Understanding Autism

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

CHILD DEVELOPMENT

Understanding Autism

Jon Matthew Farber, MD, Arnold J. Capute, MD

Although much is known about autism, misconceptions persist. A case report is presented and
used as a framework for dispelling some common myths about the condition. A simple, practical
model for understanding autism is proposed and recommendations are provided for the practitioner
managing a child with autistic-like features.

LUTISM WAS FIRST described by Kanner in but was clumsy. She achieved independence in self-care skills
1943.' The condition has been both fascinating and prior to the time of admission. She spoke in phrases at age
9. At age 11, she received a psychological and educational
confusing to the medical profession and allied disci- evaluation at another university center. She was felt to have
plines. Despite the extensive knowledge that has ac- near-average intelligence but to be functioning at a much
cumulated during the years, many myths persist, and lower level because of emotional disturbance. Evaluation at
improper understanding remains all too common. The a "developmental center" at age 14 was interpreted as show-
ing a "biochemical imbalance" and poor binocularity; no
following case report will highlight some ofthe pitfalls cognitive testing was performed. A diet low in concentrated
in making the diagnosis of autism and in interpreting sweets and eye exercises were prescribed but they did not
it properly. improve her performance.
Early reports commented upon her autistic-like behavior.
Case Report At the time of admission, she still did not like changes in
her environment and would engage in occasional self-stim-
A 16-year-old black girl was admitted for evaluation as to ulatory behavior.
"whether she had a psychosis or a learning disability." Her By report, the mother was schizophrenic. Information
grandparents (legal guardians) and the school had requested about the father was not available.
help in planning her school program. Physical and neurological examination was normal apart
She did not say any words until the age of 30 months. At from mild obesity, poor visual acuity, and mild clumsiness.
4 years of age, she had been diagnosed as schizophrenic at She was evaluated by an interdisciplinary team witb repre-
a university center specializing in developmental disorders. sentatives from pediatrics, psychiatry, psychological testing,
In early adolescence, she had been felt to be schizophrenic speech and language, special education, liehavioral psychol-
by two psychiatrists, one of whom continued to see her ogy, occupational therapy, and social work. Test results in-
monthly. Thioridazine had been tried without success. She cluded a Wechsler Intelligence Scale For Children-Revised
was in a school program for the emotionally disturbed but (WISC-R) with a full-scale IQ of 63, performance IQ of 84,
was mainstreamed for mathematics. and verbal of 52. She could repeat eight digits forward and
Developmental milestones were not known to the grand- six reversed (adult-level performance) but not a sentence
parents. She reportedly had walked at an appropriate time from memory at the 8-year level. On the Wide Range
Achievement Test (WRAT) level II, grade equivalents were:
From the Department of Pediatrics, The John F. Kennedy In- spelling 6.9, mathematics 8.4, and reading single words ("de-
stitute, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Balti- coding") 5.9. Reading comprehension of paragraphs was at
more, Maryland. the pre-primer level; she could answer some questions using
Supported by Project 917, Maternal and Child Health. rote memory.
Correspondence to: Jon Matthew Farber, MD, Department of
Pediatrics, The John F. Kennedy Institute, 207 N. Broadway, Bal- All observers noted that her speech was of a "cocktail"
timore, MD 21205. nature. She had several stock questions but did not engage
Received for publication June 1983, and accepted November in meaningful conversation. She did not notice if she was
1983. given an incongruous answer (e.g., green to the question

CLINICAL PEDIATRICS April 1984 199


FARBER AND CAPUTE

"what color is your mother's hair?"). When presented with ample of the psychodynamic approach is that of Bet-
a complex question, she would engage in humming and other telheim.'* Researchers^'® have been unable to find sig-
"inappropriate" behaviors. When the question was rephrased
more simply, she would attempt to respond, and her behavior nificant parental factors to account for autism, but this
would become appropriate. Answers to many questions were belief persists. The psychological trauma from "blam-
associative, focusing on key words; for example, her response ing" parents for their child's autism is readily appre-
to the question "what's the thing to do when you are cold?" ciated. Abnormal behavior is often equated, erro-
was "to freeze." Her use of language for communication neously, with emotional disturbance, although behav-
was therefore at less than a 3-year level.
A diagnosis was made of pervasive developmental disorder
ior and emotion are two distinct entities. The girl in
(autism), residual type, using DSM-III criteria. Academically, our case did have abnormal behavior, but it was part
her rote skills in spelling and word recognition were judged of her organic condition of autism.
not to be truly useful. Given her severe language and social Myth #2.' Autistic children become schizophrenic adults.
difficulties, it was felt that she would not be able to live There are several features in common between schizo-
independently as an adult, and it was recommended that the
school program be aimed towards future sheltered com- phrenia and autism. Indeed, autism was at one time
munity living. equated with childhood schizophrenia, but they are
Copies of our findings, interpretations, and conclusions not similar. The genetics of the two conditions are
were clearly spelled out and sent to the psychiatrist and the different,' and autistic children usually are retarded
school. After receiving them, the psychiatrist asked whether as adults, not schizophrenic* One ofthe hallmarks of
an inpatient stay for treatment of her "thought disorder"
with an antipsychotic medication would be useful. The school autism is delayed and deviant language,^ as was true
asked whether she should be placed in a college preparatory of the girl in our case. Her conversational skills and
track. comprehension abilities were obviously not in line with
her chronological age, but they were in line with her
other language abilities. Her language cognitive age was
Discussion
markedly depressed. In adult schizophrenia, however,
language capabilities should be at an adult level, with
Myths of Autism
qualitatively (rather than quantitatively) abnormal
In his original paper, Kanner' listed the following thought processes. Failure to recognize this subtlety
as important features of autism: inability to relate in can result in misdiagnosis, and our case had been felt
the ordinary way to people and situations; excellent mistakenly to be schizophrenic.
rote memory; obsessive desire for sameness; good cog- Myth #3: Autistic children have normal cognitive potential.
nitive potential; and intelligent but cold parents. Now Autistic children often are described as not looking
it is known that the last feature is not correct, and retarded.^ This leads observers to feel that there is a
some of the others are not necessary (see below). Rut- core of higher intelligence that is prevented from being
ter^ subsequently described the following criteria for manifested by the autistic behavior. If only the behavior
diagnosis: serious impairment in the development of could be removed, the myth runs, then this core of
social skills; delayed and deviant language develop- intelligence would be exposed. There are some autistic
ment; ritualistic and compulsive behavior; and onset children who are of normal intelligence, but they are
before 30 months. A more recent similar attempt at rare. The majority of autistic children are retarded.
standardized classification is that of DSM-III.* The The behavior is secondary to the cognitive impairment,
patient described here was clearly autistic (actual DSM- not vice-versa.
III diagnosis: infantile autism, residual state). Nev- Many autistic children, like our case, can have iso-
ertheless, this patient has presented both a diagnostic lated islands of good cognitive skills (splinter skills).
and a treatment dilemma. It is instructive to look at Many have good performance capabilities, and the girl
five ofthe most common myths of autism, both as they
in our case has normal mathematics abilities and ex-
relate to this case and to autism in general, to see
cellent rote memory. This does not imply, however,
where the difficulties lie.
that her other cog^nitive abilities should be comparable;
Mylh # / ; Autism is an emotional disorder. K a n n e r ' this would be akin to claiming that someone with a
noted in his first set of patients that their parents were monoplegia should really have good use of the affected
cold, obsessive people, although he did point out that limb because the other three function normally. In-
he felt the condition was innate and organic. Psycho- telligence is not represented by one IQ number in any
dynamic interpretations were very much in favor then, of the developmental disabilities, but this is particularly
so his statements as to etiology were ignored. An ex- true in the autistic population. To say that a 16-year-

200 Vol. 23 No. 4 CLINICAL PEDIATRICS


UNDERSTANDING AUTISM

old with the performance skills of a 13-year-old is of Similarly, deaf children should have normal nonverbal
low-normal intelligence, when her verbal skills are at language, which is not true of autism.
a 3-year level, is to markedly misrepresent the case. Nonverbal language skills are involved in social and
Myth #4: Rote abilities indicate true understanding. Many gestural communication, so that the autistic child has
autistic children, like our case, have good rote mem- abnormal social skills: lack of eye contact, not cuddly
ories. They can thus do well on associative tasks, such as a baby, more interested in objects than people, etc.
as pointing to named pictures. This can act as an in- The impaired verbal skills lead to such features as
flator of other skills. The girl in our case was reported mutism and delayed and/or stereotypic speech. Be-
to "read" at the fifth grade level, because she could cause of delayed language acquisition, these children
pronounce written words at this level. Actually, she often will have pronounced or prolonged echolalia or
was merely decoding, and for her it was like reading pronomial reversal, both phenomena seen to a lesser
a foreign language. Yet, even after receiving our ex- degree in the normal child below 30 months (see be-
plicit reports, the school staff remained confused and low). Performance and memory capabilities are also
still wondered if she had a potential for college. An usually affected, but need not be, so that some autistic
even more striking, yet subtler, example of rote learn- children can have splinter skills. These skills, however,
ing masquerading as understanding was seen in her are not indicative of overall intelligence. As with other
conversation. She had learned a number of standard developmental disabilities {e.g., mental retardation, ce-
questions about age, schools, and so forth, which she rebral palsy), the autistic child does progress, but at
readily asked. However, she could not use the infor- his own rate. As language abilities progress, so does
mation in any meaningful way. She could only mem- social awareness. As a consequence, behavior improves.
orize it for future parroting. Autistic features are "outgrown," but the underlying
Myth #5: Autistic children are untestable. This helps to cognitive impairment remains.
perpetuate the third myth; if you do not adequately
test a child, how can you actually say anything about Recommendations
intelligence? When our case was tested at age 11, she
was given 3 of the 10 WISC-R subtests, as she could Given a better understanding of autism, where does
not obtain scores on the others. Her scores on these this lead? First, autism is to be viewed as an organic
tests were in the average range, and the interpretation developmental disorder, not an emotional condition.
was therefore that she may have average intellectual There is often a great deal invested in considering it
potential. The more important finding was that she to be emotional, because that implies a cure is possible.
could not score on the other 7 subtests, not because However, true psychoses are rarely (indeed, probably
of behavior, but because of ability. If a test geared to never) seen in early childhood. A diagnosis of psychosis
a lower cognitive age had been administered, she would is usually based on findings that are better explained
have been able to answer some questions and receive on a developmental basis. For example, pronomial re-
a score. 'Untestable' almost never means untestable; versal is commonly seen in autistic children and often
it almost always means the examiner did not aim low given a psychodynamic interpretation. In actuality,
enough. pronomial reversal is a common developmental finding,
and when seen in the older autistic child, merely reflects
his developmental language delay. Similarly, the ab-
Model of Autism normal behavior can be explained by retarded non-
Having explored and exploded these myths, what verbal language development, and is not due to dis-
then is autism? Autism is a developmental disorder ordered thought processes as may occur in a cognitively
that includes a language impairment; it is a form of normal child.
communication disorder.'" It differs from other com- Second, there is a need to obtain accurate IQ scores
munication disorders, such as developmental aphasia and interpret them properly. With all their faults, IQ
or congenital deafness, in the totality of the impair- scores are still good predictors of future functioning
ment, which includes both verbal and nonverbal lan- in autistic children.* 'Untestable' or partial results usu-
guage skills. Children with autism and developmental ally indicate that the examiner has chosen a test at too
aphasia differ, as the former have more deviant lan- high a level. Similarly, it is necessary to avoid sole
guage development, more severe comprehension de- reliance on nonverbal IQ measures, such as the Leiter
fects, and defects in the social usage of language.'" or Raven's Progressive Matrices. A child with autism

CLINICAL PEDIATRICS April 1984 201


FARBER AND CAPUTE

can have a normal nonverbal IQ and still have severe through the National Society for Autistic Children
cognitive impairment in his language abilities. The (1234 Massachusetts Avenue, Washington, D.C.).
language abilities are of greater prognostic significance
and must be determined.
Third, rote skills should be viewed properly, as iso- References
lated abilities. Over-reliance on them will result in an
1. Kanner L. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nerv Child
over-estimation of the child's functioning. Reading 1943;2:217.
words is different than reading for information, count- 2. Rutter M. The description and classification of infantile autism.
ing out loud is different than having number concepts, In: Churchill DW, Alpern GD, DeMyer, MK, eds. Infantile
and so forth. autism: proceedings of the Indiana University colloquium.
Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas, 1971:8-29.
Lastly, parents must be educated early and well. 3. American Psychiatric Association. DSM-III: diagnostic and
Autism is misunderstood still, as the present case report statistical manual of mental disorders. 3rd ed. Washington,
DC: APA, 1980.
shows. The "mystique" of autism must be removed, 4. Bettelheim B. The empty fortress. New York: Free Press,
and parents must be given a framework for under- 1967.
standing. Autism can best be viewed as a language- 5. Cox A, Rutter M, Newman S, Bartak L. A comparative study
based cognitive disorder. The family here did not un- of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive
language disorder. III. Parental characteristics. Br J Psy-
derstand their grandchild's condition, and in their chiatr 1975; 126:146.
quest for answers, the girl had been treated needlessly 6. Cantwell DC, Baker L, Rutter M. Family factors. In: Rutter
with eye exercises and dietary therapy. Both of these M, Schopler E, eds. Autism, a reappraisal of concepts and
treatment. New York: Plenum Press, 1978:269-96.
offered false hope and, by not addressing the under- 7. Hanson DR, Gottesman II. The genetics, if any, of childhood
lying language disorder, perpetuated the cycle of mis- schizophrenia. J Aut Ghild Schizo 1976;6:209.
8. DeMyer MK, Barton S, DeMyer WE, et al. Prognosis in autism:
understanding. Numerous unscientific treatment a follow-up study. J Aut Ghild Schizo 1973;3:199.
methods abound and strong claims will be made, but 9. Goleman M. Studies of the autistic syndromes. In: Katzman
there are no cures. At the present time, only educa- R, ed. Gongenital and acquired cognitive disorders. New
York: Raven Press, 1979:265-75.
tional methods (including behavioral modification) 10. Bartak L, Rutter M, Gox A. A comparative study of infantile
have been demonstrated to be effective at improving autism and specific developmental receptive language dis-
performance. Additional information for parents can order: I. The children. Br J Psychiatr 1975; 126:127.
11. Wing L. Autistic children, a guide for parents and professionals.
be obtained from well-reasoned literature," and Seacacus, NJ: Gitadel Press, 1980.

202 Vol. 23 No. 4 CLINICAL PEDIATRICS

You might also like