Martijnetal2020CCFPMeta analysisAIVAEV

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 26

A Meta-analysis Comparing Male

Adolescents Who Have Sexually Offended


Against Intrafamilial Versus Extrafamilial
Victims

Frederica M. Martijn, Elisabeth


J. Leroux, Kelly M. Babchishin & Michael
C. Seto

Clinical Child and Family Psychology


Review

ISSN 1096-4037

Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev


DOI 10.1007/s10567-020-00320-6

1 23
Your article is protected by copyright and
all rights are held exclusively by Springer
Science+Business Media, LLC, part of
Springer Nature. This e-offprint is for personal
use only and shall not be self-archived in
electronic repositories. If you wish to self-
archive your article, please use the accepted
manuscript version for posting on your own
website. You may further deposit the accepted
manuscript version in any repository,
provided it is only made publicly available 12
months after official publication or later and
provided acknowledgement is given to the
original source of publication and a link is
inserted to the published article on Springer's
website. The link must be accompanied by
the following text: "The final publication is
available at link.springer.com”.

1 23
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-020-00320-6

A Meta‑analysis Comparing Male Adolescents Who Have Sexually


Offended Against Intrafamilial Versus Extrafamilial Victims
Frederica M. Martijn1,2   · Elisabeth J. Leroux3 · Kelly M. Babchishin1 · Michael C. Seto1

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2020

Abstract
This meta-analysis examined whether theoretically and clinically relevant differences exist between male adolescents who
have sexually offended against intrafamilial victims (AIV) and male adolescents who have sexually offended against extra-
familial victims (AEV). A total of 26 independent samples (8 published and 18 unpublished) that compared a total of 2169
AIV and 2852 AEV were analyzed. The results of this meta-analysis indicate that categorizing male adolescents who
commit sexual offenses based on their relationship to victims is a meaningful distinction. We found that AIV presented
with greater atypical sexual interests, increased sexual regulation issues, more severe family dysfunction, more extensive
childhood maltreatment histories, and greater internalizing psychopathology than AEV. Conversely, AEV presented with
more indicators of antisociality than AIV, suggesting that extrafamilial sexual offending might fit better with a generalist
explanation of adolescent sexual offending. Findings highlight the value of assessing family dysfunction and maltreatment
history, sexual development and regulation, and general delinquency factors to better understand adolescents who have
committed a sexual offense.

Keywords  Incest · Adolescents · Sex offending · Intrafamilial victims · Extrafamilial victims

Introduction of adolescent sexual offenses committed in North America


are against a relative (i.e., against intrafamilial victims;
Despite making up only 5–10% of the population in Western U.S. Department of Justice 2009; Varma and Leroux 2019).
countries (Eurostat 2019; U.S. Census Bureau 2018), it is Despite this, we know relatively little about how adolescents
estimated that male adolescents between 12 and 17 years who commit sexual offenses against intrafamilial victims
old commit approximately 20% of police reported sexual (AIV) differ from adolescents who commit sexual offenses
offenses (Nationaal Rapporteur Mensenhandel en Sek- against extrafamilial victims (AEV). This meta-analysis was
sueel Geweld tegen Kinderen 2018; Planty et  al. 2013; conducted to examine theoretical and clinical factors dis-
U.S. Department of Justice 2009), often against younger tinguishing intrafamilial and extrafamilial male adolescent
children (Ueda 2017). Moreover, approximately a quarter sexual offending, in order to highlight potential avenues to
improve theory, prevention, assessment, and intervention. As
Electronic supplementary material  The online version of this almost all research about adult and adolescent sexual offend-
article (https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1056​7-020-00320​-6) contains ing has focused on males, this meta-analysis by necessity
supplementary material, which is available to authorized users. also focuses on male adolescents.
* Elisabeth J. Leroux
elisabethleroux@cmail.carleton.ca
Explaining Adolescent Sexual Offending
1
Present Address: University of Ottawa Institute of Mental
Health Research, 1145 Carling Avenue, Ottawa, Theories about sexual offending generally emphasize atypi-
ON K1Z 7K4, Canada
cal sexual interests and antisociality as individual-level
2
Leiden University, Rapenburg 70, 2311 EZ Leiden, explanations for both the onset and persistence of sexual
Zuid‑Holland, The Netherlands
offending (e.g., Finkelhor 1984; Hanson and Morton-
3
Carleton University, 1125 Colonel By Drive, Ottawa, Bourgon 2005; Seto 2019). Atypical sexual interests refer
ON K1S 5B6, Canada

13
Vol.:(0123456789)
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

to paraphilias such as pedophilia (a sexual attraction to Research has just started exploring these hypotheses about
prepubescent children, as defined by the American Psychi- incest (see Pullman et al. 2019; Seto 2018).
atric Association 2013) as well as excessive sexual preoc- While atypical sexual interests and antisociality are
cupations that can motivate sexual offending. Antisociality considered the main risk factors for adolescent sexual
refers to a propensity to engage in antisocial behavior, and is offending, especially when paired with victim access
typically operationalized in reference to specific personality opportunities, these factors may not adequately explain
traits (e.g., impulsivity and callousness), specific attitudes adolescent sexual offending against intrafamilial victims.
and beliefs (e.g., that people can do what they want if they A meta-analysis considering victim relatedness found
have the will), and a willingness to take physical and social that adults who commit sexual offenses against intrafa-
risks. Individuals who are high in atypical sexual interests milial victims generally score lower on measures of atypi-
or antisociality are more likely to sexually offend when they cal sexual interests and antisociality compared to adults
have opportunities to do so, and those who are high in both who commit sexual offenses against extrafamilial victims
are at the greatest risk (Seto 2019). (Seto et al. 2015). A second meta-analysis by Pullman
Adolescents who have committed a sexual offense et al. (2017) found that adults who offended against bio-
(ASOs) are different from adolescents who have commit- logically related victims tended to be similar or lower on
ted a non-sexual offense (ANSOs) because ASOs consist- measures of atypical sexual interests and antisociality than
ently score higher on measures reflecting atypical sexual those who offended against sociolegally related victims
interests and score lower on measures reflecting antisociality (Pullman et al. 2017). However, to date, there is limited
compared to ANSOs (Seto and Lalumière 2010). Research empirical evidence regarding differences between adoles-
considering victim age further distinguishes between ASOs cents who commit intrafamilial versus extrafamilial sexual
who commit a sexual offense against a child and ASOs who offenses.
commit a sexual offense against a peer. In general, ASOs
with child victims score higher on measures reflecting atypi-
cal sexual interests and lower on antisociality than ASOs
with peer victims (Leroux et al. 2016; Ueda 2017). Basis for Adolescent‑Specific Research

While the meta-analyses by Seto et al. (2015) and Pull-


Intrafamilial Sexual Offending man et al. (2017) comparing adults who have committed
intrafamilial versus extrafamilial sexual offenses provide
In addition to victim age, a potentially important distinguish- an empirical starting point to expect a distinction based on
ing characteristic to consider for ASOs is relationship to the victim relatedness to be relevant to ASOs, we must be cau-
victim (Seto 2018). Intrafamilial sexual offending is a puz- tious in extrapolating findings from adults to adolescents
zling phenomenon. Given time spent together and proximity (Letourneau and Miner 2005). First, the relationship to the
in nuclear families, there is plenty of opportunity for incest victim is usually different for adults and adolescents. When
to occur if someone is motivated to engage in such behav- adults commit intrafamilial sexual offenses it is most often
ior (Cyr et al. 2002; Krienert and Walsh 2011). Yet, incest against biological daughters or stepdaughters, whereas
is only reported to occur in a small minority of families, adolescents typically commit intrafamilial sexual offenses
with research suggesting a prevalence rate of approximately against younger siblings (see Seto 2018). Second, research
10% in siblings (Pullman 2018). The likelihood of incest about adolescents must consider the developmental context
is theorized to be constrained by incest taboos and incest within which sexual offending occurs (Seto and Lalumière
avoidance. Incest taboo is a concept rooted in sociologi- 2010). For example, parental and other family-of-origin fac-
cal research and refers to universal cultural rules and norms tors are more proximal factors for adolescents who commit
that discourage sexual behavior among close relatives (Wolf sexual offenses compared to adults (Aquilino 2006; Lindell
2014). Incest avoidance is a related concept rooted in evo- and Campione-Barr 2017), whereas intimate partner rela-
lutionary psychology and biology and refers to the evolved tionship factors, substance use, and access to children are
avoidance within humans and [other] animals of sexual more proximal factors for adults who commit sexual offenses
behavior with close relatives, as doing so would lead to the (Seto 2018). To account for the importance of familial fac-
negative fitness effects of inbreeding depression (Charles- tors for adolescent behavior broadly, familial dysfunction
worth and Willis 2009). For incest to occur, someone has to indicators, including childhood maltreatment history, were
act against both incest taboos and incest avoidance. Trans- prioritized in the current meta-analysis comparing adoles-
gressing these mechanisms can be facilitated either by cents who offend against intrafamilial victims (AIV) with
having weaker taboos or avoidance, or by having motiva- adolescents who offend against extrafamilial victims (AEV).
tions that are strong enough to overpower these inhibitions. Doing so also aligns with many theoretical and clinical

13
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

models regarding the onset of intrafamilial sexual offending, general recidivism is much higher (ranging from 40 to 50%;
most of which implicate family disorganization and child- Caldwell 2011; Rojas and Olver 2019; Viljoen et al. 2012).
hood maltreatment factors (e.g., Rudd and Herzberger 1999; Given the evidence of heterogeneity in terms of antisocial-
Smith and Israel 1987; Thornton et al. 2008). ity within ASOs and given evidence that adults who offend
against extrafamilial victims are more antisocial—on aver-
Indicator Domains age—than those who offend against intrafamilial victims, we
would expect differences between AIV and AEV. This is an
As the basis of our analysis, we drew on theories about intra- important domain to explore given the relationship between
familial sexual offending among adults, empirical evidence antisociality indicators and the likelihood of future sexual
regarding differences between adults who have committed a and general offending (Cottle et al. 2001).
sexual offense against intrafamilial versus extrafamilial vic- Specific offense characteristics (e.g., degree of intrusive-
tims, and empirical evidence regarding global differences ness) were also examined given that, on average, ASOs who
between ASOs and ANSOs. Taken together, these bodies of have offended against males, younger children, and multiple
work informed the five core domains we examined: (1) sexu- children, are more likely to show evidence of atypical sexual
ality; (2) antisociality and offense characteristics; (3) family interests towards prepubescent children (Seto et al. 2003).
functioning and childhood maltreatment; (4) psychopathol- Further, there is some evidence that having multiple victims,
ogy; and (5) psychoeducational and social functioning. and other offense related details (e.g., use of a weapon, under
the influence of substances) predict sexual recidivism among
Sexuality ASOs (McCann amd Lussier 2008).

Indicators related to both atypical and typical sexual inter- Family Functioning and Childhood Maltreatment
ests and behaviors were included because they are both
theoretically and clinically relevant factors. Atypical sex- Indicators that reflect disorganized and/or chaotic family
ual interests are implicated in many etiological theories of contexts were included because they are theoretically rel-
sexual offending (e.g., Finkelhor 1984; Seto 2019; Ward evant from a socio-ecological and multi-systemic perspec-
and Siegert 2002). There is also a relatively robust empiri- tive (Bronfenbrenner 1979). The family system plays a cen-
cal literature regarding the relationship between atypical tral role during the developmental period of adolescence
sexual interests—primarily sexual interest in prepubescent and is likely an important nexus for intrafamilial sexual
children—and sexual offending recidivism among adults offending (Borduin and Schaeffer 2002). Further, there is
(Hanson and Morton-Bourgon 2005; McPhail et al. 2019). a well-established association between family dysfunction
There is also some evidence to suggest that atypical sexual and general delinquency among adolescents (e.g., Murray
interest in prepubescent children is associated with victim and Farrington 2010), and families need to be involved in
type selection (i.e., child-aged victim versus peer-aged vic- evidence-based treatment for ASOs (Letourneau et al. 2009).
tim) and the use of child sexual exploitation material among To date, there has been limited empirical investigation into
ASOs (Leroux et al. 2016; Davis and Knight 2019). The the role of familial factors in the onset or maintenance of
sexuality domain also includes indicators related to sexual intrafamilial or extrafamilial adolescent sexual offending.
preoccupation, sexual regulation problems, and general However, we expect group differences on the premise that
sexual behaviors. intrafamilial sexual offending necessarily occurs within the
family system. It is therefore likely there are unique familial
Antisociality and Offense Characteristics factors associated with this form of sexual offending, com-
pared to extrafamilial sexual offending. As such, the fam-
Indicators that reflect antisociality and specific offense char- ily functioning indicators chosen broadly reflect a nested
acteristics were included because they are theoretically and family-systems approach, and include family-level factors
clinically relevant (Cottle et al. 2001; McCann and Lussier (e.g., sexualized familial environment), parent-level factors
2008). ASOs are, on average, lower risk in terms of anti- (e.g., parental substance use), and individual-level factors
sociality compared to ANSOs (Seto and Lalumière 2010); in a family context (e.g., history of being a victim of sexual
however, there is evidence of heterogeneity within ASOs in abuse, when a majority of sexual abuse is perpetrated by
terms of antisociality as well. For example, the findings from relatives).
Daversa and Knight (2007) regarding ASOs who sexually Childhood maltreatment indicators, specifically those
offend against children suggest a subgroup who are primar- related to childhood sexual abuse, were included as they
ily explained by psychopathic traits and general antisoci- are implicated in several explanations of sexual offending
ality. Further, although the base rate of sexual recidivism (e.g., sexually abused-sexual abuser hypothesis; Seto and
among ASOs is low (ranging from 7 to 11%), the base rate of Lalumière 2010). There is also a large body of empirical

13
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

research that shows being a victim of childhood sexual abuse and Gullone 2006). Poor social functioning is also implicated
is associated with an increased likelihood of committing a in explanations of adolescent sexual offending. Specifically, it is
sexual offense (e.g., Jespersen et al. 2009; Seto and Lalumi- theorized that deficits in social functioning may stunt the forma-
ère 2010). Empirical research also suggests that any form tion of age-appropriate peer and intimate relationships, leading
of childhood maltreatment is associated with an increased adolescents who are unable to form such relationships to seek
likelihood of committing sexually coercive behaviors, even out younger children or non-consensual interactions to fulfill
in twin-control designs (Forsman et al. 2012). these needs (Gunby and Woodhams 2010; Miner et al. 2016).

Psychopathology
Current Study
Indicators reflecting psychopathology were primarily included
because they are clinically relevant to the assessment and Research comparing AIV to AEV is sparse, even though the
treatment of sexual offending. These indicators were also field of adolescent sexual offending has identified the compar-
included because they are relevant to theories of adolescent ison of AIV and AEV as a priority for over 30 years (Becker
sexual offending that suggest this behavior can be the result et al. 1986; Joyal et al. 2016; Latzman et al. 2011; Tidefors
of maladaptive coping to emotional distress (Marshall and et al. 2010; Seto 2018; Worling 1995). As such, we conducted
Marshall 2000). Empirical research shows that ASOs typi- a meta-analysis of published and unpublished data to iden-
cally report significantly more psychopathology compared tify whether AIV and AEV differ on relevant theoretical and
to ANSOs, specifically regarding anxiety and self-esteem clinical factors. We based our hypotheses on theories about
(Seto and Lalumière 2010). Given psychopathology among intrafamilial sexual offending and empirical evidence regard-
adolescents is commonly conceptualized along externalizing ing differences between adults who have committed a sexual
and internalizing dimensions within clinical psychology, the offense against intrafamilial versus extrafamilial victims and
indicators included in the psychopathology domains also fol- ASOs and ANSOs. We thus predicted AIV would differ from
lowed this distinction (Cicchetti and Rogosch 2002; Forns AEV by having fewer indicators of atypical sexual interests
et al. 2018). Externalizing and internalizing problems were and antisociality, and more severe offense characteristics. We
defined in reference to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual also predicted AIV would differ from AEV by presenting with
of Mental Disorders, 5th edition (DSM-5; American Psychi- more problems in terms of family functioning, childhood mal-
atric Association 2013). Relevant indicators included either a treatment, psychopathology, psychoeducational functioning,
formal DSM-5 diagnosis (e.g., conduct disorder, social anxi- and social functioning indicators.
ety disorder), or the presence of specific traits of psychologi-
cal disorders (e.g., anxiety, impulsivity, attention problems).
Methods
Psychoeducational and Social Functioning
Selection of Studies
Indicators reflecting psychoeducational and social function-
ing were included because they are clinically relevant from a PsychINFO, PubMed, Criminal Justice Abstracts, and
socio-ecological and multi-systemic perspective (Brackney Google Scholar were searched for studies comparing ado-
and Karabenick 1995; Bronfenbrenner 1979; Suldo et al. lescents who had sexually offended against intrafamilial
2011). The school system and peers are the two other most victims (AIV) to adolescents who had sexually offended
central systems—outside of the familial system—during the against extrafamilial victims (AEV) using the following
developmental period of adolescence (Smith et al. 2016). key terms: adolescen*, juvenile*, child*, teen*, young adult,
Further, there is some evidence among adults who commit sex* offen*, sex* aggres*, sex* delinquen*. Additional arti-
sexual offenses that those with intrafamilial victims have cles were found by reviewing the reference lists of the ini-
significantly lower IQ scores compared to those with extra- tially identified articles. The initial online search ended on
familial victims (Seto et al. 2015). Differences in psychoedu- January 18, 2018 and was updated on March 11, 2020. The
cational functioning in AIV and AEV may be important, as online search (see Figure S1 in the Supplemental Material)
there is extensive research showing an association between yielded eight published papers1 in which AIV were explicitly
lower IQ and criminality among adults and adolescents (e.g.,
Cottle et al. 2001; Ogilvie et al. 2011).
1
Indicators reflecting social functioning were also included   The data received in 2018 from Dr. Van Vugt were later used in
a paper comparing AIV to AEV that was published in March 2020
because adolescence is a critical period for the development of (Van Vugt and Garofalo 2020). However, the variables reported were
the necessary social skills required to establish and maintain peer defined differently than those provided for the meta-analysis. As such,
friendships and intimate relationships (Aquilino 2006; Heinrich

13
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

compared to AEV on descriptive characteristics. We con- a specific sample). Practice coding was completed using the
sequently contacted 27 authors from the online search who first six samples, and then both coders coded the remaining 20
reported any information on familial relationships in their samples independently and generated consensus ratings after
published papers yet did not explicitly compare ASOs on coding independently. Only variables with effect sizes coded
the basis of having intrafamilial or extrafamilial victims. We in at least three separate samples were included in this meta-
asked authors whether they could provide us with raw data analysis. Some effect sizes were at different levels, for example,
or computed output to calculate effect sizes comparing intra- drug abuse and alcohol abuse both fall under the superordinate
familial and extrafamilial groups. We also contacted 185 variable of substance abuse. When presented in the tables, the
authors from the online search who reported any descrip- weighted effect sizes of the superordinate variables are pre-
tive characteristics of their sample of ASOs, even if they sented, including weighted average N (rounded total value
did not explicitly report familial relationship variables to presented). In the tables, subordinate categories are indicated
ask if they could provide raw data or computed output to with indentations. The Coding Manual including a full list of
calculate effect sizes comparing AIV and AEV. We received the coding schemes and specific variables and included in each
18 unique datasets (containing either raw data or computed domain is available upon request and as Online Supplemental
output), and one dataset that supplemented a published Material.
study. We received the last dataset on June 29, 2018. All
provided datasets have been previously used in publications Inter‑rater Analysis
in peer-reviewed journals reporting other comparisons; that
is, they did not report the AIV versus AEV comparison. Interrater reliability was calculated for 20 out of 26 samples
Overall, this meta-analysis includes data from 26 unique, that were independently coded. We conducted two sets of
non-overlapping samples. inter-rater reliability analyses: one set for the moderator vari-
ables and study descriptors (i.e., at the sample level), and one
Aggregation of Findings set for the dependent variables (i.e., at the effect size level).
The mean kappa value for the 22 categorical study descrip-
To be included in this meta-analysis, the sample had to include tors was M = 0.85, Mdn= 0.92 (range from 0.35 to 1.00).
a group of male adolescents who had sexually offended against The categorical study descriptor that had a kappa of 0.35
an intrafamilial victim and a group of male adolescents who was the descriptor “AEV with some related victims: yes/no/
had sexually offended against an extrafamilial victim and unknown”; however, the overall percent agreement on this
include sufficient information to calculate Cohen’s d effect descriptor was 86.4%. The next lowest kappa was 0.63. The
sizes for the variables of interest. We restricted the meta-anal- mean intraclass correlations (ICC) for 46 continuous study
ysis to male adolescents as almost all research about adolescent descriptors were M = 0.95, Mdn= 1.00 (range from 0.43 to
sexual offending that would be included in this meta-analysis 1.00). The continuous study descriptor that scored 0.43 was
has focused on males. Because the current meta-analysis the percentage of AIV with some unrelated victims. This con-
includes domains derived from male-specific research, we do tinuous descriptor had an ICC of 1 after deleting the largest
not know whether theories or evidence generalize to female discrepancy. The next lowest ICC was 0.65. In short, there
adolescents. This is consistent with the large body of literature was a high level of overall agreement on the coded descriptor
regarding the importance of gender-specific research for female variables.
offending generally (e.g., Steffensmeier and Allan 1996), and The reliability analysis for the dependent variables was
female sexual offending specifically (e.g., Gannon and Cortoni based on 426 effect sizes from 20 out of 26 samples. There
2010). As such, we excluded Willard (2014) completely from was a high level of agreement for all effect sizes coded
this meta-analysis, as it was impossible to disentangle effect across samples. The absolute ICC based on two raters was
sizes for female and male adolescents in this study. Two raw 0.99 (n = 426). A total of 29 out of 455 effect sizes (6.4%)
datasets contained data from female adolescents, which we were calculated by one coder but not the other. After coding
deleted before calculating the effect sizes. independently for inter-rater analyses, disagreements were
Each study was coded by the first and second authors using discussed, and a consensus was reached.
a manual with a list of target variables and explicit coding
rules. Two sets of information were coded for each sample: Analytical Strategy
sample descriptors (i.e., information describing the sample)
and sample effect sizes (i.e., all effect sizes calculated from Effect Size

Footnote 1 (continued) The effect size indicator used was the standardized mean
we chose to include the data that were originally provided as they difference Cohen’s d, defined as follows: d = (M1 − M2)/Sw,
aligned with our Coding Manual. where M1 and M2 were the group means, and Sw was the

13
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

pooled-within SD (Hasselblad and Hedges 1995). For cate- accounted for > 50% of the total variance of Q (see Hanson
gorical variables Cohen’s d was calculated using a 2 × 2 fre- and Bussière 1998). When outliers were identified (for effect
quency table, using the Sánchez-Meca et al. (2003) equation. sizes with at least 3 samples), results with and without the
A positive d indicates that AEV had more characteristics outlier(s) are presented in the tables, and the main interpreta-
that are considered problematic (e.g., antisocial attitudes), tion is focused on the result with the outliers removed.
statistically rare (e.g., learning disorder), or risk relevant
(e.g., family disruption) than AIV. Cohen (1988) suggested Moderator Analyses
a d of 0.20 is considered a small, 0.50 is a moderate, and
0.80 is a large effect. For benchmark reference, the median When there was a reasonable amount of variability in an
absolute magnitude of all main effect sizes of Seto and Lalu- aggregated effect size (I2 > 20%) and sufficient samples
mière (2010)’s meta-analysis comparing ASOs with ANSOs reporting on the moderator of interest (6 for continuous mod-
was d = 0.13 and the 25th, 50th, and 75th percentiles were, erator, 2 for each level of a categorical moderator), moderator
respectively, d = 0.07, d = 0.13, and d = 0.26, with a range of analyses were conducted. Fixed-effect meta-regression was
d = 0.00 (rounded) to 0.67. used to examine the extent to which a continuous modera-
tor variable influenced the magnitude or direction of group
Meta‑analysis differences, whereas between-level Q statistic was used for
a categorical moderator variable (Borenstein et al. 2009).
Meta-analysis is a statistical method to aggregate weighted A statistically significant meta-regression and between-level
findings from multiple samples, where the aggregation of Q-statistic analysis indicates that the moderator explains a
findings reflects a more accurate measure of the true effects significant portion of the variability across samples, above
of a particular variable of interest than individual sam- what would be expected by sampling error alone.
ples. Findings across samples were aggregated using both Based on prior research (Seto et al. 2015; Pullman et al.
fixed-effect and random-effects meta-analysis (Borenstein 2017), the between-study continuous characteristics for
et al. 2009). Fixed-effect meta-analysis produces estimates moderator analysis considered were: proportion of AIV
restricted to the particular set of samples that are included in with both intra- and extrafamilial victims; proportion of
the meta-analysis, whereas random-effects estimate effects AIV with sociolegal victims exclusively; proportion of AIV
for the population from which the sample of studies is drawn with child victims exclusively; proportion of AEV with
from. When variability across samples is low (Q < degrees child victims exclusively; proportion of AIV with male vic-
of freedom), fixed-effect and random-effects meta-analyses tims exclusively; and proportion of AEV with male victims
produce identical results. While both fixed-effect and ran- exclusively. The moderator “proportion of AIV with both
dom-effects results are presented, we focused on the fixed- intra- and extrafamilial victims” could not be meaningfully
effect model for interpretation of the results as all the results analyzed because of the restricted range of the effect sizes
are based on a small number of samples (k < 30) and the (8 out of 10 studies reported that ‘10–20%’ of their sample
between-study variability component necessary for random- had both intra- and extrafamilial victims) and was therefore
effects analyses (i.e., T2, tau) becomes imprecise as the num- excluded from further analysis.
ber of samples decreases (Schulze 2007). A statistically significant continuous moderator means
We used Cochran’s Q and I2 statistics to test the degree that, as the proportion of the moderator increases or
of heterogeneity across samples. Cochran’s Q statistic tests decreases, the magnitude of the difference between AIV and
whether variability across samples in an effect size is larger AEV also increases or decreases. As an intuitive example,
than what would be expected by chance. The I2 value is akin suppose we find the following: (1) the average victim age of
to a measure of effect size variability across samples and AIV is younger than AEV; and (2) this effect is significantly
indicates the variability due to true heterogeneity as opposed moderated in the positive direction (i.e., the moderator has a
to chance. The I2 statistic can be compared across analyses positive Z score) by proportion of child victims in AIV. This
whereas Q cannot when the number of samples (k) varies means that the magnitude of difference between AIV with
across analyses. As a general heuristic, I2 values of 25% higher proportions of child victims and AEV is larger than
are considered low, 50% moderate, and 75% high variability the magnitude of the difference between AIV with lower
(Higgins et al. 2003). proportions of child victims and AEV. Essentially, as the
We assessed for outliers to ensure single samples did not number of child victims across AIV samples increases, AIV
contribute undue weight in the aggregated results. A sam- victims are increasingly younger on average compared to
ple was considered an outlier when the overall variability AEV (i.e., the difference widens).
(Q) was statistically significant (p < 0.05), and if subsequent Finally, we assessed publication bias (i.e., published
analyses with the outlier (i.e., the sample with the most versus unpublished effect sizes) as a categorical modera-
extreme weight and most extreme effect size d) removed tor. Publication bias generally consists of two types: a bias

13
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

toward only publishing studies with significant results (also biological and sociolegal relatedness of intrafamilial victims,
known as the file drawer issue or selection bias) and a bias to with 7 samples reporting 100% biological victims, and 6
selectively report significant results (also known as p-hacking samples reporting between 60 and 99% biological victims
or inflation bias), both leading to an overestimation of sig- (M = 92.2%, Mdn= 100%). Twelve samples further speci-
nificant results. Integrating unpublished results likely gives a fied this relationship: 4 samples reported that victims were
truer reflection of the estimated differences between groups exclusively full/half siblings; 3 samples reported that victims
(Head et al. 2015; Rothstein et al. 2005). It might be that were a mix of full/half siblings and extended family; and 5
publication status influences the magnitude of an effect size: samples reported that victims were a mix of full/half, step/
for example, we might find that published studies report more foster/adopted siblings, and extended family. Eleven studies
extensive maltreatment histories than non-published studies. (42.3%) had information on known/unknown status of the
victims of AEV, reporting that on average 18.8% of AEV
had committed a sexual offense against a stranger victim.
Results Twenty samples (76.9%) reported detailed information
on the victim age of AIV. Averaged across these 20 sam-
Sample Description ples, 80.8% of AIV had a child victim (i.e., 11 years old
or younger), 9.7% had a peer-aged victim (i.e., between
The group size of AIV ranged from 13 to 314 (M = 83.4, 12–17 years old), 2.0% had an adult victim (i.e., 18 years
SD= 75.3, Mdn= 65.5, total N = 2169), and the group size old or older), and 6.7% had victims in multiple age groups.
of AEV ranged from 14 to 438 (M = 109.7, SD= 108.8, Twenty-one studies (80.8%) reported on victim age of AEV.
Mdn = 76.0, total N = 2852). The average age of the total Averaged across these samples, 54.6% of AEV had a child
sample was 15.0 (SD= 1.6), with an average of 14.7 victim, 32.7% had a peer-aged victim, 8.4% had an adult
(SD= 1.6) for AIV, and 15.1 (SD= 1.6) for AEV. Although victim, and 3.7% had victims in multiple age groups. Most of
all datasets have been used in publications in peer-reviewed the samples included detailed information on gender of the
journals, only 16% (n = 135/845) of effect sizes were coded victim (73.1%; k = 19). Averaged across samples a majority
directly from published papers that explicitly reported group had exclusively female victims (AIV 59.2%, AEV 66.4%),
comparisons of AIV and AEV. The remaining 84% of the followed by exclusively male victims (AIV 22.3%, AEV
effect sizes (n = 710/845) were calculated from the raw data 23.5%), and last a mix of both female and male victims (AIV
or outputs provided to us by the authors of the primary stud- 17.9%, AEV 9.7%). More detailed victim information can be
ies. Papers that explicitly reported comparisons of AIV and found in Table S2 of the Supplemental Material.
AEV were published between 1995 and 2017 (Mdn= 2010).
All samples grouped adolescents based on victim infor- Indicator Domains
mation from official arrests, charges, or convictions. Some
samples also grouped adolescents based on substantiated Forest plots that present the individual effect sizes and weighted
victim information from child protection or welfare services effect size can be found in Figures S2–S9 of the Supplemental
(30.8%; k = 8), self-report (3.8%; k = 1), or other information Materials. Statistically significant effect sizes, i.e., effect sizes
(7.7%; k = 2). Most samples came from forensic assessment for which the confidence intervals do not cross zero, will be
clinics or outpatient treatment centers (46.2%; k = 12). Over discussed first, followed by discussion of other effect sizes.
half of both AIV and AEV (61.5%, k = 16) were in some
form of post-adjudication treatment. More detailed informa- Sexuality
tion can be found in Table S1 of the Supplemental Material.
Table 1 presents the comparisons of AIV to AEV on all
Victim Information sexuality indicators, with comparisons based on 3–7 stud-
ies (N ranging from 319 to 1405). Moderate to large group
Most samples (73.1%, k = 19) included information on differences were found for global atypical sexual interests
the proportional composition of intrafamilial and extrafa- (d = − 0.30), and specifically sexual interest with children
milial victims of AIV: 12 samples reported 100% exclu- (d = − 0.22), with AIV having more atypical sexual interests
sively intrafamilial victims, 4 samples reported between compared to AEV. These comparisons are based on self-
80 and 99% exclusively intrafamilial victims, and 3 sam- reported information (k = 4) and physiological measures
ples reported between 68 and 79% exclusively intrafamilial (i.e., penile plethysmography; k = 3). AIV also showed more
victims (M = 93.2%, Mdn = 100%). All samples reported typical pornography use and more problems with sexual
that AEV consisted of exclusively extrafamilial victims, regulation (d = − 0.26 and d = − 0.22) than AEV. The find-
except for one sample with missing information. Half of ings were consistent across samples, with non-significant Q
the samples (k = 13) had information on the proportion of statistics and I2 ranging from 0 to 34%. General sexuality

13
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

Table 1  Comparing adolescents who have committed an offense against intrafamilial victims versus extrafamilial victims on sexuality indicators
Variables Fixed-effect Random-effects Q I2 N (k) Samplesb
d [95% CI] d [95% CI]

Sexuality indicators
 Global atypical sexual interests − 0.30 [− 0.44, -0.16] − 0.28 [− 0.45, − 0.11] 7.85 23.6 948 (7) 10, 11, 14, 18, 19, 21, ­25+
  Sexual interest in children − 0.22 [− 0.42, − 0.01] − 0.22 [− 0.42, − 0.01] 1.59 0.0 478 (5) 14, 18, 19, 21, ­25+
 Atypical sexual attitudes 0.01 [− 0.13, 0.15] 0.01 [− 0.13, 0.15] 4.13 0.0 1245 (7) 1, 2, 10, 11, 16, 22, 25
 Pornography use − 0.26 [− 0.44, − 0.07] − 0.26 [− 0.51, − 0.01] 6.08 34.2 668 (5) 2, 11, 18, 21, 25
 Sexual regulation − 0.15 [− 0.27, − 0.03] − 0.18 [− 0.41, 0.05] 14.87* 59.7 1405 (7) 2, 4, 10, 12, 21, 22, 25
  Outlier removeda − 0.22 [− 0.35, − 0.09] − 0.26 [− 0.45, − 0.07] 7.44 32.8 1239 (6) 4, 10, 12, 21, 22, 25
  Sexual preoccupation − 0.09 [− 0.22, 0.05] − 0.09 [− 0.35, 0.16] 8.79 54.5 1138 (5) 2, 4, 10, 12, 21
 Problematic sexual behavior 0.12 [− 0.09, 0.34] 0.12 [− 0.09, 0.34] 1.24 0.0 598 (4) 10, 11, 21, 25
  Low sexual knowledge 0.22 [− 0.11, 0.54] 0.22 [− 0.11, 0.54] 0.03 0.0 444 (3) 10, 11, 25
 Conventional sexual experiences 0.02 [− 0.15, 0.18] 0.02 [− 0.23, 0.28] 10.81 53.8 1004 (6) 1, 10, 11, 18, 21, 25
  Previous sexual experience 0.04 [− 0.13, 0.22] 0.09 [− 0.19, 0.38] 10.62 52.9 960 (6) 1, 10, 11, 18, 21, 25
  Previous romantic relationship − 0.12 [− 0.45, 0.20] − 0.14 [− 0.51, 0.22] 2.29 12.5 319 (3) 11, 18, 25

A positive d indicates that adolescents with extrafamilial victims had more characteristics that were considered problematic (e.g., antisocial atti-
tudes), statistically rare (e.g., learning disorder), or risk relevant (e.g., family disruption) than adolescents with intrafamilial victims
Bolded values indicate that the group differences were statistically significant, p < 0.05. Indented variables with distinct labels represent subcat-
egories
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001
a
 One outlier removed
b
 The corresponding references to the sample numbers can be found in the reference list. + Studies 10, 11, 21, and 25 based their assessment of
sexual interest on clinical judgment (structured interview or rating scale). Studies 14, 18, and 19 based their assessment of sexual interest on
penile plethysmography

variables indicated more problematic sexual behavior, lower (d = 0.15), more antisocial attitudes (d = 0.23), and higher
sexual knowledge, and fewer conventional sexual experi- rates of general substance abuse, drug abuse, and alcohol
ences for AEV than for AIV, with statistically non-signifi- abuse (d = 0.07, d = 0.07, and d = 0.02, respectively.
cant and small to moderate effect sizes (d = 0.02–0.22).  There was a
small and statistically non-significant difference between AIV
Antisociality and Offense Characteristics and AEV on number of victims (d = − 0.06). By definition,
adolescents with both intrafamilial and extrafamilial victims
Table 2 presents the comparisons between AIV and AEV on were included in the AIV group. It is therefore possible victim
all antisociality indicators, with comparisons based on 4–18 number was influenced by this categorization, as some AIV
samples (N ranging from 434 to 3420). Overall, AEV were would have multiple victims. As such, we conducted a post hoc
more antisocial compared to AIV, with higher scores on gen- analysis restricting the comparison to AIV with exclusively
eral violence risk instruments (d = 0.19) and greater global (100%) intrafamilial victims and AEV. This result was consist-
antisociality (d = 0.09). AEV also had more extensive previ- ent, with a small effect size of d = − 0.02.
ous general, violent, and non-sexual/non-violent (e.g., prop-
erty offense) offense histories than AIV (d = 0.19, d = 0.27, Family Functioning and Childhood Maltreatment
and d = 0.36, respectively). The variability across studies for
any and violent offense histories was significant and moder- Table 3 presents the comparisons of AIV to AEV on fam-
ate, indicating that around 55% of the observed variability is ily functioning, with comparisons based on 3–13 samples
beyond what can be expected by sampling error alone. There (N ranging from 300 to 2693). Overall, AIV came from
was no to small heterogeneity across studies for general vio- more disturbed family environments, with moderate to
lence risk instruments, non-sexual/non-violent offense histo- large effect sizes. AIV reported more overall family dys-
ries (after removing an outlier), and global antisociality. The function (d = − 0.13), a higher prevalence of divorced par-
other, statistically non-significant effect sizes followed the ents (d = − 0.13), more disruptions in parenting presence
same pattern of the antisociality domain. Effect sizes were (d = − 0.28), higher prevalence of foster care placement
small to moderate, with AEV having more delinquent peers (d = − 0.32), and a higher number of siblings (d = − 0.12).

13
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

Table 2  Comparing adolescents who have committed an offense against intrafamilial victims versus extrafamilial victims on antisociality and
offense characteristics indicators
Variables Fixed-effect Random-effects Q I2 N (k) Samplesb
d [95% CI] d [95% CI]

Antisociality indicators
 Sexual violence risk − 0.04 [− 0.16, 0.08] − 0.07 [− 0.25, 0.12] 10.56 43.2 1227 (7) 2, 4, 9, 11, 12, 19, 22
 General violence risk 0.19 [0.09, 0.29] 0.19 [0.09, 0.29] 1.85 0.0 1590 (4) 2, 12, 14, 22
 Any previous history 0.19 [0.11, 0.26] 0.22 [0.09, 0.35] 37.09** 56.9 3413 (17) 1, 3, 4, 5, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, 15,
18, 20, 21, 22, 23, 25, 26
  Any previous sexual − 0.01 [− 0.10, 0.09] 0.03 [− 0.15, 0.20] 25.63** 57.1 2640 (12) 4, 10, 11, 12, 13, 15, 18, 20,
offense 21, 22, 23, 26
  Any previous violent 0.27 [0.14, 0.39] 0.31 [0.10, 0.51] 21.58* 53.7 1904 (11) 5, 9, 10, 11, 13, 15, 18, 21, 23,
offense 25, 26
  Any previous non-sexual/ 0.28 [0.16, 0.40] 0.30 [0.08, 0.53] 29.37*** 69.4 1723 (10) 5, 10, 11, 13, 15, 18, 21, 23,
non-violent offense 25, 26
  Outlier removeda 0.36 [0.24, 0.48] 0.36 [0.24, 0.49] 8.52 6.2 1541 (9) 5, 10, 11, 13, 15, 18, 21, 25,
26
 Global antisociality 0.09 [0.01, 0.17] 0.10 [− 0.001, 0.21] 25.12 32.3 3420 (18) 1, 2, 4, 5, 9, 10, 11, 12, 14, 15,
16, 18, 20, 21, 22, 24, 25, 26
  Delinquent peers 0.15 [− 0.09, 0.38] 0.22 [− 0.17, 0.62] 6.12 51.0 536 (4) 1, 5, 11, 25
  Substance abuse 0.07 [− 0.02, 0.15] 0.08 [− 0.05, 0.21] 18.17 39.5 3028 (12) 1, 2, 10, 11, 12, 14, 15, 18, 20,
21, 25, 26
  Drug abuse 0.07 [− 0.11, 0.25] 0.06 [− 0.15, 0.28] 4.16 27.9 820 (4) 1, 18, 21, 26
  Alcohol abuse 0.02 [− 0.15, 0.20] 0.02 [− 0.15, 0.20] 2.07 0.0 812 (4) 1, 18, 21, 26
  Antisocial attitudes 0.23 [− 0.01, 0.47] 0.23 [− 0.01, 0.47] 2.39 0.0 434 (4) 2, 11, 16, 25
Offense characteristics indicators
 Age at first offense − 0.21 [− 0.30, − 0.13] − 0.24 [− 0.41, − 0.07] 35.91*** 69.4 2387 (12) 2, 5, 8, 11, 12, 14, 17, 18, 19,
20, 23, 24
 Victim age − 0.43 [− 0.50, − 0.36] − 0.58 [− 0.76, − 0.40] 63.04*** 79.4 2803 (14) 6, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 15, 17, 18,
20, 21, 23, 25, 26
  Outlier removeda − 0.55 [− 0.63, − 0.47] − 0.61 [− 0.75, − 0.47] 26.52** 54.7 2784 (13) 6, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 15, 17, 18,
20, 21, 23, 26
 Use of substances during 0.56 [0.22, 0.90] 0.56 [0.22, 0.90] 1.49 0.0 1043 (4) 9, 14, 15, 21
offense
 Degree of intrusiveness − 0.22 [− 0.34, -0.09] − 0.21 [− 0.51, 0.08]43.61*** 79.4 2002 (10) 1, 2, 5, 9, 10, 15, 17, 21, 22,
25
  Outlier removeda − 0.36 [− 0.49, − 0.22] − 0.33 [− 0.53, − 0.14] 14.32 44.1 1846 (9) 1, 2, 5, 9, 10, 15, 17, 21, 25
 Use of force during the 0.03 [− 0.07, 0.13] 0.06 [− 0.09, 0.21] 20.93 42.7 2701 (13) 1, 2, 5, 9, 12, 14, 15, 17, 18,
offense 21, 22, 25, 26
 Number of victims − 0.06 [− 0.14, 0.01] − 0.09 [− 0.22, 0.05] 33.34** 58.0 3053 (15) 2, 4, 5, 9, 11, 12, 13, 15, 17,
19, 21, 22, 23, 25, 26
 Number of victims (AIV − 0.02 [− 0.13, 0.10] − 0.01 [0.15, 0.13] 6.98 14.1 1614 (7) 4, 5, 9, 11, 12, 13, 15
with 100% intrafamilial
victims)

A positive d indicates that adolescents with extrafamilial victims had more characteristics that were considered problematic (e.g., antisocial atti-
tudes), statistically rare (e.g., learning disorder), or risk relevant (e.g., family disruption) than adolescents with intrafamilial victims
Bolded values indicate that the group differences were statistically significant, p < 0.05. Indented variables with distinct labels represent subcat-
egories
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001
a
 One outlier removed
b
 The corresponding references to the sample numbers can be found in the reference list

13
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

Table 3  Comparing adolescents who have committed an offense against intrafamilial victims versus extrafamilial victims on family functioning
and childhood maltreatment indicators
Variables Fixed-effect Random-effects Q I2 N (k) Samplesb
d [95% CI] d [95% CI]

Family functioning indicators


 Family dysfunction − 0.13 [− 0.23, − 0.04] − 0.16 [− 0.32, − 0.01] 20.68 42.0 2693 (13) 2, 5, 6, 7, 9, 10, 12, 15, 18,
21, 23, 24, 25
  Divorce − 0.13 [− 0.25, -0.0005] − 0.16 [− 0.33, 0.01] 10.65 24.9 2007 (9) 6, 9, 10, 12, 18, 21, 23, 24, 25
 Parental disruptions − 0.19 [− 0.31, − 0.08] − 0.21 [− 0.44, 0.03] 30.64*** 73.9 1871 (9) 9, 10, 11, 14, 15, 20, 21, 22,
26
  Outlier removeda − 0.28 [− 0.40, − 0.16] − 0.28 [− 0.45, − 0.12] 11.98 41.6 1525 (8) 9, 10, 11, 14, 20, 21, 22, 26
  Foster care − 0.32 [− 0.51, − 0.12] − 0.32 [− 0.53, − 0.10] 4.54 11.9 796 (5) 10, 11, 20, 21, 26
 Number of siblings − 0.12 [− 0.23, − 0.01] − 0.25 [− 0.47, − 0.02] 13.66* 63.4 1463 (6) 11, 12, 18, 21, 24, 26
 Parental relationship − 0.08 [− 0.32, 0.15] 0.02 [− 0.45, 0.50] 5.56 64.0 300 (3) 4, 21, 12 
 Attachment 0.02 [− 0.17, 0.22] 0.03 [− 0.20, 0.26] 2.64 24.2 656 (3) 10, 18, 21
 Parental substance abuse − 0.07 [− 0.23, 0.09] − 0.07 [− 0.26, 0.13] 11.78 23.6 1347 (10) 6, 9, 10, 11, 18, 20, 21, 23,
25, 26
  Paternal substance abuse − 0.20 [− 0.48, 0.08] − 0.20 [− 0.48, 0.08] 0.08 0.0 306 (3) 9, 21, 26
 Parental criminal involve- 0.12 [− 0.09, 0.32] 0.06 [− 0.27, 0.39] 11.81* 57.7 766 (6) 3, 10, 18, 20, 21, 26
ment
 Parental psychological − 0.14 [− 0.36, 0.07] − 0.14 [− 0.36, 0.07] 4.94 0.0 980 (7) 10, 11, 18, 20, 21, 23, 25
disorder
 Sexualized family environ- − 0.23 [− 0.49, 0.04] − 0.23 [− 0.52, 0.06] 2.24 10.7 491 (3) 1, 11, 25
ment
Childhood maltreatment indicators
 Global child maltreatment − 0.32 [− 0.39, − 0.25] − 0.32 [− 0.39, − 0.25] 17.78 0.0 3474 (21) 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 10, 11,
12, 14, 15, 17, 18, 20, 21,
22, 24, 25, 26
  Sexual abuse − 0.35 [− 0.44, − 0.27] − 0.35 [− 0.44, − 0.27] 14.50 0.0 2843 (20) 1, 2, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 10, 11, 12,
14, 15, 17, 18, 20, 21, 22,
24, 25, 26
  Sexual abuse by a rela- − 0.28 [− 0.47, − 0.09] − 0.28 [− 0.47, − 0.09] 5.50 0.0 840 (7) 2, 10, 17, 18, 20, 21, 26
tive
  Sexual abuse by a parent − 0.57 [− 0.91, − 0.22] − 0.57 [− 0.91, − 0.22] 0.05 0.0 198 (3) 20, 21, 26
  Physical abuse − 0.27 [− 0.35, − 0.18] − 0.27 [− 0.39, − 0.15] 24.60 39.0 3285 (16) 1, 2, 6, 8, 10, 11, 12, 14, 17,
18, 20, 21, 22, 24, 25, 26
  Physical abuse by a − 0.08 [− 0.33, 0.18] − 0.08 [− 0.33, 0.18] 1.28 0.0 367 (3) 2, 18, 20
relative
  Emotional abuse − 0.23 [− 0.38, − 0.08] − 0.23 [− 0.38, − 0.08] 7.00 0.0 1304 (8) 2, 6, 8, 10, 11, 14, 18, 25
  Neglect − 0.21 [− 0.31, − 0.11] − 0.19 [− 0.32, − 0.06] 17.74 32.4 2940 (13) 1, 2, 8, 10, 11, 12, 14, 17, 18,
21, 24, 25, 26
 Exposure to domestic − 0.18 [− 0.31, − 0.05] − 0.20 [− 0.37, − 0.03] 10.54 24.1 1698 (9) 2, 5, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 23, 24
violence

A positive d indicates that adolescents with extrafamilial victims had more characteristics that were considered problematic (e.g., antisocial atti-
tudes), statistically rare (e.g., learning disorder), or risk relevant (e.g., family disruption) than adolescents with intrafamilial victims
Bolded values indicate that the group differences were statistically significant, p < 0.05. Indented variables with distinct labels represent subcat-
egories
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001
a
 One outlier removed
b
 The corresponding references to the sample numbers can be found in the reference list

Variability across these variables was non-significant and were found for paternal substance abuse (d = − 0.20), paren-
small to moderate, except for number of siblings (high and tal psychological disorders (d = − 0.14), and sexualized fam-
significant variability, 63%). Effect sizes in the same pattern ily environments (d = − 0.23). AIV and AEV presented with

13
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

Table 4  Comparing adolescents who have committed an offense against intrafamilial victims versus extrafamilial victims on psychopathology
indicators
Variables Fixed-effect Random-effects Q I2 N (k) Samplesb
d [95% CI] d [95% CI]

Externalizing problems
 Conduct problems − 0.07 [− 0.23, 0.10] 0.01 [− 0.32, 0.34] 21.96** 68.1 1392 (8) 1, 4, 5, 9, 10, 18, 21, 22
 Global externalizing 0.004 [− 0.10, 0.11] − 0.01 [− 0.17, 0.16] 26.26* 50.5 1463 (14) 1, 4, 5, 7, 8, 11, 12, 18, 20, 21, 22,
24, 25, 26
  Disruptive disorder − 0.02 [− 0.16, 0.11] − 0.02 [− 0.22, 0.18] 11.96 41.5 1151 (8) 1, 8, 11, 18, 21, 24, 25, 26
   ADHD − 0.04 [− 0.21, 0.13] − 0.01 [− 0.23, 0.21] 9.30 35.5 1083 (7) 1, 8, 11, 18, 21, 24, 26
   ODD − 0.02 [− 0.40, 0.35] − 0.02 [− 0.40, 0.35] 0.67 0.0 519 (3) 11, 18, 21
  Aggression 0.02 [− 0.12, 0.16] 0.01 [− 0.24, 0.26] 16.41* 57.3 840 (8) 1, 4, 5, 7, 11, 20, 21, 22
   Childhood aggression − 0.15 [− 0.37, 0.08] − 0.15 [− 0.37, 0.08] 1.30 0.0 475 (3) 1, 11, 20
Internalizing problems
 Global internalizing − 0.20 [− 0.29, − 0.11] − 0.21 [− 0.39, − 0.03] 20.00* 55.0 1816 (10) 4, 7, 11, 12, 14, 18, 21, 24, 25, 26
  Outlier removeda − 0.15 [− 0.24, − 0.06] − 0.15 [− 0.24, − 0.06] 5.33 0.0 1677 (9) 4, 7, 11, 12, 14, 18, 21, 24, 25
  Anxiety − 0.09 [− 0.50, 0.33] − 0.27 [− 1.06, 0.53] 9.80* 69.4 472 (4) 4, 21, 25, 26
  Outlier removeda 0.12 [− 0.33, 0.56] 0.05 [− 0.65, 0.75] 4.30 53.5 333 (3) 4, 21, 25
  Depression − 0.004 [− 0.25, 0.24] 0.01 [− 0.30, 0.32] 6.27 36.2 532 (5) 4, 7, 21, 25, 26
  Suicidal behaviors − 0.14 [− 0.27, − 0.02] − 0.14 [− 0.27, -0.02] 0.30 0.0 1496 (4) 12, 14, 18, 25
Clinical presentation
 Self-regulation 0.004 [− 0.12, 0.13] 0.004 [− 0.12, 0.13] 1.14 0.0 1083 (4) 11, 12, 21, 25
 Denial 0.13 [− 0.05, 0.31] 0.11 [− 0.15, 0.37] 5.66 47.0 936 (4) 9, 12, 14, 22
 Empathy − 0.02 [− 0.19, 0.15] − 0.02 [− 0.19, 0.15] 3.03 0.0 940 (6) 9, 10, 16, 21, 22, 25
 Treatment readiness 0.16 [− 0.12, 0.43] 0.16 [− 0.12, 0.43] 0.83 0.0 301 (3) 2, 3, 25

A positive d indicates that adolescents with extrafamilial victims had more characteristics that were considered problematic (e.g., antisocial atti-
tudes), statistically rare (e.g., learning disorder), or risk relevant (e.g., family disruption) than adolescents with intrafamilial victims
Bolded values indicate that the group differences were statistically significant, p < 0.05. Indented variables with distinct labels represent subcat-
egories
ADHD Attention Deficit Hyperactive Disorder, ODD Oppositional Defiant Disorder
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001
a
 One outlier removed
b
 The corresponding references to the sample numbers can be found in the reference list

small group and statistically non-significant differences in Psychopathology


terms of parental relationships, attachment, and parental
substance use (ds ranging from − 0.08 to 0.02). Table 4 presents the comparisons of AIV to AEV on psy-
Table 3 also presents the comparisons of AIV to AEV on chopathology indicators with comparisons based on 3–14
childhood maltreatment histories based on 3–21 samples (N samples (N ranging from 301 to 1816). AIV presented with
ranging from 198 to 3474). AIV consistently reported more more internalizing problems than AEV, specifically global
severe childhood maltreatment histories than AEV, as they internalizing problems (d = − 0.15) and indicators of sui-
experienced more global childhood maltreatment, general cidal behaviors/ideation (d = − 0.14), with little variability
sexual abuse as well as sexual abuse by a relative or par- across studies. Broadly, AIV presented with more conduct
ent, physical and emotional abuse, neglect, and exposure to problems and externalizing behaviors than AEV, but with
domestic violence (ds ranging from moderate to large − 0.18 overall small to moderate and statistically non-significant
to − 0.57). Variability across studies in this domain was non- differences (d = − 0.15 to 0.02). AEV exhibited more denial
significant and small, indicating that childhood maltreatment and less treatment readiness (d = 0.13 and d = 0.16) than
indicators were consistent across samples. AIV.

13
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

Table 5  Comparing adolescents who have committed an offense against intrafamilial victims versus extrafamilial victims on psychoeducational
and social functioning indicators
Variable Fixed-effect Random-effects Q I2 N (k) Samplesb
d [95% CI] d [95% CI]

Psychoeducational functioning indicators


 IQ 0.10 [− 0.001, 0.20] 0.13 [0.002, 0.26] 11.45 30.1 2178 (9) 1, 3, 12, 14, 18, 21, 22, 23, 25
  Verbal IQ − 0.001 [− 0.14, 0.14] − 0.001 [− 0.14, 0.14] 2.50 0.0 800 (4) 12, 21, 22, 25
  Performance IQ 0.04 [− 0.10, 0.19] 0.11 [− 0.20, 0.41] 8.30* 63.9 795 (4) 12, 21, 22, 25
   Outlier removeda − 0.05 [− 0.21, 0.11] − 0.05 [− 0.21, 0.11] 1.65 0.0 639 (3) 12, 21, 25
 Learning disorder 0.03 [− 0.12, 0.18] 0.04 [− 0.13, 0.21] 7.75 9.7 1247 (8) 5, 8, 10, 11, 14, 18, 24, 25
 Alternative school program 0.08 [− 0.04, 0.20] 0.08 [− 0.04, 0.20] 3.00 0.0 1884 (7) 10, 11, 12, 14, 18, 23, 25
 School maladjustment 0.05 [− 0.05, 0.15] 0.08 [− 0.07, 0.23] 19.70* 44.2 1857 (12) 2, 5, 9, 10, 11, 13, 14, 18, 20, 21,
22, 25
   Outlier removeda 0.03 [− 0.07, 0.13] 0.03 [− 0.07, 0.13] 9.58 0.0 1813 (11) 2, 9, 10, 11, 13, 14, 18, 20, 21,
22, 25
  Truancy 0.06 [− 0.12, 0.23] 0.07 [− 0.22, 0.36] 10.30* 61.2 1095 (5) 10, 11, 13, 14, 18
   Outlier removeda 0.15 [− 0.04, 0.34] 0.18 [− 0.09, 0.44] 5.11 41.3 950 (4) 10, 11, 13, 14
 Employment 0.13 [− 0.17, 0.42] 0.14 [− 0.24, 0.52] 3.17 36.9 640 (3) 11, 13, 15
Social functioning indicators
 Global social problems − 0.05 [− 0.18, 0.08] − 0.05 [− 0.18, 0.08] 5.37 0.0 1012 (8) 4, 7, 10, 12, 18, 21, 22, 25
  Social skills − 0.22 [− 0.46, 0.03] − 0.22 [− 0.46, 0.03] 2.28 0.0 548 (4) 10, 12, 18, 25
  Self-esteem 0.11 [− 0.10, 0.32] 0.08 [− 0.21, 0.37] 3.15 36.4 580 (3) 7, 10, 21
 Lonely − 0.04 [− 0.19, 0.12] − 0.07 [− 0.26, 0.13] 8.28 27.5 1082 (7) 1, 7, 10, 11, 16, 18, 25
 Young friends 0.24 [− 0.03, 0.50] − 0.003 [− 0.61, 0.60] 4.48 55.3 609 (3) 1, 21, 25
 Bullying − 0.16 [− 0.48, 0.15] − 0.16 [− 0.48, 0.15] 1.42 0.0 457 (3) 10, 11, 24

A positive d indicates that adolescents with extrafamilial victims had more characteristics that were considered problematic (e.g., antisocial atti-
tudes), statistically rare (e.g., learning disorder), or risk relevant (e.g., family disruption) than adolescents with intrafamilial victims
Bolded values indicate that the group differences were statistically significant, p < 0.05. Indented variables with distinct labels represent subcat-
egories
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001
a
 One outlier removed
b
 The corresponding references to the sample numbers can be found in the reference list

Psychoeducational and Social Functioning Sociolegal Victims of AIV

Table 5 presents the comparisons of psychoeducational and Compared to AEV, AIV samples with more sociolegal vic-
social functioning of AIV to AEV with comparisons based tims had more extensive non-sexual non-violent offense
on 3–12 samples (N ranging from 457 to 2178). None of the histories than AIV samples with fewer sociolegal victims
effect sizes in these domains reached statistical significance. (Z = − 1.49, p = 0.19, k = 6). Reversely, AIV samples with
AIV and AEV presented with mostly small and some moder- higher proportions of sociolegal victims presented with
ate differences on psychoeducational functioning, globally fewer indicators of antisociality and externalizing problems
indicating lower psychoeducational functioning for AEV (ds than AIV samples with lower proportions of sociolegal vic-
ranging from − 0.05 to 0.15). The directions of the effect tims. Larger proportions of AIV with sociolegally related
sizes for group differences on social functioning were small victims exclusively were related to larger effect sizes for
to moderate in size but in inconclusive directions for AIV global antisociality (Z = 1.73, p = 0.001, k = 9), global exter-
and AEV (ds ranging from − 0.22 to 0.24). nalizing (Z = 2.85, p = 0.001, k = 8), and disruptive disor-
ders (Z = 2.70, p = 0.003, k = 6). The effect size “flipped”
Moderator Analyses for externalizing problems (from d = − 0.50 to d = 0.20) and
disruptive disorders (from d = − 0.29 to d = 0.21), indicating
Figure representations of the moderator analyses can be that AIV samples with fewer sociolegal victims had more
found in the Supplemental Materials and Figures S10–S29. externalizing problems and disruptive disorders than AEV;

13
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

AIV samples with more sociolegal victims had fewer exter- samples were also related to fewer global externalizing
nalizing problems and disruptive disorders than AEV. problems (Z = − 3.04, p = 0.017, k = 11), disruptive disorders
(Z = − 2.44, p = 0.039, k = 7), and Attention Deficit/Hyper-
Child Victims of AIV and AEV activity Disorder (ADHD; Z = − 2.26, p = 0.042, k = 6) than
in AEV samples with fewer male victims, when compared
Proportion of child victims in AIV changed the effect size to AIV.
of number of victims from d = − 0.59 to d = 0.22. When
compared to AEV, AIV fewer child victims had a higher Publication Bias
overall number of victims than AIV samples with more child
victims (Z = 2.92, p = 0.030, k = 13). Compared to AEV, AIV Publication bias was assessed using between-level Q sta-
samples with larger proportions of child victims had more tistics to examine the extent to which publication status
externalizing problems than AIV samples with fewer child (published versus unpublished) influenced the direction or
victims (Z = − 2.89, p = 0.047, k = 12). magnitude of the effect size. In order to assess the effect of
When analyzing the effect of proportion of child victims publication, the analysis required at least 2 published and 2
in AEV on conventional sexual experiences, the effect size unpublished studies on any given effect size, leading to 20
changed direction from positive to negative (from d = 0.35 to dependent outcome variables analyzed. Larger effect sizes
d = − 0.55) for conventional sexual experiences (Z = − 2.62, for published samples were present in 15 of the 20 variables
p = 0.048, k = 6). This means that AEV with smaller propor- analyzed (published studies’ ds ranging from − 0.97 to 0.27,
tions of child victims had fewer conventional sexual experi- Mdn= − 0.15; unpublished studies’ ds ranging from − 0.54
ences than AIV. On the other hand, AEV samples with larger to 0.18, Mdn= − 0.01). Publication bias, however, did not
proportions of child victims had more conventional sexual significantly moderate any effect sizes (see Table S3 of the
experiences than AIV. Supplemental Materials).
Compared to AIV, AEV samples with more child
victims also presented as less antisocial (Z = −  3.28,
p = 0.029, k = 16), with fewer global externalizing prob- Discussion
lems (Z = − 3.85, p = 0.011, k = 13), and less aggression
(Z = − 3.18, p = 0.026, k = 8) relative to AEV samples with Summary
fewer child victims. AEV samples with more child victims
also more often had parents with substance abuse problems This meta-analysis found that victim relatedness is an impor-
and more internalizing symptoms than AEV samples with tant distinction in adolescent sexual offending. Adolescents
fewer child victims (Z = 2.29, p = 0.025, k = 9; Z = 1.78, who have committed a sexual offense against an intrafamil-
p = 0.042, k = 8). ial victim (AIV) reported greater atypical sexual interests
and sexual regulation issues, greater family dysfunction,
Male Victims of AIV and AEV more extensive maltreatment histories, and more internal-
izing psychopathology compared to adolescents who have
Male victims in AIV samples moderated the direction of the committed a sexual offense against an extrafamilial victim
effect size of divorce rates, indicating that AIV samples with (AEV). In contrast, AEV tended to commit sexual offenses
higher proportions of male victims had divorce rates more as part of a more general pattern of antisocial behavior. The
similar to AEV (Z = 3.27, p = 0.002, k = 7). The effect of observed differences were generally small to moderate in
male victims in AIV on parental psychological disorder was size and variability across studies ranged from none to large.
inconclusive (Z = 1.89, p = 0.006, k = 7). Compared to AEV, The type of relatedness, age, and gender of victims were
AIV samples with larger proportions of male victims had also moderators of the differences between AIV and AEV
more global externalizing problems than AIV samples with in conventional sexual experiences, antisociality, and other
smaller proportions of male victims (Z = − 2.79, p = 0.029, externalizing behaviors. The key findings and implications
k = 11). are discussed in the next sections.
For AEV samples, male victims moderated effect sizes
of sexual abuse and sexual abuse by a relative: AEV with Sexuality
higher proportions of male victims had more extensive
sexual abuse histories and were more likely to have been The finding that AIV consistently presented with more
sexually abused by relatives than AEV with lower pro- atypical sexual interest indicators than AEV across studies
portions of male victims (from d = − 1.04 to d = − 0.07; is particularly striking, considering they are the opposite
Z = 2.23, p = 0.046, k = 16; and from d = − 0.56 to d = 0.12; of what is found in the adult literature. Specifically, adults
Z = 2.23, p = 0.036, k = 6). More male victims in AEV who commit sexual offenses against intrafamilial victims

13
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

typically score lower on measures of atypical sexual interest sociolegal victims. This finding was unexpected, as these
than adults who commit sexual offenses against extrafamil- results are contrary to previous research that indicates that
ial victims (Seto et al. 2015). Measures of general sexual- adults who offend against sociolegal victims tend to present
ity indicated that AEV had more problems in these areas, with more antisociality than adults who offend against bio-
but the differences were small, extending previous findings logical victims (Pullman et al. 2017). Firstly, it is important
that conventional sexual experiences do not seem to play to qualify that these findings are based on a small number
a differentiating role in adolescent sexual offending (Seto of samples and limited range in the moderator variables.
and Lalumière 2010). As also previously noted by Seto and We view these findings as interesting puzzles that warrant
Lalumière (2010), it is surprising that only 7 out of 26 (27%) further investigation. A possible explanation of why higher
included studies reported on atypical sexual interest vari- proportions of sociolegal victims in AIV was associated
ables. Considering the pattern of findings regarding atypical with less antisocial and externalizing problems than AIV
sexual interests in AIV, and the key role atypical interests with fewer sociolegal victims may be that incest taboo and
plays in adult sexual recidivism (Hanson and Morton-Bour- avoidance effects are weaker for adolescents and their socio-
gon 2005; Seto 2019), it seems that atypical sexual interests, legally related relatives, thus requiring less antisociality to
and likely sexuality more broadly, is an important domain to overcome incest prohibitions and commit a sexual offense.
better understand intrafamilial and extrafamilial adolescent However, this relationship is likely influenced by the age
sexual offending. of becoming stepsiblings and the gender of the stepsib-
lings. An interesting avenue for future research would be
Antisociality to explore how sociolegal and biological relatedness and
timing of family disruption are related differently to risk in
AEV consistently showed more indicators of antisociality adolescents and adults who commit sexual offenses against
than AIV. These results echo results of a previous meta-anal- intrafamilial victims, as well as how these variables influ-
ysis showing the same general pattern for adults (Seto et al. ence differences between AIV and AEV on antisocial and
2015). The finding that antisociality in AEV was negatively externalizing problems.
moderated by proportion of child victims—meaning that
AEV samples with more child victims were less antisocial Offense Characteristics
than AEV samples with fewer child victims when compared
to AIV—lends further support for the hypothesis that ado- The findings that AIV presented with more severe offense
lescents with extrafamilial peer or adult victims are more characteristics than AEV are in line with findings across the
antisocial than adolescents who commit offenses against intrafamilial sexual offending literature (e.g., Carlson et al.
intrafamilial child victims (Leroux et al. 2016). The pattern 2006; Cyr et al. 2002; Krienert and Walsh 2011). The greater
of effect sizes for AEV in the domain of antisociality might severity of offense characteristics also fit with the pattern of
further extend previous research concluding that adolescents more atypical sexual interest indicators of AIV, though this
who have committed a sexual offense (ASOs) are less anti- is not found among adults who offend against intrafamilial
social than adolescents who have committed a non-sexual victims, who also have more severe offense characteristics
offense (ANSOs; Seto and Lalumière 2010). The results of yet are less likely to show atypical sexual interests (Seto
this meta-analysis suggest that merging AIV and AEV may et al. 2015). On the other hand, more severe offense char-
obscure important differences in antisociality among ASOs. acteristics may also be explained by opportunity, with AIV
This meta-analysis, however, only tells us about the relative having longer and more frequent access to younger victims
difference between AIV and AEV, not about the absolute as a result of committing an offense within their family and
prevalence of problems or risk factors within these groups. family homes (Leclerc et al. 2015). Substance abuse during
In this meta-analysis, AEV were more antisocial than AIV; the offense showed a reverse relationship compared to the
this does not tell us how antisocial AIV or AEV are com- rest of the domain, with AEV presenting more instances of
pared to adolescents with no criminal record or with a gen- substance abuse than AIV. However, substance use during
eral offense history. the commission of an offense is a well-established corre-
late of general delinquency (Felson et al. 2008; White et al.
Moderating Effects of Biological Versus Sociolegal 2002), and the group finding is thus consistent with the find-
Relationship ing that AEV are globally more antisocial than AIV. The
observed differences within this domain, however, showed
As compared to AEV, AIV samples with a higher propor- moderate to high variability across studies, indicating that
tion of sociolegal victims presented with fewer problems other factors play a crucial role in the differences between
on global antisociality, global externalizing, and disrup- AIV and AEV in some of the offense characteristics. For
tive disorders than AIV samples with lower proportions of example, hypothetically the variability observed in number

13
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

of victims could be accounted for by the specific operation- within the family unit. Future research should prioritize
alization of intrafamilial victims in some studies. Studies these factors.
who defined their intrafamilial group as having “any intra- Family dynamics and relationships are promising theo-
familial victim” may have a higher average of number of retical risk factors for intrafamilial adolescent sexual offend-
victims since adolescents with both intra- and extrafamilial ing. Victims of sibling sexual offending report strained and
victims would have been included in that group. However, unequal relationships between them and their siblings, and
the overall difference in number of victims between AIV and general negative family dynamics with absent and unsup-
AEV were small (d = − 0.06) and remained small when the portive parents, creating environments where the offending
comparison to AEV was limited to AIV with 100% intrafa- could take place, go unnoticed, or be ignored (Laviola 1992;
milial victims (d = − 0.02). Katz and Hamama 2017; McDonald and Martinez 2017;
Monahan 2010). These relationships are likely negatively
The Family Context influenced by the sexual offense, and due to the retrospec-
tive assessment, the temporal association of relationship
AIV consistently presented with more dysfunctional family quality to the onset of sexual offending behavior is hard to
environments than AEV. These disorganized family struc- determine. Nonetheless, family treatment studies report an
tures might reflect a lack of parental oversight and stability, increase in positive relationships between the person who
creating environments where intrafamilial sexual offending has committed an intrafamilial sexual offense, the victim,
is more likely to take place (Seto 2019; Leclerc et al. 2015). and other family members (Bass et al. 2006; Halse et al.
It is likely these indicators precede the sexual offense and 2012; Haskins 2003; Thornton et al. 2008). Longitudinal
could therefore potentially play a causal role in the onset of research is sorely needed to further test the role of par-
intrafamilial sexual offending. Sexualized family environ- ent–child and sibling–sibling relationships in theories and
ments are also hypothesized to play a key role in the onset, clinical interventions addressing intrafamilial adolescent
and possible maintenance, of intrafamilial adolescent sexual sexual offending.
offending (Smith and Israel 1987). In the current meta-anal-
ysis only three studies, comprising 491 adolescents, reported The Role of Maltreatment
on this variable. However, this factor has promise in distin-
guishing AIV from AEV, as the magnitude of this effect size AIV reported more extensive maltreatment histories than
is considered large relative to our benchmark (d = − 0.23, AEV, consistent with findings in the adult literature (Seto
95% CI [− 0.49, 0.04]). et al. 2015). Given that child maltreatment is often com-
Family maltreatment is among the most commonly mitted by parents or other caregivers (Finkelhor et  al.
reported indicators of family dysfunction. This is unsurpris- 2014; Radford et al. 2013), these findings are consistent
ing given childhood maltreatment is thought to play a key with the concept of intergenerational continuity of mal-
role in the onset of intrafamilial sexual offending, as well as treatment (Schofield et al. 2013; Thornberry and Henry
adolescent offending generally. To illustrate, all eight pub- 2013). The notably large effect sizes for sexual abuse
lished studies and thirteen unpublished studies reported on histories (d = − 0.35)—especially parental sexual abuse
family maltreatment. Regrettably, information about other, (d = − 0.57)—as more severe for AIV suggest that a history
more general indicators of family dysfunction was mostly of sexual abuse may be especially pertinent to the onset of
superficial and unstandardized. For instance, only three stud- intrafamilial adolescent sexual offending. These findings
ies, representing 300 adolescents, reported on the quality lend further support for the association between sexual abuse
of parental relationships. Other hypothetically important history and perpetration of sexual offending, also known as
variables, such as parenting styles, parental supervision, or the sexually abused-sexual abuser hypothesis (Burton 2003;
maternal and paternal attachment and relationships, had few Jespersen et al. 2009; Seto and Lalumière 2010; Seto et al.
studies and effect sizes indicated inconsistent directions. 2015; Ward et al. 2006). While sexual abuse histories may
Surprisingly, none of the studies reported on sibling–sib- not predict sexual recidivism in adults (Hanson and Morton-
ling relationships. This is understandable as it is difficult Bourgon 2005), there is tentative evidence that sexual abuse
to infer the change in sibling–sibling relationships pre-to histories may be a risk factor for adolescent sexual recidi-
post-offense, especially if the intrafamilial sexual offending vism (Mallie et al. 2011), again indicating differential devel-
was perpetrated against a sibling. Nevertheless, understand- opmental contexts of risk factors for adults and adolescents.
ing sibling–dynamics and relationships may be especially There may also be a specific association between sexual
important in explaining the onset, continuation, and ces- abuse histories and atypical sexual interests in adolescents.
sation of intrafamilial sexual offending, particularly if the Previous research has indicated that ASOs with sexual abuse
offending was directed at one (or more) specific siblings histories show bigger erectile responses to sexual stimuli
pertaining children or coercion (Becker et al. 1992; Murphy

13
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

et al. 2001). Among adults who commit sexual offenses, for AIV, however, are puzzling and indicate further research
childhood sexual abuse is related to greater paraphilic inter- is needed to understand how externalizing behavior and vic-
ests, more paraphilic experiences, and more extensive sexual tim age, gender, and relatedness might intersect.
life histories (Pedneault et al. 2019). Our results are also
consistent with hypotheses that physical and sexual abuse Psychoeducational and Social Functioning
can lead to negative affect, which in turn leads to maladap-
tive coping through sexual behavior, for instance, in the The magnitude of effect size differences between AIV and
form of sexual preoccupation and atypical sexual fantasies AEV on psychoeducational functioning were small over-
(Cortoni and Marshall 2001; Knight and Sims-Knight 2004; all. The effect sizes in the social functioning domain were
Maniglio 2011). small to moderate and in inconsistent directions. Moderator
analysis indicated that, when compared to AIV, AEV with
lower proportions of exclusive child victims (i.e., a higher
Psychopathology proportion of peer/adult victims) had fewer conventional
sexual experiences than AEV with higher proportions of
AIV presented with more psychopathology overall com- exclusive child victims. This might indicate that AEV with
pared than AEV. It should, however, be noted that informa- peer victims who lack the skills to acquire consensual sexual
tion on the timing of psychopathological assessments was experiences—further supported by the less extensive con-
lacking. It is likely that these assessments were performed ventional sexual histories of AEV—sexually offend to fulfill
after the adolescent was identified as having committed a these peer-related sexual and romantic needs.
sexual offense. These assessments, especially concerning These findings are somewhat counter-intuitive as it could
internalizing problems, may therefore be influenced by the also be expected that male adolescents with lower social
consequences of being identified as a person who has com- skills will be more likely to offend against children because
mitted a sexual offense, such as experiencing depression or peer relationships may seem intimidating to initiate and/
anxiety due to the stigma associated with sexual offending or difficult to maintain. Although this explanation would
(Jeglic et al. 2012; Leroux 2019; Tewksbury 2012). This align with the heterosocial skills deficit hypothesis of ado-
would be applicable to both AIV and AEV, but the stigma lescent sexual offending (Finkelhor 1984), the proportion
may be worse for AIV considering the taboos associated of child victims in AEV did not moderate the social skills
with incest as well as offending against young children effects, and AIV had overall poorer social skills than AEV
(Paine and Hansen 2002). (d = − 0.22). Notwithstanding small differences between
Inconsistent with the pattern observed in the antisociality AIV and AEV, interventions within the psychoeducational
domain, AIV presented with more externalizing problems, and social domains are well supported to decrease recidi-
albeit with small and non-significant effect sizes. This find- vism in adolescents adjudicated for sexual offenses (Van den
ing is inconsistent because antisociality and externalizing Berg et al. 2017; Yoder et al. 2016).
problems (especially conduct disorder) are positively corre-
lated (Vess and Skelton 2010). Consistent with the pattern in
the antisociality domain, AEV presented with greater denial Clinical Implications
and less treatment readiness, which may more likely be a
reflection of antisociality rather than psychopathology. This The results of this meta-analysis indicate that AIV and AEV,
is important to consider from a clinical perspective, because on average, are different in some ways that need to be con-
it may indicate that AEV may be more difficult to engage in sidered in explanations of intrafamilial and extrafamilial
the treatment process. adolescent sexual offending. While we must base decisions
about treatment and interventions on individual assessments,
Moderating Effect of Age and Gender of Victim it is expected that AIV and AEV require a differentiated
treatment approach. The results of this study also emphasize
Having a larger proportion of child or male victims was that we should not extrapolate findings from adults to ado-
associated with more externalizing problems among AIV, lescents who have sexually offended. In this meta-analysis,
but fewer externalizing problems among AEV. The modera- we find that AIV are consistently more likely to show atypi-
tor results—limited by restricted range of the moderator and cal sexual interests compared to AEV, whereas the reverse
number of samples included—for AEV seem to mimic the pattern is found for adults (Seto et al. 2015). It would be a
adult literature, wherein adults who have committed sexual mistake if treatment programs assume that atypical sexual
offenses against child victims tend to present with fewer interests do not warrant attention in AIV. The results of the
externalizing traits than adults who commit sexual offenses study also suggest that AEV are more likely to commit sex-
against adult victims (Vess and Skelton 2010). The results ual offenses within a pattern of general delinquent behavior

13
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

and, thus, may benefit more than AIV from a validated treat- maltreatment could be at increased risk of intrafamilial sex-
ment approach targeting general criminogenic needs. ual offending. Similarly, sexualized family norms, sexual
The results highlight the important role that family dys- dysregulation, and increased pornography use should also
function plays in adolescent sexual offending, especially for be attended to, especially when combined with evidence of
AIV. It is clear that more research is needed to understand maltreatment and family dysfunction. This meta-analysis
what role family dysfunction plays, and how to best inter- can raise the awareness of practitioners to commonly clus-
vene to prevent sexual offending. Systemic approaches are tered factors that are associated with intrafamilial adolescent
shown to be effective for a broad range of adolescent delin- sexual offending.
quent behaviors (Van der Stouwe et al. 2014), but might be
especially pertinent for adolescent sexual offending (Dopp
et al. 2017; Letourneau et al. 2013; Seto and Lalumière Limitations
2010). Systemic treatment with families in which adoles-
cent sexual offending and abuse has taken place may not be This meta-analysis is based on studies that used official
a simple endeavor. For example, housing and the placement records of criminal charges and/or convictions. The “dark
of the adolescent in- or outside of the family home after the number” of undetected sexual offenses committed by adoles-
sexual offense is a complex decision (Grant et al. 2006). cents is thought to be high, as most sexual offenses are not
The threat of out-of-home placement might impact willing- officially reported (Daly and Bouhours 2010; Planty et al.
ness of victims to disclose the sexual abuse if the victim is 2013). The dark number for intrafamilial adolescent sexual
scared that it would break up the family (Seto 2018). Fur- offenses may be even higher, as taboo, stigma, and denial
ther, while the disruption of parenting seems not to be in of incest is widespread (Ginzburg et al. 2006; Paine and
the best interest of adolescents in terms of mental health, Hansen 2002). Complex and possibly differential selection
attachment needs, and risk reduction (Thornton et al. 2008), effects may influence whether AIV and AEV are reported to
the needs of the victim and other family members must also the justice system and are consequently included in foren-
be considered. sic and clinical samples. This complicates a straightforward
The results of this meta-analysis could aid practitioners comparison, as risk domains may be influenced by these
in identifying factors that predate the onset of sexual offend- selection effects.
ing, thereby contributing to prevention efforts. Some of the For example, it is possible that AIV have a longer dura-
differences identified in this meta-analysis likely predate the tion of offending because their victims are too young to dis-
onset of sexual offending, such as atypical sexual interests close sexual abuse or family dysfunction reduces reporting.
in children (Seto 2019) or family histories of divorce, foster Alternatively, it might be that AEV have less opportunity
care, or parental problems such as substance abuse or crim- and/or access to younger victims to commit intrusive sexual
inal involvement. Other differences, however, may follow offenses, even though they might be similar to AIV in terms
the onset of sexual offending or may be conflated with pre- of their motivation to offend against young children. It is
existing differences, particularly psychopathology and fam- also reasonable to assume that perpetration by a stranger
ily conflict (Seto and Lalumière 2010; Seto et al. 2015. As (which is very unlikely for intrafamilial sexual offenses)
we have suggested in our previous work, an important direc- contributes to whether a sexual offense is more likely to be
tion for research to understand both adolescent and adult reported (Finkelhor and Ormrod 1999).
sexual offending—and a necessary condition for suggesting These potential selection effects are a limitation of the
causal influence—is a better sense of temporal precedence. literature as well as of this meta-analysis. We had too few
Given the often-hidden nature of sexual offending in studies to be able to perform analyses to investigate all these
general, especially intrafamilial sexual offending, mental possible moderation effects. Future studies should examine
health, child protection, and school professionals must rely if, for example, observed differences between AIV and AEV
on behavioral flags to initiate prevention and/or intervention in intrusiveness are explained by differences in duration of
responses. Most professionals working with male adoles- sexual offending, where offenses against a related victim can
cents will come across some who are at risk of committing span months or even years, as opposed to something intrin-
sexual offenses, some who are at risk of becoming victims sically different in intrafamilial versus extrafamilial offend-
of sexual offending within their families, or some who are ing motivations. Future studies could also aim to include
at risk of becoming both victims and perpetrators of sexual detailed indicators of coercion (e.g., victim injury and dura-
abuse. The results of this meta-analysis give information tion), information on the source of disclosure to the justice
about possible avenues to develop primary and secondary system, and information on access and supervision. This
interventions and screening tools within families at risk would enable investigation into the contextual risk factors
for both sexual offending and victimization. For instance, related to reporting intra- and extrafamilial sexual offense.
families with high levels of dysfunction and poly-form

13
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

A limitation of every meta-analysis is that analyses are offenses, except for two studies that grouped based on index
dependent on the variables included in the primary stud- offenses only. This may have misclassified some AIV and
ies, and how those variables were reported. Constructs were AEV because they had disparate sexual offense histories.
operationalized inconsistently across studies. For example, Paying attention to adolescents who have committed a
sexual interest in children was assessed in three studies with sexual offense against both intrafamilial and extrafamilial
penile plethysmography, while four other studies used self- victims might be important as several studies indicate that
report or clinical opinion. Many psychological and behav- persons who commit sexual offenses against polymorphic
ioral variables were dependent on clinical judgment, which victim types may differ from persons who commit sexual
could have influenced their reliability and consistent opera- offenses against a single victim type. There is some evidence
tionalization. Most variables were recorded retrospectively, that adult men who commit polymorphic sexual offenses are
and as such, might have be prone to bias: for instance, child- more likely to have atypical sexual interests (e.g., Cann et al.
hood sexual abuse can be both under- and over-reported in 2007; Stephens et al. 2018). It is possible the observed dif-
sexual offending research (Baldwin et al. 2019; Jespersen ference in atypical sexual interests between AIV and AEV in
et al. 2009). We had insufficient data to examine how method the current meta-analysis is affected by the presence of AIV
type (self-report, clinical judgement, risk assessments) mod- with multiple, polymorphic victims. Unfortunately, it was
erated effect sizes. not possible to directly test this, as the moderator “intrafa-
Most notable was the inconsistent definition of intrafa- milial and extrafamilial victims” could not be meaningfully
milial adolescent sexual offending across studies. Only half analyzed due to the limited range of this moderator (8 out of
of the studies reported detailed information on victim relat- 10 studies reporting on this moderator only had 10–20% of
edness. Of those, some studies included only biologically their sample with both intra- and extrafamilial victims). We
related victims (full/half siblings), while others included could also not perform post-hoc analyses of the influence of
sociolegally related victims (step/foster/adopted siblings). monomorphic victim type (i.e., exclusively intrafamilial vic-
Some studies included only first degree related, minor-aged tims) on atypical sexual interest variables by analyzing AIV
victims, while others also included second or third degree samples with 100% intrafamilial victims. This was because
related, adult-aged victims. For this study, we chose to none of the sexuality variables had sufficient AIV groups
include all types of relatives as representing intrafamilial consisting of 100% intrafamilial victims to meta-analyze,
relationships to increase power—and because were limited as we only meta-analyzed effect sizes of which at least three
in how data were presented in the published samples or in samples reported information. As such, an important con-
the database/output provided to us. We expect that consid- sideration in future research on victim type polymorphism
erable contributions to the understanding of intrafamilial is intrafamilial and/or extrafamilial relatedness.
adolescent sexual offending could be made by examining The theoretical foundations of this meta-analysis were
the extent to which relatedness—especially biological and based on male-specific research. Considering only one
sociolegal relatedness—is related to risk factors and out- published manuscript reported on female AIV and AEV
comes for AIV, such as recidivism, management, and treat- (Willard 2014), we decided to omit female ASOs from the
ment needs. analyses, as we could not conduct meaningful comparisons.
Four out of eight published samples included adolescents The findings of this study should not be extrapolated to
who had committed a sexual offense against both intrafa- female AIV and AEV. There is evidence that risk factors
milial and extrafamilial victims as AIV in their sample. In for general and sexual offending and recidivism rates are
order to maximize includability of studies, we decided to different for adult men and women, influenced by, for exam-
use this definition for AIV going forward when coding and ple, relationships, antisocial attitudes, and societal response
analyzing the unpublished samples. When we received raw (Gannon and Cortoni 2010). This might also be true for
data, we coded AIV and AEV such that any of the adoles- adolescent girls and boys, although the lack of research on
cents in the dataset who had committed a sexual offense women, and more particularly, adolescent girls who com-
against both an intra- and extrafamilial victim was coded as mit sexual offenses is still severely lacking when compared
AIV. When authors provided us with output, we requested to to male-specific research. As such, there is a considerable
adhere to this definition of AIV as well (as also provided in need for more primary studies about female ASOs, espe-
our Coding Manual, available in the Supplemental Materi- cially female adolescents who have committed intrafamilial
als). This means the results of our meta-analysis should be sexual offenses. Anonymized surveys may be highly suitable
understood as a quantitative review of studies that compare for such research, given self-reported victimization data sug-
adolescents who have committed any intrafamilial sexual gest that, while girls do sexually offend, they are very rarely
offenses with adolescents who have committed extrafamilial seen by the justice system or clinics (Stemple et al. 2017). It
sexual offenses exclusively. Almost all samples indicated should also be noted that all the studies in this meta-analysis
they grouped the adolescents based on a complete history of

13
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

are from Anglophone and Western European countries and Compliance with Ethical Standards 
might not generalize to other countries and cultures.
Lastly, it is striking how few published studies have Conflict of interest  The authors declare there is no conflict of interest
reported on the comparison of AIV and AEV. We were able and this work was not supported by a funding agency.
to locate only 9 published studies in over 6000 screened Ethical Approval  The study was not required to undergo ethical review.
studies. When contacting authors from our search, it became
clear that victim relatedness is often recorded but is not
reported in published research. It is conceivable that more
datasets have victim relatedness recorded, which we were References
not able to find and include in this meta-analysis. Also,
many variables thought to be theoretically important did
not have enough data to meta-analyze, or had the minimum References marked with an asterisk indicate
amount required. While meta-analyses are a powerful tool samples included in the meta-analysis. The study
to explore broad relations and propose relevant domains for numbers in brackets correspond with the study
future research, primary studies are needed to more precisely numbers in the tables. Numbers with a decimal
and efficiently explore how different variables intersect with indicate unpublished datasets.
factors associated with adolescent sexual offending.
*Aebi, M. (2019). Offender types and criminality dimensions in male
juveniles convicted of sexual offenses. Unpublished dataset
[Study no. 9.1].
Conclusion *Aebi, M., Vogt, G., Plattner, B., Steinhausen, H. C., & Bessler, C.
(2012). Offender types and criminality dimensions in male juve-
niles convicted of sexual offenses. Sexual Abuse, 24, 265–288.
The field of research regarding adult sexual offending is https​://doi.org/10.1177/10790​63211​42044​9 [Study no. 9].
increasingly aware of the importance of victim relatedness *Almond, L. (2019). Youths who sexually harm: A multivariate model
in terms of theory, risk assessment, and treatment. Research of characteristics. Unpublished dataset [Study no. 10.1].
*Almond, L., Canter, D., & Salfati, G. C. (2006). Youths who sexu-
into adolescent sexual offending should also move forward
ally harm: A multivariate model of characteristics. Journal of
to include victim relatedness, as the findings of this meta- Sexual Aggression, 12, 97–114. https​://doi.org/10.1080/13552​
analysis show it is a relevant theoretical and clinical consid- 60060​08236​05 [Study no. 10].
eration for this population. A strength of this meta-analysis American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical
manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Ameri-
is the inclusion of unpublished data, adding to the confi-
can Psychiatric Association.
dence of the findings. The key findings of this meta-analysis Aquilino, W. S. (2006). Family relationships and support systems
are that AIV show a pattern of more pronounced atypical in emerging adulthood. In J. J. Arnett & J. L. Tanner (Eds.),
sexual interests and family dysfunction, whereas AEV show Emerging adults in America: Coming of age in the 21st cen-
tury (pp. 193–217). Washington, DC: American Psychological
a pattern of more pronounced general antisocial behaviors
Association.
and attitudes. The main implication of this is that offense- Baldwin, J. R., Reuben, A., Newbury, J. B., & Danese, A. (2019).
specific and general antisocial factors must be considered Agreement between prospective and retrospective measures of
when developing comprehensive theories of adolescent sex- childhood maltreatment: A systematic review and meta-analysis.
JAMA Psychiatry, 76, 584–593. https​://doi.org/10.1001/jamap​
ual offending. These results also indicate that familial factors
sychi​atry.2019.0097.
are crucial to include in research about adolescent sexual Bass, L. B., Taylor, B. A., Knudson-Martin, C., & Huenergardt, D.
offending. After all, the central distinguishing characteristic (2006). Making sense of abuse: Case studies in sibling incest.
between AIV and AEV is whether the sexual offense was Contemporary Family Therapy, 28, 87–109. https ​ : //doi.
org/10.1007/s1059​1-006-9697-0.
committed against someone within or outside of the family.
Becker, J. V., Kaplan, M. S., Cunningham-Rathner, J., & Kavoussi, R.
While this meta-analysis cannot determine timing and (1986). Characteristics of adolescent incest sexual perpetrators:
causality, it highlights several promising domains as a focus Preliminary findings. Journal of Family Violence, 1, 85–97. https​
for future research. Given most clinical practices will come ://doi.org/10.1007/bf009​77034​.
Becker, J. V., Kaplan, M. S., & Tenke, C. E. (1992). The relationship of
across intrafamilial and extrafamilial sexual offending, it is
abuse history, denial and erectile response profiles of adolescent
important that any comprehensive assessment includes these sexual perpetrators. Behavior Therapy, 23, 87–97. https​://doi.
factors. Large, longitudinal studies should aim to investigate org/10.1016/s0005​-7894(05)80310​-7.
the complex nature and putatively causal relationships of Borduin, C. M., & Schaeffer, C. M. (2002). Multisystemic treatment
of juvenile sexual offenders. Journal of Psychology & Human
risk factors related to intrafamilial or extrafamilial adoles-
Sexuality, 13, 25–42. https​://doi.org/10.1300/j056v​13n03​_03.
cent sexual offending. Our hope is the findings presented Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R.
here will be used as a jumping off point for more specific (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester: Wiley.
investigation into the onset, maintenance, and desistance of Brackney, B. E., & Karabenick, S. A. (1995). Psychopathology and
academic performance: The role of motivation and learning
intrafamilial and extrafamilial adolescent sexual offending.

13
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

strategies. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 42, 456–465. https​ of Criminology, 46, 241–267. https​://doi.org/10.1177/00048​
://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.42.4.456. 65812​47038​3 [Study no. 15].
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cam- Daversa, M. T., & Knight, R. A. (2007). A structural examination
bridge: Harvard University Press. of the predictors of sexual coercion against children in ado-
*Brown, S. (2019). Can systemic interventions designed to reduce reof- lescent sexual offenders. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 34,
fending by youth also reduce their victimization? Unpublished 1313–1333. https​://doi.org/10.1177/00938​54807​30241​1.
dataset [Study no. 11.1]. Davis, K. A., & Knight, R. A. (2019). The relation of childhood
*Burkhart, B. R. (2019). Childhood sexual abuse in adolescents adju- abuse experiences to problematic sexual behaviors in male
dicated for sexual offenses: Mental health consequences and sex- youths who have sexually offended. Archives of Sexual Behav-
ual offending behaviors. Unpublished dataset [Study no. 12.1]. ior, 48, 1–21. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1050​8-018-1279-3.
Burton, D. L. (2003). Male adolescents: Sexual victimization and sub- Dopp, A. R., Borduin, C. M., White, I. I., Mark, H., & Kuppens, S.
sequent sexual abuse. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, (2017). Family-based treatments for serious juvenile offenders:
20, 277–296. https​://doi.org/10.1023/a:10245​56909​087. A multilevel meta-analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Caldwell, M. F. (2011). Study characteristics and recidivism base rates Psychology, 85, 335–354. https​://doi.org/10.1037/ccp00​00183​.
in juvenile sex offender recidivism. International Journal of *Edwards, R. (2019). Predicting dropout from a residential pro-
Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 54, 197–212. gramme for adolescent sexual abusers using pre-treatment
https​://doi.org/10.1177/03066​24x08​33001​6f. variables and implications for recidivism. Unpublished dataset
*Cale, J. (2019). Offense trajectories, the unfolding of sexual and [Study no. 16.1].
non-sexual criminal activity, and sex offense characteristics of *Edwards, R., Beech, A., Bishopp, D., Erikson, M., Friendship, C.,
adolescent sex offenders. Unpublished dataset [Study no. 13.1]. & Charlesworth, L. (2005). Predicting dropout from a resi-
*Cale, J., Smallbone, S., Rayment-McHugh, S., & Dowling, C. dential programme for adolescent sexual abusers using pre-
(2016). Offense trajectories, the unfolding of sexual and non- treatment variables and implications for recidivism. Journal of
sexual criminal activity, and sex offense characteristics of ado- Sexual Aggression, 11, 139–155. https​://doi.org/10.1080/13552​
lescent sex offenders. Sexual Abuse, 28, 791–812. https​://doi. 60050​00636​41 [Study no. 16].
org/10.1177/10790​63215​58096​8 [Study no. 13]. Eurostat. (2019). Being young in Europe today: Demographic trends.
Cann, J., Friendship, C., & Gozna, L. (2007). Assessing crossover Retrieved from https​://ec.europ​a.eu/euros​t at/stati​stics​-expla​
in a sample of sexual offenders with multiple victims. Legal ined/index ​ . php/Being ​ _ young ​ _ in_Europ ​ e _today ​ _ -_demog​
and Criminological Psychology, 12, 149–163. https​: //doi. raphi​c_trend​s.
org/10.1348/13553​2506x​11243​9. Finkelhor, D. (1984). Child sexual abuse: New theory and research.
Carlson, B. E., Maciol, K., & Schneider, J. (2006). Sibling incest: New York: The Free Press.
Reports from forty-one survivors. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, Finkelhor, D., & Ormrod, R. (1999). Reporting Crimes Against Juve-
15(4), 19–34. https​://doi.org/10.1300/j070v​15n04​_02. niles (Juvenile Justice Bulletin, Publication No. NCJ178887).
Charlesworth, D., & Willis, J. H. (2009). The genetics of inbreeding Retrieved from: https ​ : //schol ​ a rs.unh.edu/cgi/viewc ​ o nten​
depression. Nature Reviews Genetics, 10, 783–796. https​://doi. t.cgi?artic​le=1001&conte​xt=ccrc.
org/10.1038/nrg26​64. Finkelhor, D., Vanderminden, J., Turner, H., Hamby, S., & Shat-
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2002). A developmental psy- tuck, A. (2014). Child maltreatment rates assessed in a national
chopathology perspective on adolescence. Journal of Con- household survey of caregivers and youth. Child Abuse &
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 6–20. https ​ : //doi. Neglect, 38, 1421–1435. https ​ : //doi.org/10.1016/j.chiab​
org/10.1037/0022-006x.70.1.6. u.2014.05.005.
*Clift, R. J. (2019). Background, offence characteristics, and crimi- Felson, R., Savolainen, J., Aaltonen, M., & Moustgaard, H. (2008).
nal outcomes of Aboriginal youth who sexually offend: A closer Is the association between alcohol use and delinquency causal
look at Aboriginal youth intervention needs. Unpublished dataset or spurious? Criminology, 46, 785–808. https​://doi.org/10.111
[Study no. 14.1]. 1/j.1745-9125.2008.00120​.x.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Forns, M., Abad, J., & Kirchner, T. (2018). Internalizing and exter-
Hillside: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. nalizing problems. In R. J. R. Levesque (Ed.), Encyclopedia of
Cortoni, F., & Marshall, W. L. (2001). Sex as a coping strategy and adolescence (pp. 1965–1970). Berlin: Springer.
its relationship to juvenile sexual history and intimacy in sexual Forsman, M., Johansson, Santtila, Sandnabba, & Långström, N. (2012).
offenders. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, Child maltreatment and adult violent offending: population-based
13, 27–43. https​://doi.org/10.1023/a:10095​62312​658. twin study addressing the ‘cycle of violence’ hypothesis. Psycho-
Cottle, C. C., Lee, R. J., & Heilbrun, K. (2001). The prediction of logical Medicine, 42, 1977–1983. https​://doi.org/10.1017/s0033​
criminal recidivism in juveniles: A meta-analysis. Criminal Jus- 29171​10030​60.
tice and Behavior, 28, 367–394. https​://doi.org/10.1177/00938​ Gannon, T. A., & Cortoni, F. (2010). Female sexual offenders: The-
54801​02800​3005. ory, assessment and treatment. New York: Wiley. https​://doi.
Cyr, M., Wright, J., McDuff, P., & Perron, A. (2002). Intrafamilial sex- org/10.1002/97804​70666​715.
ual abuse: Brother sister incest does not differ from father daugh- Ginzburg, K., Arnow, B., Hart, S., Gardner, W., Koopman, C., Classen,
ter and stepfather stepdaughter incest. Child Abuse & Neglect, C. C., et al. (2006). The abuse-related beliefs questionnaire for
26, 957–973. https​://doi.org/10.1016/s0145​-2134(02)00365​-4. survivors of childhood sexual abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 30,
*Daly, K. (2019). Youth sex offending, recidivism and restorative jus- 929–943. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiab​u.2006.01.004.
tice: Comparing court and conference cases. Unpublished data- Grant, J., Thornton, J., & Chamarette, C. (2006). Residential place-
set [Study no. 15.1]. ment of intra-familial adolescent sex offenders. Trends & Issues
Daly, K., & Bouhours, B. (2010). Rape and attrition in the legal pro- in Crime and Criminal Justice, 315. Retrieved from: https​://aic.
cess: A comparative analysis of five countries. Crime and Jus- gov.au/publi​catio​ns/tandi​/tandi​315.
tice, 39, 565–650. https​://doi.org/10.1086/65310​1. Gunby, C., & Woodhams, J. (2010). Sexually deviant juveniles: Com-
*Daly, K., Bouhours, B., Broadhurst, R., & Loh, N. (2013). Youth parisons between the offender and offence characteristics of
sex offending, recidivism and restorative justice: Comparing ‘child abusers’ and ‘peer abusers’. Psychology, Crime & Law,
court and conference cases. Australian & New Zealand Journal 16, 47–64. https​://doi.org/10.1080/10683​16080​26219​66.

13
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

Halse, A., Grant, J., Thornton, J., Indermaur, D., Stevens, G., & *Latzman, N. E., Viljoen, J. L., Scalora, M. J., & Ullman, D. (2011).
Chamarette, C. (2012). Intrafamilial adolescent sex offenders’ Sexual offending in adolescence: A comparison of sibling
response to psychological treatment. Psychiatry, Psychology and offenders and nonsibling offenders across domains of risk and
Law, 19, 221–235. https:​ //doi.org/10.1080/132187​ 19.2011.56176​ treatment need. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 20, 245–263.
3. https​://doi.org/10.1080/10538​712.2011.57123​3 [Study no. 2].
Hanson, R. K., & Bussière, M. T. (1998). Predicting relapse: A Laviola, M. (1992). Effects of older brother-younger sister incest:
meta-analysis of sexual offender recidivism studies. Journal of A study of the dynamics of 17 cases. Child Abuse & Neglect,
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 348–362. https​://doi. 16, 409–421. https​://doi.org/10.1016/0145-2134(92)90050​-2.
org/10.1037/0022-006x.66.2.348. Leclerc, B., Smallbone, S., & Wortley, R. (2015). Prevention nearby:
Hanson, R. K., & Morton-Bourgon, K. E. (2005). The characteristics The influence of the presence of a potential guardian on the
of persistent sexual offenders: A meta-analysis of recidivism severity of child sexual abuse. Sexual Abuse, 27, 189–204.
studies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, https​://doi.org/10.1177/10790​63213​50459​4.
1154–1163. https​://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006x.73.6.1154. *Leroux, E. J. (2019). Victim age and the generalist versus specialist
Haskins, C. (2003). Treating sibling incest using a family systems distinction in adolescent sexual offending. Unpublished dataset
approach. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 25, 337–350. [Study no. 18.1].
https​://doi.org/10.17744​/mehc.25.4.r0vm1​whayc​tmlmw​w. Leroux, E. J. (2019). Understanding the experiences of justice-
Hasselblad, V., & Hedges, L. V. (1995). Meta-analysis of screening and involved youth at the intersection of the criminal justice and
diagnostic tests. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 167–178. https​:// mental health systems (Unpublished doctoral dissertation).
doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.167. Ottawa: Carleton University.
Head, M. L., Holman, L., Lanfear, R., Kahn, A. T., & Jennions, M. D. Leroux, E. J., Pullman, L. E., Motayne, G., & Seto, M. C. (2016).
(2015). The extent and consequences of p-hacking in science. Victim age and the generalist versus specialist distinction in
PLoS Biology. https​://doi.org/10.1371/journ​al.pbio.10021​06. adolescent sexual offending. Sexual Abuse, 28, 79–95. https​://
Heinrich, L. M., & Gullone, E. (2006). The clinical significance of doi.org/10.1177/10790​63214​53581​4 [Study no. 18].
loneliness: A literature review. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, Letourneau, E. J., Henggeler, S. W., Borduin, C. M., Schewe, P. A.,
695–718. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2006.04.002. McCart, M. R., Chapman, J. E., et al. (2009). Multisystemic
*Hendriks, J., & Bijleveld, C. C. J. H. (2004). Juvenile sexual delin- therapy for juvenile sexual offenders: 1-Year results from a
quents: Contrasting child abusers with peer abusers.  Crimi- randomized effectiveness trial. Journal of Family Psychology,
nal Behaviour and Mental Health, 14, 238–250. https​://doi. 23, 89–102. https​://doi.org/10.1037/a0014​352.
org/10.1002/cbm.591 [Study no. 23]. Letourneau, E. J., Henggeler, S. W., McCart, M. R., Borduin, C. M.,
Higgins, J., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G. (2003). Schewe, P. A., & Armstrong, K. S. (2013). Two-year follow-up
Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. British Medical Jour- of a randomized effectiveness trial evaluating MST for juve-
nal, 327, 557–560. https​://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557. niles who sexually offend. Journal of Family Psychology, 27,
Jeglic, E. L., Mercado, C. C., & Levenson, J. S. (2012). The prevalence 978–985. https​://doi.org/10.1037/a0034​710.
and correlates of depression and hopelessness among sex offend- Letourneau, E. J., & Miner, M. H. (2005). Juvenile sex offenders: A
ers subject to community notification and residence restriction case against the legal and clinical status quo. Sexual Abuse: A
legislation. American Journal of Criminal Justice, 37, 46–59. Journal of Research and Treatment, 17, 293–312. https​://doi.
https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1210​3-010-9096-9. org/10.1177/10790​63205​01700​304.
Jespersen, A. F., Lalumière, M. L., & Seto, M. C. (2009). Sexual Lindell, A. K., & Campione-Barr, N. (2017). Continuity and change
abuse history among adult sex offenders and non-sex offenders: in the family system across the transition from adolescence to
A meta-analysis. Child Abuse & Neglect, 33, 179–192. https​:// emerging adulthood. Marriage & Family Review, 53, 388–416.
doi.org/10.1016/j.chiab​u.2008.07.004. https​://doi.org/10.1080/01494​929.2016.11842​12.
*Joyal, C. C., Carpentier, J., & Martin, C. (2016). Discriminant factors *Mackaronis, J. E. (2019). Assessing sexual interest in adolescents
for adolescent sexual offending: On the usefulness of consider- who have sexually offended. Unpublished dataset [Study no.
ing both victim age and sibling incest. Child Abuse & Neglect, 19.1].
54, 10–22. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiab​u.2016.01.006 [Study *Mackaronis, J. E., Byrne, P. M., & Strassberg, D. S. (2016). Assessing
no. 1]. sexual interest in adolescents who have sexually offended. Sexual
Katz, C., & Hamama, L. (2017). From my own brother in my own Abuse, 28, 96–115. https​://doi.org/10.1177/10790​63214​53581​8
home: Children’s experiences and perceptions following alleged [Study no. 19].
sibling incest. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 32, 3648–3668. Mallie, A. L., Viljoen, J. L., Mordell, S., Spice, A., & Roesch, R.
https​://doi.org/10.1177/08862​60515​60087​6. (2011). Childhood abuse and adolescent sexual re-offending: A
*Kemper, T. S. (2019). Offense history and recidivism in three victim- meta-analysis. Child & Youth Care Forum, 40, 401–417. https​://
age-based groups of juvenile sex offenders. Unpublished dataset doi.org/10.1007/s1056​6-010-9136-0.
[Study no. 17.1]. Maniglio, R. (2011). The role of childhood trauma, psychological prob-
*Kemper, T. S., & Kistner, J. A. (2007). Offense history and recidivism lems, and coping in the development of deviant sexual fantasies
in three victim-age-based groups of juvenile sex offenders. Sex- in sexual offenders. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 748–756.
ual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 19, 409–424. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2011.03.003.
https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1119​4-007-9061-4 [Study no. 17]. Marshall, W. L., & Marshall, L. E. (2000). The origins of sexual
Knight, R. A., & Sims-Knight, J. E. (2004). Testing an etiological offending. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 1, 250–263. https​://doi.
model for male juvenile sexual offending against females. Jour- org/10.1177/15248​38000​00100​3003.
nal of Child Sexual Abuse, 13, 33–55. https​://doi.org/10.1300/ McCann, K., & Lussier, P. (2008). Antisociality, sexual deviance, and
j070v​13n03​_03. sexual reoffending in juvenile sex offenders: A meta-analytical
Krienert, J. L., & Walsh, J. A. (2011). Sibling sexual abuse: An investigation. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 6, 363–385.
empirical analysis of offender, victim, and event characteristics https​://doi.org/10.1177/15412​04008​32026​0.
in National Incident-Based Reporting System (NIBRS) Data, *McCuish, E. C. (2019). Abuse experiences of family members, child
2000–2007. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 20, 353–372. https​ maltreatment, and the development of sex offending among incar-
://doi.org/10.1300/j070v​13n03​_03. cerated adolescent males: Differences between adolescent sex

13
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

offenders and adolescent non-sex offenders. Unpublished dataset *Parks, G. A., & Bard, D. E. (2006). Risk factors for adolescent sex
[Study no. 20.1]. offender recidivism: Evaluation of predictive factors and com-
*McCuish, E. C., Cale, J., & Corrado, R. R. (2017). Abuse experiences parison of three groups based upon victim type. Sexual Abuse:
of family members, child maltreatment, and the development of A Journal of Research and Treatment, 18, 319–342. https​://doi.
sex offending among incarcerated adolescent males: Differences org/10.1177/10790​63206​01800​402 [Study no. 22].
between adolescent sex offenders and adolescent non-sex offend- Pedneault, C. I., Babchishin, K. M., Lalumière, M. L., & Seto, M. C.
ers. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative (2019). The association between childhood sexual abuse and sex-
Criminology, 61, 127–149. https​://doi.org/10.1177/03066​24x15​ ual coercion in men: A test of possible mediators. Journal of Sex-
59749​2 [Study no. 20]. ual Aggression. https​://doi.org/10.1080/13552​600.2019.16135​
McDonald, C., & Martinez, K. (2017). Victims’ retrospective explana- 75.
tions of sibling sexual violence. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, *Perez, E. M. (2017). Differences Between Intrafamilial and Extrafa-
26, 874–888. https​://doi.org/10.1080/10538​712.2017.13549​53. milial Adolescent Sex Offenders (Unpublished doctoral disserta-
*McKillop, N., Brown, S., Johnson, I., Smallbone, S., & Ogilvie, J. M. tion). Alliant International University, Fresno, United States of
(2017). Can systemic interventions designed to reduce reoffend- America [Study no. 3].
ing by youth also reduce their victimization? Journal of Child Planty, M., Langton, L., Krebs, C., Berzofsky, M., & Smiley-McDon-
& Adolescent Trauma, 10, 41–50. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s4065​ ald, H. (2013). Female Victims of Sexual Violence, 1994-2010
3-016-0123-y [Study no. 11]. (U.S. Bureau of Justice Statistics, NCJ 240655). Retrieved from:
McPhail, I. V., Hermann, C. A., Fernane, S., Fernandez, Y. M., Nunes, https​://www.bjs.gov/conte​nt/pub/pdf/fvsv9​410.pdf.
K. L., & Cantor, J. M. (2019). Validity in phallometric testing for Pullman, L. E. (2018). Examining the roles of early proximity, degree
sexual interests in children: A meta-analytic review. Assessment, of genetic relatedness, and disgust in explaining father-daughter
26, 535–551. https​://doi.org/10.1177/10731​91117​70613​9. and brother-sister incest (Unpublished doctoral dissertation).
*Miner, M. H. (2019). Understanding sexual perpetration against chil- Ottawa: University of Ottawa.
dren: Effects of attachment style, interpersonal involvement, and Pullman, L. E., Babchishin, K., & Seto, M. C. (2019). An examination
hypersexuality. Unpublished dataset [Study no. 21.1]. of the Westermarck hypothesis and the role of disgust in incest
*Miner, M. H., Robinson, B. E., Knight, R. A., Berg, D., Swinburne avoidance among fathers. Evolutionary Psychology. https​://doi.
Romine, R., & Netland, J. (2010). Understanding sexual perpe- org/10.1177/14747​04919​84992​4.
tration against children: Effects of attachment style, interpersonal Pullman, L. E., Sawatsky, M. L., Babchishin, K. M., McPhail, I.
involvement, and hypersexuality. Sexual Abuse, 22, 58–77. https​ V., & Seto, M. C. (2017). Differences between biological and
://doi.org/10.1177/10790​63209​35318​3 [Study no. 21]. sociolegal incest offenders: A meta-analysis. Aggression and
Miner, M. H., Swinburne Romine, R., Robinson, B. B. E., Berg, D., Violent Behavior, 34, 228–237. https ​ : //doi.org/10.1016/j.
& Knight, R. A. (2016). Anxious attachment, social isolation, avb.2017.01.003.
and indicators of sex drive and compulsivity: Predictors of child Radford, L., Corral, S., Bradley, C., & Fisher, H. L. (2013). The
sexual abuse perpetration in adolescent males? Sexual Abuse: prevalence and impact of child maltreatment and other types of
A Journal of Research and Treatment, 28, 132–153. https​://doi. victimization in the UK: Findings from a population survey of
org/10.1177/10790​63214​54758​5. caregivers, children and young people and young adults. Child
Monahan, K. (2010). Themes of adult sibling sexual abuse survivors Abuse & Neglect, 37, 801–813. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiab​
in later life: An initial exploration. Clinical Social Work Journal, u.2013.02.004.
38, 361–369. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1061​5-010-0286-1. *Rayment-McHugh, S., & Nisbet, I. (2003, May). Sibling incest offend-
*Morais, H. B., Alexander, A. A., Fix, R. L., & Burkhart, B. R. ers as a subset of adolescent sexual offenders. Paper presented at
(2018). Childhood sexual abuse in adolescents adjudicated the Child Sexual Abuse: Justice Response or Alternative Reso-
for sexual offenses: Mental health consequences and sexual lution Conference, Adelaide, Australia. Retrieved from: https​://
offending behaviors.  Sexual Abuse,  30, 23–42. https​: //doi. epubs​.scu.edu.au/ccyp_pubs/6/. [Study no. 4].
org/10.1177/10790​63215​62522​4 [Study no. 12]. *Richardson, G., Kelly, T. P., Bhate, S. R., & Graham, F. (1997).
Murphy, W. D., DiLillo, D., Haynes, M. R., & Steere, E. (2001). An Group differences in abuser and abuse characteristics in a Brit-
exploration of factors related to deviant sexual arousal among ish sample of sexually abusive adolescents. Sexual Abuse: A
juvenile sex offenders. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Journal of Research and Treatment, 9, 239–257. https​://doi.
Treatment, 13, 91–103. https​://doi.org/10.1023/a:10266​48220​ org/10.1177/10790​63297​00900​307 [Study no. 5].
419. *Rojas, E. Y., & Gretton, H. M. (2007). Background, offence char-
Murray, J., & Farrington, D. P. (2010). Risk factors for conduct disor- acteristics, and criminal outcomes of Aboriginal youth who
der and delinquency: Key findings from longitudinal studies. Los sexually offend: A closer look at Aboriginal youth intervention
Angeles: SAGE Publications. needs. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 19,
Nationaal Rapporteur Mensenhandel en Seksueel Geweld tegen Kin- 257–283. https​://doi.org/10.1177/10790​63207​01900​306 [Study
deren. (2018). Dadermonitor seksueel geweld tegen kinderen no. 14].
2013–2017. Den Haag: Nationaal Rapporteur Mensenhandel en Rojas, E. Y., & Olver, M. E. (2019). Validity and reliability of the
Seksueel Geweld tegen Kinderen. violence risk scale-youth sexual offense version. Sexual Abuse.
Ogilvie, J. M., Stewart, A. L., Chan, R. C., & Shum, D. H. (2011). Neu- https​://doi.org/10.1177/10790​63219​85806​4.
ropsychological measures of executive function and antisocial Rothstein, H. R., Sutton, A. J., & Borenstein, M. (2005). Publication
behavior: A meta-analysis. Criminology, 49, 1063–1107. https​ bias in meta-analysis. Hoboken: Wiley.
://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-9125.2011.00252​.x. Rudd, J. M., & Herzberger, S. D. (1999). Brother-sister incest—
Paine, M. L., & Hansen, D. J. (2002). Factors influencing children father-daughter incest: A comparison of characteristics and
to self-disclose sexual abuse. Clinical Psychology Review, 22, consequences. Child Abuse & Neglect, 23, 915–928. https​://doi.
271–295. https​://doi.org/10.1016/s0272​-7358(01)00091​-5. org/10.1016/s0145​-2134(99)00058​-7.
*Parks, G. A. (2019). Risk factors for adolescent sex offender recidi- Sánchez-Meca, J., Chacón-Moscoso, S., & Marín-Martínez, F.
vism: Evaluation of predictive factors and comparison of three (2003). Effect-size indices for dichotomized outcomes in
groups based upon victim type. Unpublished dataset [Study no. meta-analysis. Psychological Methods, 8, 448–467. https​://doi.
22.1]. org/10.1037/1082-989x.8.4.448.

13
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

Schofield, T. J., Lee, R. D., & Merrick, M. T. (2013). Safe, stable, Ueda, M. (2017). Developmental risk factors of juvenile sex offend-
nurturing relationships as a moderator of intergenerational ers by victim age: An implication for specialized treatment pro-
continuity of child maltreatment: A meta-analysis. Journal grams. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 37, 122–128. https​://
of Adolescent Health, 53, S32–S38. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j. doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2017.09.006.
jadoh​ealth​.2013.05.004. U.S. Census Bureau, Population Division. (2018). Projected 5-year
Schulze, R. (2007). Cur rent methods for meta-analysis: age groups and sex composition: Main projections series for the
Approaches, issues, and developments. Zeitschrift für Psy- United States, 2017–2060. Retrieved from: https​://www.censu​
chologie/Journal of Psychology, 215, 90–103. https​: //doi. s.gov/data/table ​ s /2017/demo/poppr ​ o j/2017-summa ​ r y-table​
org/10.1027/0044-3409.215.2.90. s.html.
Seto, M. C. (2019). The motivation-facilitation model of sex- U.S. Department of Justice. (2009). Juveniles who commit sexual
ual offending. Sexual Abuse, 31(1), 3–24. https ​ : //doi. offenses against minors (The Office of Juvenile Justice and Delin-
org/10.1177/10790​63217​72091​9. quency Prevention, NCJ Publication No. 227763). Retrieved
Seto, M. C. (2018). Pedophilia and sexual offending against chil- from: https​://www.ncjrs​.gov/pdffi​les1/ojjdp​/22776​3.pdf.
dren: Theory, assessment, and intervention (2nd ed.). Wash- *Van den Berg, C. (2019). Juvenile sexual delinquents: Contrasting
ington, DC: American Psychological Association. child abusers with peer abusers. Unpublished dataset [Study no.
Seto, M. C., Babchishin, K. M., Pullman, L. E., & McPhail, I. V. 23.1].
(2015). The puzzle of intrafamilial child sexual abuse: A meta- Van den Berg, C., Bijleveld, C., & Hendriks, J. (2017). The juvenile
analysis comparing intrafamilial and extrafamilial offenders sex offender: Criminal careers and life events. Sexual Abuse, 29,
with child victims. Clinical Psychology Review, 39, 42–57. 81–101. https​://doi.org/10.1177/10790​63215​58096​7.
https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2015.04.001. Van der Stouwe, T., Asscher, J. J., Stams, G. J. J., Deković, M., &
Seto, M. C., & Lalumière, M. L. (2010). What is so special about Van der Laan, P. H. (2014). The effectiveness of multisystemic
male adolescent sexual offending? A review and test of expla- therapy (MST): A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review,
nations through meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 34, 468–481. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2014.06.006.
526–575. https​://doi.org/10.1037/a0019​700. *Van Vugt, E. S. (2019). Similarities and differences between youth
Seto, M. C., Murphy, W. D., Page, J., & Ennis, L. (2003). Detecting who engaged in intrafamilial and extrafamilial sexually abusive
anomalous sexual interests in juvenile sex offenders. Annals behavior: An exploratory study. Unpublished dataset [Study no.
of the New York Academy of Sciences, 989, 118–130. https​:// 24.1].
doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2003.tb072​98.x. *Van Vugt, E., & Garofalo, C. (2020). Similarities and differences
Smith, E. P., Faulk, M., & Sizer, M. A. (2016). Exploring the meso- between youth who engaged in intrafamilial and extrafamilial
system: The roles of community, family, and peers in ado- sexually abusive behavior: An exploratory study. International
lescent delinquency and positive youth development. Youth Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology.
& Society, 48, 318–343. https​://doi.org/10.1177/00441​18x13​ https​://doi.org/10.1177/03066​24x20​91189​7 [Study no. 24].
49158​1. Varma, K. N., & Leroux, E. J. (2019). Understanding the nature and
Smith, H., & Israel, E. (1987). Sibling incest: A study of the dynamics scope of adolescent sexual offending across Canada. In S. Kohm,
of 25 cases. Child Abuse & Neglect, 11, 101–108. https​://doi. K. Walby, K. Gorkoff, & K. Maier (Eds.), The annual review of
org/10.1016/0145-2134(87)90038​-x. interdisciplinary justice research (Vol. 8, pp. 120–145). Win-
Steffensmeier, D., & Allan, E. (1996). Gender and crime: Toward a nipeg: Centre for Interdisciplinary Justice Studies.
gendered theory of female offending. Annual Review of Sociol- *Veneziano, C. (2019). The relationship between adolescent sex
ogy, 22, 459–487. https​://doi.org/10.1146/annur​ev.soc.22.1.459. offender behaviors and victim characteristics with prior victimi-
Stemple, L., Flores, A., & Meyer, I. H. (2017). Sexual victimization zation. Unpublished dataset [Study no. 25.1].
perpetrated by women: Federal data reveal surprising preva- *Veneziano, C., Veneziano, L., & LeGrand, S. (2000). The relationship
lence. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 34, 302–311. https​:// between adolescent sex offender behaviors and victim charac-
doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2016.09.007. teristics with prior victimization. Journal of Interpersonal Vio-
Stephens, S., Seto, M. C., Goodwill, A. M., & Cantor, J. M. (2018). The lence, 15, 363–374. https​://doi.org/10.1177/08862​60000​15004​
relationships between victim age, gender, and relationship poly- 002 [Study no. 25].
morphism and sexual recidivism. Sexual Abuse, 30(2), 132–146. Vess, J., & Skelton, A. (2010). Sexual and violent recidivism by
https​://doi.org/10.1177/10790​63216​63098​3. offender type and actuarial risk: Reoffending rates for rapists,
Suldo, S., Thalji, A., & Ferron, J. (2011). Longitudinal academic out- child molesters and mixed-victim offenders. Psychology, Crime
comes predicted by early adolescents’ subjective well-being, & Law, 16, 541–554. https​://doi.org/10.1080/10683​16080​26129​
psychopathology, and mental health status yielded from a dual 08.
factor model. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 6, 17–30. https​ *Viljoen, J. L. (2019). Sexual offending in adolescence: A comparison
://doi.org/10.1080/17439​760.2010.53677​4. of sibling offenders and nonsibling offenders across domains of
Tewksbury, R. (2012). Stigmatization of sex offenders. Deviant Behav- risk and treatment need. Additional unpublished data [Study no.
ior, 33, 606–623. https​://doi.org/10.1080/01639​625.2011.63669​ 2.1].
0. Viljoen, J. L., Mordell, S., & Beneteau, J. L. (2012). Prediction of
Thornberry, T. P., & Henry, K. L. (2013). Intergenerational continu- adolescent sexual reoffending: A meta-analysis of the J-SOAP-II,
ity in maltreatment. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, ERASOR, J-SORRAT-II, and Static-99. Law and Human Behav-
555–569. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1080​2-012-9697-5. ior, 36, 423–438. https​://doi.org/10.1037/h0093​938.
Thornton, J. A., Stevens, G., Grant, J., Indermaur, D., Chamarette, Ward, T., Polaschek, D. L., & Beech, A. R. (2006). Theories of sexual
C., & Halse, A. (2008). Intrafamilial adolescent sex offenders: offending (Vol. 21). Chichester: Wiley.
Family functioning and treatment. Journal of Family Studies, 14, Ward, T., & Siegert, R. J. (2002). Toward a comprehensive theory of
362–375. https​://doi.org/10.5172/jfs.327.14.2-3.362. child sexual abuse: A theory knitting perspective. Psychology,
*Tidefors, I., Arvidsson, H., Ingevaldson, S., & Larsson, M. (2010). Crime and Law, 8, 319–351. https:​ //doi.org/10.1080/106831​ 6020​
Sibling incest: A literature review and a clinical study. Journal of 84018​23.
Sexual Aggression, 16, 347–360. https​://doi.org/10.1080/13552​ *Way, I. (2019). Delinquent histories of adolescents adjudicated for
60090​35116​67 [Study no. 6]. criminal sexual conduct. Unpublished dataset [Study no. 26.1].

13
Author's personal copy
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

*Way, I., & Urbaniak, D. (2008). Delinquent histories of adolescents adolescent nonsibling sex offenders. Child Abuse & Neglect, 19,
adjudicated for criminal sexual conduct. Journal of Interpersonal 633–643. https​://doi.org/10.1016/0145-2134(95)00021​-y [Study
Violence, 23, 1197–1212. https​://doi.org/10.1177/08862​60508​ no. 7].
31429​6 [Study no. 26]. *Yates, P., Allardyce, S., & MacQueen, S. (2012). Children who dis-
White, H. R., Tice, P. C., Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2002). play harmful sexual behaviour: Assessing the risks of boys abus-
Illegal acts committed by adolescents under the influence of alco- ing at home, in the community or across both settings. Journal
hol and drugs. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, of Sexual Aggression, 18, 23–35. https​://doi.org/10.1080/13552​
39, 131–152. https​://doi.org/10.1177/00224​27802​03900​201. 600.2011.63452​7 [Study no. 8].
Willard, S. (2014). The relationship between incest victimization and Yoder, J. R., Hansen, J., Ruch, D., & Hodge, A. (2016). Effects of
incest perpetration in adolescent registered juvenile sex offend- school-based risk and protective factors on treatment success
ers (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Pittsburgh: University among youth adjudicated of a sexual crime. Child Sexual Abuse,
of Pittsburgh. 25, 310–325. https​://doi.org/10.1080/10538​712.2016.11376​68.
Wolf, A. P. (2014). Incest avoidance and the incest taboos: Two aspects
of human nature. Redwood City: Stanford University Press. Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
*Worling, J. R. (1995). Adolescent sibling-incest offenders: Differ- jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
ences in family and individual functioning when compared to

13

You might also like