Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

Brief Communication 29

How birds use their eyes: Opposite left-right specialization for


the lateral and frontal visual hemifield in the domestic chick
Giorgio Vallortigara*†, Claudio Cozzutti‡, Luca Tommasi‡,
and Lesley J. Rogers§
Recent evidence has demonstrated that, in animals lateralization of attack and some other visually guided
with laterally placed eyes, functional cerebral responses in the chick [8, 9], we used both chicks hatched
asymmetry is revealed by preferential use of either from eggs maintained in the dark until hatching (D-chicks)
the left or right eye in a range of behaviors (birds: and chicks hatched from eggs exposed to light during
[1, 2, 3]; fish: [4, 5]; reptiles: [6, 7]). These findings the last three days before hatching (L-chicks). For light
pose a theoretical problem. It seems that there exposure, a 60 W lamp provided 300 Lux within the
would be disadvantages in having a substantial incubator. We reared 40 L-chicks and 36 D-chicks in pairs
degree of asymmetry in the use of the two eyes; a at a controlled temperature (30–35⬚C) in metal cages (30 ⫻
deficit on one side would leave the organism 30 ⫻ 40 cm) that were illuminated from above by fluores-
vulnerable to attack on that side or unable to cent lamps. For each pair of companion chicks, another
exploit resources appearing on one side. We here pair was selected, and the two pairs were tested simultane-
report a possible solution to the problem. We have ously (either as pairs of companions or pairs of strangers)
found that domestic chicks show selective use of in two separate cages (50 ⫻ 60 ⫻ 30 cm). The behavior
the lateral visual field of the left eye and of the of the chicks was videorecorded for 5 min. Since data for
right hemifield in the binocular, frontal visual field an individual subject could not be considered to be en-
when they peck at strangers but not at cagemates. tirely independent of the other chick in the pair, data for
Thus, during social recognition, there seems to be each pair were considered together as one score (light
opposite and complementary left-right companions, N ⫽ 10; light strangers, N ⫽ 10; dark com-
specialization for the lateral and frontal visual fields panions, N ⫽ 8; dark strangers, N ⫽ 10). The numbers
of the two eyes. These findings can reconcile the of pecks directed to the conspecific and to the environ-
computational advantages associated with ment (at the floor or walls of the cage) were measured
asymmetry of the left and right sides of the brain from the videotapes. In addition, we measured the fre-
with the ecological demands for an animal to quency with which the chick used its lateral and frontal
perceive and respond equally well to the left and fields of vision to fixate before pecking. We made these
right sides of its midline. measurements by using transparencies to superimpose
protractors on the monitor screen (Figure 1). We classified
Addresses: *Department of Psychology, Animal Cognition and angles between ⫾ 15 degrees of the midline as use of the
Comparative Neuroscience Laboratory, University of Trieste, and
†B.R.A.I.N. Centre for Neuroscience, Trieste 34123, Italy. ‡Department frontal (binocular) visual field and the remaining angles
of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova 35131, Italy. as use of the lateral visual field [1]. Note that, although
§School of Biological Sciences, Division of Zoology, University of New the chick pecks by a ballistic movement of its head for-
England, Armidale NSW 2351, Australia. ward from its midline, it fixates and decides whether to
peck or not prior to this by turning its head to view the
Correspondence: Giorgio Vallortigara
E-mail: vallorti@univ.trieste.it stimulus and using the high-acuity regions of its retina.

Received: 6 October 2000


Revised: 8 November 2000
Figure 2 shows that, as expected, both L- and D-chicks
Accepted: 21 November 2000 pecked more at strangers than at companions. Pecks at
the environment, in contrast, were higher in D-chicks
Published: 9 January 2001 than in L-chicks and tended to be higher among strangers
in L-chicks and among companions in D-chicks.
Current Biology 2001, 11:29–33

0960-9822/01/$ – see front matter Use of the lateral and frontal visual fields before pecking
 2001 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. shows opposite directions of lateralization in D-chicks
(Figure 3): D-chicks preferentially used the left, lateral
Results and discussion monocular field and the right, frontal hemifield before
Chicks were tested, on day 3 after hatching, in pairs com- pecking at strangers. L-chicks showed significant use of
posed of companions (cagemates) or strangers. Social the left, lateral monocular field but, in contrast to the
pecking by each chick at its test partner was scored. Since D-chicks, no differences with respect to the frontal hemi-
previous work had revealed that exposure of the embryo fields. A more detailed analysis of the different targets of
to light during the final days of incubation determines pecking (Figure 4) confirmed these data and showed that
30 Current Biology Vol 11 No 1

Figure 1

Schematic representation of a chick’s use of


the frontal or lateral field of vision before
pecking (␣ indicates the angle used to peck
at the target in this case).

Figure 2

Number of pecks directed at a conspecific and at the environment in but not for pecks at the feet [F(1, 34) ⫽ 0.286 (ns)]. Pecking at the
pairs of companion and stranger chicks. There were more pecks at environment was the same among pairs of companions and
strangers than at companions in both L- and D-chicks [test condition: strangers [F(1, 34) ⫽ 0.470, p ⫽ 0.498]; however, pecking at the
F(1, 34) ⫽ 16.313, p ⬍ 0.001; hatching condition: F(1, 34) ⫽ 0.050 environment was higher among D- than among L-chicks [F(1, 34) ⫽
(not significant, or “ns”); test ⫻ hatching: F(1, 34) ⫽ 0.300 (ns)]. 6.087, p ⫽ 0.019], and there was a tendency [interaction: F(1,34) ⫽
Separate analyses showed that the companions/strangers difference 3.815, p ⫽ 0.059] in L-chicks for higher pecking among strangers
was evident (see bottom row) only for pecks at the head [F(1, 34) ⫽ and in D-chicks for higher pecking among companions.
10.108, p ⫽ 0.003], and the body [F(1, 34) ⫽ 19.804, p ⬍ 0.001]
Brief Communication 31

Figure 3

Chicks’ use of the lateral field before pecking at a conspecific showed (ns)]; the test ⫻ hatching interaction was, however, significant [F(1,34) ⫽
a significant effect of testing conditions [F(1, 34) ⫽ 7.387, p ⫽ 0.010] 5.335, p ⬍ 0.05]. D-chicks, but not L-chicks, showed a significant
in both L- and D-chicks; there were no other statistically significant preference (one-sample t-test, two-tailed; see figure) for using the right
effects [hatching: F(1,34) ⫽ 0.18 (ns); test ⫻ hatching: F(1,34) ⫽ frontal field when tested as pairs of strangers.
0.003 (ns)]. Chicks showed a significant preference (one-sample There were no statistically significant effects associated with eye use
t-test; two-tailed; see figure) for using the left lateral field when they before pecking at the environment [lateral field — hatching, F(1,34) ⫽
were tested in pairs of strangers. Use of the frontal field before pecking 0.023; test, F(1,34) ⫽ 0.066; test ⫻ hatching, F(1, 34) ⫽ 0.137.
at a conspecific showed a significant effect of testing conditions Frontal field — hatching, F(1,34) ⫽ 0.035; test, F(1,34) ⫽ 1.820;
[F(1,34) ⫽ 4.175, p ⬍ 0.05], but not of hatching [F(1,34) ⫽ 0.165 test ⫻ hatching, F(1,34) ⫽ 0.810].

the effects were largely confined to hemifield use before grounds; other things being equal, these stimuli might
pecks at the head. No significant preferences in hemifield happen to be located on either side at random. The answer
use were apparent before pecking at the environment that has been provided so far is that the computational
(Figure 3). advantages associated with possession of an asymmetric
brain [18, 19, 20] should compensate for the ecological
These findings provide a solution to a puzzling phenome- disadvantages of perceiving and responding with less effi-
non. A number of independent reports have provided ciency to one or the other side of the body. However, our
convincing evidence that animals with laterally placed results suggest a different answer. The two eyes of the
eyes show preferential left and right use in different tasks domestic chick seem to provide visual information that
(reviewed in [10]). For instance, chicks use the left eye can be used for social recognition, but they utilize differ-
to look at an aerial predator [2] and the right eye to look ent parts of the visual field; of the two lateral fields, the
at a familiar imprinting stimulus [1, 11]. Anolis lizards [6, left is used preferentially, whereas in the binocular field
7] and toads [12, 13] use the left eye during agonistic the area to the right of the midline (right binocular hemi-
encounters, and fish use the right eye to look at a danger- field) is used preferentially. Interestingly, previous evi-
ous stimulus [14] and the left eye to look at conspecifics dence had shown that chicks wearing monocular eye
[5]. Results of studies that use selective occlusion of one patches were unable to discriminate between companions
eye complement these results by showing that birds’ per- and strangers when they were using their right eye [21,
formance in a variety of tasks depends on which eye they 22] and that, in contrast, chicks with blinkers covering
use (reviews in [15, 16, 17]). However, having one eye the right frontal field (but leaving the lateral field unob-
that is better at responding to a predator or recognizing structed) showed difficulty in inhibiting pecks at compan-
a partner appears to be a disadvantage on ecological ions [23]. This is now understandable because use of eye
32 Current Biology Vol 11 No 1

Figure 4

Chicks’ use of the lateral field before pecking at different parts of the both pecks at the head [F(1, 34) ⫽ 5.510, p ⫽ 0.025] and pecks at
conspecific revealed a significant difference between companions the feet [F(1, 34) ⫽ 0.047]. There were no other statistically
and strangers for pecks at the head [F(1, 34) ⫽ 12.226, p ⫽ 0.001], significant effects.
whereas use of the frontal field revealed a significant difference for

patches prevents (or disturbs) use of the frontal visual stranger, but they use both the left and right binocular
field and thus reveals only part of the true specialization fields. These findings may indicate better integration of
of the two eyes. the hemispheres, which could result because early experi-
ence with light allows better coordination of visual input
Unlike the pigeon, the chick has only one region of high from the two eyes.
ganglion-cell density, providing high-acuity vision, that is
directed primarily to the lateral monocular field [24, 25]. It is interesting to note that, in both D- and L-chicks,
This area of the retina of only the left eye is used before the left eye sends its input via the thalamofugal pathway
a peck is directed toward a stranger. An area of high but more strongly to the right hemisphere than to the left
somewhat lower ganglion-cell density extends away from hemisphere and via the fast relay system of the tecto-
this central region in the horizontal plane and just into fugal pathway also only to the right hemisphere (i.e., via
the temporal retina (i.e., it receives input from the frontal the rotundal-ectostriatal connections without collateral
field). This provides acuity vision into the binocular fields, branches [26]). (Note that it is likely that the slower sys-
but there is no overlap across the midline of the high- tem of the tectofugal pathway, via the unmyelinated ro-
acuity regions of each eye (see Figure 10 of [24]) unless tundal-ectostriatal projections with collateral branches
the eyes are converged. It is, therefore, likely that when that cross the midline, is not used for attack pecking [26].)
a chick fixates binocularly before pecking, it may be using Inputs from the right eye differ in D- and L-chicks. In
only the right half of the binocular field. We might there- L-chicks, the right eye sends inputs to both hemispheres
fore conclude that, before pecking a stranger, the chick via the thalamofugal pathway and, again, via the fast sys-
uses either the lateral field of its left eye (right hemi- tem of the tectofugal pathway to only the left hemisphere
sphere) or the right frontal field (left hemisphere or per- [27]. In contrast, the right eye of D-chicks sends inputs
haps both hemispheres if the eyes are converged). Thus, almost entirely to the left hemisphere via both systems.
it appears that there is a form of complementary specializa- The left and right frontal fields of D-chicks are, therefore,
tion of the eye fields, at least in the case of chicks incu- less integrated than those of L-chicks, and that could
bated in the dark. Chicks exposed to light also show explain why the frontal-field asymmetry is revealed only
preferential use of the left lateral field for pecking a in D-chicks.
Brief Communication 33

In conclusion, there is complementary specialization of a stroll through left and right animals’ perceptual worlds.
Brain and Language 2000, 73:189-219.
the left lateral and right frontal visual fields for fixation 20. Güntürkün O, Diekamp B, Manns M, Nottelmann F, Prior H, Schwarz
before pecking. These preferences make use of different A, et al.: Asymmetry pays: visual lateralization improves
discrimination success in pigeons. Curr Biol 2000, 10:1079-
high-acuity regions of each retina and, presumably, might 1081.
provide qualitatively different information to the visual 21. Vallortigara G, Andrew RJ: Lateralization of response to change
regions of the hemispheres. One might ask why eye (and in a model partner by chicks. Animal Behaviour 1991, 41:187-
194.
hemisphere) use is lateralized in this way, and the answer 22. Vallortigara G: Right hemisphere advantage for social
could lie in the specialization of the right hemisphere for recognition in the chick. Neuropsychologia 1992, 9:761-768.
23. McKenzie R, Andrew RJ, Jones RB: Lateralization in chicks and
processing topographical information [28] because this hens: new evidence for control of response by the right eye
might require use of the wide-angle, lateral field of the system. Neuropsychologia 1998, 36:51-58.
left eye. No such requirement would be demanded by 24. Ehrlich D: Regional specialization of the chick retina as
revealed by the size and density of neurons in the ganglion
the left hemisphere (and right eye) even though it, too, cell layer. J Comp Neurol 1981, 195:643-657.
directs agonistic pecking. 25. Morris VB: An afoveate area centralis in the chick retina. J
Comp Neurol 1982, 210:198-203.
26. Deng C, Rogers LJ: Bilaterally projecting neurons in the two
visual pathways of chicks. Brain Res 1998, 794:281-290.
References 27. Rogers LJ, Deng C: Light experience and lateralization of the
1. Dharmaretnam M, Andrew RJ: Age- and stimulus-specific use of two visual pathways in the chick. Behavioural Brain Res 1999,
right and left eyes by the domestic chick. Animal Behaviour 98:277-287.
1994, 48:1395-1406. 28. Rashid N, Andrew RJ: Right hemisphere advantage for
2. Evans CS, Evans L, Marler P: On the meaning of alarm calls: topographical orientation in the domestic chick.
functional references in the avian vocal system. Animal Neuropsychologia 1989, 27:937-948.
Behaviour 1993, 46:23-28.
3. Vallortigara G, Regolin L, Bortolomiol G, Tommasi L: Lateral
asymmetries due to preferences in eye use during visual
discrimination learning in chicks. Behavioural Brain Res 1996,
74:135-143.
4. Bisazza A, De Santi A, Vallortigara G: Laterality and cooperation:
mosquitofish move closer to a predator when the
companion is on their left side. Animal Behaviour 1999, 57:1145-
1149.
5. Sovrano V, Rainoldi C, Bisazza A, Vallortigara G: Roots of brain
specializations: preferential left-eye use during mirror-
image inspection in six species of teleost fish. Behavioural
Brain Res 1999, 106:175-180.
6. Deckel AW: Laterality of aggressive responses in Anolis.
Journal of Experimental Zoology 1995, 272:194-200.
7. Deckel AW: Hemispheric control of territorial aggression in
Anolis carolinensis: effects of mild stress. Brain Behaviour
and Evolution 1998, 51:33-39.
8. Rogers LJ: Light experience and asymmetry of brain function
in chickens. Nature 1982, 297:223-225.
9. Rogers LJ: Light input and the reversal of functional
lateralization in the chicken brain. Behavioural Brain Res
1990, 38:211-221.
10. Vallortigara G, Rogers LJ, Bisazza A: Possible evolutionary
origins of cognitive brain lateralization. Brain Res Reviews
1999, 30:164-175.
11. Vallortigara G, Regolin L, Pagni P: Detour behaviour, imprinting,
and visual lateralization in the domestic chick. Cognitive
Brain Res 1999, 7:307-320.
12. Robins A, Lippolis G, Bisazza A, Vallortigara G, Rogers LJ:
Lateralization of agonistic responses and hind-limb use in
toads. Animal Behaviour 1997, 56:875-881.
13. Vallortigara G, Rogers LJ, Bisazza A, Lippolis G, Robins A:
Complementary right and left hemifield use for predatory
and agonistic behaviour in toads. NeuroReport 1998, 9:3341-
3344.
14. Facchin L, Bisazza A, Vallortigara G: What causes lateralization
of detour behaviour in fish? Evidence for asymmetries in
eye use. Behavioural Brain Res 1999, 103:229-234.
15. Andrew RJ: The nature of behavioural lateralization in the
chick. In Neural and Behavioural Plasticity. Edited by Andrew RJ.
Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1991:536-554.
16. Güntürkün O: Avian visual lateralization: a review. Neuroreport
1997, 8:3-11.
17. Rogers LJ: Behavioral, structural and neurochemical
asymmetries in the avian brain: a model system for studying
visual development and processing. Neuroscience and
Biobehavioral Reviews 1996, 20:487-503.
18. Rogers LJ: Evolution of hemispheric specialization:
advantages and disadvantages. Brain and Language 2000,
73:236-253.
19. Vallortigara G: Comparative neuropsychology of the dual brain:

You might also like