Functional and Structural Indices of Empathy

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Neuropsychology © 2014 American Psychological Association

2015, Vol. 29, No. 3, 463– 472 0894-4105/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/neu0000155

Functional and Structural Indices of Empathy: Evidence for


Self-Orientation as a Neuropsychological Foundation of Empathy
Brick Johnstone, Dan Cohen, Kirk R. Bryant, Bret Glass, and Shawn E. Christ
University of Missouri

Objective: To evaluate a model that hypothesizes that empathy is associated with decreased right parietal
lobe (RPL)-related self-orientation (i.e., increased selflessness), which allows individuals to more easily
empathize with others. Methods: Participants: Thirty one individuals with documented neuroradiological
abnormalities due to traumatic brain injury (TBI) referred for clinical evaluations. Measures: Cerebral
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

integrity was measured with both functional (i.e., neuropsychological tests) and structural indices (i.e.,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

MRI). Participants were administered comprehensive neuropsychological tests associated with general
bilateral frontal, temporal, and parietal lobe functioning, a self-report measure of empathy (i.e., Penner’s
Prosocial Personality Battery), and an objective measure of empathy (i.e., Prisoner’s Dilemma). Twenty
participants also completed structural MRI analysis of the bilateral frontal, temporal, parietal, and insular
cortices measured in terms of volume. Results: Pearson correlations indicated that empathy was related
to increased neuropsychological indices of RPL and frontal lobe (primarily left frontal) functioning. The
only MRI indices associated with empathy were the bilateral insula. Neither functional nor structural
cerebral indices were significantly related to objective measures of empathy. Conclusions: Contrary to
hypotheses, empathy appears to be associated with increased RPL functioning. It is suggested that to
incorporate the experiences of others into the experience of the self (i.e., to be empathetic), one must have
an intact sense of the self.

Keywords: affective empathy, cognitive empathy, self-orientation, right parietal lobe, neuropsychology

Neuroanatomy of Empathy Peretz, 2004; Zaki & Ochsner, 2012). Considered together, these
studies implicate most of the brain in empathy, which is not
There has been growing interest in determining the neuroanat- surprising given its complexity. This variability in findings is
omical and neuropsychological foundations of empathy (hereby likely related to the lack of clear definitions of empathy, method-
defined as “the action of understanding, being aware of, being
ological weaknesses inherent in neuroradiological scanning (Logo-
sensitive to, and vicariously experiencing the feelings, thoughts,
thetis, 2008), and other methodological issues (e.g., use of self-
and experience of another”). A meta-analysis of empathy research
report vs. objective measures of empathy; “artificiality” of
concluded that empathy is a complex construct that is related to
objective empathy measures; structural vs. functional measures of
multiple neurologic networks and cognitive and emotional abilities
cerebral integrity; use of clinical and nonclinical samples; Zaki &
(Zaki & Ochsner, 2012). Specifically, empathy has been shown to
Ochsner, 2012).
be related to the inferior parietal lobe, temporoparietal junction,
The majority of research on the neurology of empathy has
anterior insula, posterior superior temporal sulcus, temporal pole,
involved neuroradiological evaluation of nonclinical populations
premotor cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, anterior cingulate cor-
engaged in objective computer-based tasks. However, determining
tex, medial prefrontal cortex, insula, right temporoparietal region,
associations between empathy and cerebral regions is complicated
and various regions of the frontal lobes (Banissy, Kanai, Walsh, &
given the numerous other cognitive processes that are engaged
Rees, 2012; Decety & Jackson, 2004; Decety & Jackson, 2006;
during objective tasks of empathy (e.g., attending to stimuli, lis-
Eslinger, 1998; Grattan, Bloomer, Archambault, & Eslinger, 1994;
tening to instructions, perceiving stimuli, cognitively processing
Grattan & Eslinger, 1989; Grattan & Eslinger, 1992; Mutschler,
economic games commonly used in empathy research, etc.; Logo-
Reinbold, Wankerl, Seifritz, & Ball, 2013; Preston & de Waal,
2002; Shamay-Tsoory, Tomer, Goldsher, Berger, & Aharon- thetis, 2008). As a result, the neuroanatomical basis of empathy
has been difficult to determine. An alternative research method has
involved the investigation of populations with brain dysfunction in
which persons with various lesions/disorders are compared with
This article was published Online First November 17, 2014. one another (e.g., persons with left vs. right frontal lobe lesions),
Brick Johnstone, Department of Health Psychology, University of Missouri; or to healthy control groups, in terms of empathy (Beadle &
Dan Cohen, Department of Religious Studies, University of Missouri; Kirk R.
Tranel, 2013; de Sousa et al., 2010; Eslinger, 1998; Grattan et al.,
Bryant, Department of Health Psychology, University of Missouri; Bret Glass,
1994; Grattan & Eslinger, 1989; Grattan & Eslinger, 1992; Rankin,
Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri; Shawn E.
Christ, Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri. Kramer, & Miller, 2005; Rankin et al., 2006; Shamay-Tsoory et
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Brick John- al., 2004; Shamay-Tsoory, Aharon-Peretz, & Perry, 2009). Using
stone, Department of Health Psychology, University of Missouri, DC116.88, this method, empathy can be associated with different cerebral
Columbia, MO 65212. E-mail: johnstoneg@health.missouri.edu areas (i.e., damage to areas associated with empathy will be
463
464 JOHNSTONE, COHEN, BRYANT, GLASS, AND CHRIST

indicative of decreased empathy). However, a weakness in such 2012) and seizures (Johnstone et al., 2014). These studies suggest
research is that only general relationships can be inferred given the that to forgive, one must be willing to decrease focus on the
diffuse nature of most brain disorders. Whereas the first method perceived wrong to the self.
may lead to overidentification of the cerebral areas/functions spe- Based on these studies, it is hypothesized that empathy may also
cifically associated with empathy, the latter one can only be used be related to decreased RPL-related self-orientation. Specifically,
to make general inferences regarding brain-empathy relationships. it is suggested that one must be selfless to experience spiritual
In fact, it has been suggested that the two methodologies are transcendence, be willing to forgive, and to be empathetic. This
appropriate complements to one another (Rorden & Karnath, hypothesis is also based on studies that indicate that the right
2004). hemisphere, and particularly the RPL, is primarily related to pro-
cessing information that is related to the self (Austin, 2009;
Northoff et al., 2006). Numerous studies have consistently dem-
Neuropsychology of Empathy
onstrated that the right hemisphere is related to several self-
In addition to identifying the cerebral areas associated with referential processes, such as recognizing pictures of one’s self and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

empathy, research has also focused on determining the specific processing autobiographical information/memories (Decety &
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

affective and cognitive components of empathy. Empathy has been Sommerville, 2003; Keenan, McCutcheon, & Pascual-Leone,
conceptualized as relating to numerous cognitive processes includ- 2001; Keenan, Nelson, O’Connor, & Pascual-Leone, 2001; Leigh
ing attention, memory, perspective taking, theory of mind, abstract et al., 2013; Lou et al., 2004; Platek, Wathne, Tierney, & Thom-
reasoning, cognitive flexibility, generational fluency, and set shift- son, 2008; Uddin, Molnar-Szakacs, Zaidel, & Iacoboni, 2006).
ing (Rankin et al., 2005). More concisely, empathy is generally The relationship between the RPL and self-orientation is also
conceptualized as involving two primary neuropsychological pro- supported by research with individuals with RPL dysfunction.
cesses including the ability to cognitively take the perspective of They often experience “disorders of the self,” including anosag-
others (i.e., cognitive empathy) and the ability to feel the emotions nosia and left-sided neglect, in which they have difficulties
of others as if they were one’s own (i.e., affective empathy; creating/processing a coherent sense of the physical and/or
Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2009; Shamay-Tsoory, 2011). Studies of psychic self (Feinberg & Keenan, 2005b; Feinberg & Keenan,
empathy involving persons with brain dysfunction have primarily 2005a; McGlynn & Schacter, 1989; Mesulam, 2000). When
focused on the associations between empathy and cognitive flex- considered together, these studies suggest that decreased RPL-
ibility (Grattan et al., 1994; Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2009; Grattan & related self-orientation (which can be conceptualized as in-
Eslinger, 1989), with suggestions that increased cognitive flexibil- creased “selflessness”) may also allow individuals to more
ity allows individuals to take the perspective of others. Other easily understand and relate to the experiences of others (i.e., to
studies suggest that empathy is related to declarative memory be empathetic).
(Beadle & Tranel, 2013), as well as empathetic concern (Rankin et
al., 2005; Rankin et al., 2006). Rationale for Current Study
It was hypothesized that decreased integrity of the RPL (which
Right Hemisphere, Self-Orientation, and Empathy is associated with decreased self-orientation) would be related to
Empathy has been hypothesized to be related to “self/other increased empathy. To address weaknesses in empathy research to
differentiation” and “self/other overlap” (Zaki & Ochsner, 2012). date, empathy was measured by subjective and objective measures,
However, the specific nature of such self/other neuropsychological and cerebral integrity was measured in terms of both functional
processes has not been fully elaborated. Recent research on other and structural indices. Although the focus of the study was on the
complex human experiences/traits (i.e., transcendence, forgive- relationship between the RPL and empathy, relationships among
ness) suggests that empathy may have its neuropsychological empathy and indices of other cerebral areas were also investigated
foundations in decreased self-orientation associated with the right to assist in clarifying the neurologic and neuropsychological foun-
hemisphere, and particularly the right parietal lobe (RPL). Re- dations of empathy.
search indicates that persons with increasing RPL dysfunction,
from either brain injuries or tumors, report increased spiritual Methods
transcendence (Johnstone, Bodling, Cohen, Christ, & Wegryzn,
2012; Johnstone & Glass, 2008; Urgesi, Aglioti, Skrap, & Fabbro, Participants
2010). This increased transcendence is hypothesized to be related
to decreased self-orientation, or what can be termed increased The sample included 31 individuals with TBI referred for out-
“selflessness.” These studies are further supported by single- patient neuropsychological evaluations at a Midwestern university.
photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) research on Bud- All participants had abnormal clinical radiologic evaluations of the
dhist monks and Franciscan nuns who demonstrate decreased RPL brain. Neuropsychological tests and self-report measures of empa-
activity during advanced spiritual practices while reporting in- thy were completed by all 31 participants. MRI data for 20
creased feelings of selflessness (Newberg, Alavi, Baime, Mozley, participants was obtained and analyzed for research purposes. Data
& d’Aquili, 1997; Newberg et al., 2001; Newberg, Pourdehand, were unavailable for the remaining participants due to either
Alavi, & d’Aquili, 2003). preexisting counterindications (e.g., metal in body; n ⫽ 7) or
Two recently published studies suggest that decreased RPL technical problems at the time of testing (n ⫽ 4). Chi-square and
functioning is also related to increased willingness to forgive in t tests conducted between the 20 participants with and 11 partic-
populations with both traumatic brain injury (TBI; Johnstone et al., ipants without MRI scanning indicated there were no significant
SELF-ORIENTATION AND EMPATHY 465

differences in neuropsychological, demographic, or outcome mea- Table 2


sures between the groups, other than marital status (with the group Sample Brain Injury Characteristics
not receiving MRI having a higher percentage of divorce).
The average age of the sample was 34.6 years (SD ⫽ 16.2; range ⫽ N Percent
17– 87). The mean Wechsler Adult Scale of Intelligence III, Full Scale Loss of Consciousness
IQ (WAIS-3 FSIQ) of the sample was in the average range (WAIS-3 Yes 18 58.1
FSIQ ⫽ 93.4; SD ⫽ 15.9; Wechsler, 1997a). The WAIS-3 FSIQ was No 5 16.1
not significantly correlated with any measure of empathy. Demo- Unknown/Missing 8 25.8
Retrograde Amnesia
graphic characteristics of the entire sample are presented in Table 1. Yes 9 29.0
TBI injury severity characteristics are presented in Table 2. No 0 0.0
Unknown/Missing 22 71.0
Posttraumatic Amnesia
Procedure Yes 14 45.2
No 0 0.0
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

All participants were referred for clinical neuropsychological


Unknown/Missing 17 54.8
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

evaluations and had evidence of abnormal neuroradiological eval- Effected Brain Regions on Imaging
uations at the time of their injuries. They were administered Left Frontal 9 29.0
measures of the functional integrity of the brain including intelli- Right Frontal 7 22.6
gence, memory, language, visual-spatial skills, attention, and Left Temporal 7 22.6
Right Temporal 7 22.6
sensory-motor skills by psychometricians (see Table 3). Partici-
Left Parietal 6 19.4
pants provided informed consent and completed subjective mea- Right Parietal 4 12.9
sures of empathy (see Table 4). Participants were then scheduled Occipital 2 6.5
for MRIs (i.e., measures of the structural integrity of the brain),
where they were screened for contraindications to scanning and
also completed an objective measure of empathy. Participants were
compensated $100. The present study was approved by the uni-
versity’s research review board.

Table 1 Measures of Empathy


Sample Demographics
Penner Prosocial Personality Battery (Penner, Fritzsche,
N Percent Craiger, & Freifeld, 1995). This measure was used to assess
several dimensions of empathy and has been used in several
Gender peer-reviewed studies (Finkelstein, Penner, & Brannick, 2005;
Male 21 67.7
Female 10 32.3 Penner, 2002; Penner & Finkelstein, 1998; Skoe, Cumberland,
Handedness Eisenberg, Hansen, & Perry, 2002). The Empathy scale assesses
Right 28 90.3 the ability to feel responsibility for and concern about the welfare
Left 3 9.7 of others. This scale is divided into four different scores, including
Ethnicity
White 28 90.0
a total empathy score, as well as individual scores for each of the
African American 1 3.2 following three dimensions of empathy: Empathetic Concern, Per-
Biracial 2 6.5 spective Taking, and Personal Distress. All questions are rated on
Marital Status
Married 9 29.0
Divorced 5 16.1
Separated 1 3.2 Table 3
Single 16 51.6 Neuropsychological Measure Descriptive Statistics
Education
Some HS 4 12.9 N Min. Max. Mean SD
HS Diploma 13 41.9
Some College 7 22.6 TMT (z score) 31 ⫺19.52 1.18 ⫺3.58 4.86
Bachelor’s Degree 3 9.7 WMS-3 LM I
Master’s Degree 3 9.7 (scaled score) 31 2.00 13.00 7.35 3.01
PhD/JD/MD 1 3.2 BVMT-R Delay
Employment Status (t score) 30 20.00 66.00 46.27 13.77
Currently Employed 12 38.7 JOLO (z score) 31 ⫺1.75 1.08 ⫺0.02 0.86
Student 5 16.1 Letter Fluency
Unemployed, not on disability 12 38.7 (scaled score) 31 3.00 19.00 8.42 3.85
Unemployed, on disability 1 3.2 Agnosia RH
Retired 1 3.2 (errors) 30 0 4 0.70 1.06
Annual Income Agnosia LH
⬍$20,000 25 80.6 (errors) 30 0 2 0.37 0.56
$20,001–$40,000 3 9.7
Note. TMT ⫽ Trail Making Test Part B; WMS-3 LM I ⫽ Wechsler
$40,001–$60,000 1 3.2
Memory Scale III Logical Memory I; BVMT-R ⫽ Benton Visual Memory
$60,001–$80,000 1 3.2
Test Revised; JOLO ⫽ Judgment of Line Orientation; RH ⫽ right hand;
⬎$80,000 1 3.2
LH ⫽ left hand.
466 JOHNSTONE, COHEN, BRYANT, GLASS, AND CHRIST

Table 4 trial. The total score is the amount of money earned over all trials.
Subjective and Objective Empathy Measures Higher dollar amounts are indicative of higher levels of coopera-
Descriptive Statistics tion (and theoretically empathy).

N Min. Max. Mean SD Functional and Structural Indices of Cerebral


Prosocial Personality Battery Integrity
Total Empathy 31 32 46 39.93 3.79
Perspective Taking 31 10 22 17.32 2.91 The current study evaluated both the functional and structural
Empathic Concern 31 12 19 15.61 2.08 integrity of gross cerebral structures that have been shown to be
Personal Distress 31 3 12 7.0 2.56 related to empathy (i.e., bilateral frontal, temporal, parietal, insular
Prisoner’s Dilemma Total Score 20 61 79 71.50 4.35 cortices). Both structural and functional indices of cerebral integ-
rity were used given that neuropsychological tests can provide
different information than neuroradiological techniques (Gomar et
al., 2011; Schmand et al., 2014). For example, cerebral abnormal-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

a five point scale, from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).


ities were detected in only 18% of MRI scans of persons with TBI,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

The score for each scale is the total number endorsed for each
compared with 40% of SPECT scans and 86% of magnetoen-
respective scale. The Total Empathy score is the sum of each of the
cephalography evaluations (Lewine et al., 2007).
following three subscales.
Empathetic concern. This subscale includes four questions
Neuropsychological Tests
and assesses the degree to which individuals are emotionally
concerned with the well-being of others. It includes questions such The following neuropsychological tests were administered as
as: “When I see someone being taken advantage of, I feel kind of indices of the general functional integrity of each of the following
protective toward them”; “I am often quite touched by things that cerebral lobes. Specifically, the tests were not used as indicators of
I see happen.” To be consistent with the terminology of previous specific cognitive functions, but rather to infer the functional
studies, Empathetic Concern is conceptualized as affective empa- integrity of the cerebral lobe that is generally associated with the
thy. Higher scores suggest more empathetic concern. Cronbach’s test (i.e., finger agnosia tests used as indices of parietal lobe
alpha for the measure was .67 for the original sample (Penner, functioning, rather than measures of tactile sensitivity; Lezak,
2002; n ⫽ 1,111). Howieson, & Loring, 2004). However, it is acknowledged that
Perspective taking. This subscale includes five questions and only general relationships could be inferred regarding empathy and
assesses the degree to which individuals can take the perspective neuroanatomical foundations, which was determined to be appro-
of others. It includes questions such as: “I sometimes try to priate given the unclear nature of the neurological foundations of
understand my friends better by imagining how things look from empathy (Zaki & Ochsner, 2012). For all tests, higher scores
their perspective”; “When I am upset at someone, I usually try to indicate more intact ability, other than for the finger agnosia tests
‘put myself in their shoes’ for a while.” To be consistent with the (i.e., higher scores equal worse performance). Age was controlled
terminology from previous studies, Perspective Taking is concep- for by calculating standard scores for all tests based on age, other
tualized as cognitive empathy. Higher scores suggest a greater than for finger agnosia.
capacity to take other’s perspective. Cronbach’s alpha for the RPL. The Judgment of Line Orientation (JOLO; Benton,
measure was .66 for the original sample (Penner, 2002; n ⫽ Hamsher, Varney, & Spreen, 1983) and left-hand finger agnosia
1,111). tests were used to infer RPL functioning. The JOLO is a measure
Personal distress. This subscale includes three questions and of spatial perception with the total score equaling the number
assesses the degree to which individuals have difficulties dealing correct of the 30 total items. For this study, the score is presented
with stressful situations. Example questions include: “I tend to lose as a z-score calculated according to normative data published in
control during emergencies”; “I am usually pretty effective in the test manual, with higher z -scores indicative of better perfor-
dealing with emergencies”; and “When I see someone who badly mance. The JOLO, and similar measures of the perception of
needs help in an emergency, I go to pieces.” Higher scores suggest angulation, have been associated with right hemisphere and par-
a higher degree of distress. Cronbach’s alpha for the measure was ticularly RPL functioning (Benton, Hannay, & Varney, 1975;
.77 for the original sample (Penner, 2002; n ⫽ 1,111). Lezak, Howieson, & Loring, 2004; Meador et al., 1993). Left-
Prisoner’s Dilemma task. This computer task that has been sided finger agnosia is a measure of tactile perception that is
used as an objective measure of empathy/altruism (Batson & associated with the RPL (Lezak et al., 2004). On this test, each of
Moran, 1999). Participants are engaged in a task in which they are the five fingers is touched four times and identified by the partic-
told they can cooperate with, or not cooperate with (i.e., defect), ipant, with the score equaling the total number of errors. More
another person (i.e., the same confederate face on the computer agnosia errors suggest greater RPL dysfunction.
screen for each trial), not knowing if the confederate will them- Left parietal lobe. Right-sided finger agnosia is a measure of
selves cooperate or defect. If the participant and confederate both tactile perception associated with the left parietal lobe (Lezak et
choose to cooperate, then they both earn $5. If the participant al., 2004), during which each of the five fingers is touched four
defects but the confederate cooperates, then the participant earns times and identified by the participant, with the score equaling the
$6 and the confederate $1. If the participant cooperates but the total number of errors. More agnosia errors suggest greater left
confederate defects, then the participant earns $1 and the confed- parietal dysfunction.
erate $6. The participants are administered 15 trials twice, and the Right temporal lobe. The Brief Visuospatial Memory Test
participant is informed of the confederate’s response after each Revised (BVMT-R; Benedict, 1997) is a test of visual memory that
SELF-ORIENTATION AND EMPATHY 467

has been associated with right temporal lobe functioning (Tranel & (the upsampling is required for use of BrainVoyager’s advanced
Damasio, 2002). It involves presenting the participant with three segmentation tools). BrainVoyager’s built-in segmentation pro-
trials during which they are visually exposed to six geometric cess, which uses local intensity histograms and computed gradient
figures for 10 s, after which they are asked to draw the figures from fields to adaptively calculate WM, GM, and cerebrospinal fluid
memory. The score is the total number of details recalled for all (CSF) boundaries, was then applied. Importantly, before proceed-
three trials, which is presented as a t score based on age-related ing, the resulting segmentations were visually inspected (slice-by-
normative data (average t score ⫽ 50, SD ⫽ 10). slice) for accuracy and manually corrected where necessary. Cor-
Left temporal lobe. The Wechsler Memory Scale III tical thickness measurements were calculated using a Laplace
(WMS-3) Logical Memory (LM) I subtest (Wechsler, 1997b) is a method (Jones, Buchbinder, & Aharon, 2000) as implemented
measure of verbal memory that has been associated with left
within BrainVoyager. For the current study, cortical volume was
temporal functioning (Tranel & Damasio, 2002). It involves read-
chosen as an index of structural cerebral integrity (i.e., cortical
ing two narrative stories to the participant that they are asked to
thickness ⫻ surface area), consistent to other neuroradiological
remember and repeat immediately after presentation. The score is
studies of empathy (Banissy et al., 2012; see Table 5).
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

the standard score according to age-related norms (i.e., M ⫽ 10,


Output from the previously described segmentation process was
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

SD ⫽ 3).
Left frontal lobe. The Delis Kaplan Executive Function Sys- also utilized to create cortical surface reconstructions of each
tem Letter Fluency Test (DKEFS; Delis & Kaplan, 2001) is a hemisphere for each participant. Anatomical alignment of these
measure of verbal fluency that has been associated with left frontal surface representations (and thereby the aforementioned cortical
lobe functioning (Johnstone, Leach, Hickey, Frank, & Rupright, thickness measurements) across participants was accomplished
1995). On this measure, participants state as many words as using BrainVoyager’s automatic cortex-based alignment (CBA)
possible in a 1-min period for three different letters. The total score process (Fischl, Sereno, Tootell, & Dale, 1999; Goebel, Esposito,
is the total number of words generated over the three trials, which & Formisano, 2006). CBA represents an iterative adaptive process
is expressed as a scaled score based on age-related normative data whereby curvature information (representing gyral and sulcal fold-
(M ⫽ 10, SD ⫽ 3). ing patterns) is used to align macroanatomical structures (gyri and
Frontal lobe. The Trail Making Test Part B (Reitan, 1992) sulci) of each participant’s brain to a standard reference brain
has been associated with general frontal lobe functioning (John- provided in BrainVoyager. Following alignment, average cortical
stone et al., 1995). It is a measure of divided attention (also thickness, surface area, and volume measurements were extracted
described as cognitive flexibility) that involves the participant for each major cortical region (frontal, parietal, temporal, insular)
completing a connect-the-dot test, alternating between a series of of each hemisphere (left, right) of each participant.
numbers and letters (i.e., 1 to A, A to 2, 2 to B, B to 3, etc.). The Analyses. Pearson product–moment correlations were con-
result is based on time to complete the measure. For this study, the ducted among the neuropsychological measures and the Penner
score is presented as a z score according to normative data (Hea- Prosocial Personality Battery Empathy scales, and the Prisoner’s
ton, Miller, Taylor, & Grant, 2004), with higher z scores indicative
Dilemma total score (see Table 6). It is noted that age was
of better performance.
accounted for in the analyses through the use of age-corrected
normative data for the neuropsychological measures, other than for
MRI finger agnosia measures. Partial-correlations using age as a cova-
Data acquisition. A 3T Siemens Trio MRI scanner with a riate were conducted among MRI-derived measures of cortical
standard eight-channel head coil was used to obtain high- volume and the Penner scores, and the Prisoner’s Dilemma total
resolution (1 mm3) T1-weighted structural images of the brain. score (see Table 7). Statistical significance was set at 0.05, ac-
Images were collected using a standard T1-weighted pulse se- knowledging weaknesses inherent in using the relatively small
quence (MP-RAGE sequence: repetition time [TR] ⫽ 1,920 ms, sample size.
echo time [TE] ⫽ 4 ms, flip angle ⫽ 8°, number of slices ⫽ 160,
resolution ⫽ 1 mm3).
Data processing and analysis. Data processing and analysis
was carried out using a surface-based approach as implemented
within BrainVoyager QX software (Brain Innovation, Maastricht, Table 5
The Netherlands). (For a more extensive description of the Brain- MRI Cortical Volume Descriptive Statistics
Voyager processing pipeline, see http://support.brainvoyager.com
or Geuze et al., 2008.) First, the structural MRI data for each N Min. Max. Mean SD
participant was rotated into anterior-posterior commissures (AC- Left Hemisphere
PC) coordinates. The skull and dural tissue were then removed by Left Temporal 20 30,628.05 53,710.81 42,241.44 5,425.12
manually deleting voxels containing non-neural tissue. Next, a Left Frontal 20 41,317.70 79,798.30 59,528.84 8,539.29
Sigma filter and BrainVoyager’s automatic intensity inhomogene- Left Parietal 20 23,028.63 37,681.60 31,215.89 3,722.84
Left Insula 20 6,319.09 9,724.08 8,121.69 900.11
ity correction tool (IIHC) were applied to enhance grey/white Right Hemisphere
matter (GM/WM) tissue contrast and correct for spatial intensity Right Temporal 19 30,052.48 47,168.65 40,602.12 4,330.72
inhomogeneities, respectively. Noncortical structures (e.g., ventri- Right Parietal 20 44,251.37 81,739.61 62,146.05 8,641.45
cles, subcortical nuclei) were then removed, and the resulting Right Frontal 20 23,802.39 34,333.18 28,024.57 3,026.48
Right Insula 20 8,123.95 11,413.30 9,915.95 894.49
image was upsampled from 1 to 0.5 mm3 using sinc interpolation
468 JOHNSTONE, COHEN, BRYANT, GLASS, AND CHRIST

Table 6 related to increased rather than decreased RPL functional integ-


Correlation Analyses Among Neuropsychological Indices and rity. The process of being empathetic apparently is not associated
Empathy Measures with a decreased focus on the self, which allows for an increased
focus on others. Instead, it appears that to empathize with others
Prisoner’s that it is necessary to have an intact and relatively strong sense of
Neuropsychological Total EC PT PD Dilemma
the self. That is, if one does not have an intact sense of the self,
Left Temporal then one cannot fully understand and emotionally relate to the
WMS-3 LM I 0.24 ⫺0.16 0.36ⴱ 0.14 0.28 experiences of others. As stated by Cozolino:
Left Frontal
DKEFS Letter Fluency 0.52ⴱ 0.42ⴱ 0.42ⴱ ⫺0.08 0.04 empathy is an attempt to experience the inner life of another while
Left Parietal retaining objectivity. In other words, we hold our own perspective in
Right Finger Agnosia 0.07 0.11 ⫺0.06 0.08 ⫺0.30
mind while simultaneously imagining what it is like to be the other. In
Right Temporal
BVMT-R total 0.03 ⫺0.24 0.09 0.12 0.07 order to have empathy, we need to maintain an awareness of our inner
Right Parietal world as we imagine the inner world of others. (Cozolino, 2006, p.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

JOLO 0.04 0.38ⴱ 0.12 ⫺0.49ⴱ 0.20 203)


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Left Finger Agnosia ⫺0.21 ⫺0.37ⴱ ⫺0.27 0.37ⴱ 0.15


Frontal Although the results were contrary to those hypothesized, they
Trail Making Test 0.35ⴱ ⫺0.01 0.39ⴱ 0.31 0.16 support others studies that suggest that relatively stronger right
Note. Total ⫽ Penner Prosocial Personality Total score; EC ⫽ Empa- hemisphere functioning, and particularly RPL functioning, is re-
thetic Concern; PT ⫽ Perspective Taking; PD ⫽ Personal Distress; Pris- lated to increased empathy (Decety & Batson, 2009; Grattan &
oner’s Dilemma ⫽ Prisoner’s Dilemma Total Earnings; WMS-3 LM I ⫽ Eslinger, 1989; Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2004; Shamay-Tsoory et al.,
Wechsler Memory Scale III Logical Memory I; BVMT-R ⫽ Benton Visual 2009). For example, Shamay-Tsoory and colleagues (2004) re-
Memory Test Revised; DKEFS ⫽ Delis Kaplan Executive Function
Scales; JOLO ⫽ Judgment of Line Orientation. ported that individuals with RPL lesions, but not left parietal lobe
ⴱ lesions, demonstrated significantly decreased empathy. Similarly,
p ⬍ .05.
Grattan and Eslinger (1989) found that the JOLO (i.e., the same
measure used in the current study to infer RPL dysfunction) was
significantly and positively related to empathy in a sample of
Results individuals with brain dysfunction (r ⫽ .30 vs. r ⫽ .38 in the
current study). However, they conceptualized this test as a measure
Neuropsychological Indices of perceptual judgment and did not mention this finding in the
Contrary to hypotheses, the results suggest that empathy is abstract given the focus of their study was on the frontal lobes. In
generally related to increased functional integrity of the RPL. contrast, in the current study, the JOLO was used to infer the
Specifically, measures of increased RPL functioning (i.e., JOLO functional integrity of the RPL. Whereas Grattan and Eslinger
and left-hand finger agnosia tests) were significantly correlated concluded that “impairments in visual-spatial perception could
with both increased affective empathy and less personal distress. potentially be a shared variable among the flexibility and empathy
Measures associated with frontal lobe functioning, and particu- measures,” it is suggested that it may be more appropriate to
larly the left frontal lobe (i.e., Trail Making Test and DKEFS interpret these tests as indicators of general RPL functional integ-
Letter Fluency subtest), were significantly and positively corre- rity. Based on this premise, it can then be inferred that empathy is
lated with cognitive empathy. significantly related to two measures of RPL integrity (i.e., JOLO,
A measure of left temporal lobe functioning (i.e., WMS-3 LM I left finger agnosia), rather than suggesting that empathy is related
scale) was significantly and positively associated with cognitive
empathy.
Table 7
No neuropsychological measures were related to Prisoner’s Di-
Correlation Analysis Among MRI Cortical Volume and
lemma scores.
Empathy Measures
MRI Indices Prisoner’s
MRI Brain Region Total EC PT PD Dilemma
Right and left insular cortices were the only cerebral structures
shown by MRI to be significantly related to subjective measures of Left Hemisphere
empathy (i.e., Total Empathy, Personal Distress). No MRI indices Left Temporal 0.32 0.18 0.19 0.05 0.04
Left Frontal 0.12 0.08 ⫺0.13 0.20 ⫺0.11
of cerebral integrity were significantly related to Prisoner’s Di- Left Parietal 0.33 0.27 0.25 ⫺0.04 0.04
lemma scores. Left Insula 0.48ⴱ 0.06 0.25 0.46ⴱ 0.15
Right Hemisphere
Right Temporal 0.12 ⫺0.10 ⫺0.13 0.26 ⫺0.07
Discussion Right Frontal ⫺0.15 ⫺0.17 ⫺0.23 0.03 ⫺0.12
Right Parietal ⫺0.02 ⫺0.04 ⫺0.02 ⫺0.07 0.12
Empathy and the RPL Right Insula 0.43ⴱ ⫺0.18 0.29 0.56ⴱ 0.04
Note. Total ⫽ Total Empathy; EC ⫽ Empathetic Concern; PT ⫽ Per-
The primary goal of the study was to determine if decreased spective Taking; PD ⫽ Personal Distress; Prisoner’s Dilemma ⫽ Prison-
RPL functioning was related to increased empathy. Contrary to the er’s Dilemma Total Earnings.

hypothesis, the results suggest that certain aspects of empathy are p ⬍ .05.
SELF-ORIENTATION AND EMPATHY 469

to perceptual judgment and left-sided tactile sensitivity. If concep- The lack of significant findings between empathy and other
tualized in this manner, it can then be inferred that other RPL cerebral areas does not suggest that empathy is solely based in the
functions, such as general self-orientation processes (Austin, 2009; insula. The lack of findings is more likely related to the fact that
Decety & Sommerville, 2003; Leigh et al., 2013; Lou et al., 2004; many individuals with even severe TBI have normal neuroradio-
Uddin et al., 2006; Feinberg & Keenan, 2005b, 2005a; McGlynn & logical findings (Brasure et al., 2012; Lobato et al., 1986). This
Schacter, 1989; Mesulam, 2000) are also likely to be diminished. suggests that quantitative structural MRI data may help differen-
Of note, the results also indicated that decreased RPL function- tiate groups with brain dysfunction for comparison (e.g., frontal vs.
ing (i.e., JOLO, left-sided finger agnosia) is associated with higher nonfrontal lesions; Grattan, Bloomer, Archambault, & Eslinger
levels of personal distress. That is, less intact RPL functioning 1994), but may not be appropriate for correlational analyses such
(and inferentially the sense of self) is associated with a decreased as those conducted in the current study. Regardless, the current
ability to keep composure under stressful situations (e.g., “I tend to results clearly suggest that future empathy research will benefit
lose control during emergencies”; “When I see someone who from focusing on the role of the insula.
badly needs help in an emergency, I go to pieces”). This suggests
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

that a stronger sense of the self allows individuals to retain com- Self-Report Versus Objective Measures of Empathy
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

posure in stressful situations and increase their ability to focus on


The results indicate that self-report measures of empathy (i.e.,
others. This finding is consistent with beliefs that one must have a
Penner’s Prosocial Personality measure) were significantly related
strong sense of the self to empathize with others (Cozolino, 2006).
to neuropsychological indices of cerebral integrity, although the
objective measure (i.e., Prisoner’s Dilemma) was not. This finding
Empathy and Other Cerebral may be reflective of the “artificiality” associated with using ex-
Structures/Neuropsychological Functions perimental tasks to measure empathy (Zaki & Ochsner, 2012), and
particularly for persons with TBI. Although subjective and objec-
In the current study, empathy (i.e., affective, cognitive, total)
tive measures of a construct may frequently be administered to-
was associated with frontal lobe functioning (and particularly the
gether to assure the construct validity of an attribute (e.g., empa-
left frontal lobe), consistent with several other studies (Decety &
thy), in this case the constructs measured by the Penner Prosocial
Batson, 2009; Decety & Jackson, 2004; Grattan et al., 1994;
Personality Measure and Prisoner’s Dilemma appear to be differ-
Grattan & Eslinger, 1989; Grattan & Eslinger, 1992; Rankin et al.,
ent.
2005; Rankin et al., 2006; Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2004; Shamay-
Tsoory et al., 2009). In general, current and previous results
Limitations and Future Directions
consistently suggest that cognitive flexibility associated with fron-
tal lobe functioning is necessary for individuals to take the cogni- It is acknowledged that empathetic processes are much more
tive perspective of others. complicated from neuroanatomical and neuropsychological per-
The results also support previous studies that indicate that the spectives than the conclusions presented in this article. However,
temporal lobes are involved in empathy (Decety & Batson, 2009; given the weaknesses in empathy research to date (Zaki &
Leigh et al., 2013; Rankin et al., 2006). It has been suggested that Ochsner, 2012), the current results can provide suggestions for
empathy may be related to the emotional processing that has been future research, and particularly how self-orientation relates to
shown to be related to both the temporal lobes and limbic system, empathy. The current study suggests that different neuropsycho-
given their close neural connectivity (Lee & Siegle, 2012; Lezak et logical processes associated with different neurological networks
al., 2004; Rankin et al., 2005). However, somewhat unexpectedly, interact in the experience of empathy (e.g., RPL-related self-
in the current study, cognitive empathy was associated with in- orientation; frontal lobe-related cognitive flexibility; temporal
creased left temporal lobe functioning, whereas affective empathy lobe-related emotional processing; insula-related processing of
was not. If anything, it could be hypothesized that affective em- internal physical states and social emotions).
pathy would be related to temporal lobe functioning. The rationale To date researchers have generally conceptualized empathy as
for this finding is unclear. involving processes such as “self/other discrimination,” “self-
reflection,” “self/other overlap,” and “theory of mind” (Shamay-
Tsoory et al., 2009; Zaki & Ochsner, 2012). However, these terms
Empathy and Structural Indices of Cerebral Integrity
are relatively nonspecific and such terminology may not best
The results indicate that the bilateral insular cortices were the explain the manner by which individuals incorporate the experi-
only structural indices that were significantly associated with ences of others into the experience of the self. A more nuanced
empathy (i.e., Total Empathy and Personal Distress). This finding explanation for the manner by which specific dimensions of self-
is generally consistent with several other studies that implicate the orientation relate to empathy is needed. Just as “language” can be
insula in the experience of empathy (Banissy et al., 2012; used as a general umbrella term for other more specific commu-
Mutschler et al., 2013). It is speculated that the insula may be nication skills (e.g., expression, comprehension, semantics, syntax,
related to empathy given that it has been associated with emotional etc.), “self-orientation” may be best conceptualized as a general
processing (and particularly negative emotions such as personal umbrella descriptor for more specific self-oriented processes (e.g.,
distress), awareness of internal bodily states (i.e., self-orientation autobiographical memory, body schema perception, self-
of internal states), and connecting physiological states of the body awareness, self-recognition, self-evaluation, etc.). In fact, a general
with social emotions such as empathy (Craig, 2009; Critchley, model of self-orientation has been proposed, with suggestions that
Wiens, Rotshtein, Ohman, & Dolan, 2004; Karnath, Baier, & the self can be conceptualized in terms of a physical self, mental
Nägele, 2005). self, and autobiographical self (Northoff et al., 2006).
470 JOHNSTONE, COHEN, BRYANT, GLASS, AND CHRIST

Further research regarding the expression of increased and de- Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feel—Now? The anterior insula and
creased self-orientation processes is also warranted given the mul- human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 59 –70. http://dx
tiple studies that suggest that decreased RPL functioning (as mea- .doi.org/10.1038/nrn2555
sured by the JOLO and/or finger agnosia tests) is associated with Critchley, H. D., Wiens, S., Rotshtein, P., Ohman, A., & Dolan, R. J.
(2004). Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nature Neu-
increased transcendence (Johnstone et al., 2012; Johnstone &
roscience, 7, 189 –195. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1176
Glass, 2008) and increased willingness to forgive (Johnstone et al., Decety, J., & Batson, C. D. (2009). Empathy and morality: Integrating
2012; Johnstone et al., 2014), but decreased empathy (Grattan et social and neuroscience approaches. In J. Braeckman, J. Verplaetse, & J.
al., 1994; current study). Future research may benefit from deter- De Schrijver (Eds.), The moral brain (pp. 109 –127). Berlin: Springer-
mining how increased and decreased self-orientation may be re- Verlag. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4020-6287-2_5
lated to other human experiences (e.g., altruism, antisocial person- Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of lesion
ality, etc.), as well as the manner in which self- and other-oriented empathy. Behavior, Cognition, & Neuroscience, 3, 71–100.
neuropsychological processes interact in different human experi- Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2006). A social-neuroscience perspective on
ences/relationships (Austin, 2009; Fox et al., 2005; Ochsner et al., empathy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 54 –58.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00406.x
2004).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Decety, J., & Sommerville, J. A. (2003). Shared representations between


A primary limitation of the current study is the relatively small
self and other: A social cognitive neuroscience view. Trends in Cogni-
sample size, as the use of a larger sample may lead to a more clear tive Sciences, 7, 527–533. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.004
understanding of the specific nature of empathy (e.g., allow for Delis, D., & Kaplan, E. (2001). Delis Kaplan Executive Function System.
investigation of more specific brain regions). The results are fur- San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
ther limited in that the current sample included persons with TBI, de Sousa, A., McDonald, S., Rushby, J., Li, S., Dimoska, A., & James, C.
which is an injury that typically involves generalized impairment (2010). Why don’t you feel how I feel? Insight into the absence of
throughout the brain (Lezak et al., 2004). As a result, only general empathy after severe traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia, 48,
inferences can be drawn regarding the neuroanatomical and neu- 3585–3595. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.08.008
ropsychological bases of empathy. Relatedly, the current study is Eslinger, P. J. (1998). Neurological and neuropsychological bases of em-
pathy. European Neurology, 39, 193–199. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/
limited as the MRI data involving cortical volume was viewed by
000007933
examining large regions as a whole (i.e., left frontal lobe) rather Feinberg, T. E., & Keenan, J. P. (2005a). The lost self: Pathologies of the
than examining more specific regions within each brain region that brain and identity. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. http://dx
have been suggested to be implicated in different dimensions of .doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195173413.001.0001
empathy (Eslinger, 1998; Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2009). Not focus- Feinberg, T. E., & Keenan, J. P. (2005b). Where in the brain is the self?
ing on more specific brain regions may have decreased the strength Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 14, 661– 678.
of the observed relationships and may have contributed to the http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2005.01.002
insignificant MRI findings. Finkelstein, M. A., Penner, L. A., & Brannick, M. T. (2005). Motive, role
identity, and prosocial personality as predictors of volunteer activity.
Social Behavior and Personality, 33, 403– 418. http://dx.doi.org/
References 10.2224/sbp.2005.33.4.403
Fischl, B., Sereno, M. I., Tootell, R. B., & Dale, A. M. (1999). High-
Austin, J. H. (2009). Selfless insight. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical
Banissy, M. J., Kanai, R., Walsh, V., & Rees, G. (2012). Inter-individual surface. Human Brain Mapping, 8, 272–284. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
differences in empathy are reflected in human brain structure. Neuro- (SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:4⬍272::AID-HBM10⬎3.0.CO;2-4
Image, 62, 2034 –2039. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05 Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D. C.,
.081 & Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into
Batson, C. D., & Moran, T. (1999). Empathy induced altruism in a dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proceedings of the Na-
prisoner’s dilemma. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 909 – tional Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102,
924. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(199911)29:7⬍909:: 9673–9678. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0504136102
AID-EJSP965⬎3.0.CO;2-L Geuze, E., Westenberg, H. G. M., Heinecke, A., de Kloet, C. S., Goebel,
Beadle, J. N., & Tranel, D. (2013). Empathy in hippocampal amnesia. R., & Vermetten, E. (2008). Thinner prefrontal cortex in veterans with
Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 091. posttraumatic stress disorder. NeuroImage, 41, 675– 681. http://dx.doi
Benedict, R. (1997). Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised professional .org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.007
manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Goebel, R., Esposito, F., & Formisano, E. (2006). Analysis of functional
Benton, A. L., Hamsher, K. D., Varney, N. R., & Spreen, O. (1983). image analysis contest (FIAC) data with BrainVoyager QX: From
Judgement of line orientation manual. New York, NY: Oxford Univer- single-subject to cortically aligned group general linear model analysis
sity Press. and self-organizing group independent component analysis. Human
Benton, A., Hannay, H. J., & Varney, N. R. (1975). Visual perception of Brain Mapping, 27, 392– 401. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20249
line direction in patients with unilateral brain disease. Neurology, 25, Gomar, J. J., Bobes-Bascaran, M. T., Conejero-Goldberg, C., Davies, P.,
907–910. http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/WNL.25.10.907 Goldberg, T. E., & the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative.
Brasure, M., Lamberty, G. J., Sayer, N. A., Nelson, N. W., MacDonald, R., (2011). Utility of combinations of biomarkers, cognitive markers, and
Ouellette, J., . . . Wilt, T. J. (2012). Multidisciplinary postacute rehabil- risk factors to predict conversion from mild cognitive impairment to
itation for moderate to severe traumatic brain injury in adults. Agency for Alzheimer disease in patients in the Alzheimer’s disease neuroimaging
Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ) Comparative Effectiveness initiative. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68, 961–969. http://dx.doi
Reviews, 72, ES1–ES20. .org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.96
Cozolino, L. (2006). The neuroscience of human relationships. New York, Grattan, L. M., Bloomer, R. H., Archambault, F. X., & Eslinger, P. J.
NY: Norton. (1994). Cognitive flexibility and empathy after frontal lobe lesions.
SELF-ORIENTATION AND EMPATHY 471

Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, & Behavioral Neurology, 7, 251– Logothetis, N. K. (2008). What we can do and what we cannot do with
259. fMRI. Nature, 453, 869 – 878. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature06976
Grattan, L. M., & Eslinger, P. J. (1989). Higher cognition and social Lou, H. C., Luber, B., Crupain, M., Keenan, J. P., Nowak, M., Kjaer,
behavior: Changes in cognitive flexibility and empathy after cerebral T. W., . . . Lisanby, S. H. (2004). Parietal cortex and representation of
lesions. Neuropsychology, 3, 175–185. the mental Self. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
Grattan, L. M., & Eslinger, P. J. (1992). Long-term psychological conse- United States of America, 101, 6827– 6832. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/
quences of childhood frontal lobe lesion in patient DT. Brain and pnas.0400049101
Cognition, 20, 185–195. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0278-2626 McGlynn, S. M., & Schacter, D. L. (1989). Unawareness of deficits in
(92)90068-W neuropsychological syndromes. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Heaton, R. K., Miller, W., Taylor, M. J., & Grant, I. (2004). Revised Neuropsychology, 11, 143–205. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead-Reitan Battery. Lutz, 01688638908400882
FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Meador, K. J., Moore, E. E., Nichols, M. E., Abney, O. L., Taylor, H. S.,
Johnstone, B., Bayan, S., Gutierrez, L., Lardizabal, D., Lanigar, S., Yoon, Zamrini, E. Y., & Loring, D. W. (1993). The role of cholinergic systems
D. P., & Judd, K. (2014). Neuropsychological correlates of forgiveness. in visuospatial processing and memory. Journal of Clinical and Exper-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Religion, Brain & Behavior. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/2153599X.2014 imental Neuropsychology, 15, 832– 842.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

.880069 Mesulam, M.-M. (2000). Behavioral neuroanatomy. In M.-M. Mesulam


Johnstone, B., Bodling, A., Cohen, D., Christ, S. E., & Wegrzyn, A. (Ed.), Principles of behavioral and cognitive neurology (2nd ed.). New
(2012). Right parietal lobe-related “selflessness” as the neuropsycholog- York, NY: Oxford Press.
ical basis of spiritual transcendence. International Journal for the Psy- Mutschler, I., Reinbold, C., Wankerl, J., Seifritz, E., & Ball, T. (2013).
chology of Religion, 22, 267–284. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10508619 Structural basis of empathy and the domain general region in the anterior
.2012.657524 insular cortex. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 177. http://dx.doi
Johnstone, B., & Glass, B. A. (2008). Evaluation of a neuropsychological .org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00177
model of spirituality in persons with traumatic brain injury. Zygon, 43, Newberg, A. B., Alavi, A., Baime, M., Mozley, P. D., & d’Aquili, E. G.
861– 874. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9744.2008.00964.x (1997). The measurement of cerebral blood flow during the complex
cognitive task of mediation using HMPAO-SPECT imaging. Journal of
Johnstone, B., Leach, L. R., Hickey, M. L., Frank, R. G., & Rupright, J.
Nuclear Medicine, 38, 95.
(1995). Some objective measurements of frontal lobe deficits following
Newberg, A., Alavi, A., Baime, M., Pourdehnad, M., Santanna, J., &
traumatic brain injury. Applied Neuropsychology, 2, 24 –28. http://dx.doi
d’Aquili, E. (2001). The measurement of regional cerebral blood flow
.org/10.1207/s15324826an0201_4
during the complex cognitive task of meditation: A preliminary SPECT
Jones, S. E., Buchbinder, B. R., & Aharon, I. (2000). Three-dimensional
study. Psychiatry Research, 106, 113–122. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
mapping of cortical thickness using Laplace’s equation. Human Brain
S0925-4927(01)00074-9
Mapping, 11, 12–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1097-0193(200009)11:
Newberg, A., Pourdehnad, M., Alavi, A., & d’Aquili, E. G. (2003).
1⬍12::AID-HBM20⬎3.0.CO;2-K
Cerebral blood flow during meditative prayer: Preliminary findings and
Karnath, H. O., Baier, B., & Nägele, T. (2005). Awareness of the functioning
methodological issues. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 97, 625– 630. http://
of one’s own limbs mediated by the insular cortex? The Journal of Neuro-
dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.2003.97.2.625
science, 25, 7134 –7138. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI
Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greck, M., Bermpohl, F., Dobrowolny, H., &
.1590-05.2005
Panksepp, J. (2006). Self-referential processing in our brain—A meta-
Keenan, J. P., McCutcheon, N. P., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2001). Functional
analysis of imaging studies on the self. NeuroImage, 31, 440 – 457.
magnetic resonance imaging and event-related potentials suggest right
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.002
prefrontal activation for self-related processing. Brain and Cognition, Ochsner, K. N., Knierim, K., Ludlow, D. H., Hanelin, J., Ramachandran,
47, 87–91. T., Glover, G., & Mackey, S. C. (2004). Reflecting upon feelings: An
Keenan, J. P., Nelson, A., O’Connor, M., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2001). fMRI study of neural systems supporting the attribution of emotion to
Self-recognition and the right hemisphere. Nature, 409, 305. http://dx self and other. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1746 –1772.
.doi.org/10.1038/35053167 http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/0898929042947829
Lee, K. H., & Siegle, G. J. (2012). Common and distinct brain networks Penner, L. A. (2002). Dispositional and organizational influences on sus-
underlying explicit emotional evaluation: A meta-analytic study. Social tained volunteerism: An interactionist perspective. Journal of Social
Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 521–534. http://dx.doi.org/ Issues, 58, 447– 467. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1540-4560.00270
10.1093/scan/nsp001 Penner, L. A., & Finkelstein, M. A. (1998). Dispositional and structural
Leigh, R., Oishi, K., Hsu, J., Lindquist, M., Gottesman, R. F., Jarso, S., . . . determinants of volunteerism. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Hillis, A. E. (2013). Acute lesions that impair affective empathy. Brain: chology, 74, 525–537. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.525
A Journal of Neurology, 136, 2539 –2549. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ Penner, L. A., Fritzsche, B. A., Craiger, J. P., & Freifeld, T. S. (1995).
brain/awt177 Measuring the prosocial personality. In J. Butcher & C. D. Spielberger
Lewine, J. D., Davis, J. T., Bigler, E. D., Thoma, R., Hill, D., Funke, M., (Eds.), Advances in personality assessment (Vol. 10). Hillsdale, NJ:
. . . Orrison, W. W. (2007). Objective documentation of traumatic brain LEA.
injury subsequent to mild head trauma: Multimodal brain imaging with Platek, S. M., Wathne, K., Tierney, N. G., & Thomson, J. W. (2008).
MEG, SPECT, and MRI. The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, Neural correlates of self-face recognition: An effect-location meta-
22, 141–155. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.HTR.0000271115.29954.27 analysis. Brain Research, 1232, 173–184. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsycho- .brainres.2008.07.010
logical assessment (4th ed.). New York, NY: Oxford Press. Preston, S. D., & de Waal, F. B. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and
Lobato, R. D., Sarabia, R., Rivas, J. J., Cordobes, F., Castro, S., Muñoz, proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 1–20.
M. J., . . . Lamas, E. (1986). Normal computerized tomography scans in Rankin, K. P., Gorno-Tempini, M. L., Allison, S. C., Stanley, C. M., Glenn,
severe headinjury. Prognostic and clinical management implications. S., Weiner, M. W., & Miller, B. L. (2006). Structural anatomy of
Journal of Neurosurgery, 65, 784 –789. http://dx.doi.org/10.3171/jns empathy in neurodegenerative disease. Brain: A Journal of Neurology,
.1986.65.6.0784 129, 2945–2956. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl254
472 JOHNSTONE, COHEN, BRYANT, GLASS, AND CHRIST

Rankin, K. P., Kramer, J. H., & Miller, B. L. (2005). Patterns of cognitive Skoe, E. A., Cumberland, A., Eisenberg, N., Hansen, K., & Perry, J.
and emotional empathy in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Cognitive (2002). The influences of sex and gender-role identity on moral cogni-
and Behavioral Neurology, 18, 28 –36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.wnn tion and prosocial personality traits. Sex Roles, 46, 295–309. http://dx
.0000152225.05377.ab .doi.org/10.1023/A:1020224512888
Reitan, R. M. (1992). Trail making test: Manual for administration and Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (2002). Neurobiological foundations of
scoring. Tucson, AZ: Reitan Neuropsychological Laboratory. human memory. In A. D. Baddeley, M. D. Kopelman, & B. A. Wilson
Rorden, C., & Karnath, H.-O. (2004). Using human brain lesions to infer (Eds.), The handbook of memory disorders (2nd ed., pp. 17–56). Chich-
function: A relic from a past era in the fMRI age? Nature Reviews ester: Wiley.
Neuroscience, 5, 812– 819. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1521 Uddin, L. Q., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Zaidel, E., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). rTMS
Schmand, B., Rienstra, A., Tamminga, H., Richard, E., van Gool, W. A., to the right inferior parietal lobule disrupts self-other discrimination.
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 65–71. http://dx.doi.org/
Caan, M. W., & Majoie, C. B. (2014). Responsiveness of magnetic
10.1093/scan/nsl003
resonance imaging and neuropsychological assessment in memory clinic
Urgesi, C., Aglioti, S. M., Skrap, M., & Fabbro, F. (2010). The spiritual
patients. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 40, 409 – 418.
brain: Selective cortical lesions modulate human self-transcendence.
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2011). The neural bases for empathy. The Neuro-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Neuron, 65, 309 –319. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.026


scientist, 17, 18 –24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073858410379268 Wechsler, D. (1997a). WAIS III. Administration and scoring manual. San
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Aharon-Peretz, J., & Perry, D. (2009). Two systems Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
for empathy: A double dissociation between emotional and cognitive Wechsler, D. (1997b). WMS-III. Administration and scoring manual. San
empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 132, 617– 627. http://dx.doi.org/ Zaki, J., & Ochsner, K. N. (2012). The neuroscience of empathy: Progress,
10.1093/brain/awn279 pitfalls and promise. Nature Neuroscience, 15, 675– 680. http://dx.doi
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Tomer, R., Goldsher, D., Berger, B. D., & Aharon- .org/10.1038/nn.3085
Peretz, J. (2004). Impairment in cognitive and affective empathy in
patients with brain lesions: Anatomical and cognitive correlates. Journal Received March 7, 2014
of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26, 1113–1127. http:// Revision received July 24, 2014
dx.doi.org/10.1080/13803390490515531 Accepted September 22, 2014 䡲

You might also like