Book Review: Act II: Confrontation

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 2

10.

1177_17470218211013677
book-review2021
QJP0010.1177/17470218211013677Quarterly Journal of Experimental PsychologyBook review

Book review

Quarterly Journal of Experimental

Book review Psychology


2021, Vol. 74(7) 1315­–1316
© Experimental Psychology Society 2021
Article reuse guidelines:
sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/17470218211013677
https://doi.org/10.1177/17470218211013677
qjep.sagepub.com

Paul Gulino and Connie Shears. The Science of Screenwriting: The characters who make questionable decisions (to put it
Neuroscience Behind Storytelling Strategies. London: Bloomsbury, mildly), Silver Linings Playbook is presented as a study in
2018, 164 pp., £16.99, ISBN 9781501327254 (pbk). conflict. Third, a simple summary, along with some thought
experiments and exercises, round off most chapters. These
Reviewed by: Robin SS Kramer and Abi MB Davis, School support readers in their understanding of the concepts pre-
of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Lincoln, UK sented and provide “homework” aimed at developing some
of the techniques discussed.
As avid film fans, and taking inspiration from the authors’
discussion of how best to structure a script (Chapter 9), we
Act II: confrontation
have decided to present this review in line with Field’s (2005)
“Three Act Structure.” So let’s dim the lights, open the cur- Combining psychology and screenwriting surely makes for
tains, and sit back with our overpriced confectionaries . . . interesting and entertaining subject matter. However, this
combination also represents the book’s biggest challenge.
What kind of audience will enjoy and/or gain the most from
Act I: setup reading it? The authors seem to target screenwriters and
By joining forces, a cognitive psychologist and a professor filmmakers, acknowledging that upon completion of the
of screenwriting aimed to shed some much needed light on book, readers will be “more interested in success in writing
the science behind storytelling. In recent years, scientists a screenplay or making a film than in continuing any scien-
have learned a lot about the psychology and physiology tific inquiry” (p. 157). Although personally, our background
that make humans particularly attracted to film and televi- is in psychological research, we suspect that any informa-
sion. It must be noted that our species did not specifically tion such readers would gain is fairly rudimentary (and
evolve to try and make sense of static images displayed in likely covered in more detail in other texts, frequently cited
rapid succession before our eyes. But then we also didn’t by the authors). And as experimental psychologists, the sci-
evolve to eat puffed up kernels of corn drowning in butter entific coverage is somewhat unsatisfactory.
and/or salt. On both counts, we have certainly adapted well To give an example, filmmakers know that conflict and
to our environment. Indeed, this ability to confuse the drama are vital ingredients that hold the attention of view-
brain (and entice it with the perfect combination of sweet ers (Chapter 7). As viewers, we like to see how characters
and salty) is precisely why films work so well on us. We will overcome obstacles to achieve their goals—think of
can be transported beyond the mundane, to other worlds the initial rivalry between Woody and Buzz in Toy Story.
and times, and it would seem that we’re very much built to However, the authors approach the scientific underpin-
enjoy such things. nings of this concept through the example of the classic
This book embraces the overlap between science and Müller-Lyer illusion, where lines appear to differ in length
screenwriting, with the goal of applying what we know due to the arrows featured at their ends. This is quite the
about psychology to the process of making a better film. For stretch in terms of relevance. A second illustration of this
example, how can screenwriters make sure that viewers mismatch can be found in Chapter 3, where the authors
connect with the main character (Chapter 2) and don’t lose discuss the spectacle enjoyed by viewers at the beginning
interest along the way (Chapter 6)? To this end, the book is of Star Wars when a tiny spacecraft is pursued by a giant
successful in a number of ways. First, it is surprisingly short, Imperial Stardestroyer. Here, the scientific explanation is
and perhaps as a result, is very readable. The light-hearted couched in terms of photoreceptors housed in the retina,
writing style emphasises the authors’ enjoyment of the sub- firing as a result of changes in light levels. Although inter-
ject matter, and readers might easily consume the material in esting to the inquiring non-scientist, we would very much
one sitting. Second, the book is filled with plenty of exam- have preferred more high-level and in-depth explorations
ples to illustrate each point, taken from both television and of psychological explanations which better address why
film. While One Flew Over the Cuckoo’s Nest and Breaking such phenomena result in some of the most memorable
Bad suitably epitomise the difficulties with presenting main scenes in film history.
1316 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 74(7)

This criticism applies throughout the book. Where sci- Act III: resolution
entific parallels were drawn, we always felt like more rel-
evant literature could have been found. Let’s consider Don’t get us wrong, we both enjoyed reading this book. The
Chapter 2. Here, the authors tackle the concept of the audi- nuances regarding filmmaking were both interesting and
ence’s connection with the main character, which might be informative, and some of the screenwriting concepts were
particularly difficult if screenwriters have opted for a less new to us as psychologists. The problem for scientists is the
traditional “nonhero” (someone we wouldn’t typically level at which the content is pitched, along with the target
admire and who lacks many positive qualities). The authors audience. Our recommendation is that this book best serves
suggest that first impressions are important so it’s best to screenwriters with some scientific curiosity. However, we
introduce the character when in between transgressions. don’t think that they will learn to take full advantage of the
Gulino and Shears argue that our connection to the charac- brain’s inner workings. But they will enjoy an evening or
ter is brought about by our evolved mechanisms that cause two with a fun read. And perhaps slightly higher scores on
them to be “adopted” by us as “family” (our in-group) via the next pub quiz. For those of us with a background in psy-
survival instincts (p. 33). Problematically, no specific evi- chology and an interest in films or screenwriting, we sug-
dence is presented to support these ideas. Alternatively, we gest approaching this book with an open mind and don’t
as psychologists might have incorporated more directly expect too much. The information won’t revolutionise your
related findings about how first impressions of social traits thinking but the delivery will amuse and satisfy. Think Two
are unavoidable, and once formed, are difficult to inhibit Weeks Notice rather than Inception.
(e.g., Goller et al., 2018; Ritchie et al., 2017). Or that sim-
ply being exposed to any character over time may result in ORCID iD
a liking or preference for that person (termed the “mere Robin SS Kramer https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8339-8832
exposure effect”—Zajonc, 1968; which we know applies
to faces too—Kramer & Parkinson, 2005). Or that others References
disclosing personal information about themselves results Collins, N. L., & Miller, L. C. (1994). Self-disclosure and liking: A
in our increased liking of them (Collins & Miller, 1994). meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 116(3), 457–475.
Again, we feel like the authors missed out on the opportu- Field, S. (2005). Screenplay: The foundations of screenwriting.
nity to present more relevant research. Random House.
Our final criticism will depend entirely on how enam- Goller, J., Leder, H., Cursiter, H., & Jenkins, R. (2018).
oured the reader is with the Star Wars franchise. From our Anchoring effects in facial attractiveness. Perception,
perspective, we found the book to be inexplicably focussed 47(10–11), 1043–1053.
Kramer, R. S. S., & Parkinson, B. (2005). Generalization of mere
on these works. Along with mentions in virtually every
exposure to faces viewed from different horizontal angles.
chapter, the authors chose to spend all of Chapter 10 (16%
Social Cognition, 23(2), 125–136.
of the book!) taking us through a literal “blow by blow” Ritchie, K. L., Palermo, R., & Rhodes, G. (2017). Forming
account of the first film, framed as an illustration of many impressions of facial attractiveness is mandatory. Scientific
of the concepts discussed throughout the earlier chapters. Reports, 7, 469.
For us, this was too much, and we sympathise with those Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposures. Journal
readers who actively disliked the films. of Personality and Social Psychology, 9(2, Pt. 2), 1–27.

You might also like