in 2012) in fourth- and fifth-grade classrooms
the least restrictive environment and as much compared to a business-as-usual comparison
as possible with their peers without disabili-
ties. Yet, although the amount of time students 1
University of Colorado Boulder
with LD spend in general education class- 2
University of Texas at Austin
rooms has increased steadily over time, their
Corresponding Author:
academic achievement continues to be far Alison G. Boardman, University of Colorado Boulder,
behind that of their peers without disabilities 247 UCB, Boulder, CO 80309, USA.
(Cortiella & Horowitz, 2014). E-mail: alison.boardman@colorado.edu

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com by guest on April 10, 2016


2 Exceptional Children

group who used their typical instructional to actively construct meaning by self-­
strategies. Specifically, we were interested in monitoring and applying tactics to facilitate
the influence of CSR on the reading achieve- understanding throughout the reading process
ment of students with LD who participated in (e.g., Duke, Pearson, Strachan, & Billman,
grade-level reading instruction in their gen- 2011; Kendeou, van de Broek, Helder, &
eral education classrooms. This study extends Karlsson, 2014). It can be challenging for
existing CSR research by increasing the sam- teachers to teach students who are poor read-
ple size of students and teachers under inves- ers to enact the reading comprehension prac-
tigation and by looking closely at grade levels tices that are often used effortlessly by more
that have had mixed results in other CSR stud- capable ­ readers. Comprehension strategy
ies, some in favor of students who receive instruction can provide actionable steps that
CSR (Klingner, Vaughn, & Schumm, 1998) students learn to apply before, during, and
and others that have shown no differences after reading to overcome barriers to under-
between CSR and typical instruction (Hitchcock, standing and enhance knowledge related to
Dimino, Kurki, Wilkins, & Gersten, 2010). In the text.
addition, we use fidelity of implementation
measures to look closely at the similarities Effective Reading
and differences across conditions to establish
how implementation of CSR in authentic
Comprehension Practices for
classrooms settings compares with instruction Students With LD
in classrooms without CSR. A large number of syntheses or meta-analyses
report positive outcomes from comprehension
Comprehension strategy instruction interventions for struggling elementary and ado-
can provide actionable steps that lescent readers, including those with and with-
students learn to apply before, out identified LD (e.g., Butler, Urrutia, Buenger,
& Hunt, 2010; Edmonds et al., 2009; Flynn,
during, and after reading to
Zheng, & Swanson, 2012; Gajria, Jitendra,
overcome barriers to understanding Sood, & Sacks, 2007; Gersten, Fuchs, Williams,
and to enhance knowledge related & Baker, 2001; Vaughn, Gersten, & Chard,
to the text. 2000; Wanzek, Wexler, Vaughn, & Ciullo,
2010). A number of these reviews have grouped
Reading comprehension has been described fourth and fifth graders with those in middle and
as “the most critical skill students need to be high school (i.e., Grades 4–12) based on the
successful in school” (Watson, Gable, Gear, & rationale that those in upper elementary grades
Hughes, 2012, p. 80), and conceptual, subject- face reading demands with more similarities to
matter knowledge is enhanced when students those of older readers than those of the lower
are able to access and understand text (Reed elementary grades (i.e., Grades 1–3) where
& Vaughn, 2012). Yet, for students who have foundational reading skills are emphasized (e.g.,
difficulty understanding what they read, phonemic awareness, phonics, fluency). Consis-
focusing on mechanisms for enhancing text- tent with this notion, findings from 82 studies
based reading comprehension is essential to reviewed in a recent analysis of reading inter-
improve overall understanding (Kamil et al., ventions (Scammacca, Roberts, Vaughn, &
2008; McKeown, Beck, & Blake, 2009). As Stuebing, 2015) suggested that fourth- through
noted by the RAND Reading Study Group 12th-grade students benefit similarly.
(2002), “because meaning does not exist in Wanzek and colleagues (2010) conducted a
text, but rather must be actively constructed, synthesis of interventions focused exclusively
instruction in how to employ strategies is nec- on students in Grades 4 and 5 and found strong
essary to improve comprehension” (p. 32) evidence to support using direct and explicit
Nearly four decades of research has con- comprehension instruction for students with
firmed that reading comprehension is a cogni- reading difficulties and disabilities. Further,
tively demanding task that requires individuals Wanzek et al. (2010) explained that for upper

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com by guest on April 10, 2016


Boardman et al. 3

elementary students, ­comprehension practices conducted in language arts settings with nar-
that provide opportunities for students to pre- rative text, most of the models are not specific
view text and connect with their background to a particular content area or text type. Find-
knowledge, use self-questioning and self-reg- ings such as these ground the consensus rec-
ulating practices while reading, and summa- ommendations (Kamil et al., 2008) that many
rize what they are learning are associated with instructional techniques in reading compre-
moderate to high effects. These results are hension are widely applicable across teachers
consistent with other research syntheses on and content areas (e.g., explicitly teaching
reading comprehension outcomes with older vocabulary, providing comprehension strat-
students (Edmonds et al., 2009; Scammacca egy instruction, incorporating text-based
et al., 2007; Solis et al., 2012). The vast major- ­discussion).
ity of interventions in middle grades have uti-
lized instruction related to main idea or Collaborative Strategic
summarization (Solis et al., 2012).
Scammacca and her colleagues (2007,
Reading
2015) conducted two meta-analyses on older CSR (Klingner & Vaughn, 1999; Klingner
students (Grades 4–12) and confirmed that the et al., 2012) is a multicomponent reading com-
research base supports teaching reading strat- prehension instructional model theoretically
egies to those who struggle with reading. In grounded in cognitive psychology (Flavell,
particular, they reported higher impact (i.e., 1979) with a focus on explicit instruction for
effect sizes) from multicomponent interven- struggling learners. Whereas strong readers
tions, which include word-level and text-level may “pick up” strategies for reading and apply
instruction. Further, the Flynn et al. (2012) them automatically, struggling readers can be
synthesis on struggling readers (Grades 5–9) taught those same strategies explicitly with
emphasized a need for future research endeav- information about what the strategy is, how to
ors that incorporate vocabulary and compre- apply it, why it is important, and in what con-
hension instruction to better support the texts the strategy will be the most useful. In
reading challenges faced by older readers. addition, sociocultural theory (Vygostky,
Additional recommendations include 1978) informs the structure of CSR with an
incorporating discussion about text into emphasis on scaffolding and peer-mediated
instruction as a means to increase engagement learning. In sociocultural theory, cognition
and comprehension (Lawrence & Snow, 2011; occurs through both individual processing and
Murphy, Wilkinson, Soter, Hennessey, & social interaction and is thus increased when
Alexander, 2009). Murphy and colleagues individuals can listen to, learn from, and build
(2009) reviewed 40 studies of discourse- on each other’s ideas. The teacher’s role is to
focused comprehension models, most con- create learning contexts that encourage col-
ducted with fourth through sixth graders, and laboration and that include guidance and sup-
found that many approaches increased stu- ports so that students learn more than they
dents’ comprehension and were also effective would have been able to learn on their own
at increasing student talk and decreasing (Wertsch, 1991). In CSR, collaboration occurs
teacher talk. Findings were generally more in student-led cooperative groups where each
robust for students reading below average lev- student is given a specific role (e.g., question
els than for those at or above grade level. In expert), using the tenets of cooperative learn-
another review of the influence of discourse ing (Johnson & Johnson, 1989, 2008).
on reading comprehension, Lawrence and Students use before, during, and after read-
Snow (2011) found that asking important ing strategies during CSR to access challeng-
questions, providing a focus for the discus- ing text. Before reading, the teacher leads a
sion, and defining rules for how to interact short preview of the text that includes stating
during discussions increases the quality of the topic, presenting a few key vocabulary
student talk about text. Though much of the concepts, having students connect to their
research on text-based discussions has been background knowledge, and setting the pur-

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com by guest on April 10, 2016


4 Exceptional Children

pose for reading (preview). During reading, Vaughn, Arguelles, Hughes, & Ahwee, 2004).
students monitor their understanding while Again, students in CSR made significantly
reading the text out loud in small groups. greater gains in reading comprehension on the
They stop after each short section of text to GMRT when compared to students in com-
identify words and ideas they don’t under- parison classrooms who did not receive CSR
stand and to use context clues and morphemic (d = .19, p < .01). Both low-achieving students
analysis to figure out word meanings (click and students identified as LD made the great-
and clunk). Next, students independently gen- est relative gains (d = .25 for high- and
erate a brief main idea statement, share with average-achieving students, .51 for low-
­
group members, and give one another feed- achieving students, and .38 for students with
back (get the gist). After reading the entire LD). In an experimental study that randomly
text, students ask and answer each other’s assigned 74 fifth-grade classrooms across five
questions; write a short review statement; and school districts to treatment or a control condi-
discuss why their ideas are the most impor- tion, there were no statistically significant dif-
tant, using evidence from the text (wrap up). ferences in reading comprehension outcomes
Initially, teachers use explicit instruction to between conditions (Hitchcock et al., 2010).
introduce each strategy one at a time over Researchers reported that low fidelity to the
about five or six short lessons. Then, teachers full CSR model might have contributed to the
move students into small groups of approxi- results. For example, only one fifth of study
mately four students, fine-tuning strategy use teachers were observed using all of the core
and promoting collaboration and discussion CSR components. This study also reported a
skills that are needed to manage small group smaller amount of follow-up support than has
learning. Students utilize a variety of resources been provided in other CSR studies.
to scaffold their use of the reading strategies, Several experimental studies have also been
including role cue cards (i.e., leader, clunk conducted on CSR at the middle school level.
expert, gist expert, question expert), learning In one study, using a within-teacher design,
logs to record their ideas, and lists of affixes four sections with 34 sixth-, seventh-, and
to support word analysis. Teachers are encour- eighth-grade students with LD in reading and
aged to embed CSR into content instruction language arts were randomly assigned to use a
using curriculum-based expository text. computer-adapted version of CSR two times a
Several studies have examined the efficacy week for 10 to 12 weeks or to a comparison
of CSR in Grades 4 through 8. In one quasi- condition (Kim et al., 2006). Reading growth
experimental design (QED) study, researchers was statistically significant on a researcher-
provided daily CSR instruction in inclusive developed measure of reading comprehension
classrooms of fourth graders, about 50% of and on a standardized measure, the Woodcock
whom were English language learners (ELLs), Reading Mastery Test-Revised (d = .50,
during an 11-day social studies unit, while a p < .05), favoring the students who received
second group of students received their typical computerized CSR (Kim et al., 2006). In
instruction without CSR. Relative to students another experimental study that also used a
in the comparison condition, students who within-teacher design, 387 students in seventh-
received CSR instruction made statistically and eighth-grade language arts and reading
significant gains in reading as measured by the classrooms were randomly assigned to CSR or
Gates MacGinitie Reading Test (GMRT, fourth to a comparison condition of matched classes
edition; MacGinitie, MacGinitie, Maria, & without CSR (Vaughn et al., 2011). Students
Dreyer, 2000; effect size d = .44, p < .001; who received CSR instruction approximately
Klingner et al., 1998). In another QED study, once weekly over the school year scored sig-
fourth-grade students from five culturally and nificantly higher on the GMRT than students in
linguistically diverse inclusive classrooms a comparison group, with struggling readers
received CSR two to three times each week for making the greatest gains (g = .12 for the
1 month during social studies (Klingner, overall sample, .36 for struggling readers;
­

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com by guest on April 10, 2016


Boardman et al. 5

p < .05). Finally, in a randomized control trial Setting and Participants


that used a within-teacher design, this time in
middle school science and social studies The study took place in 14 elementary schools
classes, 1,074 students in social studies and sci- located in three urban/near urban school dis-
ence classrooms were assigned to receive CSR tricts, two in one state (Site 1) and one in
instruction weekly in each content area or to a another state (Site 2). Each district served a
comparison condition with no CSR (Boardman, diverse population of students.
Klingner, Buckley, Annamma, & Jensen, Sixty teachers, 31 randomly assigned to
2015). Students who received CSR instruction treatment and 29 to control, participated in the
scored higher on the GMRT than their peers in study. Selection and consent of both teachers
comparison classrooms (g = .18, p < .05). The and students followed Institutional Review
present study investigated further the imple- Board procedures at the participating univer-
mentation and outcomes of using CSR in sities. Due to uneven numbers of teachers by
fourth- and fifth-grade classrooms. site (31 in Site 1 and 29 in Site 2), randomiza-
tion resulted in an additional teacher in the
treatment condition at each site (16 treatment
Method
and 15 control in Site 1 and 15 treatment and
We employed a multisite cluster randomized 14 control in Site 2). Ninety-five percent of
control trial (RCT) in which teachers were ran- the teachers were female. The majority (65%)
domly assigned to condition (i.e., the CSR were Caucasian, followed by Hispanic (27%),
group or the business-as-usual “typical” group). African American (6%), and Asian (2%).
We conducted an intent-to-treat analysis where Teaching experience ranged from 1 to 30
we analyzed the groups exactly as they existed years, with a mean of 9.23 and a median of 7
on randomization. This design controls for years. Roughly 40% of teachers had a mas-
observed and unobserved teacher differences ter’s degree. Nearly half of the teachers (48%)
because these factors are independent of condi- held at least one certification in addition to
tion, thus minimizing the risk of teacher-level being elementary certified (e.g., English lan-
confounds. The RCT is recommended for this guage arts, bilingual, reading, special educa-
purpose (Coalition for ­Evidence-Based Policy, tion, English as a second language).
2003). There were 1,372 students in the initial
Because our study took place in upper ele- sample (686 in treatment and 686 in the com-
mentary classes, where teachers provide parison condition; see Table 1). The average
instruction in different content areas to the age was 10.6 for students in CSR treatment
same students, we randomly assigned teachers and 10.52 for students in comparison. The
to use the CSR instructional model or to con- majority of the student sample was Hispanic,
tinue with their typical instructional practices. and about half were ELLs. Approximately
We addressed the following research questions: 10% of the students in the treatment group
received special education services compared
1. What is the efficacy of CSR on the to about 12% of students in the comparison
reading comprehension of fourth- and group. Five percent (n = 32) of the CSR stu-
fifth-grade students in general educa- dents were identified with a learning disabil-
tion classrooms compared with stu- ity compared to 8% (n = 55) of the students in
dents in a comparison group? the comparison group. Because teachers were
2. What is the efficacy of CSR taught in randomly assigned to condition such that in
general education classrooms for a each school some teachers taught CSR and
subgroup of students with learning dis- others did not, we are not aware of any sys-
abilities? tematic differences in the way students with
3. How does fidelity to the CSR model disabilities were distributed across classrooms
distinguish instruction between CSR or in the types or amount of special education
and comparison classrooms? services they received. Considering the

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com by guest on April 10, 2016


6 Exceptional Children

Table 1.  Demographics for Initial Student Sample.

CSRa TYPb

Demographics n % n %
Gender
 Male 357 52.0 326 47.5
 Female 329 48.0 360 52.5
Race/Ethnicity
  African American 42 6.1 43 6.3
 Hispanic 470 68.5 475 69.2
 Asian 5 0.7 5 0.7
  Pacific Islander 12 1.7 12 1.7
 White 331 48.3 331 48.3
 Multiracial 4 0.6 1 0.1
English language learner 360 52.5 342 49.9
Special education total 55 8.0 73 10.8
  Learning disability (LD) 32 4.7 55 8.0
  Special education other than LD 22 3.2 18 2.6

Note. The percentages associated with ethnicity may add up to more than 100 because students were allowed to
be categorized to more than one ethnicity. CSR = Collaborative Strategic Reading (Klingner, Vaughn, Boardman, &
Swanson, 2012); TYP = typical practice.
a
N = 686. bN = 686.

d­ istribution of students with LD across class- opportunities for teachers to practice and to plan
rooms, classes were similar (e.g., similar ratio how to integrate CSR into existing practices.
of students with LD to students without LD) All teachers received introductory lessons with
with the exception that in the CSR condition, texts as well as the teacher and student materials
only 42% of classrooms had students with (e.g., strategy resource cards) needed to imple-
LD, whereas 76% of classrooms in the control ment CSR. Texts in the introductory lessons
condition included students with LD. Twenty- ranged in level from Grades 3 to 4. A follow-up
two additional students in the CSR group and 2-hour booster session was provided in each
18 additional students in the comparison district midway through the study that empha-
group had a disability other than LD. Catego- sized using student work to plan CSR instruc-
ries included deaf, emotional disability, physi- tion and that addressed problems of practice
cal disability, speech and language, autism unique to specific districts. Initial professional
spectrum disorder, significant limited intellec- development and follow-up booster sessions
tual capacity, and multiple disabilities. Our were planned collaboratively by the research
sample was representative of national per- team and provided by a lead researcher in each
centages of students with LD and other dis- state to ensure consistency of presentation con-
abilities (Cortiella & Horowitz, 2014). tent, materials, and quality. In addition, teachers
were offered biweekly coaching sessions by
Professional Development and university graduate students who were experi-
enced coaches and experts in CSR. Coaches
Teacher Support would typically observe, co-teach, or model and
Teachers participated in one full day of CSR then debrief with teachers, emphasizing what
professional development that included the was going well and areas for improvement.
rationale for implementing strategy instruction,
how to teach the CSR strategies, methods for
Description of Intervention
supporting discussion in student-led groups,
and how to introduce CSR to students. Profes- Students in both conditions (CSR and business
sional development included modeling and as usual, or comparison) were taught in diverse

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com by guest on April 10, 2016


Boardman et al. 7

general education classrooms including ELLs, subtest is designed to measure students’ abili-
struggling readers, and students with LD, as ties to read and understand different types of
well as average and above-average readers. prose from passages in published books and
Teachers assigned to the intervention condition periodicals. Some questions relate to a literal
were asked to implement CSR two to three ses- understanding of the passage whereas others
sions each week, for approximately 50 min per require students to make inferences or draw
session, while reading predominately exposi- conclusions. In addition, the comprehension
tory text (either the textbook or supplemental subtest is designed to measure students’ ability
readings matched to the curriculum). To ensure to determine the meaning of words in an
that all students received CSR instruction, we authentic text context. Two parallel forms per-
requested that CSR be used during Tier 1 mit pre- and posttesting. Alternate-forms reli-
instruction, and coaches supported this request ability for the GMRT is estimated to range
when planning with teachers so that lessons from .90 to .95, and test-retest reliability has
included all or most students in the class. CSR been estimated as above .88 (MacGinitie et al.,
was usually implemented as part of teachers’ 2000).
literacy blocks, although teachers also chose to
use CSR in social studies or science, and some
teachers varied when they delivered CSR
Implementation Measures
instruction. In complex classroom environ- Fidelity of implementation denotes the degree
ments, there is a fair amount of movement in to which a program is implemented as origi-
and out of class throughout the day in elemen- nally planned (O’Donnell, 2008). Fidelity is
tary settings, particularly for students who operationalized in this study in terms of dos-
receive special services. Although there was age (frequency of implementation), quality
some movement of students recorded during (how well CSR instruction was delivered),
observations, classrooms were relatively intact and program differentiation (the degree to
during CSR instruction. Both treatment and which instructional strategies within each
comparison classes followed the district’s condition can be differentiated). To measure
fourth- and fifth-grade curriculum and selected dosage, we used teacher logs, in which teach-
readings at their discretion. Teachers in both ers in the treatment condition self-reported the
conditions used a combination of textbook and number of CSR sessions and minutes they
supplementary resources. In comparison taught throughout the intervention. The
classes, we asked teachers to continue using Implementation Validity Checklist (IVC;
their typical instructional practices without the Vaughn et al., 2011, 2013) classroom observa-
weekly segments of CSR. The study took place tion tool was used to assess quality of CSR
during the fall semester of 2010 for approxi- instruction and program differentiation.
mately 14 weeks of instruction between when
students were pre- and posttested. Teacher Logs.  Researchers collected logs com-
pleted by 16 of the 31 CSR teachers to estimate
dosage (i.e., the frequency of CSR implemen-
Student Outcome Measure
tation). Teacher participants in one site did not
The student outcome was reading comprehen- agree to complete implementation logs.
sion performance, as measured by the reading Coaches collaborated closely with teachers
comprehension subtest of the Gates-MacGini- throughout the study and were present in all
tie Reading Test (fourth edition; MacGinitie, schools on a weekly basis. Though unable to
MacGinitie et al., 2000). The GMRT is a timed confirm exact implementation at Site 2,
paper-and-pencil, group-administered survey researchers reported that the number of ses-
that assesses student achievement in reading sions, including variation among teachers,
and was administered by researchers prior to appeared to be similar across sites. The mean
beginning the intervention (August) and prior to number of self-reported CSR sessions was 39
winter break (December). The comprehension (SD = 12.7; median = 37 sessions; range, 23–62

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com by guest on April 10, 2016


8 Exceptional Children

sessions), and the mean number of minutes was each other while working in groups, partici-
1,795 min (SD = 580.6; range, 1,065–3,480 pating, and using reading strategies profi-
min) or approximately 30 hr of CSR instruction ciently. Responses are rated on a scale of 0
(median = 1,673 min) with sessions that were (not observed) to 4 (observed and done well).
typically around 40 min each. Teachers were The third section of the IVC includes global
asked to implement CSR two to three times quality items that are rated on a scale from 1
each week for about 50 min per session. On (low) to 7 (high), including quality of teach-
average, teachers taught slightly more sessions er’s instruction and quality of teacher’s class-
than expected for a slightly shorter amount of room management.
time per session. Dosage, as reported by teach- Observers participated in trainings to
ers, was in the acceptable range. ensure that an interrater agreement of 90% on
all IVC items was reached. This training pro-
Implementation Validity Checklist (IVC).  We mea- cess has been followed in other CSR studies
sured quality of implementation and program that report on fidelity using the IVC (e.g.,
differentiation in both sites using the IVC, a Boardman et al., 2015; Vaughn et al., 2011).
classroom observational tool (Vaughn et al., Another reason to conduct IVC observa-
2011, 2013). The IVC contains items linked to tions in both treatment and comparison classes
components of CSR as well as core teacher was to measure program differentiation, or the
and student behaviors that are needed to imple- extent to which instruction similar to CSR
ment CSR with high quality but are not unique was present in comparison classrooms.
to CSR (e.g., promoting collaboration and pro- Describing the counterfactual, or what the
viding feedback). Observers used the IVC intervention is being compared to, helps con-
rubric to record the extent to which teachers textualize results in randomized control trials
implemented CSR as intended. The instrument (Lemons, Fuchs, Gilbert, & Fuchs, 2014).
has been used to determine the quality and
variation in delivery of CSR and to detect sim-
ilarities in instructional practices that might
Data Analysis Procedures
occur in comparison classrooms (e.g., if a To analyze outcomes, raw scores for GMRT
main idea strategy is taught in a comparison tests were converted into a standard score with
classroom). For this study, we collected three a mean of 100 and standard deviation of 15 to
preannounced rounds of IVC observations assist with interpretation. Using the software
with all 60 teachers (CSR and comparison) program Hierarchical Linear Modeling 7.0, we
approximately once each 4 weeks between ran a multilevel, random intercepts model to
October and December. estimate the effects of CSR on GMRT posttest
Divided into three sections, the first part of scores while controlling for GMRT pretest
the IVC measures procedural fidelity, in scores. We also included s­ tudent-level dummy
which teachers are rated on each CSR compo- coded special education status variables
nent (i.e., teacher/student preview, clunks, fix because we were interested in this subgroup of
up strategies, get the gist, questions, review). students. To model special education status,
Scores are assigned according to quality and students who received no special education
adherence to the intervention, ranging from 0 services served as the reference group. The
(not observed) to 4 (highly aligned with CSR dummy codes included learning disability
strategy). The second section of the IVC mea- (LD; 1 = yes, 0 = no) and special education
sures six teacher and student behaviors that other than LD (Sped_other; 1 = yes, 0 = no).
are essential to high-quality implementation Students were regarded as nested in teachers
of CSR. Teacher behaviors are scored accord- who were nested in schools. Condition (1 =
ing to the extent to which the teacher monitors treatment, 0 = comparison) was modeled at
and provides feedback, reinforces learning, Level 2 because that is where the random
and encourages group collaboration. Student assignment took place, and LD and Sped_
items include observations of students helping other were each interacted with condition. The

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com by guest on April 10, 2016


Boardman et al. 9

effect of treatment and the effect of treatment disability than for those who received no spe-
interacted with LD and with a disability other cial education services.
than LD were all modeled as random in esti- We conducted multiple analyses to investi-
mating main and interaction effects. Pretest gate implementation. We ran frequencies and
GMRT scores were entered into the model as descriptives to analyze dosage, as measured by
grand mean centered. When running the analy- the number of CSR components and the teacher/
ses explained further in the findings section, student behaviors promoting CSR application
the effect of condition interacted with Sped_ observed within each round of the fidelity
other was nonsignificant. The LD × Condition observations. In addition, we conducted an
effect, however, was significant regardless of ­independent-samples t test to examine program
whether condition was interacted with Sped_ differentiation between CSR and typical condi-
other. Thus, to run the most parsimonious tions, as measured by the Implementation Valid-
model, we dropped the interaction effect ity Checklist. We conducted three separate
between treatment and Sped_other but kept analyses, one assessing differences between
the interaction of condition with LD in the conditions in procedural fidelity, the second
model. Thus, using a three-level notation assessing differences in teacher/student behav-
(Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002), the final model iors, and the third analyzing differences in over-
took the following form: all teacher quality. The procedural fidelity score
was computed as a composite component score
Level 1 of seven items (i.e., teacher preview, student pre-
PostGMRTijk = π0jk + π1jk × (PreGMRTijk) view, clunks, fix-up strategies, gist, questions,
+ π2jk × LDijk) + π3jk × review) that we averaged within and across the
(Sped_OTHERijk) + eijk three rounds of observations. Similarly, the
teacher/student behaviors score was computed
Level 2 by averaging the sum of the three teacher behav-
ior items and three student behavior items within
π0jk = β00k + β01k × (CONDITIONjk) + r0jk each round and then across rounds. Global qual-
π1jk = β10k ity included two items, each averaged within
and across rounds: overall quality of teacher’s
π2jk = β20k + β21k × (CONDITIONjk) + r2jk instruction and quality of teacher’s classroom
management.
π3jk = β30k

Level 3 Missing Data


β00k = γ000 + u00k Although randomizing teachers to conditions
should result in statistically equivalent groups,
As shown previously, the model including missing data due to attrition could threaten the
the interaction term also included controls for initial balance and result in a biased impact
the main effects of LD identification and CSR estimate. For this study, there were 1,372 stu-
treatment. To test the main effect, GMRT dents at random assignment (686 in treatment
posttest scores were regressed on GMRT pre- and 686 in control) and 1,303 students in the
test scores, special education status dummy- analytic sample (653 in treatment and 650 in
coded variables, and treatment indicator to control). Thus, the overall attrition rate was
examine differences in outcomes between 5.0%, and the differential attrition rate was
­students in the treatment and students in the 0.4%. There were no ­missing data at either the
comparison condition. An additional model teacher or school cluster levels. Using a con-
was fit with an interaction term between LD servative attrition threshold (What Works
status and the treatment variable to examine Clearinghouse, 2014), attrition for this study
the hypothesis that the CSR treatment might was low. Missing student data were therefore
be more effective for students with a learning handled using listwise deletion.

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com by guest on April 10, 2016


10 Exceptional Children

Results There was no significant main effect of


CSR on student outcomes, as indicated in the
Maintaining the same standard score on the summary of mixed and random effects in
GMRT from fall to winter represents average Table 3. There was, however, a significant,
growth in reading over that time period (Mac- positive interaction effect between condition
Ginitie et al., 2002). Our results show that on and posttest scores for students with LD. The
average, although students with LD scored adjusted posttest means were higher for stu-
lower at pretest and at posttest than students dents with LD who received CSR in general
without LD, the average gains from pretest to education classrooms than for students with
posttest for all students were positive. Further, LD who did not receive CSR (β = 4.86, p =
students with LD in CSR outperformed stu- .03). It is estimated that students with LD
dents with LD in the comparison condition, score an average of 4.86 points higher on the
on average (see Figure 1). GMRT when in a class implementing CSR,
compared to students with LD who received
Although students with LD scored typical instruction. This difference is equiva-
lower at pretest and at posttest than lent to a Hedges’s g effect size of 0.52.
students without LD, the average
gains from pretest to posttest for all Implementation
students were positive.
Implementation results show the percentage
of teachers observed implementing each of
The observed values (descriptive statistics)
the CSR components in CSR and compari-
show similar pre- and posttest means and
son classrooms during the third round of IVC
standard deviations for treatment and com-
data collection (i.e., receiving a score on a
parison groups. These findings are presented
CSR component of 1 or above) was 79%
in Table 2.

95

90

85 CSR - Students with LD

80
CSR - Students without LD
75

70 TYP - Students with LD

65
TYP - Students without LD

60

55

50
Mean pre GMRT Mean post GMRT

Figure 1.  The differential influence of condition (CSR or TYP) on GMRT scores.
Note. CSR = Collaborative Strategic Reading (Klingner, Vaughn, Boardman, & Swanson, 2012); GMRT = Gates-
MacGinitie Reading Test (MacGinitie, MacGinitie, Maria, & Dreyer, 2000); TYP = typical; LD = learning disability.

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com by guest on April 10, 2016


Boardman et al. 11

Table 2.  Means and Standard Deviations on the GMRT for Study Groups.

Pretest Posttest

  M SD n M SD n
Main effect
  Intervention (CSR) 88.56 13.77 679 93.06 12.35 670
 TYP 89.18 14.92 680 93.09 14.11 683
Interaction effect
  Learning disability
  Intervention (CSR) 79.69 10.70 32 85.21 7.89 31
  TYP 77.15 9.76 55 79.23 9.95 52
  No special education services
  Intervention (CSR) 89.13 13.8 627 93.67 12.53 604
  TYP 90.36 14.79 608 94.53 13.73 581

Note. CSR = Collaborative Strategic Reading (Klingner, Vaughn, Boardman, & Swanson, 2012); GMRT = Gates-
MacGinitie Reading Test (MacGinitie, MacGinitie, Maria, & Dreyer, 2000); TYP = typical.

(CSR) and 87% (comparison) for teacher strategy use in typical classrooms (as mea-
preview, 84% (CSR) and 55% (comparison) sured by procedural fidelity items on the IVC),
for student preview, 100% (CSR) and 34% teachers in the CSR condition implemented
(comparison) for identify clunks, 100% CSR-aligned reading comprehension strate-
(CSR) and 28% (comparison) for use fix-up gies to a greater degree than teachers in the
strategies, 97% (CSR) and 31% (compari- comparison condition, thus providing ade-
son) for get the gist, 58% (CSR) and 41% quate distinction between the two study condi-
(comparison) for ask and answer questions, tions. The curricula in the study school districts
and 45% (CSR) and 31% (comparison) for included some amount of reading strategy
review. Thus, inasmuch as using the full CSR instruction, and this was observed in many
model was emphasized in professional devel- classrooms. For example, more than 80% of
opment and coaching, teachers in CSR class- both CSR and typical teachers were engaging
rooms frequently did not complete the model students in previewing activities prior to read-
(through questions and review) within each ing across observation rounds. Previewing the
observation session. Although some teachers text is part of CSR but is also common in typi-
reported covering all the strategies over 2 cal instruction in upper elementary class-
days of instruction, many students were rooms. There was also a s­ ignificant difference
probably exposed to only the first three or in teacher/student behavior scores between
four of the expected five CSR strategies on a CSR (M = 2.9, SD = .6) and typical (M = 2.3,
regular basis. SD =.6) teachers; t(58) = 4.5, p = .000. Results
Table 4 shows means and standard devia- from the teacher/student behaviors scale in the
tions for procedural fidelity (i.e., each compo- IVC indicate that in CSR classrooms, teachers
nent of the CSR model) as well as teacher and provided more feedback on the quality of stu-
student behaviors promoted in the CSR dents’ strategy use and there was more student-
model, indicating the extent of the alignment led collaborative group work. There was not a
of teachers’ instruction with the CSR model. significant difference in overall teacher quality
The data are provided by condition for each of or classroom management rating scores for
the three rounds of IVC observations. teachers in CSR and comparison conditions.
As presented in Table 5, there was a signifi-
cant difference in the procedural fidelity scores
Discussion
in CSR (M = 2.2, SD = 0.7) and typical (M = .7,
SD = 0.3) conditions; t(58) = 7.7, p = .008. We were interested in the extent to which a
Despite a fair amount of reading comprehension multicomponent reading strategy model (i.e.,

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com by guest on April 10, 2016


12 Exceptional Children

Table 3.  Fixed and Random Effects of Condition, Demographics, and Pretest Scores on Posttest
Scores.

Fixed effects

Predictor Estimate (SE) t Ratioa p Value Hedges’s g


Intercept, β0 95.53 (0.70) 138.37 <.001  
 Condition, γ010 –0.41 (0.79) –0.75 .606 –0.03
GMRT pretest, β10 0.63 (0.02) 26.54 <.001  
LDb, β20 –7.15 (1.30) –4.71 <.001
 Condition, γ210 4.86 (2.11) 2.00 .026 .52
SPED_Otherb, β30 –1.68 (1.44) –1.54 .243  

Random effects

  Variance (SD) t Ratioa p Value % of total variation


Level 1 (individual) 62.22 (7.89) 2.433 <.001 88.52
Level 2 (teacher) 5.92 (2.43) 2.43 .005 8.42
Level 3 (school) 2.15 (1.47) 1.47 3.06

Note. LD = learning disability; SPED_Other = special education status other than LD; GMRT – Gates-MacGinitie
Reading Test (MacGinitie, MacGinitie, Maria, & Dreyer, 2000); CSR = Collaborative Strategic Reading (Klingner,
Vaughn, Boardman, & Swanson, 2012); condition (CSR = 1, comparison = 0).
a
The t ratio for fixed effects was determined by dividing the estimate by its standard error; for random effects, the
t ratio was determined by dividing the variance component by its standard deviation. bReference group is students
that receive no special education services.

CSR; Klingner et al., 2012), that incorporates study included some form of reading strate-
features of effective instruction for students gies and other instructional moves likely to
with comprehension difficulties would posi- promote reading comprehension such as peer-
tively influence the reading comprehension of mediated discussions about text. In addition,
students with LD in general education class- the overall quality of instruction was rated as
rooms. Overall, students without LD in both equivalent in CSR and non-CSR classrooms,
conditions made gains in a short period of and overall, instruction in both conditions was
time with no s­tatistical ­ difference between above average. In this study, students without
treatment and comparison groups. This dem- LD were exposed to adequate or better read-
onstrates that students w­ ithout LD appeared ing instruction in both conditions and made
to benefit from the instruction they were strong gains in reading comprehension, indi-
receiving in their classrooms, regardless of cating that the specific nature of reading
whether or not CSR was used. However, stu- instruction was not important to their reading
dents with LD who received CSR delivered growth. Thus, the use of CSR strategies did
by their general education teacher as part of not add or detract from the reading gains of
their grade-level reading instruction approxi- students without LD; however, for students
mately two times each week for 14 weeks with LD, CSR was particularly beneficial (g =
made significantly greater gains in reading .52).
comprehension than students with LD who Second, although we did not interview
did not receive CSR instruction. These results teachers in this study, in previous studies
are promising for several reasons. First, these teachers have reported that CSR supports all
gains were achieved despite a good deal of learners with a particular benefit to students
similar instruction across conditions. Instruc- who read below grade level (e.g., Klingner,
tion in most comparison classrooms in this Boardman, Boele, & Annamma, 2010;

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com by guest on April 10, 2016


Boardman et al. 13

Table 4.  Implementation Validity Checklist Observation Results.

CSR TYP

  R1 R2 R3 R1 R2 R3

  M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)


Procedural fidelity
  Teacher preview 1.3 (1.3) 2.5 (1.4) 2.9 (1.4) 1.8 (1.2) 1.6 (1.2) 1.7 (1.3)
  Student preview 2.0 (1.4) 2.3 (1.5) 2.7 (1.4) 1.0 (1.0) 0.9 (1.2) 1.0 (1.1)
 Clunks 2.7 (1.2) 3.0 (1.4) 3.3 (0.9) 0.8 (0.9) 0.4 (0.6) 0.4 (0.6)
  Fix-up strategies 2.1 (1.2) 2.6 (1.1) 2.9 (0.9) 0.6 (0.6) 0.4 (0.7) 0.3 (0.6)
  Get the gist 2.1 (1.3) 2.3 (1.3) 2.9 (1.0) 0.5 (0.8) 0.8 (1.1) 0.4 (0.7)
 Questions 1.7 (1.3) 2.0 (1.5) 1.8 (1.7) 0.6 (0.9) 0.8 (1.0) 0.7 (1.0)
 Review 0.6 (1.0) 1.0 (1.3) 1.1 (1.3) 0.6 (0.8) 0.6 (0.9) 0.5 (0.7)
Teacher behaviors
  Monitors and provides feedback 2.8 (0.8) 3.0 (0.8) 3.3 (0.9) 1.8 (0.8) 1.9 (1.2) 1.8 (1.1)
  Reinforces learning 2.6 (0.9) 2.8 (1.0) 2.9 (1.0) 2.6 (1.0) 2.9 (1.0) 3.0 (1.0)
  Promotes collaboration in groups 2.8 (0.9) 3.1 (1.0) 3.3 (0.9) 2.4 (1.1) 2.7 (1.1) 2.7 (1.1)
Student behaviors
  Help each other while in groups 2.8 (0.9) 2.9 (0.9) 3.1 (0.9) 2.1 (1.1) 1.8 (1.1) 2.0 (1.2)
  All participate in their groups 2.9 (0.8) 3.0 (0.9) 3.3 (0.8) 2.8 (0.8) 2.7 (1.2) 3.2 (0.9)
  Proficiency in strategy use 2.3 (0.6) 2.7 (0.8) 2.9 (0.9) 1.7 (0.8) 1.3 (0.8) 1.4 (.07)

Note. CSR = Collaborative Strategic Reading (Klingner, Vaughn, Boardman, & Swanson, 2012); TYP = typical practice;
R1 = Round 1; R2 = Round 2; R3 = Round 3; procedural fidelity scores (low = 1 to high = 4); not observed = 0;
Teacher and student behaviors scores (low = 1 to high = 4).

Table 5.  Group Differences for Implementation Fidelity Between CSR and Typical Classes.

CSR TYP

Implementation Fidelity Measure M SD M SD t(58) p Cohen’s d


Procedural fidelity 2.23 0.67 0.79 0.32 10.54 .008 2.743
Teacher and student behaviors 2.91 0.56 2.26 0.56 4.52 .000 1.16
Global rating 5.17 1.11 5.23 1.07 –0.196 .846 –0.055

Note. Procedural fidelity includes seven items (scale 0–4). Teacher and student behaviors include six items
(scale 0–4). Global quality is based on one item (scale 1–7). All items are averaged across observation rounds.
CSR = Collaborative Strategic Reading (Klingner, Vaughn, Boardman, & Swanson, 2012); TYP = typical practice;
IVC = Implementation Validity Checklist (Vaugn et al., 2011, 2013).

­ lingner et al., 2004). For example, in an ear-


K present study, the majority of teachers were
lier study conducted during the same project responsive to coaching, were active partici-
reported here, 15 of 17 teachers reported that pants in the professional development and the
CSR supported struggling readers the most follow-up support sessions, and most reported
and also commonly noted the benefits to all that they would continue using CSR with their
learners. “It [CSR] gets kids thinking about entire class after the study ended. Despite the
how you read successfully, and what the pro- lack of significant effects for students without
cess is and they practice it together, and they’re LD, teachers appeared to see benefits for a
learning from each other in the cooperative variety of learners of using CSR as an instruc-
groups” (Klingner et al., 2010, p. 25). In the tional tool to promote reading comprehension.

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com by guest on April 10, 2016


14 Exceptional Children

Finally, providing instruction that is aligned words or concepts) and human resources that
to the CSR model, as measured by the IVC are employed when students share, discuss, and
observation protocol, appears to benefit stu- help each other make meaning from text. In
dents with LD. The strategies observed in CSR addition, the structures also increase account-
classrooms were taught explicitly to students ability for participation by having each student
and then applied in small student-led groups in complete his or her own learning log that is a
a reading routine that was repeated over the record of his or her thinking and ideas and a
course of the semester. Explicit instruction is a way for teachers to monitor progress and pro-
key aspect of CSR and is known to support vide feedback. Students then use the ideas gen-
struggling readers to become more effective erated on the learning log to guide their
readers (Kamil et al., 2008). Teachers used a discussion. The expectations in CSR are that
variety of texts while teaching CSR from basal everyone in the group shares ideas and the role
readers and content area textbooks to supple- expert cue cards guide the group to include all
mental readings that were selected because students. Cooperative learning that incorporates
they aligned with the content or because they individual accountability is the most effective at
would be of high interest to ­students. It may be improving achievement for students with LD
that the intentional structures of CSR increased (McMaster & Fuchs, 2002). Research has con-
opportunities for students with LD to read and sistently shown that students with LD partici-
interact with text. Although we did not collect pate less than students without LD in general
data on the reading levels of teacher-selected education classrooms (e.g., Newman, 2006)
texts, it may also be that because text selection and usually receive reading instruction in large
was addressed during the professional devel- group settings (Swanson, 2008). Intentionally
opment session and by coaches during indi- structuring discussions to value all students is
vidual planning sessions that CSR teachers an important aspect of increasing participation
selected texts that were more accessible to stu- (Berry, 2006). Using structures that equalize
dents with LD than those used in classes with- participation through roles, accountability, and
out CSR. grouping structures may also be critical to
increasing access to new learning for students
Students with LD who received CSR with LD (Lawrence & Snow, 2011). In addition,
delivered by their general education our results indicate that when teachers imple-
teacher as part of their grade-level ment CSR, they provide more feedback to stu-
dents on the quality of their work than in
reading instruction . . . made
comparison classrooms. Feedback is an essential
significantly greater gains in component of instruction for all students but may
reading comprehension than be particularly important for students with LD
students with LD who did not (Hattie & ­Timperley, 2007). Gersten and col-
receive CSR instruction. leagues (2001) also found that peer interactions
accompanied with explicit feedback from the
Further, in CSR, students are assigned to het- teacher support reading comprehension for stu-
erogeneous small groups, and students in this dents with LD. In CSR, teachers facilitate learn-
study were observed working in small groups ing throughout the lesson by listening in on group
more often in CSR classrooms than in compari- discussions and providing verbal feedback to
son classrooms. Within those groups, students individuals and to groups to support strategy use
are given multiple resources and use expert and content learning. Teachers may also provide
roles to guide the group through the reading written feedback on learning logs and use that
process. For students with LD, this provides a data to guide the content of future lessons.
process that they can apply with built-in scaf- When implementing CSR strategies in this
folds that include both material resources (e.g., study, the quality of instruction was generally
a list of fix-up strategies to identify unknown acceptable. However, when considering fidelity

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com by guest on April 10, 2016


Boardman et al. 15

to the full CSR model (i.e., all five strategies), possibility that additional data collection such as
the majority of teachers were observed teaching interview data would have provided greater
consistently only three of the five CSR compo- explanatory power. Further, because we col-
nents (i.e., preview, click and clunk, get the gist). lected implementation logs for only half of our
Other intervention studies that included investi- sample, we were limited in our ability to esti-
gations of implementation factors have also mate overall dosage. We also did not have access
demonstrated low fidelity to parts of the inter- to students’ individualized education program
vention’s model even when there is an overall information or the services that students received
positive effect of the intervention (e.g., May outside of the general education classroom. Still,
et al., 2015). In addition, other studies of CSR researchers were in close contact with teachers
have found that teachers most often do not com- and regularly visited classrooms. This presence
plete the full CSR model (Boardman et al., 2015, allowed us to use observation and field note data
in press; Hitchcock et al., 2010). This finding to make determinations about how CSR instruc-
may simply reflect expectations that are unreal- tion was delivered and to whom.
istic or too constraining given the demands Another key limitation of this study is that
placed on teachers’ time to teach, plan, collabo- the findings showed no main effect whereas
rate and still accommodate all the federal-, an interaction effect was detected. In the
state-, district-, and school-level testing require- absence of a main effect, more credence is
ments. Though the implications for not complet- typically given to positive subgroup findings
ing the CSR model are not yet clear, it is possible when the estimated full sample effect is posi-
that the lack of significant gains by students tive and statistically significant. When con-
without learning disabilities may be attributed in ducting applied research studies, however,
part to students not regularly applying the full Bloom and Michalopouos (2013) noted that
suite of CSR reading comprehension strategies. often there is interest not only in the overall
In other words, the change in their strategy use average effect of an intervention but also the
may not be robust enough to yield substantial effects for different subgroups of the overall
growth. For example, “asking and answering sample of participants. The authors suggested
questions,” a strategy associated with improved several criteria for determining when an
reading comprehension (Rosenshine, Meister, & effect for a specific subgroup provides strong
Chapman, 1996), is one of the often omitted evidence of an intervention’s effectiveness,
components. including whether a research design is used
Educating students with diverse learning that supports valid inferences, if findings are
needs within general education classrooms is consistent with prior empirical findings and
essential. CSR supports teachers and students existing theory, whether results are statisti-
in inclusive classrooms to use reading strate- cally significant, and if the magnitude in dif-
gies with structured cooperative learning ferences is large enough to be ­important. The
supports that promote student engagement,
­ present study include all of these criteria.
peer interactions, and reading comprehension.
The findings from this study demonstrate that Implications for Practice and for
students with LD in inclusive fourth- and
fifth-grade classrooms benefit from CSR
Future Research
instruction when compared to students with Over the past 10 years, individuals with dis-
LD who do not receive CSR instruction. abilities have made inadequate progress in
reading despite the incremental gains of stu-
dents without disabilities (Vaughn & Wanzek,
Limitations 2014). Although the reasons for these limited
Although we used multiple data sources to gains in reading comprehension for students
determine how CSR was taken up by teachers with disabilities are not clear, it is likely that
and the extent to which students might have with the increasing time students with disabili-
benefited from instruction, there is always the ties spend in general education classrooms

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com by guest on April 10, 2016


16 Exceptional Children

(National Center for Education Statistics, ing disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research
2014), these settings may not be providing & Practice, 21, 211–232. doi:10.1111/j.1540-
enough instruction that specifically benefits 5826.2006.00219.x
students with disabilities. Identifying instruc- Bloom, H. S., & Michalopoulos, C. (2013). When
is the story in the subgroups? Strategies for
tional practices that positively influence all
interpreting and reporting intervention effects
learners is a high priority for improving class-
for subgroups. Prevention Science, 14, 179–
room instruction. Although CSR did not differ- 188. doi:10.1007/s11121-010-0198-x
entially benefit typical learners, it was Boardman, A., Buckley, P., Vaughn, S., Roberts,
associated with the same gains that typical G., Scornavacco, K., & Klingner, J. (in press).
learners get in classrooms not using CSR. The relationship between implementation of
Thus, the differential benefit for students with Collaborative Strategic Reading and student
disabilities in CSR classrooms warrants con- outcomes for adolescents with disabilities.
sideration by teachers as they aim to provide Journal of Learning Disabilities.
beneficial instruction in reading comprehen- Boardman, A., Klingner, J., Buckley, P., Annamma,
sion for all students. S., & Jensen, C. (2015). Collaborative strate-
gic reading in content classes: Results from
There are many implications for future
year 1 of a randomized control trial. Reading
research, including studies that systematically
and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 28,
examine which elements of CSR are most ben- 1257–1283. doi:10.1007/s11145-015-9570-3.
eficial and whether there are elements of CSR Butler, S., Urrutia, K., Buenger, A., & Hunt, M.
that can be enhanced in ways to demonstrate (2010). A review of the current research
greater impact for all students. For example, on comprehension instruction (No.
team-based learning has been examined in ED-08-CO-0123). Washington, DC: National
social studies classes as a means of enhancing Reading Technical Assistance Center, RMC
outcomes and might be a valuable way of Corporation: U.S. Department of Education.
adjusting CSR (Wanzek et al., 2014). Address- Retrieved from http://www2.ed.gov/programs/
ing the influence of teachers’ text selection readingfirst/support/compfinal.pdf.
Coalition for Evidence-Based Policy. (2003).
within reading comprehension strategies
Identifying and implementing educational
instruction could also provide useful informa-
practices supported by rigorous evidence:
tion to educators. Although the students with A user-friendly guide. Washington, DC:
LD in the present study made gains in the gen- U.S. Department of Education, Institute of
eral education classroom using CSR, these Education Sciences, National Center for
gains were still incremental. In our study, CSR Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance.
was implemented up to three times each week Retrieved from https://www2.ed.gov/rschstat/
in approximately 40-min sessions. For students research/pubs/rigorousevid/rigorousevid.pdf
with LD to move closer to meeting grade-level Cortiella, C., & Horowitz, S. H. (2014). The state
expectations, we think it would be interesting of learning disabilities: Facts, trends and
to determine the effects of a supplemental ­emerging issues. New York, NY: National
Center for Learning Disabilities.
intervention paired with CSR for students with
Duke, N., Pearson, D., Strachan, S., & Billman,
disabilities. Similarly, further coordination of
S. (2011). Essential elements of foster-
instruction across settings could offer opportu- ing and teaching reading comprehension.
nities for broader application and transfer of In S. Samuels & A. Farstrup (Eds.), What
reading strategy use. Future research may also research has to say about reading instruction
consider the level of gains that can be realisti- (4th ed., pp. 51–93). Newark, DE: International
cally expected when reading interventions are Reading Association.
provided in general education settings. Edmonds, M. S., Vaughn, S., Wexler, J.,
Reutebuch, C. K., Cable, A., Tackett, K.,
& Wick, J. (2009). A synthesis of reading
References interventions and effects on reading out-
Berry, R. A. W. (2006). Teacher talk during whole- comes for older struggling readers. Review
class lessons: Engagement strategies to support of Educational Research, 79(1), 262–300.
the verbal participation of students with learn- doi:10.3102/0034654308325998

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com by guest on April 10, 2016


Boardman et al. 17

Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cogni- Kendeou, P., van den Broek, P., Helder, A., &
tive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-­ Karlsson, J. (2014). A cognitive view of read-
developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, ing comprehension: Implications for reading
34, 906–911. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.906 difficulties. Learning Disabilities Research
Flynn, L. J., Zheng, X., & Swanson, H. (2012). & Practice, 29(1), 10–16. doi:10.1111/
Instructing struggling older readers: A ldrp.12025
selective meta-analysis of intervention Kim, A., Vaughn, S., Klingner, J. K., Woodruff, A.
research. Learning Disabilities Research & L., Reutebuch, C. K., & Kouzekanani, K. (2006).
Practice, 27, 21–32. doi:10.1111/j.1540- Improving the reading comprehension of middle
5826.2011.00347.x school students with disabilities through com-
Gajria, M., Jitendra, A. K., Sood, S., & Sacks, G. puter-assisted collaborative strategic reading.
(2007). Improving comprehension of exposi- Remedial and Special Education, 27, 235–249.
tory text in students with LD: A research syn- doi:10.1177/07419325060270040401
thesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40(3), Klingner, J. K., Boardman, A. G., Boele, A., &
210–225. doi:10.1177/00222194070400030301 Annamma, S. (2010, April). Implementing col-
Gersten, R., Fuchs, L. S., Williams, J. P., & laborative strategic reading with adolescent
Baker, S. (2001). Teaching reading compre- struggling readers. Presentation at American
hension strategies to students with learning Educational Research Association Annual
disabilities: A review of research. Review Meeting, Denver, CO.
of Educational Research, 71(2), 279–320. Klingner, J. K., & Vaughn, S. (1999). Promoting
doi:10.3102/00346543071002279 reading comprehension, content learning and
Hattie, J., & Timperley, H. (2007). The power of English acquisition through collaborative
feedback. Review of Educational Research, strategic reading. The Reading Teacher, 52,
77(1), 81–112. doi:10.3102/003465430298487 738–747.
Hitchcock, J., Dimino, J., Kurki, A., Wilkins, Klingner, J. K., Vaughn, S., Argüelles, M.
C., & Gersten, R. (2010). The impact of E., Hughes, M. T., & Ahwee, S. (2004).
Collaborative Strategic Reading on the Collaborative strategic reading: “Real world”
Reading Comprehension of Grade 5 Students lessons from classroom teachers. Remedial
in Linguistically Diverse Schools (NCEE and Special Education, 25, 291–302. doi:10.1
2011-4001). Washington, DC: National 177/07419325040250050301
Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Klingner, J. K., Vaughn, S., Boardman, A., &
Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences, Swanson, E. (2012). Now we get it! Boosting
U.S. Department of Education. Retrieved from comprehension with collaborative strategic
http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/edlabs/regions/south- reading. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
west/pdf/REL_20114001.pdf Klingner, J. K., Vaughn, S., & Schumm, J.
Individuals With Disabilities Education Act, 20 (1998). Collaborative strategic reading during
U.S.C. §§ 1400 et seq. (2006 & Supp. V. social studies in heterogeneous fourth-grade
2011). ­classrooms. Elementary School Journal, 99,
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T (1989). 3–22. doi:10.1086/461914
Cooperation and competition: Theory and Lawrence, J. F., & Snow, C. E. (2011). Oral dis-
research. Edina, MN: Interaction Book course and reading. In M. L. Kamil, P. B.
Company. Rosenthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.),
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T (2008). Handbook of reading research: Volume IV
Cooperation in the classroom (8th ed.). Edina, (pp. 320–338). New York, NY: Routledge.
MN: Interaction Book Company. Lemons, C. J., Fuchs, D., Gilbert, J. K., & Fuchs,
Kamil, M. L., Borman, G. D., Dole, J., Kral, L. S. (2014). Evidence-based practices in a
C. C., Salinger, T., & Torgesen, J. (2008). changing world: Considering the counter-
Improving adolescent literacy: Effective class- factual in education research. Educational
room and intervention practices: A practice Researcher, 43, 242–252. doi:10.3102/00131
guide (NCEE#2008-4027). Washington, DC: 89X14539189
National Center for Education Evaluation and MacGinitie, W. H., MacGinitie, R. K., Maria, K.,
Regional Assistance, Institute of Education & Dreyer, L. G. (2000). Gates-MacGinitie
Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. Reading Tests (4th ed.). Itasca, IL: Riverside.
Retrieved from http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/ MacGinitie, W. H., MacGinitie, R. K., Maria, K.,
pdf/practice_guides/adlit_pg_082608.pdf & Dreyer, L. G. (2002). Gates-MacGinitie

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com by guest on April 10, 2016


18 Exceptional Children

Reading Tests technical report: Forms S & T. Rosenshine, B., Meister, C., & Chapman, S.
Itasca, IL: Riverside. (1996). Teaching students to generate ques-
May, H., Gray, A., Sirinides, P., Goldsworthy, tions: A review of the intervention studies.
M. A., Sam, C., Gillespie, J. N., & Tognatta, Review of Educational Research, 66, 181–221.
N., (2015). Year one results from the mul- doi:10.3102/00346543066002181
tisite randomized evaluation of the i3 Scammacca, N., Roberts, G., Vaughn, S., Edmonds,
scale-up of Reading Recovery. American M., Wexler, J., Reutebuch, C. K., & Torgesen,
Educational Research Journal, 52, 547–581. J. K. (2007). Reading interventions for ado-
doi:10.3102/0002831214565788. lescent struggling readers: A meta-analysis
McKeown, M. G., Beck, I. L., & Blake, R. G. with implications for practice. Portsmouth,
(2009). Rethinking reading comprehen- NH: RMC Research Corporation, Center on
sion instruction: A comparison of instruc- Instruction. Retrieved from http://files.eric
tion for strategies and content approaches. .ed.gov/fulltext/ED521837.pdf.
Reading Research Quarterly, 44(3), 218–253. Scammacca, N., Roberts, G., Vaughn, S., &
doi:10.1598/RRQ.44.3.1 Stuebing, K. K. (2015). A meta-analy-
McMaster, K. N., & Fuchs, D. (2002). The effects sis of interventions for struggling read-
of cooperative learning on the academic ers in Grades 4–12: 1980–2011. Journal
achievement of students with learning disabili- of Learning Disabilities, 48, 369–390.
ties: An update of Tatayama-Sniezek’s review. doi:10.1177/0022219413504995
Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, Solis, M., Ciullo, S., Vaughn, S., Pyle, N.,
17, 107–117. doi:10.1111/1540-5826.00037 Hassaram, B., & Leroux, A. (2012). Reading
Murphy, P. K., Wilkinson, I. A., Soter, A. O., comprehension interventions for middle
Hennessey, M. N., & Alexander, J. F. (2009). school students with learning disabilities:
Examining the effects of classroom discussion A synthesis of 30 years of research. Journal
on students’ comprehension of text: A meta- of Learning Disabilities, 45, 327–340.
analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, doi:10.1177/0022219411402691
101(3), 740. doi:10.1037/a0015576 Swanson, E. A. (2008). Observing reading instruc-
National Center for Education Statistics. (2014). tion for students with learning disabilities: A
Children and youth with disabilities. Retrieved synthesis. Learning Disability Quarterly, 3,
from http://nces.ed.gov/programs/coe/indicator_ 115–133.
cgg.asp. Vaughn, S., Gersten, R., & Chard, D. J. (2000).
National Governors Association Center for Best The underlying message in LD intervention
Practices, Council of Chief State School research: Findings from research syntheses.
Officers. (2010). Common Core State Standards. Exceptional Children, 67, 99–114.
Washington, DC: Author. Vaughn, S., Klingner, J., Swanson, E. A., Boardman,
Newman, L. (2006). Facts from NLTS2. Retrieved A. G., Roberts, G., Mohammed, S., & Still-
from www.nlts2.org. Spisak, S. J. (2011). Efficacy of collaborative
O’Donnell, C. L. (2008). Defining, conceptu- strategic reading with middle school students.
alizing, and measuring fidelity of imple- American Educational Research Journal, 48,
mentation and its relation to outcomes in 938–964. doi:10.3102/0002831211410305
K–12 curriculum intervention research. Vaughn, S., Roberts, G., Klingner, J., Swanson,
Review of Educational Research, 78, 33–84. E., Boardman, A., Stillman-Spisak, S. J.,
doi:10.3102/0034654307313793  . . . Leroux, A. (2013). Collaborative strategic
RAND Reading Study Group. (2002). Reading for reading: Findings from experienced imple-
understanding: Toward an R&D program in menters. Journal of Research on Educational
reading comprehension. Santa Monica, CA: Effectiveness, 6, 137–163. doi:10.1080/193457
RAND. 47.2012.741661
Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, T (2002). Hierarchical Vaughn, S., & Wanzek, J. (2014). Intensive inter-
linear models: Applications and data analysis ventions in reading for students with reading
methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE. disabilities: Meaningful impacts. Learning
Reed, D. K., & Vaughn, S. (2012). Comprehension Disabilities Research & Practice, 2, 46–53.
instruction for students with reading dis- doi:10.1111/ldrp.12031
abilities in grades 4 through 12. Learning Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The devel-
Disabilities: A Contemporary Journal, 10(1), opment of higher psychological processes.
17–33. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com by guest on April 10, 2016


Boardman et al. 19

Wanzek, J., Vaughn, S., Kent, S. C., Swanson, What Works Clearinghouse. (2014). What Works
E. A., Roberts, G., Haynes, M., & Solis, M. Clearinghouse procedures and standards
(2014). The effects of team-based learning on handbook (Version 3.0). Retrieved from
social studies knowledge acquisition in high http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/DocumentSum
school. Journal of Research on Educational .aspx?sid=19.
Effectiveness, 7, 183–204. doi:10.1080/19345
747.2013.836765
Wanzek, J., Wexler, J., Vaughn, S., & Ciullo, S.
Authors’ Note
(2010). Reading interventions for struggling This research was supported by grant R305A080608
readers in the upper elementary grades. A synthe- from the Institute of Education Sciences, U.S.
sis of 20 years of research. Reading and Writing, Department of Education. The content is solely the
23, 889–912. doi:10.1007/s11145-009-9179-5 responsibility of the authors and does not necessar-
Watson, S. M. R., Gable, R., Gear, S. B., & ily represent the official views of the U.S. Depart-
Hughes, K. C. (2012). Evidence-based strat- ment of Education.
egies for improving the reading comprehen- Dr. Janette Klingner passed away in March
sion of secondary students: Implications for 2014. The authors would like to acknowledge her
students with learning disabilities. Learning substantial intellectual contribution to the develop-
Disabilities Research & Practice, 27, 79–89. ment and study of Collaborative Strategic Reading
doi:10.1111/j.1540-5826.2012.00353.x and to the project described here.
Wertsch, J. V. (1991). Voices of the mind: A
sociocultural approach to mediated action. Manuscript received April 2015; accepted
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. September 2015.

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com by guest on April 10, 2016


View publication stats

You might also like