Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

Australasian Marketing Journal 20 (2012) 282–296

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Australasian Marketing Journal


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/amj

A critical review of classical conditioning effects on consumer behavior


Chanthika Pornpitakpan
University of Macau, Taipa, Macau, China

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This paper reviews extant research in classical conditioning effects in consumer behavior and advertising
Received 15 July 2011 contexts to determine whether they are real or illusory. The empirical results reveal that in cases where
Revised 14 June 2012 classical conditioning effects were found, they could be countermined by the deficiencies in research
Accepted 3 July 2012
methodologies, demand artifacts, the mediating role of contingency awareness, or some alternative
Available online 28 July 2012
mechanisms. In cases where the effects were not observed, the failure could be attributed to violations
of the conditions for classical conditioning to occur or absence of contingency and demand awareness.
Keywords:
It is concluded that thus far there has been no convincing evidence for classical conditioning effects on
Classical conditioning
Pavlovian conditioning
consumer behavior. Suggestions for future research in this area are presented.
Literature review Ó 2012 Australian and New Zealand Marketing Academy. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction and De Houwer, 1995; Baeyens et al., 1998; Hammerl and Grabitz,
1996). In the signal-learning notion of classical conditioning, an
Inspired by classical conditioning principles, many ads show the organism engages a higher cognitive process and learns the ‘‘if-
advertised product together with celebrities or pleasant stimuli (ob- then’’ relationship between the CS and the US (Rescorla and
jects, scenes, persons, and so forth) once or several times with a Wagner, 1972). Repeated pairings of the two stimuli to be associ-
hope that positive feelings from those stimuli will transfer to the ated is essential in establishing and strengthening their associative
product and thus inducing its liking. Classical conditioning has been link (Martindale, 1991). Through a signal learning process
generally accepted in consumer behavior literature as a mechanism (Rescorla, 1988), increased repetition of the pairing of two stimuli
producing advertising effects (Schiffman and Kanuk, 2010), as a pos- fortifies confidence that the presence of one stimulus predicts the
sible mechanism in the peripheral route of persuasion (Edell and presence of the other. The contingency or statistical correlation
Burke, 1984; Petty et al., 1983), and as pertinent in passive con- between the CS and the US is an important determinant of signal
sumption context (Gorn, 1982; Greenwald and Leavitt, 1984). learning.
According to the classical conditioning model of learning, which Evaluative (attitude) conditioning, on the other hand, concerns
is based on Pavlov’s (1927) work, an unconditioned stimulus (here- the acquisition of preferences and refers to the change in valence of
after referred to as US or USs for the plural form) is a biologically initially neutral CSs after pairing with positive or negative USs.
significant stimulus such as food, pain, electric shock that gener- Evaluative conditioning is usually conceptualized as a form of eval-
ates a response (for example, salivation when seeing certain foods) uative learning that occurs without awareness of the CS–US con-
from the start; this response is referred to as an unconditioned re- tingencies (De Houwer et al., 2001; Stahl et al., 2009). In a
sponse. Repeated pairings of a conditioned stimulus (hereafter re- typical evaluative conditioning study (e.g., De Houwer et al.,
ferred to as CS or CSs for the plural form, for example, the ring of a 2001; Walther, 2002), a subjectively neutral stimulus is repeatedly
bell) with an US (for example, meat paste) will enable the CS to eli- paired with a subjectively liked or disliked stimulus, leading to a
cit a conditioned response (for example, salivation) in an uncon- valence shift in the formerly neutral stimulus. That is, the CS in
scious and automatic manner. When the US is an affect (Razran, an evaluative conditioning paradigm does not attain a predictive
1938), for instance, music and humor, the conditioning may be re- value but simply obtains the affective qualities of the US.
ferred to as affective conditioning. Three major characteristics of evaluative conditioning are as
follows. First, evaluative conditioning does not seem to depend
1.1. Types of classical conditioning on contingency awareness of the CS and the US (Baeyens et al.,
1990; De Houwer et al., 2001). Second, it does not appear to rely
Within the paradigm of classical conditioning, it has been pro- on the statistical CS–US contingency but seems to be sensitive to
posed that a distinction be made between different types of condi- contiguity, that is, to spatiotemporal CS–US co-occurrences
tioning, namely, signal learning and evaluative learning (Baeyens (Baeyens et al., 1993; De Houwer et al., 2001). Therefore, weak
contingency in an evaluative learning paradigm (e.g., single CS or
US presentations in the acquisition phase) does not automatically
E-mail addresses: ynvynv@yahoo.com, ynvynv@gmail.com

1441-3582/$ - see front matter Ó 2012 Australian and New Zealand Marketing Academy. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ausmj.2012.07.002
C. Pornpitakpan / Australasian Marketing Journal 20 (2012) 282–296 283

reduce conditioning, as would be the case in signal learning (Baey- 2.1. The CS lacking predictiveness of the US
ens et al., 1993). Third, after successful evaluative conditioning,
single CS presentations may not alter its valence; in other words, In order for classical conditioning to occur, the CS should pre-
evaluative conditioning seems to be extinction resistant (Baeyens dict the US. The CS will have low predictiveness of the US if (i) both
et al., 1988; De Houwer et al., 2001). stimuli are presented simultaneously; (ii) the CS is presented con-
However, it has been criticized that the conclusions about eval- stantly, followed occasionally by the US (Brown and Jenkins,
uative conditioning without CS–US contingency awareness often 1968); (iii) either the US or the CS is frequently encountered alone;
relied on questionable experimental designs or failed to capture (iv) the US comes before the CS (i.e., backward conditioning); and
subtle but substantial manifestations of such awareness (Field, (v) the CS and the US are presented randomly with respect to each
2000; Field and Davey, 1999; Hammerl, 2000; Lovibond and other. When this predictive relation is violated, the conditioned
Shanks, 2002). This view/critique implies that the differentiation behavior will not be established.
between signal learning and evaluative conditioning as two types However, Shurtleff and Ayres (1981) found backward condi-
of classical conditioning is rather obscure and in fact, evaluative tioning. Spetch et al. (1981) reviewed experimental evidence and
conditioning is merely a situation when CS–US contingency aware- concluded that backward conditioning could produce effects simi-
ness cannot be detected and the focus is on the change in valence lar to those obtained from forward conditioning. In addition, such
of initially neutral CSs after pairing with positive or negative USs. It effects could not be explained by factors other than stimulus pair-
follows from this view/critique that CS–US contingency awareness ing. Rachman’s (1991) review suggests that conditioning can occur
underlies any observed classical conditioning effect. even when the CS and the US are not contiguous.
In order to say that conditioning occurs in an experiment,
1.2. Objectives and contributions of the study appropriate control groups are needed for comparison with the
conditioning groups (Rescorla, 1967). The experimental group is
Consumer researchers have empirically investigated classical exposed to the CS, followed by the US. A random control group is
conditioning effects after Gorn’s (1982) experiments. The results, exposed to the same quantity of CSs and USs as the experimental
unfortunately, have been mixed. Given three decades of classical group, but these stimuli are presented randomly with respect to
conditioning studies in consumer behavior and the prevalent each other. Classical conditioning can be said to occur only if there
application of classical conditioning principle knowingly or is a response in the experimental group but not in the random con-
unknowingly in advertising, this study aims to review classical trol group. This way, the differences between the two groups can
conditioning research in the realm of consumer behavior and be explained by the degree to which the CS predicts the US and
advertising to find out whether the effects are real or illusory. In cannot be attributed to differences in familiarity with the CS or
addition, the study discusses the common weaknesses of research the US, i.e., the mere exposure effect (Zajonc, 1968) or to any inter-
in this area and suggests what future research in this field should action between them (pseudoconditioned responses).
improve. Sharing the same view discussed in the preceding para- Even though the CS precedes and predicts the US, conditioning
graph, this review covers studies in both signal learning and eval- may not occur under the situations detailed below.
uative conditioning paradigms of classical conditioning as long as
they involve consumer behavior and advertising. Due to space con-
2.2. Overshadowing
straint, not all studies reviewed will be evaluated in detail.
The contributions of this study are two. First, in terms of aca-
Overshadowing can prevent classical conditioning from occur-
demic contribution, this study pinpoints common weaknesses of
ring (Pavlov, 1927). In an overshadowing procedure, two CSs dif-
research in classical conditioning and suggests ways to improve re-
ferent in salience, such as a loud noise and a dim light, are
search in this area in order to increase internal validity and yield
presented together before the US. Conditioning may occur to the
results that are more credible.
more salient CS (in this example, the loud noise) only because
Second, in terms of managerial contribution, this study provides
the less salient CS is overshadowed by the more salient one. This
guidelines for advertisers whether they should use the classical
suggests that salient stimuli coinciding with the target CS (for in-
conditioning principle in designing and producing ads, which are
stance, a highly sexy model and a brand in an ad) be removed.
usually costly. For example, a television commercial involving
non-celebrity actors, storyboarding, and script writing with a
length of 30-s costs on average US$3500–US$35000 to produce 2.3. Blocking
(Maus Media Group, 2011). The cost to create a full page colored
ad by a freelancer may start from US$2500 and much more by a Blocking can forbid classical conditioning (Kamin, 1969). In a
large advertising agency. The media expenses associated with blocking procedure, an individual is given experience that CS1 is
showing the ads are even much higher. For instance, the average predictive of an US (for example, dark clouds predict rain). Later,
price of a 30-s television spot in the first quarter of 2011 in the CS1 and CS2 (for example, a barometer that also predicts rain)
USA was US$108,956 (Crupi, 2011). A full-page ad in Cosmopolitan are presented together, followed by the same US. No conditioned
magazine costs US$244,100 for colored and US$195,300 for black- response occurs to CS2, however. In effect, prior experience with
and-white ads, respectively (Cosmopolitan, 2012). Clearly, it is CS1 blocks conditioning to CS2. In marketing, this indicates that
essential that marketers know whether ads based on classical con- a familiar US should not be used. For instance, using a celebrity en-
ditioning concepts are effective given such large expenditure of dorser who has been well established as an endorser for other
producing and displaying ads. products suppresses forming an association between that celebrity
and a newly endorsed product (Till, 1998).
2. Obstacles and methodological requirements for classical
conditioning 2.4. US pre-exposure effect

It is essential that the conditions hindering classical condition- Classical conditioning will not occur if individuals have encoun-
ing be examined so that conflicting studies can be assessed. Pri- tered the US alone (Mis and Moore, 1973; Rescorla, 1973). The
marily based on McSweeney and Bierley’s (1984) review, the implication is the same as that for the blocking effect, namely, a
obstructions to classical conditioning are as follows. familiar US, such as a famous song, should not be used.
284 C. Pornpitakpan / Australasian Marketing Journal 20 (2012) 282–296

2.5. Latent inhibition which seems to be unnecessarily complicated. Therefore, the three
patterns of ‘‘order of CSs and USs in the pairing’’ are merged but
Latent inhibition also forestalls classical conditioning (Lubow, will be mentioned for each study. Some groups contain no studies
1973). In a latent inhibition procedure, the CS is presented alone and therefore do not appear as sub-section headings.
several times before it is paired with the US, resulting in little con-
ditioning. A marketing implication of latent inhibition is that using 3.1. Studies using familiar CSs, familiar USs, and a single CS–US pairing
a familiar CS, such as a well-known product, will not be effective,
and it is easier to classically condition responses to unfamiliar Gorn (1982) provided the first support for classical conditioning
CSs such as new brands. in consumer behavior. He showed students slides of either a beige
or a blue pen (a CS) while having them listen to either liked or dis-
2.6. Garcia effect liked music (an US). When given a choice of taking a beige or a blue
pen as a gift, participants chose the pen associated with the liked
Garcia effect (Garcia and Koelling, 1966) refers to an improper music and avoided the one associated with the disliked music.
match between the CS and the US. It cannot be assumed, as a re- Gorn’s (1982) findings generated some concerns that inspired
sult, that any stimulus can be used as a CS or an US. For example, many later studies to replicate, extend, or refute his findings. It
in Garcia and Koelling’s experiments, an aversion (conditioned re- seemed rather difficult for a single simultaneous pairing of the
sponse) was easily conditioned to a flavor (CS1) that was followed CS and the US to produce the conditioned response because the
by sickness (US1) but not by shock (US2). In other words, the fla- CS will not predict the US if they are presented simultaneously.
vor-sickness CS–US pairing was appropriate but the flavor-shock While classical conditioning may occur after one trial (Shurtleff
pairing was not. Similarly, an aversion (conditioned response) and Ayres, 1981), it usually entails a very strong US such as an in-
could develop to a light and a noise (CS2) that were followed by tense shock or a nauseating drug (Bierley et al., 1985). In addition,
shock (US2) but not by sickness (US1). That is, the light/noise- participants in the liked music condition (one-minute extract of
shock CS–US pairing was suitable but the light/noise-sickness pair- music from the movie ‘‘Grease’’) were likely to be familiar with
ing was not. the US (the music), constituting an US pre-exposure effect or even
It has been speculated that stimuli that biologically belong to- a blocking effect if it had been elsewhere constantly associated
gether, such as tastes and sickness, results in effective condition- with some other CSs. Although no evidence in the study indicated
ing. Generally, it is easier to build an associative link between that participants had heard the music with other stimuli, since the
two stimuli when they are similar or related to each other (McS- music was from a popular movie, the chance of participants not
weeney and Bierley, 1984). In marketing, the choice of the US having heard this music before participating in the experiment
should correspond to the associations the brand already has or should be rather low. Beige and blue pens are very typical to stu-
seeks to have. For instance, Kim et al. (1996) demonstrated that dents, giving rise to a latent inhibition effect, i.e., a situation where
the pairing of pizza delivery (CS) with a race car (US) improved the CS has been encountered alone several times before it is paired
respondents’ beliefs about the speed of the pizza delivery service. with the US. All these made it rather unlikely for classical condi-
‘‘Starter’’ athletic apparel employed the durable and venerable tioning to emerge.
baseball player Cal Ripken to emphasize that Starter jackets were The observed classical conditioning effect in Gorn (1982) might
also durable. In the 1990’s, Pepsi used many endorsers such as Mi- have been due to demand characteristics, which are rather preva-
chael J. Fox, Madonna, and Michael Jackson to enhance its youth lent in classical conditioning studies involving human beings.
image. Authoritative, genteel, and cultured John Houseman fit well Some aspects of his procedure might have clued participants to
with the investment firm Smith Barney. However, McDonald’s the study’s objectives. For example, participants were told that
used John Houseman for only a short time because his image did the purpose was to evaluate ad music and pen color choice. The
not go well with a family-oriented fast food restaurant (Till, 1998). pen color choice was obtrusively measured by telling participants
wanting different pen colors to walk to the opposite sides of the
room, hence underscoring the importance of color choice in their
2.7. Insufficient pairings
minds. Furthermore, the experiment was administered in a large
class, so interaction among participants was possible and could
The acquisition speed of conditioned responses can range from
have affected the findings in an unpredictable manner. Finally, be-
one to many pairings of the CS and the US. The acquisition of con-
cause no control procedure was used, alternative explanations
ditioned responses will be faster for CSs that are more salient
such as the mere exposure effect and pseudoconditioned responses
(Kamin and Brimer, 1963; Rescorla, 1972), for USs that are stronger
could not be ruled out.
(Pavlov, 1927; Wagner et al., 1964), and for longer inter-trial inter-
Feinberg (1986) conducted a series of experiments in which he
vals (Terrace et al., 1975), which refers to longer time between suc-
manipulated the presence or absence of credit card stimuli in the
cessive CS–US pairings. Salient CSs are those that are intense
context of experimental tasks requiring participants to evaluate
physically (for example, brighter lights) or have acquired some
the product’s worth and to indicate how much they would donate
psychological importance to a person (for example, his or her
money actually or be willing to donate to a charity. The credit card
own name) (McSweeney and Bierley, 1984).
served as an US while the product or charity activities served as
CSs. The results showed that the mere presence of a credit card
3. Evidence for classical conditioning effects led to greater valuations and donations and that participants’ deci-
sion time was faster in the presence of a credit card symbol.
The evaluation of most studies below is based on the above The classical conditioning effects observed in Feinberg’s (1986)
impediments and methodological requirements for classical condi- experiments were subject to some limitations. First, Feinberg did
tioning. The studies can be grouped by the following methodolog- not remove the US – the credit card itself – when measuring the
ical scheme: 2 (familiarity of the CSs: familiar, unfamiliar) by 2 conditioned response, which means participants who viewed cata-
(familiarity of the USs: familiar, unfamiliar) by 2 (number of re- log pictures of products in the presence of credit card stimuli also
peated CS–US pairings: single, multiple) by 3 (order of CSs and evaluated the worth of these products in the presence of a credit
USs in the pairing: CSs preceding USs, simultaneous, USs preceding card. Second, the CS lacked predictiveness of the US because a
CSs). The full classification scheme results in 24 groups in total, credit card (a US) is frequently encountered alone in our daily life.
C. Pornpitakpan / Australasian Marketing Journal 20 (2012) 282–296 285

Third was the US pre-exposure effect – conditioning will not occur completion of the tasks, participants answered questionnaires
if consumers have been preexposed to the US alone. Finally, all his measuring prior credit card use and awareness.
experiments employed a single simultaneous paring of the CS and Task A consisted of a paper catalog of 12 consumer items, all of
the US. While classical conditioning may occur after one trial, it which were brand-neutral and were selected because they were
usually entails a very strong US such as an intense shock or a nau- similar to those used by Feinberg (1986). Each photo was presented
seating drug. Demand artifacts might be responsible for the ob- in the middle of a white sheet of paper, with two questions written
served classical conditioning effects. beneath the photo. The first question (a distracter question) asked
The mood induction experiment in Groenland and Schoormans about the most distinctive feature of the item, while the second
(1994) is included here to demonstrate a single pairing effect with question asked how much money the participant was willing to
the US preceding the CS (backward conditioning). The mood induc- spend on the item. Participants were individually tested and seated
tion was operationalized in a way that only mood induction could at a table with a consumer products catalog placed in front of them.
explain the occurrence of classical conditioning. Based on the pre- For participants in the credit card present condition, a laminated
test, a fragment of popular classical music (pleasant music) and a picture of the VISA and MasterCard logos was located on the upper
fragment of hard rock music (unpleasant music) were selected. left corner of the table, and these participants were told the credit
Both fragments were instrumental. Participants’ personal favorite card picture was left over from another experiment. For participants
colors were also pretested, with blue and green pens being selected in the credit card absent condition, the credit card picture was ab-
for the experiment. sent from the table. All participants were instructed to view each
Participants were split into two groups: one exposed to pleasant of the catalog items and answer the two questions for each item.
music while the other to unpleasant one. The experimenters ex- Task B was a computer-based version of task A written in Visu-
plained that a pen manufacturer wished to know whether music alBasic.NET and consisted of 12 trials, each of which presented one
could be used in a pen commercial. A 7-min musical fragment of the consumer items (same as those in task A) in the center of the
was played to induce mood. Five seconds after the music ended, screen. A question-and-answer box was displayed beneath each
a series of three slides of a green pen was presented for 10 s each. picture, with an ‘‘Accept’’ button located beneath the answer box.
Between slides was a break for 2 s. Then respondents filled a short Participants were tested either individually or in groups of up to
questionnaire, which included questions evaluating the pen shown 12 persons and were presented with each of the 12 trials (i.e.,
on the slide, and chose a pen (from a box containing many green items). For each item, participants were first asked, ‘‘What is the
and blue pens in equal number) to evaluate its physical attributes. most distinctive feature of the product?’’ followed by ‘‘Enter
Next, they answered another short questionnaire containing ques- amount you would spend ($) (only enter a single amount, not a
tions evaluating the music, mood, physical qualities of the pen cho- range).’’ Participants typed their answer and pressed the ‘‘Accept’’
sen, filler questions, and others. button to move onto the next question or trial. The time taken be-
The results revealed that the pen evaluation was more positive tween pressing the ‘‘Accept’’ button for the first question and
in the positive mood induction (pleasant music) than in the nega- pressing the ‘‘Accept’’ button for the second question was recorded
tive mood induction (unpleasant music), indicating that mood as a measure of response time for the price estimation. In the credit
influenced product evaluation. Furthermore, the expected pen col- card present condition, a picture of four credit card logos was
or was chosen more often by participants in a positive mood induc- shown on the bottom left-hand corner of the screen, whereas in
tion (pleasant music) than by those in the negative mood induction the credit card absent condition, the bottom left-hand corner of
(unpleasant music), meaning mood induction influenced not only the screen was blank.
product evaluation but also product choice. The results showed that price evaluations were significantly
Groenland and Schoormans (1994) used a familiar CS (a green lower for participants who viewed the items in the presence of
pen) and a familiar US (a fragment of popular classical music) with the credit card symbols compared to those who viewed the items
the US preceding the CS (backward conditioning) once only. All of without the symbols, and this effect emerged regardless of the
these procedures are obstacles to classical conditioning. method of item presentation. These effects were taken as support-
Tom (1995) attempted to replicate Gorn’s (1982) results by ing the premise that New Zealand students’ negative conditioning
using music (positive music using Kenny G’s Song Bird song versus history with credit card stimuli (i.e., credit cards being associated
negative music using John Lennon’s Number 9 Dream song) as an more with debt than spending) led to a negative credit card effect,
US. Two-hundred and twenty-seven students saw an advertised whereby credit cards discouraged rather than encouraged spend-
pen on a slide while the music was played for 60 s and had to ing, and that the absence (Hunt et al., 1990; Shimp and Moody,
pay attention to the presentation (attended stimulus), during 2000) or presence (McCall and Belmont, 1996; McCall et al.,
which a neutral Chinese ideograph (unattended stimulus) was 2004; Monger and Feinberg, 1997; Prelec and Simester, 2001; Rag-
flashed 12 times by a tachistoscope on the same screen for hubir and Srivastava, 2008) of a positive credit card effect in earlier
0.02 s. Then, participants chose a pen and a symbol as a logo for studies depended on past associations with credit card stimuli.
the pen. The results revealed that positive music led to higher Moreover, contrary to Feinberg’s (1986) results, the mean re-
choice rates for the advertised pen and Chinese ideograph, com- sponse time for the items was not consistently affected by the
pared to the unadvertised pen and Chinese ideograph. Neverthe- presence or absence of the credit card stimuli. Analysis of the ques-
less, the use of negative music as an US did not affect the tionnaire data on credit card use indicated that 67% of the partici-
preference for either the advertised pen or advertised Chinese pants reported owning or having owned a credit card, and there
ideograph. This null result will be discussed again in Section 4. was little effect of credit card use experience on price estimation
Same as Gorn’s (1982) study, Tom (1995) used a familiar CS and when the credit card stimulus was absent. However, a notable ef-
US with a single simultaneous pairing of the CS and the US. The fect existed when the credit card was present in that participants
instruction for participants to help select music for a pen ad could who did not have credit card use experience perceived the items
have heightened the importance of both the pen and the music. as having lower value. As explained by the authors, this might be
Lie et al. (2010) conducted two experiments in New Zealand to because in the absence of any personal experience where credit
replicate Feinberg’s (1986) study. In Experiment 1, 80 undergradu- cards were linked to consumption, the negative connotations asso-
ate students were randomly assigned to either task A or task B, and ciated with credit cards in the media gave rise to negative associ-
within each task, they were randomly assigned to either the credit ations with credit cards. This negative association, nevertheless,
card present condition or the credit card absent condition. Upon decreased with personal experience.
286 C. Pornpitakpan / Australasian Marketing Journal 20 (2012) 282–296

Sixteen year 4 students participated in Experiment 2. The pro- analysis if participants had poor verbalization or were not attentive
cedure of Experiment 2 was identical to task B in Experiment 1 ex- enough to describe fully what they thought.
cept two changes were made to the product items. Overall, the In Experiment 1 of Janiszewski and Warlop (1993), 54 under-
results of Experiment 2 replicated those of Experiment 1. graduates entered a laboratory one at a time to watch soft drink
Lie et al.’s (2010) results might be viewed as supporting a clas- commercials and received either forward conditioning or random
sical conditioning explanation of the credit card effect. In New Zea- conditioning. The original commercials were two 30-s TV commer-
land, credit cards have acquired negative valence through repeated cials: a Mountain Dew commercial featuring white-water surfing
pairings with negative emotions portrayed in media reports and and a Canada Dry commercial featuring scenes of couples having
advertising. Therefore, credit card logos might act as second-order fun. For each commercial, the 18 segments arranged to catchy jin-
CSs, with cautious spending as the conditioned response (Lie et al., gles were divided into three groups: six segments showing the
2010). However, their observed classical conditioning effects de- product only (serving as CSs), six segments being interesting or
serve some cautions. First, same as Feinberg (1986), the study used fun (serving as USs), and six segments of the product being con-
a single simultaneous CS–US pairing, lowering the CS’s predictive- sumed (serving as fillers).
ness of the US. Second was the latent inhibition – a credit card Two experimental commercials were made from each original.
(serving as an US) is commonly seen in our daily life before pairing The forward conditioning version of each commercial consisted
with the product items (serving as CSs) in the experiments, result- of six trials, with each trial composed of a product segment (CS),
ing in little classical conditioning. In addition, participants without followed by an entertaining segment (US), and then by a prod-
credit card use experience should not be included in the study. uct-consumption segment (filler). The random conditioning ver-
Alternatively, credit card use experience should be manipulated sion of the commercials consisted of a random order of the six
as another independent variable. The reason for the result that trials representing the six possible orders of the CS, the US, and
price evaluations were significantly lower for participants who the filler segments.
viewed the items in the presence of the credit card symbols com- The experimental design was a between-subjects manipulation
pared to those who viewed the items without the symbols may be of the conditioning procedure (forward, random) with a stimulus
that the credit card present condition happened to consist mainly replication. One treatment group received 18 forward condition-
of participants without credit card use experience (because it was ing trials for Mountain Dew and 18 random conditioning trials
found that these individuals perceived the items as having lower for Canada Dry, while the other treatment group received 18 for-
value). ward conditioning trials for Canada Dry and 18 random condi-
tioning trials for Mountain Dew. The conditioning trials for both
3.2. Studies using familiar CSs, familiar USs, and multiple CS–US groups were achieved by embedding three presentations of for-
pairings ward and random conditioning commercials within a sequence
of six filler ads, all of which were moderately known brands to al-
Bierley et al. (1985), in an attempt to replicate Gorn’s (1982) low observation of a conditioning influence. The experimental
findings, used colored geometric figures as CSs and music from commercials were presented as part of a series of commercials
the movie ‘‘Star Wars’’ as an US. Student participants were divided for eight different soft drinks. Each of the two experimental tapes
into four groups: (i) a ‘‘red-predictive’’ group, in which red CSs consisted of 12 commercials, each of which was separated by 5 s
were consistently followed by music, blue CSs were followed by of black space.
music on half of their presentations, and yellow CSs were never fol- The results showed that participants receiving forward condi-
lowed by music; (ii) a ‘‘yellow-predictive’’ group; (iii) a random tioning presentation of the Mountain Dew commercial looked at
control group; and (iv) a CS-only control group. Participants were the Mountain Dew container earlier than those presented with
exposed to 84 trials (28 trials per color). the random version of the commercial, while participants receiving
Bierley et al. (1985) found that when the color of the CSs was forward conditioning presentation of the Canada Dry commercial
held constant, the CSs that were followed by music were preferred looked at the Canada Dry container sooner than those viewing
to the ones that were not. The effect of conditioning revealed by the random version. Neither the different preference between the
the group-by-color interaction was complicated. A non-significant forward conditioning and the random conditioning versions of
increase in preference for the predictive CSs (excitatory condi- each commercial nor contingency awareness of the CS–US pairing
tioned response) and a significant or non-significant decrease in (participants had little awareness of the CS–US contingency) ap-
preference for the CSs combined to predict the absence of music peared to account for the results.
(inhibitory conditioned response). The group-by-color interaction The classical conditioning effects observed in Janiszewski and
was also significant for the participants classified as unaware of Warlop (1993) were subject to some limitations. First was the US
the study’s purpose or of the relation between the figures and pre-exposure effect – conditioning will not occur if people have
the music, thus ruling out the argument that awareness was neces- been preexposed to the US alone (white-water surfing scenes for
sary for classical conditioning. the Mountain Dew commercial and couples having fun scenes for
Bierley et al.’s (1985) findings, nevertheless, may not be re- the Canada Dry commercial). Second was the blocking effect,
garded as promising supports for classical conditioning. First, com- which refers to the situation when prior experience that CS1 is pre-
paring both experimental groups with the random control group, dictive of an US blocks conditioning from happening to CS2 and
the increase in preference for the predictive CSs was not signifi- implies that a familiar US should not be used. In addition, partici-
cant. Besides, the inhibitory conditioned response was significant pants were exposed to so many commercials, leading to fatigue
only in the yellow-predictive group. Second, like Gorn’s (1982) and boredom. Third, using well-known brands (Mountain Dew
study, the chance for classical conditioning to occur was likely to and Canada Dry) as CSs posed the latent inhibition effect, that is,
be minimal due to the use of a familiar CS and a familiar US. This a situation where the CS has been seen alone several times before
US was not pretested to ensure that participants would really like it is paired with the US, resulting in little classical conditioning.
it. Although it was required that participants in the experiment like Lastly, it was not clear whether participants’ pre-existing familiar-
the music from ‘‘Star Wars,’’ there was no guarantee that partici- ity with the experimental brand was measured. It could be that
pants would conform to this requirement. Third, the cover story participants in the forward conditioning happened to be more
asking participants to try to predict music might have led to familiar with the experimental brand, so they looked at the brand
demand artifacts, which could not be detected by the post hoc sooner than those in the random conditioning.
C. Pornpitakpan / Australasian Marketing Journal 20 (2012) 282–296 287

In the affective conditioning experiment in Groenland and The testing stage followed the training stage immediately with
Schoormans (1994), two conditions (an experimental condition 20 matching-to-sample trials, in which each of the four equiva-
and a random control condition) were manipulated, with music lence tasks were presented five times in a quasi-random order,
serving as an affect-producing US and a green pen serving as a and no feedback was given after any test trial. Participants who
CS. Both conditions were split into two groups: one being pre- matched in accordance with the equivalence relations on at least
sented pleasant music (a fragment of popular classical instrumen- 17 of these 20 trials were deemed to have passed the test. The rat-
tal music pretested to be pleasant) whereas the other being ing stage took place right after the testing stage. A participant was
presented unpleasant music (a fragment of hard rock instrumental seated at a table, on which a tray contained the following: (i) on
music pretested to be unpleasant). In the experimental condition, the left side of the tray were a glass and a 1.25-l bottle of cola la-
the presentation sequence consisted of seven trials. Each trial beled BRAND X, and (ii) on the right side of the tray were a glass
started with a 10-s presentation of a new slide, showing the pen and an identical 1.25-l bottle of cola labeled BRAND Y. Participants
in different user contexts. Four seconds after the slide presentation tasted and then rated the pleasantness of both colas. The order of
began, the music was played for one minute. The time interval be- tasting across participants was counterbalanced.
tween the CS and the US was thus similar to that used in other clas- The result showed that participants who passed the equivalence
sical conditioning studies. Between trials was a break for 15 s with test rated the cola that had a label in an equivalence class with the
neither music nor slides being presented. In the random control word holidays (i.e., holidays-cola) higher than the cola that had a
condition, the same procedure applied except both the slide and label in an equivalence class with the word cancer (i.e., cancer-
the music presentation were randomly scheduled within the total cola), and no significant difference emerged between ratings of
presentation period so that no sequential association between the the two colas for participants who failed the equivalence test. This
CS and the US could be established. The total music presentation indicated that the stimulus equivalence procedure was an effective
time was seven minutes for both the experimental and the control method by which preference functions could be transferred to a
conditions. third order.
The results of the affective conditioning experiment showed a Experiment 2 tested whether the transfer of function to a third
more positive evaluation of the pen in the positive music experi- order in Experiment 1 was facilitated by the subsequent test for
mental condition than in the negative music experimental condi- equivalence, during which the stimuli were presented contigu-
tion, whereas in the control condition, no such difference was ously on the screen. The procedure was identical to that used in
found, indicating that affective conditioning occurred at the level Experiment 1, excluding the equivalence testing stage. The result
of product evaluation. For both the experimental and the control showed a significant preference for the holidays-cola over the can-
conditions, the proportion of participants choosing the expected cer-cola, suggesting the test for equivalence in Experiment 1 was
color of pen (green in the positive conditions, blue in the negative not necessary for the transfer of preference function to a third or-
conditions) was compared to the proportion of the relevant per- der by means of a stimulus equivalence procedure.
sonal favorite color (green in the positive conditions, blue in the Experiment 3 replicated Experiment 2, but several control mea-
negative conditions). The results showed that in the experimental sures and procedures were included in order to determine whether
condition, participants chose the expected pen color significantly the conditional discrimination training clearly influenced the
more often, while in the control condition, they did not, and this pleasantness ratings of the colas. First, participants rated the two
could be interpreted that affective conditioning occurred at the le- colas before the conditional discrimination. Second, before and
vel of product choice. after the experiment, participants rated the words ‘‘cancer’’ and
Groenland and Schoormans (1994) utilized a random control ‘‘holidays’’ as emotionally negative or positive, so that these ratings
condition. However, the experiment used a familiar CS (a green could be compared to the pleasantness ratings obtained for the co-
pen) and a familiar US (a fragment of popular classical instrumen- las. Third, after participants had gone through the conditional dis-
tal music). Both aspects hinder classical conditioning. crimination training and pleasantness ratings, they were exposed
Barnes-Holmes et al. (2000) exposed 38 undergraduates to a again to the training and testing procedure except the labels Brand
series of third-order conditioning experiments. In Experiment 1, X and Brand Y were swapped within the training. Overall, the re-
each participant was trained, tested, and did the rating task indi- sults of Experiment 3 provided strong evidence that the condi-
vidually. In the training stage, each participant was trained on four tional discrimination training functioned as a powerful
matching-to-sample tasks of the following types: CANCER ? VEK; determinant of participants’ preference ratings.
VEK ? BRAND X; HOLIDAYS ? ZID; and ZID ? BRAND Y. Partici- The classical conditioning effect found in Barnes-Holmes et al.’s
pants were seated at a computer to see a text on the screen, which (2000) called for some cautions. First, participants may have real-
instructed them to press Z if they chose the comparison on the left ized what the research was attempting to achieve, and the prefer-
of the screen and to press M if they chose the comparison on the ence ratings simply reflected their tendencies to please the
right. After participants pressed the space bar, one of the four experimenter. In particular, the study’s training phase was in effect
matching-to-sample tasks appeared. In effect, a sample stimulus ensuring that participants were aware of the pairings. The pretest–
(e.g., CANCER) was presented for 2 s and then the screen cleared posttest measures, as well as the second training that swapped the
for 0.5 s before two comparison stimuli (i.e., VEK and ZID) were brands, in Experiment 3 would have clued participants as to what
shown. the experimenter wanted. Second, both colas were presented
If the choice was correct, the screen cleared and the word ‘‘COR- simultaneously on the tray and thus the presence of one brand
RECT’’ appeared on the screen for 1.5 s, accompanied by a high- may have affected participants’ ratings of the other brand. That
pitched tone. On the other hand, if the choice was incorrect, the is, the influence of the context stimuli could not be ignored. Finally,
screen cleared and the word ‘‘WRONG’’ appeared on the screen the US pre-exposure effect applied – conditioning will not occur if
for 1.5 s, accompanied by a low-pitched tone. A 2-s inter-trial people have been preexposed to the US alone.
interval was presented following the feedback, during which the In a study designed to demonstrate that classical conditioning
screen remained blank and no sounds were emitted. The four effects on attitude can happen without demand characteristics or
matching-to-sample tasks were presented in this way in a quasi- contingency awareness, Olson and Fazio (2001) found that atti-
random order until participants gave 24 correct responses tudes could develop through implicit covariation detection in a
consecutively. classical conditioning paradigm. In two experiments purportedly
288 C. Pornpitakpan / Australasian Marketing Journal 20 (2012) 282–296

about video surveillance and vigilance, undergraduate females sample and a significant interaction effect between the pretest atti-
viewed several hundred randomly presented words and images tude toward the brands and the conditioning procedure on implicit
interspersed with critical simultaneous pairings of valenced USs attitudes. That is, the conditioning procedure had no effect on im-
[either (i) positive words such as excellent, awesome and images plicit attitudes for either the Coke-lover or Pepsi-lover groups (This
such as puppies, hot fudge sundaes or (ii) negative words such as result will be presented again in Section 4). On the contrary, neu-
terrible, awful and images such as a cockroach, a man wielding a tral participants showed a significant effect of conditioning such
knife] with a CS (a colored picture of Pokémon cartoon character). that those in the Coke-positive condition showed more-favorable
The findings indicated that in a covariation estimation task implicit attitudes toward Coke, while those in the Pepsi-positive
involving the stimuli presented in the conditioning phase, partici- condition exhibited more-favorable implicit attitudes toward
pants revealed no explicit memory for the pairings, suggesting no Pepsi.
CS–US contingency awareness. In a surprise evaluation task, the As for explicit brand attitudes, participants who reported strong
CS paired with positive items was evaluated more positively than pretest preferences continued with those preferences and those
the one paired with negative items. This attitudinal conditioning who were neutral on the pretest remained neutral after the condi-
effect was observed when using an explicit measure in both exper- tioning procedure. Furthermore, there was no significant main ef-
iments and when using Greenwald et al.’s (1998) Implicit Associa- fect for the conditioning procedure and no interaction between
tion Test, which was less susceptible to demand artifacts than conditioning and pretest preference (This result will be presented
explicit measures, in Experiment 2. again in the Section 4). Therefore, Experiment 1 revealed that im-
The limitations of Olson and Fazio (2001) were twofold. First, plicit attitudes (but not explicit attitudes) for mature brands could
the study utilized a simultaneous conditioning procedure as used be altered by evaluative conditioning, and this effect occurred only
in Baker (1999), Gorn (1982), Allen and Madden (1985), Rozin for participants whose attitude toward the brand was initially neu-
et al. (1998), and Tom (1995), instead of having the CS preceding tral. In addition, contingency awareness was not necessary to
the US as suggested by McSweeney and Bierley (1984) and Groen- change implicit brand attitudes. The results of Experiment 2 repli-
land and Schoormans (1994). Second, the CS (Pokémon cartoon cated and extended the results of Experiment 1, indicating that
character) might not be novel as intended since this cartoon is brand choice was congruent with conditioning but only under cog-
quite well-known. nitive load, and the implicit brand attitudes predicted brand choice
Distinguishing between implicit attitudes [which are automatic but only under cognitive load.
and based on associations in memory, typically measured by The classical conditioning effects on implicit memory for people
Greenwald et al.’s, 1998 Implicit Association Test] and explicit atti- with neutral attitude toward the CS found in Gibson’s (2008)
tudes (which are deliberative and based on self-reported mea- experiment had a few reservations. First was the US pre-exposure
sures), Gibson (2008) extended the studies of Shimp et al. (1991) effect – conditioning will not occur if people have been preexposed
and Cacioppo et al. (1992), which will be discussed later. Based to the US alone. Second is the blocking effect, which indicates that
on the result of a pretest pertaining to an explicit measure of pref- familiar USs should not be used. Third is the latent inhibition – the
erence for soft drinks, 56 undergraduate students participated in CS is presented alone several times before pairing with the US,
Experiment 1, which employed three conditions: a strong prefer- resulting in little classical conditioning.
ence for Coke, a strong preference for Pepsi, and an approximately
equal preference for each. 3.3. Studies using familiar CSs, unfamiliar USs, and multiple CS–US
Participants were told that the experiment focused on people’s pairings
vigilance and attention to different brand images. They were ex-
posed to a large number of words and images on a computer screen Stuart et al. (1987) conducted experiments with students under
over five blocks, each of which consisted of 86 trials presented for ideal conditions for classical conditioning to occur. Experiment 1
1.5 s each. Within each block, 10 of the 86 images were of the tar- used a neutral fictitious brand as a CS and pleasant pictures as
get brand, and these were randomly interspersed throughout the USs. The design was a 4 (levels of conditioning trials: 1, 3, 10, 20
block of trials. The 76 remaining trials within each block included trials) by 2 (experimental group versus random control group) AN-
other neutral images and words, blank screens, and also four OVA. The results showed that the experimental groups had signif-
images of Coke and four images of Pepsi (the conditioning trials). icantly more-positive attitudes toward the brand than the
Thus, over the five blocks of trials, participants saw a total of 20 corresponding control groups. Experiment 2 utilized 10-trial and
images of Coke and 20 images of Pepsi, both of which were evenly one-trial levels with five conditions: (i) a latent inhibition group
spaced across the block of trials and always alternated in order. preexposed to the CS alone several times before the CS–US pair-
In the ‘‘Pepsi-positive’’ conditioning condition, Coke images ings, (ii) a conditioning control group, (iii) a random control group,
were always paired with negative photographic stimuli and words, (iv) a CS-only control group, and (v) a latent inhibition/random
whereas Pepsi images were always paired with positive ones. In control group.
contrast, in the ‘‘Coke-positive’’ condition, Coke images were al- The results showed that the conditioned response in the latent
ways paired with positive photographic and verbal stimuli, inhibition groups was significantly more positive than that in the
whereas Pepsi images were always paired with negative ones. random control groups but was less positive than that of the con-
The paired positive or negative stimuli were presented on the same ditioning groups, whose responses were more positive than the
screen as the image of Coke or Pepsi, rendering this a simultaneous other control groups. In line with Spetch et al. (1981), Experiment
conditioning procedure. Within each level of this conditioning, the 3 showed that backward conditioning did produce a conditioning
contrasting brand was always paired with negative images and effect, relative to the control group, though the effect was signifi-
words. In the conditioning trials, the Coke and Pepsi images were cantly less than that of the forward conditioning group. Experi-
placed on either the right or the left side of the screen, against a ment 4, which examined forward and backward conditioning
black background, with the USs placed adjacent to them on the procedures, confirmed that all three forward conditioning proce-
other side of the screen. In addition, the brand images were labeled dures resulted in similar conditioning effects, which were signifi-
with the word Coke or Pepsi underneath the image. Each brand ap- cantly greater than that of the control group, and replicated the
peared equally often on the right or the left of the screen. results of Experiment 3.
The results of Experiment 1 showed a significant correlation Stuart et al. (1987) utilized a careful design and attempted to
between implicit and explicit brand attitudes across the whole reduce hypothesis guessing by using filler materials. The post hoc
C. Pornpitakpan / Australasian Marketing Journal 20 (2012) 282–296 289

analysis, however, revealed that half of the participants were The results revealed an interaction between the valence of the
aware of the CS–US contingency, and this contingency awareness US (liked versus disliked faces) and participants’ mood. That is, par-
was a significant predictor of participants’ attitudes. This is in line ticipants in a sad mood were more susceptible to the negative con-
with many studies, which found that contingency awareness acted ditioning procedure than were those in a happy mood. In addition,
as a causal mediator in conditioning (Allen and Janiszewski, 1989; contingency awareness influenced the formation of consumer
Baer and Fuhrer, 1982; Biferno and Dawson, 1977; Dawson, 1973; attitudes.
Perruchet, 1985; Rescorla, 1988; Shimp et al., 1991). In addition, Walther and Grigoriadis (2004) used a very tedious condition-
because they did not employ an elaborate demand artifact assess- ing procedure and an extensive set of open-ended questions to
ment, they admitted that this possibility could not be rejected. check contingency and demand awareness. After about 40 min of
In another study, Walther and Grigoriadis (2004) investigated going through the experiment and several rounds of answering
the impact of mood on the acquisition of affective attitudes many questions, it was doubtful how participants could answer
through a 2 (mood: positive versus negative) between-subjects the questions accurately, and they might have been too exhausted
by 2 (conditioning: appetitive versus aversive) within-subjects de- to elaborate on the awareness-checking questionnaire.
sign. Forty-four students were seated individually in front of a
computer screen. The conditioning procedure contained three 3.4. Studies using unfamiliar CSs, familiar USs, and multiple CS–US
sequential phases guided by a computer program: the baseline pairings
phase, the conditioning phase, and the test phase. Participants
put on headphones and started a program of auditory instructions, Additional evidence in favor of classical conditioning was from
which informed them that the study was concerned with sponta- Allen and Janiszewski (1989). Experiment 1 assessed the role of
neous feelings people experienced towards other objects and indi- contingency awareness versus demand awareness by using five
viduals. Participants then judged different types of stimuli neutral Norwegian words as CSs and positive evaluative phrases
randomly selected by a computer program. In all conditions, 105 for participants’ performance as USs. The five words had different
black-and-white pictures of white male faces intermixed with 67 degrees in predicting the US. The experimental group was found
pictures of all kinds of shoes (without brands) selected from Ger- to have significantly more-favorable evaluation for the words pre-
man magazines and the Internet were displayed for 3 s each in a dicting the US in both the between-group test and the within-
full frontal view in the center of the screen. After 3 s, participants group test.
evaluated the stimulus. Two negative USs, two positive USs, and Experiment 2 attempted to strengthen the conditioning proce-
eight moderately rated CSs were selected from this baseline dure by increasing inter-trial intervals, to heighten contingency
evaluation. but not demand awareness, and to manipulate demand awareness.
Next was the mood manipulation. Participants were seated in The results showed that in the extended inter-trial interval group,
front of a television set. In the happy-mood condition, participants there were no effects on word evaluations or brand name ratings.
viewed two short films: one containing scenes from the movie In the contingency-aware group, there was an effect on word eval-
‘‘Shrek’’ and the other from ‘‘The Little Bear.’’ Those in the sad- uations only. In the demand-aware condition, the effects on both
mood condition were shown two films: one about child abuse variables were significant. Taken together, the two experiments
and the other about the last day of a doomed man. The films lasted did not support the conditioning-without-awareness position
about 10 min in both conditions. After the film presentation, par- (Kassarjian, 1986; MacKenzie et al., 1986; Preston, 1982; Staats
ticipants rated their mood on a 9-point scale embedded in several and Staats, 1959).
neutral questions concerning the film. In Shimp et al. (1991), the CS in each of the 21 forward-
The conditioning phase followed the mood manipulation. Pic- conditioning experiments was one of the four unknown
tures of neutral shoes (CSs) were paired with liked or disliked (Cragmont, Elf, My-te-Fine, and Target), two moderately known
faces (USs). Two sets of stimuli were used, each of which con- (RC and Shasta), or two well-known (Coke and Pepsi) cola brands.
sisted of a CS-positive US pair, a CS-negative US pair, and a con- The US was a composite of four attractive water scenes used in Stu-
trol CS–CS pair. Identical stimuli were presented in each pair. art et al.’s (1987) study (a mountain waterfall, a sunset over water,
Both sets were presented five times, intermixed with each other a boat mast against the sky, and a lavender-hued island). The filler
and with two additional neutral CS–CS distracter pairs. In keeping context for the conditioning trials included three unknown or
with previous studies, each picture was shown for 1 s with a trace known brands. Specifically, when an unknown brand served as
interval (i.e., the interval between the end of the first stimulus the CS in the context of other unknown colas, the filler items com-
and the beginning of the second stimulus of a pair) of 1 s and prised the three remaining unknown brands. When an unknown
an inter-trial interval of 4 s. The conditioning phase was instantly brand was conditioned in a known context, Coke, Pepsi, and either
followed by the test phase, which employed the same procedure RC or Shasta were used as fillers. When a moderately known brand
as in the baseline. A second mood check was conducted immedi- was conditioned in the context of known brands, the filler brands
ately after the test phase, and then the recognition test appeared included Coke, Pepsi, and the remaining moderately known brand.
on the screen. When a well-known brand was conditioned in the context of
Participants saw each CS in its original size on the left side of known brands, the filler brands were composed of RC, Shasta,
the screen and with four smaller pictures on the right. The instruc- and the remaining well-known brand. When a moderately or
tions above the pictures asked participants to select the small pic- well-known brand was conditioned in the context of unknown
ture that frequently followed the large picture. Participants had the brands, any three of the four unknown brands served as fillers.
choice between the correct US, a stimulus with the same valence as Each experiment involved a conditioning group and a random
the correct US, a stimulus that occurred with the same frequency control group. The conditioning group received 20 conditioning
as the correct US, and a stimulus of medium valence selected from trials, in which the CS always preceded an US scene, and 60 non-
the baseline. Once they made their choice, the next CS recognition conditioning trials, in which non-CS brands of cola were paired
test appeared on the screen. Finally, participants answered an with 12 pictures pretested as being neutral, e.g., a license plate,
open-ended questionnaire to check their demand awareness and weeds growing in a pond, and unpainted boards. All conditioning
verbal knowledge of the contingencies. After completing the ques- and non-conditioning trials involved displaying a cola brand on a
tionnaire, participants filled out a final mood check. The procedure screen for 7.5 s followed by a 7.5-s slide of either a positively val-
lasted for about 45 min. enced slide in the case of the conditioning trials or a neutral slide in
290 C. Pornpitakpan / Australasian Marketing Journal 20 (2012) 282–296

the case of non-conditioning trials. All 80 15-s pairings ended with those in the control group did. Besides, the non-US-preexposed
a 2-s dark-screen pause. Each of the four US water scenes followed conditioning group held significantly more-favorable attitudes to-
the CS five times, with the various water scenes randomly distrib- ward the CS than the US-preexposed conditioning group. In sum-
uted among the 20 conditioning trials. mary, the results indicated that the US induced negative
To minimize temporal conditioning, the 20 conditioning trials attitudinal conditioning toward the name Garra in participants
were interspersed among the 60 non-conditioning trials. The time who had been preexposed to the US in an unpleasant context.
between the end of a conditioning trial and the onset of the next Blair and Shimp’s (1992) study used proper control procedure
trial (i.e., the inter-trial interval) ranged from 2 to 102 s with an but faced some limitations. In the US-preexposed group, the sup-
average of 54 s. The random control group received the same num- posedly pleasant thanksgiving music in the preconditioning phase
ber of presentations of the CS and the US and the same number of was in effect a CS being paired with unpleasant winter experience
presentations of the filler brands and scenes, but all were randomly until this music was later used as a negative US. However, this pre-
assigned with respect to each other. The 23-min slide presentation conditioning procedure gave rise to the blocking effect – the US
was interrupted twice after the first and the second portion of the should have no previous associations with the CS or certain expe-
three approximately equal portions for participants to answer rience before. It would have been better if the negative US was
questions about attitudes toward filler brands. manipulated via the use of music pretested to be unfamiliar and
The 21 experiments ranged in size from 51–83 students, with unpleasant to people similar to the target respondents. Second
most experiments having approximately 30 participants each in was the Garcia effect – not any stimulus can be used as a CS or
the conditioning and random control groups. The experimental an US, and the CS and the US should logically belong together in
sessions consisted of 2–10 students, with most sessions having a classical conditioning experiment. Thanksgiving music (the US)
4–7. and sportswear (the CS) might not logically/emotionally belong to-
The results showed that the conditioning groups in 11 of the 21 gether. The negative classical conditioning effect found might have
experiments exhibited significantly more positive attitudes toward been caused by some other extraneous factors.
the CS brands than the random control groups (The non-significant In Experiment 1 of Kim et al. (1996), 66 undergraduate students
results will be discussed again in Section 4). Of the 11 instances of were administered in groups of 2–5 persons and randomly as-
statistical significance, seven employed unknown brands, three signed to one of the three conditions, namely, forward condition-
used moderately known brands, and one used well-known brands ing, backward conditioning, and control. A picture of a plain
as CSs. Attitude change was most apparent when participants no- white pizza box inscribed with the logo of a fictitious brand ‘‘L Piz-
ticed the contingency between the CS and the US. In summary, za House’’ served as a CS, while a race car, which was pretested to
evaluative conditioning could change attitudes for novel brands provoke neutral affective response and convey implicit meaning
but not for mature brands (like Coke and Pepsi). that might be used in forming beliefs about the brand, served as
One-hundred and sixty-nine participants were recruited under an US. Meanwhile, 60 pictures in the series of 80 images for three
the guise of evaluating audiocassette tapes of business programs other fictitious brands were used to detract attention from the fo-
in Blair and Shimp’s (1992) experiment, which involved two cal CS–US pairings and thus decrease hypothesis guessing.
phases: a preconditioning phase followed by a conditioning phase. Participants in each condition watched a series of 80 visual
In the preconditioning phase, participants returned to the research images displayed on a television. In the forward conditioning
site many times in winter (unpleasant experience) and each time group, they were exposed to ten trials, each of which consisted
listened to different audiotaped business books introduced and of a 7.5-s presentation of the CS followed by a 7.5-s presentation
concluded with the same music to be used as the US later. After of the US. In the backward conditioning group, all the ten presen-
the preconditioning phase, about half of the participants formed tations of the CS were preceded by the US. Participants in the con-
two conditioning groups, namely, the non-US-preexposure- trol group were exposed to the same number of presentations of
conditioning group (the conditioning group that was not preex- the CS and the US and the same 60 filler images as those in the
posed to the US) and the US-preexposure-conditioning group treatment groups. However, the sequence of images was randomly
(the conditioning group that was preexposed to the US), while scrambled, and the CS and the US never appeared contiguously.
the remainder constituted two control groups. The CS was a The results showed that the forward conditioning group had
neutral fictitious brand name of sportswear ‘‘Garra’’ and the US more-favorable attitudes toward the brand than the control and
was the ‘‘Thanksgiving’’ music positively evaluated and relatively the backward conditioning groups, which did not differ from each
unfamiliar to the participants. other. Participants who experienced forward (versus backward)
The entire presentation in the conditioning procedure included conditioning trials demonstrated higher levels of contingency
a shirt slide for 4 s, a word slide for 4 s (including the CS and three awareness, and those who were aware (versus unaware) of the
filler Portuguese names), and then a blank screen while music was contingency between the CS and the US exhibited more-positive
played for 25 s. This sequence was repeated 40 times. The 25-min beliefs about the target attribute and more-favorable brand atti-
presentation was designed such that 10 conditioning trials were tudes. Furthermore, the series of regression analyses showed that
embedded among the pairings of filler names and music snippets. the conditioning procedure accounted for significant variation in
In the conditioning groups, each word slide was followed by a 25-s beliefs about the target attribute and brand attitudes. Lastly, beliefs
snippet of the US music, and the US always followed the CS but about the target attribute mediated the effect of the conditioning
never any of the other names. Participants in the control groups re- procedure on participants’ brand attitudes.
ceived an identical number of exposures to the CS, US, and filler Experiment 2, which used a different CS (Brand L facial tissue)
materials as those in the conditioning groups; however, the CS and USs (pictures of a single kitten and two kittens, respectively)
was never immediately followed by the US in the control groups from Experiment 1, confirmed the results in Experiment 1. To-
but instead was always followed by one of the three filler snippets. gether, the results revealed that brand attitudes could be condi-
The results showed no significant difference between the non- tioned using both attractive images that induced direct affect
US-preexposed conditioning group and its control. In other words, transfer and descriptive visual images that elicited inferential be-
the US was not salient enough to produce positive attitudinal condi- lief formation (i.e., cognitive mechanisms).
tioning for participants in the non-preexposed group (this null Kim et al. (1996) used filler brands to reduce hypothesis guess-
result will be presented again in Section 4). Participants in the US- ing and a control group for comparison. However, the obtained clas-
preexposed conditioning group evaluated the CS less positively than sical conditioning effects might be subject to some reservations.
C. Pornpitakpan / Australasian Marketing Journal 20 (2012) 282–296 291

The USs in both experiments (a race car in Experiment 1 and kittens paired with a positive US maintained a favorable attitude toward
in Experiment 2) are typically encountered alone in people’s daily the CS over time. The result in Experiment 2 also showed that con-
life or television. According to the US pre-exposure effect, condi- ditioned attitudes toward the brand did persist over time. The clas-
tioning will not occur if people have been preexposed to the US sical conditioning effects obtained in Grossman and Till (1998)
alone. Demand artifacts, despite the use of filler brands, might have were subject to the same reservations as in Kim et al. (1996).
given rise to the observed classical conditioning effects. Priluck and Till (2004) conducted a 2 (conditioning group versus
Kim et al. (1998) exposed 36 undergraduates to a classical con- control group) by 2 (high versus low involvement) between-
ditioning experiment, in which the fictitious restaurant ‘‘L Pizza subjects factorial-design experiment with 195 undergraduate stu-
House’’ served as a CS and a kitten picture, which was chosen after dents. Similar to Grossman and Till (1998), the CS was a picture of
extensive pretesting that the picture was neutral and bore no belief a fictitious brand of mouthwash (Garra) that had been previously
about the CS, served as an US. Participants were randomly assigned pretested to be affectively neutral and not to evoke unusual asso-
to one of two conditions (conditioning or control). In the condition- ciations. The USs were three pleasant visual scenes: a picture of a
ing group, participants viewed the CS for 5 s, followed by a 5-s pre- boat in tropical waters, a picture of railroad tracks leading to a
sentation of the US. After 10 repetitions of the CS–US pairing, 1 s of snow-covered mountain, and a picture of a nature scene with
‘‘down time’’ occurred, followed by a filler CS and a neutrally affec- a panda. Each of these pictures had been pretested to be favorable
tive US in order to detract participants’ attention from the focal CS– to the participants.
US pairing. In the control condition, participants were exposed to Participants were exposed to a positive conditioning procedure
the same stimuli as in the conditioning group; however, the order in which the test brand was paired with favorable stimuli either 1
of stimuli presentation was randomized and there was no system- or 15 times. Participants in both the conditioning treatment and
atic pairing of the CS and the US. the conditioning control groups were exposed to a slide presenta-
The result of Experiment 1 showed that attitude toward the tion in which the stimuli appeared interspersed among filler pic-
product in the conditioning group was significantly more- tures. While the conditioning control group saw a random
favorable than that in the control group, indicating that the sequence of pictures, the conditioning treatment group was ex-
conditioning procedure influenced attitude toward the product. posed to each of the following stimuli for 4 s in the following order:
The differences in the mean values between the conditioning and the CS, the US, the CS superimposed on the US, and a blank screen.
the control groups for all product beliefs were not significant, High involvement was achieved by written instructions asking par-
showing that the US (i.e., a kitten picture) did not provoke any ticipants to pay careful attention to the slide presentation with a
beliefs that could be associated with the CS (i.e., L Pizza House). chance to win $25 if they answered questions about the presenta-
In addition, the mean values for affect toward the kitten in the con- tion correctly. In contrast, low involvement was attained by asking
ditioning and the control groups were not significantly different, participants to consider filler pictures.
suggesting that participants’ positive affect toward the kitten was The results of Experiment 1 showed that the mean attitude to-
present in both conditions. ward the CS among contingency-aware participants in the condi-
The design of Kim et al.’s (1998) study was rather simple with tioning treatment was significantly higher than that among
two groups only. The limitation in their study was similar to Kim contingency-unaware participants. The three-step regression pro-
et al.’s (1996), namely, the US pre-exposure effect, which suggests cedure supported the hypothesis that contingency awareness med-
that conditioning will not occur if consumers have been preex- iated the relationship between classical conditioning procedures
posed to the US alone. A kitten is frequently encountered in daily and attitudes toward the CS because: (i) participants in the condi-
lives. Second, the Garcia effect stipulates that the US and the CS tioning treatment were more likely to be aware of the CS–US con-
should be matched properly. It was not clear whether a kitten is tingency than those in the conditioning control; (ii) attitude
an appropriate match as an US for a pizza house. toward the brand was significant when regressed on conditioning,
In Experiment 1 of Grossman and Till (1998), groups of 12–25 showing a basic conditioning effect; and (iii) when attitude toward
participants were exposed to a fictitious brand of mouthwash (Gar- the brand was regressed on awareness and conditioning, the effect
ra), which served as a CS, and three highly favorable scenes (a trop- of conditioning decreased. Finally, individuals exposed to the CS–
ical scene with a boat, a picture of railroad tracks leading to a US pairing who were both highly involved and high in need for
snow-covered mountain, and a nature scene with a panda), which cognition were more aware of the CS–US contingency relationship
were selected as USs based on a pretest. Nine neutral pictures were than were those who were not highly involved and not high in
paired with each of the three filler brands, which were included to need for cognition.
disguise the nature of the study and prevent hypothesis guessing. Experiment 2 revealed further that individuals exposed to the
The treatment and control groups were exposed the same num- CS–US pairing who were both highly involved and high in need
ber of times to both the US and the CS; however, only the treat- for cognition developed more-favorable attitudes toward the CS
ment group was exposed to the CS–US contingency pairing. That than those who were not highly involved and not high in need
is, the control group saw a random sequence of pictures, whereas for cognition. Participants exposed to the CS–US pairing who were
the treatment group saw each of the following stimuli for 4 s in highly involved or high in need for cognition developed stronger
the following order: the CS, the US, the CS superimposed on the beliefs than those who were not highly involved or low in need
US, and a blank screen. After the experiment, participants com- for cognition. In addition, contingency awareness mediated more
pleted an immediate attitude measure, a one-week delayed mea- strongly for beliefs than for affect transfer in the relationship be-
sure with the same questionnaire as the immediate one, and a tween classical conditioning and attitude toward the CS. The find-
three-week delayed measure with identical questions as the for- ings in Experiment 2 were consistent with those of Kim et al.
mer ones but in different order, respectively. (1996, 1998), challenging the traditional view of classical condi-
The results showed that the treatment participants’ mean atti- tioning as automatic, non-cognitive learning. However, the classi-
tude toward the CS was much higher than that of the control par- cal conditioning effect observed in Priluck and Till’s experiments
ticipants. Moreover, the significant difference of mean attitude are subject to the same reservations as in Grossman and Till
toward the CS between the treatment and the control conditions (1998) and Kim et al. (1996).
persisted over time as reflected in the one-week and the three- Stahl et al.’s (2009) results supported the critical role of contin-
week delayed conditions, indicating that participants exposed to gency awareness in evaluative conditioning, albeit valence aware-
a classical conditioning procedure in which a CS was systematically ness, not identity awareness of the US. Sixteen university students
292 C. Pornpitakpan / Australasian Marketing Journal 20 (2012) 282–296

participated in Experiment 1, in which two sets (set A and set B) of effort from the participants, especially in the valence and identity
five neutrally evaluated, pronounceable nonwords were used as awareness tests. The answers given might be just random guesses.
CSs, and two sets of 25 pictures from the International Affective Second was the US pre-exposure effect – conditioning will not oc-
Pictures System were used as USs. The design was 2 (US valence: cur if people have been preexposed to the US alone. Third is the
positive versus negative) by 2 (CS set: set A and set B, both are po- blocking effect, which indicates that familiar USs should not be
sitive) with repeated measures on the US valence factor. During the used. Lastly, the study presented CS and US simultaneously on
experiment, five USs were randomly assigned to each CS for each the computer screen instead of having the CS preceding the US.
participant anew, creating 50 different CS–US pairs.
The experiment was conducted in individual computer-con-
trolled sessions in three phases: conditioning, evaluative ratings, 3.5. Studies using unfamiliar CSs, unfamiliar USs, and multiple CS–US
and awareness check. In the conditioning phase, participants pairings
watched 100 CS–US pairings (50 different CS–US pairs, each pre-
sented twice). The CS and US simultaneously appeared on the com- Cacioppo et al. (1992) conducted Experiment 1 on 43 students
puter screen for 2,000 ms with the US picture in the upper half of using a 2 (male versus female) by 3 (CS–US contingency: word fol-
the screen and the CS in the lower half. The presentation order was lowed by electric shock, nonword followed by electric shock, word
randomized anew for each participant. and nonword paired randomly with electric shock) by 2 (experi-
After the conditioning phase, participants evaluated each non- mental stimulus: word, nonword) mixed-model factorial design
word CS on a scale ranging from 1 (very unpleasant) to 8 (very in which biological sex and CS–US contingency served as be-
pleasant). Then, valence and identity awareness was assessed for tween-subjects factors and experimental stimulus served as a
each CS–US pair. Participants indicated for each CS whether they within-subjects factor. After completing a pretest, each participant
thought it had been paired with pleasant or unpleasant USs. Mean- was tested individually. Each participant determined the annoying
while, awareness for the US identity was probed through recogni- but not painful intensity of the electric shock to be used as the US.
tion tests five times for each CS, once for each of the five USs with The experimental session consisted of eight presentations of a
which it was paired. single word and eight presentations of a nonword. Each experi-
The results showed that for 15 out of 16 participants, valence- mental stimulus presentation lasted 7 s, and the inter-stimulus
aware CSs were obtained; for these valence-aware CSs, a signifi- interval was randomly varied between 30 and 40 s. In the word-
cant evaluative conditioning effect was observed across partici- shock condition, the offset of each word presentation was followed
pants. For 11 out of 16 participants, valence-unaware CSs were by a 0.25-s electric shock to the calf (US), whereas the nonword
obtained, and evaluative conditioning was absent when only va- was never followed by electric shock. Likewise, in the nonword-
lence-unaware CSs were considered. Furthermore, a regression shock condition, the offset of each nonword presentation was fol-
analysis revealed that identity awareness did not predict evalua- lowed by a 0.25-s electric shock to the calf (US), whereas the word
tive conditioning for both valence-aware and valence-unaware was never followed by electric shock. In both conditions, the order
CSs. In contrast, valence awareness significantly predicted evalua- of the 16 experimental stimulus presentations was randomized.
tive conditioning. In the random shock (pseudoconditioning) condition, the 16 7-s
Twenty-eight university students participated in Experiment 2, experimental stimulus (word and nonword) presentations were
in which the same USs as in Experiment 1 were used. However, ten randomly ordered with a 30–40 s inter-stimulus interval. A 0.25-
product pictures completed the set of CSs, five of which were s electric shock followed the offset of either the word or nonword
paired with positive USs and the remainder of which were paired on eight of the 16 experimental trials. On which eight trials the
with negative ones. The procedures of Experiment 2 were identical shock was executed was randomly determined. After the last stim-
to Experiment 1 with two exceptions. First, four evaluative ratings ulus presentation, participants rated the pleasantness and familiar-
(global impression, attractiveness, pleasantness, and willingness to ity of the same 15 six-letter words and 15 six-letter nonwords in
buy) were collected instead of only one. Second, the valence-mem- the pretest.
ory test and the identity-memory test both included an additional In line with Shimp et al. (1991), Cacioppo et al. (1992) found
‘‘don’t know’’ response option. that evaluative conditioning was most effective for unfamiliar
The results showed that in the US identity test, the correct re- CSs (unfamiliar words). In Experiment 2, 22 students read descrip-
sponse was selected in 17.3% of cases. In the valence awareness tions of the experimental stimuli and the CS–US contingency used
test, the correct response was selected in 57.1% of cases. For 26 in Experiment 1 in a 2 (experimental stimulus: word, nonword) by
participants, valence-aware CSs were present, and for these va- 3 (CS–US contingency: word followed by shock, nonword followed
lence-aware CSs, a significant evaluative conditioning effect was by shock, random pairings of word and nonword with electric
observed. For 13 participants, valence-unaware CSs were present, shock) by 2 (experimental instruction: predict participant’s rating,
and for these valence-unaware CSs, evaluative conditioning was predict experimenter’s expectation) within-subjects factorial de-
absent. The same applied to valence-undecided CSs. Identity sign. The results of Experiment 2 revealed that although contingen-
awareness did not predict evaluative ratings for valence-aware, va- cies between the CSs and the USs were explained and participants
lence-unaware, and valence-undecided CSs. tried to predict the experimenter’s hypothesis, they were unable to
Experiments 3 and 4 obtained similar results as those in the predict accurately the differential attitude change observed in
first two experiments. All of the experiments confirmed the impor- Experiment 1.
tant role of awareness (defined as recognition memory for the CS– Schemer et al. (2008) exposed 78 undergraduates to an evalua-
US pairings) for the emergence of evaluative conditioning effects tive conditioning experiment, in which previously neutral and un-
and supported the view that the impact of valence awareness on known brands represented CSs, and the actors who were not
evaluative ratings observed across all the experiments reflected known to the broad public in a music video functioned as a posi-
genuine valence acquisition rather than demand effects or other tively or negatively valenced US. The 2 by 2 between-subjects fac-
artifacts of the explicit evaluation process. torial design contained conditioning valence obtained through the
Because the evaluative conditioning found in Stahl et al.’s affective image of rap actors in a music video (positive versus neg-
(2009) research entails awareness of the CS–US contingencies, it ative image) and occurrence of brand placements (placement pres-
may be viewed as classical conditioning. Some cautions apply to ent versus absent, and participants were randomly assigned to one
the obtained effects. First, the experiments required tremendous of the four conditions.
C. Pornpitakpan / Australasian Marketing Journal 20 (2012) 282–296 293

Understanding that the study dealt with viewers’ evaluation of In testing the effect of attitude toward the ad on attitude toward
rap music videos, participants read a newspaper article, which the brand from a classical conditioning perspective, Gresham and
manipulated the image of the rap actors. Then they viewed the Shimp (1985) paired an advertised brand with an affectively valen-
rap video on a personal computer for slightly more than four min- ced TV commercial. It was found that only a small portion of the
utes. Equipped with headsets, they were administered in groups of positive- and negative-affect ads showed attitude toward the ad
6–8 individuals in an advertising laboratory with no disturbance to be a predictor of attitude toward the brand, thus failing to sup-
among participants. After watching the clip, they filled an online port the classical conditioning mechanism. Negative-affect ads
questionnaire consisting of measures of manipulation checks, seemed to elicit more of a classical conditioning response than
dependent variables, music preferences, and demographics. In did positive-affect ones. The hypotheses, which were designed to
the placement version of the clip, the placement of the target brand rule out mechanisms other than classical conditioning, were not
occurred 29 times simultaneously with the rap actors. The single supported. Part of the inability to support the classical condition-
appearance duration did not exceed 2 s. ing mechanism in Gresham and Shimp’s study might be traced to
The manipulation checks showed that participants reading the use of familiar TV commercials for mostly mature brands in
about negative characteristics of the artists evaluated the artists established product categories. Thus, attitude toward the brand
more negatively than did those reading about positive characteris- might influence attitude toward the ad, not the other way around.
tics, indicating a successful manipulation of the image of the rap Macklin (1986) exposed preschool children thrice to either an
actors (or the conditioning valence). The results showed that neg- orange or a yellow pencil under three conditions (forward condi-
atively conditioned individuals had a more negative attitude to- tioning, simultaneous conditioning, and random control) with a
ward the brand in the placement condition compared to those picture of Smurf as an US. No conditioning effect was found –
who saw the rap video without placement, while positively condi- the color selected did not relate to the buy-back variable, replicat-
tioned participants had a more positive attitude toward the brand ing Allen and Madden (1985). The lack of support for any classical
in the placement condition compared to those who saw the rap vi- conditioning effect in Macklin’s study might be due to insufficient
deo without placement. In contrast, in the no-placement condition, number of pairings, a Garcia effect, an US pre-exposure effect, a
the attitude toward the brand did not differ as a function of condi- blocking effect, a latent inhibition effect, a small sample size, and
tioning valence, indicating that the attitudes toward the brand im- absence of contingency and demand awareness in children
proved as a function of the pairing of a brand with favorable rap participants.
actors, and the evaluation of a brand decreased when the brand Another case against the classical conditioning theory was by
co-occurred with negatively evaluated rap actors. Moreover, attitu- Kellaris and Cox (1989). Experiment 1 followed Gorn’s (1982) pro-
dinal conditioning effects were stronger in individuals with higher cedures but used classical music, a yellow pen, a less obtrusive
preference for rap music, suggesting that participants liking rap choice procedure, and a new cover story. It used a 2 (liked versus
music could be conditioned both positively and negatively. Finally, disliked music) by 2 (Gorn’s versus new cover story) factorial de-
evaluative conditioning effects were more likely to occur when sign. The music appeal was found to have no effect on choice
viewers did not recognize the brand embeds in the rap video. behavior. The cover story treatment did not affect the guessing of
Some reservations applied to the evaluative conditioning effect true hypotheses. Experiment 2 examined the role of demand arti-
found in Schemer et al. (2008). First, the Garcia effect suggests that facts in Gorn’s experiment. Treatments like those used by Gorn
the US and the CS should be matched properly. Not any stimulus were described rather than administered, as suggested by Sawyer
can be used as a CS or an US, and they should belong together log- (1975). Strikingly, Gorn’s results were replicated without actual
ically. Second, the study utilized a simultaneous conditioning pro- treatments. Twenty-nine percent of the participants asked to
cedure instead of having the CS preceding the US. report the purpose of the study correctly guessed the main
hypothesis.
Experiment 3 followed Gorn’s (1982) study with an equally
4. Evidence against classical conditioning effects strong music appeal, using a 2 (high versus low music appeal) by
2 (Gorn’s cover story and choice procedures versus more disguised
Due to the small number of studies in this section, they are ar- procedures) design. Neither procedures yielded significant rela-
ranged by publication year instead of the methodological classifi- tionships between the music appeal and the pen choice. However,
cation scheme used in Section 3. Several studies did not find there was a significant relationship between the procedure and the
support for the classical conditioning effects. Allen and Madden guessing of true hypotheses in that 12 of the 15 participants who
(1985) adapted Gorn’s (1982) procedures by processing partici- correctly guessed the hypothesis had received Gorn’s procedure.
pants individually, using a different pen choice method, conduct- Kellaris and Cox (1989) was one of the apparent demonstrations
ing a more systematic post-experimental inquiry, using humor as of demand artifacts in classical conditioning literature although
an US, using a green and a black pen as CSs, and adding a buy-back some drawbacks existed in the experiments. The critical choice
measure. They could not replicate Gorn’s results. Only in the liked measurement would be void if participants did not follow the
US condition was there an effect significantly different from a ran- experimenter’s suggestion of trying the new pen in answering
dom choice, but Allen and Madden (1985, p. 309) attributed it to the questionnaires (the ink color differed to designate which pen
the demand awareness among participants. There were no rela- color the participants had chosen) or if the color participants in-
tionships between the color-selection and buy-back variables. tended to pick up had all gone from the box. Furthermore, because
The results of Allen and Madden were not surprising because the procedures were similar to Gorn’s, there was little chance that
the design was not much stronger than Gorn’s (1982). The experi- classical conditioning could occur.
mental conditions were difficult for classical conditioning to occur As presented earlier, the results of Shimp et al. (1991) showed
due to a single simultaneous pairing of the CS and the US, the use that the conditioning groups in 11 of the 21 experiments exhibited
of familiar CSs, and possibly a Garcia effect (i.e., an improper match significantly more positive attitudes toward the CS brands than the
between the CS and the US). Perhaps the most crucial factor for the random control groups while the remaining 10 experiments did
lack of any conditioning effect was the fact that participants re- not obtain significant classical conditioning effects. Blair and
vealed very little contingency awareness of the CS and the US. This Shimp (1992) found no significant difference between the non-
furnished another support for the above-mentioned view that clas- preexposed conditioning group and the control group. In
sical conditioning is mediated by contingency awareness. other words, the US was not salient enough to produce positive
294 C. Pornpitakpan / Australasian Marketing Journal 20 (2012) 282–296

attitudinal conditioning for participants in the non-preexposed procedures employed to communicate information and the mea-
group. Tom (1995) found that the use of negative music as an US sures of the effect of these communications. Small procedural
did not affect the preference for either the advertised pen or adver- changes can significantly affect the learning that is obtained in a
tised Chinese ideograph. One of the explanations for this null effect session. The findings of each study thus depend on the selection
of the negative US [which was in line with the findings of Allen and of the CS and the US, the strength of the unconditioned response,
Madden (1985)] offered by Tom was that the negative music em- inter-trial interval, temporal priority of the CS and the US, number
ployed was not only disliked but also perceived to be incongruent of trials, training environment, test environment, test distracters,
for the stated purpose. CS consistency, control conditions, subject populations, and so
Shimp and Moody (2000) exposed 69 undergraduates to three forth.
conditions, namely (i) Feinberg (1986) condition – VISA card stim- Even though classical conditioning effects might really exist,
ulus present both at product encoding and rating, (ii) VISA card ab- they might not provide much usefulness in marketing given that
sent – VISA card stimulus present during encoding of product the affective response changes in consumers do not necessarily
information but not at rating, and (iii) control – VISA card stimulus lead to actual purchases and the ideal conditions for classical con-
absent during both encoding of product information and rating. ditioning effects to occur are rather difficult to meet in real life.
The VISA card served as an US. Participants were told they were Specifically, the advertised brand (the CS) will have low predictive-
participating in a study simulating a new catalog order center ness of the US and thus low chance to elicit the desired uncondi-
method of shopping. No significant effect was found. The absence tioned response if (i) both the CS and the US are presented
of classical conditioning effect in Shimp and Moody’s study might simultaneously (which is the case in print ads and even in some
be due to the US pre-exposure effect and the CS lacking predictive- television commercials); (ii) the CS is presented constantly, fol-
ness of the US. Besides, participants revealed very little contin- lowed occasionally by the US (consumers are likely to encounter
gency and demand awareness. the brand many times in real life without the presence of the US
Finally, as presented earlier, Gibson (2008) found that the portrayed in the ad); and (iii) either the US or the CS is frequently
conditioning procedure had no effect on implicit attitudes for encountered alone (which is usually the case in consumers’ daily
either the Coke-lover or Pepsi-lover groups. As for explicit brand life). Marketers who are keen to utilize a classical conditioning pro-
attitudes, participants who reported strong pretest preferences cedure to elicit certain consumer responses should therefore be
continued with those preferences and those who were neutral on aware of its limited chance of success in real marketplace.
the pretest remained neutral after the conditioning procedure. Most studies reviewed shared the following weaknesses: (i)
Furthermore, there was no significant main effect for the condi- using familiar CSs and USs, thus constituting the CS lacking predic-
tioning procedure and no interaction between conditioning and tiveness of the US, the blocking effect, the US pre-exposure effect,
pretest preference. and the latent inhibition effect; and (ii) lacking any pretest to en-
sure that the CS and the US match each other logically and percep-
5. Conclusion tually. Some studies (e.g., Feinberg, 1986; Gorn, 1982; the mood
induction experiment of Groenland and Schoormans, 1994; Lie
This study reviews empirical research regarding classical condi- et al., 2010) employed a single pairing of CSs and USs and yet found
tioning effects on consumer behavior. The majority of the reviewed significant classical conditioning effects. While classical condition-
studies reported significant results supporting classical condition- ing might occur after one trial, it usually entails a very strong US
ing effects. This is not surprising given the fact that studies with such as an intense shock or a nauseating drug, which has hardly
significant findings have a higher chance to appear in international been used in classical conditioning research in consumer behavior
refereed journals than studies with null effects. for ethical reasons.
The review shows that in cases where classical conditioning ef- Given the prevalent methodological deficiencies and strong
fects were found, the results were rather dubious due to deficien- demand artifacts of past studies, future research testing classical
cies in methodology and/or possible demand artifacts. In most of conditioning effects on consumer behavior should try to improve
these studies, CS–US contingency awareness was necessary for classi- all of the following aspects within the same experiment to increase
cal conditioning effects to emerge. Alternatively, the effects could be internal validity of the research:
explained by some other mechanisms such as the mere exposure
effect and pseudoconditioned responses if no proper control proce- (i) Making sure that the CS predicts the US by using unfamiliar
dures were employed. In cases where classical conditioning effects CSs and USs and by having the CS precede the US.
were not supported, the non-significant results could be attributed (ii) Pretesting that the CS and the US match each other logically
to violation of the conditions for classical conditioning to occur, ab- and perceptually.
sence of CS–US contingency awareness and demand awareness, or (iii) Using striking CSs and USs and allowing longer inter-trial
lack of statistical power due to a small sample size. intervals.
Based on the above evaluation, it seems legitimate to conclude (iv) Including various filler materials to reduce participants’
that to date, not much convincing evidence exists for classical condi- hypothesis guessing.
tioning effects in consumer behavior and advertising contexts. (v) Including a random control group, which is exposed to the
Whether this is because (i) the theory itself is false (although a the- same number of CSs and USs as the experimental group
ory cannot be falsified by lack of evidence), (ii) the theory is valid but these stimuli are presented randomly with respect to
but cannot be extended to affective responses in consumers who each other.
are far different from animals on which the theory is based, (iii) (vi) Including a demand-artifact-checking group, which does not
the methodology for testing and measuring classical conditioning receive the same treatment as the experimental group but
effects on consumer behavior has not been up to the task, or (iv) merely reads the description of the treatment, in order to
classical conditioning effects on humans may occur under specific compare the results with the experimental group.
boundary conditions only, remains to be uncovered. As raised by (vii) Measuring CS–US contingency awareness and hypothesis
Janiszewski and Warlop (1993), one reason underlying the incon- guessing in every group.
sistencies in consumer conditioning literature is the flexibility in (viii) Using both negative and positive USs to see whether classi-
human learning mechanisms. Humans are very sensitive to the cal conditioning effects are present in both.
C. Pornpitakpan / Australasian Marketing Journal 20 (2012) 282–296 295

(ix) Manipulating the number of CS–US pairings and the inter- Gibson, B., 2008. Can evaluative conditioning change attitudes toward mature
brands? New evidence from the implicit association test. Journal of Consumer
trial intervals to see at what levels classical conditioning
Research 35 (June), 178–188.
effects can be observed. Gorn, G.J., 1982. The effects of music in advertising on choice behavior: A classical
conditioning approach. Journal of Marketing 46 (Winter), 94–101.
Once studies with more valid methodology have been Greenwald, A.G., Leavitt, C., 1984. Audience involvement in advertising: Four levels.
Journal of Consumer Research 11 (June), 581–592.
conducted and enough supporting evidence has been accumulated, Greenwald, A.G., McGhee, D.E., Schwartz, J.L.K., 1998. Measuring individual
future research can then proceed to examine the boundary condi- differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of
tions for classical conditioning effects to emerge and not to Personality and Social Psychology 74 (June), 1464–1480.
Gresham, L.G., Shimp, T.A., 1985. Attitude toward the advertisement and brand
emerge. Cross-cultural experiments may also be conducted to attitudes: A classical conditioning perspective. Journal of Advertising 14 (1), 10–
investigate whether cultural upbringing prompts people to re- 17, 49.
spond differentially to classical conditioning procedures. Groenland, E.A.G., Schoormans, J.P.L., 1994. Comparing mood-induction and
affective conditioning as mechanisms influencing product evaluation and
product choice. Psychology & Marketing 11, 183–197.
Grossman, R.P., Till, B.D., 1998. The persistence of classically conditioned brand
References attitudes. Journal of Advertising 27 (1), 23–31.
Hammerl, M., 2000. I like it, but only when I’m not sure why: Evaluative
Allen, C.T., Janiszewski, C.A., 1989. Assessing the role of contingency awareness in conditioning and the awareness issue. Consciousness and Cognition 9, 37–40.
attitudinal conditioning with implications for advertising research. Journal of Hammerl, M., Grabitz, H.J., 1996. Human evaluative conditioning without
Marketing Research 26 (February), 30–43. experiencing a valued event. Learning and Motivation 27, 278–293.
Allen, C.T., Madden, T.J., 1985. A closer look at classical conditioning. Journal of Hunt, J.M., Florsheim, R.A., Chatterjee, A., Kernan, J.B., 1990. Credit cards as
Consumer Research 12 (December), 301–315. spending-facilitating stimuli: A test and extension of Feinberg’s conditioning
Baer, P.E., Fuhrer, M.J., 1982. Cognitive factors in the concurrent differential hypothesis. Psychological Reports 67, 323–330.
conditioning of eyelid and skin conductance responses. Memory & Cognition 10 Janiszewski, C., Warlop, L., 1993. The influence of classical conditioning procedures
(March), 135–140. on subsequent attention to the conditioned brand. Journal of Consumer
Baeyens, F., Crombez, G., Van den Bergh, O., Eelen, P., 1988. Once in contact always Research 20 (September), 171–189.
in contact: Evaluative conditioning is resistant to extinction. Advances in Kamin, L.J., 1969. Predictability, surprise, attention and conditioning. In: Campbell,
Behavioral Research and Therapy 10, 179–199. B.A., Church, R.M. (Eds.), Punishment and Aversive Behavior. Appleton-Century-
Baeyens, F., De Houwer, J., 1995. Evaluative conditioning is a qualitatively distinct Crofts, New York, NY, pp. 279–296.
form of classical conditioning. Behaviour Research and Therapy 33, 825–831. Kamin, L.J., Brimer, C.J., 1963. The effects of intensity of conditioned and
Baeyens, F., De Houwer, J., Vansteenwegen, D., Eelen, P., 1998. Evaluative unconditioned stimuli on a conditioned emotional response. Canadian Journal
conditioning is a form of associative learning: On the artifactual nature of of Psychology 17 (2), 194–200.
Field and Davey’s (1997) artifactual account of evaluative learning. Learning Kassarjian, H.H., 1986. Consumer research: Some recollections and a commentary.
and Motivation 29, 461–474. In: Lutz, R.J. (Ed.), Advances in Consumer Research, vol. 13. Association for
Baeyens, F., Eelen, P., Van den Bergh, O., 1990. Contingency awareness in evaluative Consumer Research, Provo, UT, pp. 6–8.
conditioning: A case of unaware affective-evaluative learning. Cognition and Kellaris, J.J., Cox, A.D., 1989. The effects of background music in advertising: A
Emotion 4, 3–18. reassessment. Journal of Consumer Research 16 (June), 113–118.
Baeyens, F., Hermans, D., Eelen, P., 1993. The role of CS–US contingency in human Kim, J., Allen, C.T., Kardes, F.R., 1996. An investigation of the mediational
evaluative conditioning. Behaviour Research and Therapy 31, 731–737. mechanisms underlying attitudinal conditioning. Journal of Marketing
Baker, W.E., 1999. When can affective conditioning and mere exposure directly Research 33 (August), 318–328.
influence brand choice? Journal of Advertising 28, 31–46. Kim, J., Lim, J.S., Bhargava, M., 1998. The role of affect in attitude formation: A
Barnes-Holmes, D., Keane, J., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Smeets, P.M., 2000. A derived classical conditioning approach. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science
transfer of emotive functions as a means of establishing differential preferences 26 (2), 142–152.
for soft drinks. Psychological Record 50 (3), 493–511. Lie, C., Hunt, M., Peters, H.L., Veliu, B., Harper, D., 2010. The ‘negative’ credit card
Bierley, C., McSweeney, F.K., Vannieuwkerk, R., 1985. Classical conditioning of effect: Credit cards as spending-limiting stimuli in New Zealand. Psychological
preferences for stimuli. Journal of Consumer Research 12 (December), 316–323. Record 60, 399–412.
Biferno, M.A., Dawson, M.E., 1977. The onset of contingency awareness and Lovibond, P.F., Shanks, D.R., 2002. The role of awareness in Pavlovian conditioning:
electrodermal classical conditioning: An analysis of temporal relationships Empirical evidence and theoretical implications. Journal of Experimental
during acquisition and extinction. Psychophysiology 14 (March), 164–171. Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 28, 3–26.
Blair, M.E., Shimp, T.A., 1992. Consequences of an unpleasant experience with Lubow, R.E., 1973. Latent inhibition. Psychological Bulletin 79 (6), 398–407.
music: A second-order negative conditioning perspective. Journal of Advertising MacKenzie, S.B., Lutz, R.J., Belch, G.E., 1986. The role of attitude toward the ad as a
21 (1), 35–43. mediator of advertising effectiveness: A test of competing explanations. Journal
Brown, P.L., Jenkins, H.M., 1968. Auto-shaping of the pigeon’s key-peck. Journal of of Marketing Research 23 (May), 130–143.
the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 11 (1), 1–8. Macklin, M.C., 1986. Classical conditioning effects in product/character pairings
Cacioppo, J.T., Marshall-Goodell, B.S., Tassinary, L.G., Petty, R.E., 1992. Rudimentary presented to children. In: Lutz, R.J. (Ed.), Advances in Consumer Research, vol.
determinants of attitudes: Classical conditioning is more effective when prior 13. Association for Consumer Research, Provo, UT, pp. 198–203.
knowledge about the attitude stimulus is low than high. Journal of Maus Media Group, 2011, March 8. How Much Does It Cost to Make a TV
Experimental Social Psychology 28 (May), 207–233. Commercial? Available from: <http://mausmediagroup.wordpress.com/2011/
Cosmopolitan, 2012. 2012 Display Advertising Rates. Available from: <http:// 03/08/how-much-does-it-cost-to-make-a-tv-commercial>.
www.cosmomediakit.com/hotdata/publishers/cosmopoli2521681/categories/ Martindale, C., 1991. Cognitive Psychology: A Neural-Network Approach. Brooks/
Rates_Section.pdf>. Cole, Pacific Grove, CA.
Crupi, A., 2011. In Their Prime: Broadcast Spot Costs Soar. Available from: <http:// McCall, M., Belmont, H.J., 1996. Credit card insignia and restaurant tipping:
www.adweek.com/news/television/their-prime-broadcast-spot-costs-soar- Evidence for an associative link. Journal of Applied Psychology 81, 609–613.
132805>. McCall, M., Trombetta, J., Gipe, A., 2004. Credit cues and impression management: A
Dawson, M.E., 1973. Can classical conditioning occur without contingency learning? preliminary attempt to explain the credit card effect. Psychological Reports 95,
A review and evaluation of the evidence. Psychophysiology 10 (January), 82–86. 331–337.
De Houwer, J., Thomas, S., Baeyens, F., 2001. Associative learning likes and dislikes: McSweeney, F.K., Bierley, C., 1984. Recent developments in classical conditioning.
A review of 25 years of research on human evaluative conditioning. Journal of Consumer Research 11 (September), 619–631.
Psychological Bulletin 127, 853–869. Mis, F.W., Moore, J.W., 1973. Effect of preacquisition UCS stimulus exposure on
Edell, J.A., Burke, M.C., 1984. The moderating effect of attitude toward an ad on ad classical conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. Learning
effectiveness under different processing conditions. In: Kinnear, T.C. (Ed.), and Motivation 4 (1), 108–114.
Advances in Consumer Research, vol. 11. Association for Consumer Research, Monger, J.E., Feinberg, R.A., 1997. Mode of payment and formation of reference
Provo, UT, pp. 644–649. prices. Pricing Strategy and Practice 5, 142–147.
Feinberg, R., 1986. Credit cards as spending facilitating stimuli: A conditioning Olson, M.A., Fazio, R.H., 2001. Implicit attitude formation through classical
interpretation. Journal of Consumer Research 13 (3), 348–356. conditioning. Psychological Science 12 (5), 413–417.
Field, A.P., 2000. I like it, but I’m not sure why: Can evaluative conditioning occur Pavlov, I.P., 1927. Conditioned Reflexes. Oxford University Press, Oxford, England.
without conscious awareness? Consciousness and Cognition 9, 13–36. Perruchet, P., 1985. Expectancy for airpuff and conditioned eyeblinks in humans.
Field, A.P., Davey, G.C.L., 1999. Reevaluating evaluative conditioning: A Acta Psychologica 58 (January), 31–44.
nonassociative explanation of conditioning effects in the visual evaluative Petty, R.E., Cacioppo, J.T., Schumann, D., 1983. Central and peripheral routes to
conditioning paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior advertising effectiveness: The moderating role of involvement. Journal of
Processes 25, 211–224. Consumer Research 10 (September), 135–146.
Garcia, J., Koelling, R.A., 1966. Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Prelec, D., Simester, D., 2001. Always leave home without it: A further investigation
Psychonomic Science 4 (3), 123–124. of the credit-card effect on willingness to pay. Marketing Letters 12, 5–12.
296 C. Pornpitakpan / Australasian Marketing Journal 20 (2012) 282–296

Preston, I.L., 1982. The association model of the advertising communication process. Shimp, T.A., Stuart, E.W., Engle, R.W., 1991. A program of classical conditioning
Journal of Advertising 11 (2), 3–15. experiments testing variations in the conditioned stimulus and context. Journal
Priluck, R., Till, B.D., 2004. The role of contingency awareness, involvement, and of Consumer Research 18 (June), 1–12.
need for cognition in attitude formation. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Shurtleff, D., Ayres, J.J.B., 1981. One-trial backward excitatory fear conditioning in
Science 32 (3), 329–404. rats: Acquisition, retention, extinction and spontaneous recovery. Animal
Rachman, S., 1991. Neo-conditioning and the classical theory of fear acquisition. Learning and Behavior 9 (1), 65–74.
Clinical Psychology Review 11 (2), 155–173. Spetch, M.L., Wilkie, D.M., Pinel, J.P., 1981. Backward conditioning: A reevaluation of
Raghubir, P., Srivastava, J., 2008. Monopoly money: The effect of payment coupling the empirical evidence. Psychological Bulletin 89 (January), 163–175.
and form on spending behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied Staats, A.W., Staats, C.K., 1959. Effect of number of trials on the language
14, 213–225. conditioning of meaning. Journal of General Psychology 61 (October), 211–223.
Razran, G., 1938. Conditioning away social bias by the luncheon technique. Stahl, C., Unkelbach, C., Corneille, O., 2009. On the respective contributions of
Psychological Bulletin 35, 693. awareness of unconditioned stimulus valence and unconditioned stimulus
Rescorla, R.A., 1967. Pavlovian conditioning and its proper control procedures. identity in attitude formation through evaluative conditioning. Journal of
Psychological Review 74 (1), 71–80. Personality and Social Psychology 97 (3), 404–420.
Rescorla, R.A., 1972. Informational variables in Pavlovian conditioning. The Stuart, E.W., Shimp, T.A., Engle, R.W., 1987. Classical conditioning of consumer
Psychology of Learning and Motivation 6, 1–46. attitudes: Four experiments in an advertising context. Journal of Consumer
Rescorla, R.A., 1973. Effect of US habituation following conditioning. Journal of Research 14 (December), 334–349.
Comparative and Physiological Psychology 82 (1), 137–143. Terrace, H.S., Gibbon, J., Farrell, L., Baldock, M.D., 1975. Temporal factors influencing
Rescorla, R.A., 1988. Pavlovian conditioning: It’s not what you think it is. American the acquisition and maintenance of an autoshaped keypeck. Animal Learning
Psychologist 43 (March), 151–160. and Behavior 3 (1), 53–62.
Rescorla, R.A., Wagner, A.R., 1972. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in Till, B.D., 1998. Using celebrity endorsers effectively: Lessons from associative
the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In: Black, A.H., learning. Journal of Product and Brand Management 7 (5), 400–409.
Prokasy, W.F. (Eds.), Classical Conditioning II. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New Tom, G., 1995. Classical conditioning of unattended stimuli. Psychology &
York, pp. 64–99. Marketing 12, 79–87.
Rozin, P., Wrzesniewski, A., Byrnes, D., 1998. The elusiveness of evaluative Wagner, A.R., Siegel, S., Thomas, E., Ellison, G.D., 1964. Reinforcement history and
conditioning. Learning and Motivation 29, 397–415. the extinction of a conditioned salivary response. Journal of Comparative and
Sawyer, A.G., 1975. Demand artifacts in laboratory experiments in consumer Physiological Psychology 58 (3), 354–358.
research. Journal of Consumer Research 1 (March), 20–29. Walther, E., 2002. Guilty by mere association: Evaluative conditioning and the
Schemer, C., Matthes, J., Wirth, W., Textor, S., 2008. Does ‘passing the courvoisier’ spreading attitude effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 82, 919–
always pay off? Positive and negative evaluative conditioning effects of brand 934.
placements in music videos. Psychology & Marketing 25 (10), 923–943. Walther, E., Grigoriadis, S., 2004. Why sad people like shoes better: The influence of
Schiffman, L.G., Kanuk, L.L., 2010. Consumer Behavior, 10th ed. Prentice Hall, Upper mood on the evaluative conditioning of consumer attitudes. Psychology &
Saddle River, NJ. Marketing 21, 75–773.
Shimp, T.A., Moody, M.P., 2000. In search of a theoretical explanation for the credit Zajonc, R.R., 1968. Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and
card effect. Journal of Business Research 48 (1), 17–23. Social Psychology Monograph Supplement 9 (Part 2, June), 1–27.

You might also like