Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

The Price Knowledge and Search of Supermarket Shoppers

Author(s): Peter R. Dickson and Alan G. Sawyer


Source: Journal of Marketing, Vol. 54, No. 3 (Jul., 1990), pp. 42-53
Published by: American Marketing Association
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1251815
Accessed: 26-05-2015 20:04 UTC

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/
info/about/policies/terms.jsp

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content
in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship.
For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

American Marketing Association is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Journal of Marketing.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 138.251.14.35 on Tue, 26 May 2015 20:04:21 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Peter R. Dickson & Alan G. Sawyer

The Price and


Knowledge Search
of Supermarket
Shoppers
A model of grocery shopper response to price and other point-of-purchase information was developed
and hypotheses were tested by using observations and interviews. The findings suggest that shoppers
tended to spend only a short time making their selection and many did not check the price of the item
they selected. Perhaps as a consequence, more than half could not correctly name the price of the item
just placed in the shopping cart and more than half of the shoppers who purchased an item that was
on special were unaware that the price was reduced. Other results on point-of-purchase information pro-
cessing and behavior are discussed.

THE point-of-purchase behavior of grocery shop- (Monroe 1971), adaptation level (Helson 1964), as-
pers is of interest to three major disciplines. In similation-contrast (Sherif 1963), acceptable range
classical economics, consumers are assumed to have (Gabor and Granger 1961), and prospect theory
high knowledge at the point of purchase about the price (Kahnemann and Tversky 1979) explicity posit the
of at least the chosen alternative. Marketing science existence of referent prices that form the basis for per-
models of choice behavior assume, at least implicitly, ceptions of new prices (see reviews by Monroe 1973,
that shoppers are aware of both price and any tem- Nagle 1987, Sawyer and Dickson 1984, and Winer
porary price reduction (e.g., Guadagni and Little 1983; 1986).
Gurumurthy and Little 1987; Raman and Bass 1989; The price knowledge results of past studies (e.g.,
Winer 1986). Finally, psychological theories of con- Allen, Harrell, and Hutt 1976; Conover 1986, Pro-
sumer information processing propose that price in- gressive Grocer 1964, 1975) have suggested that about
formation at the point of purchase will be encoded, 50% of shoppers know the exact price of items they
evaluated, and integrated into memory. For example, have purchased. The consistent reaction of research-
psychological models of price perception such as the ers is surprise and concern that knowledge is so low.
Weber-Fechner law of just noticeable differences In reviewing these studies, Zeithaml (1988, p. 10) re-
marked that price knowledge appears to be "consid-
erably lower than necessary for consumers to have ac-
PeterR.Dickson is the CraneProfessor of Marketing,
TheOhioState curate internal reference prices for many products."
University.AlanG.Sawyeris Professor of MarketingandDepartment One explanation for this low knowledge is that shop-
Chair,Universityof Florida.Theauthorsthankthe Marketing Science
Insititue
forpartial pers may note price at the point of purchase but do
fundingof the project. Bothauthorsalsoreceived
not try to remember it for any length of time. In pre-
support fromTheOhioStateUniversity Collegeof Administrative
Sci-
enceintheformof a Dean'sResearch Grantanda Marketing Faculty vious studies shoppers were asked to recall price at
Research Grant.TheyalsothankJoeAlba,ChrisJaniszewski, Rosemary least several minutes after item choice. Thus shop-
Kalapurakal,RichLutz,JohnLynch, DianeSchmalensee,
LeighMcAlister, pers' price knowledge at the point of choice may be
BartWeitz, andfouranonymous JMreviewers forusefulcommentsand
criticismson draftsof the article.Finally, high but quickly drops as the shopper progresses to
theythankthe supermarket
company managers andthestoremanagers whopermittedthestudy. considering the next purchase.
We examine the behavior of supermarket shoppers

Journal of Marketing
42 / Journalof Marketing,July 1990 Vol. 54 (July 1990), 42-53

This content downloaded from 138.251.14.35 on Tue, 26 May 2015 20:04:21 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
at the point of purchase. The study objectives are to without attending to or evaluating its actual price
describe price checking and price knowledge imme- (Inman, McAlister, and Hoyer, in press). The Figure
diately after the shopper has chosen an item and to 1 feedback loop from encoding and categorizationto
assess the level of awareness of whether the chosen attentionto the objective price occurs because point-
item was on special (a temporaryreduction).The con- of-purchasesignalsof a specialpricemay lead to greater
sistent finding from past research that price knowl- checking of the item's price against the prices of al-
edge is not high is curiousgiven shoppers'statedhigh ternatives. However, the different sources of objec-
importance of price (Progressive Grocer 1975, 1981; tive informationabout the price statusand price of an
Zbytniewski 1979), the many studies that find strong item may be informationsubstitutes.For example, if
price sensitivity (e.g., Guadagni and Little 1983; noting an item's special price status in an advertise-
Wilkinson, Mason, and Paksoy 1982), and the price ment leads to a purchaseintention, it may reducelater
knowledge and search assumptions underlying most in-store price search.
economic and psychological theories of price usage The amount of attention paid to the price, the
and perception.Our study was designed to updatethe memorabilityof characteristicsof the price, and the
results of past studies undertakenin informationen- use of the price in makingthe purchasedecision affect
vironments very different from those in today's su- both the encoding into memory and recall from mem-
permarketsand to improveon their methods (Dickson ory of the price information.This storedprice, either
and Sawyer 1986a). Asking shoppersabouttheirprice- alone or combined with past prices, then becomes the
checking behavior and price knowledge immediately expected or reference price (see Helson 1964). Sev-
afterthey had selected the item in questionminimized eral price perception theories posit the existence of
any measurementlag biasesdue to forgetting.Ourstudy internalreference prices that form the basis for eval-
is the first field study to investigate shoppers'knowl- uation of new prices, helping the shopperjudge the
edge about the presence and size of price deals and price of a new brandand the extent of any price dis-
to comparedifferencesin price knowledgefor regular- count (see reviews by Monroe 1973, Monroe and
priced and special-priceditems. Petroshius 1981, and Sawyer and Dickson 1984).
Several models that explain individual shopping
A Conceptual Model of Price behavior over time provide indirect evidence that
shoppersrememberprices and develop internalstan-
Encoding and Knowledge dards. For example, Winer (1986) reportedthat the
The conceptualmodel underlyingthe hypothesestested addition of the consumer's normal reference price of
is illustratedin Figure 1. It is a modificationof Jacoby coffee brands, as mathematicallyestimatedfrom past
and Olson's (1977) informationprocessingmodel (see actualprices paid, improveda brandchoice model that
also Zeithaml 1982, 1984), which makes an important originallycontainedonly the observedprice at the time
distinctionbetween objective and psychological price of purchase. Raman and Bass (1989) similarly as-
information.The shopper's psychological price may sumed quantitative reference prices in consumers'
be representedin the shopper'smind as a quantitative minds. They found supportfor predictionsfrom pros-
price estimate and/or a subjective image. pect theorythatthe negativepurchasereactionsto prices
We include store advertisingof a special price and above reference prices would be larger than the pos-
the point-of-purchase label as additionalobjectiveprice itive reactions to prices lower than the assumed ref-
cues in Jacoby and Olson's model. The initial cate- erence prices.
gorizationand perceptionof an item's price as either The outcome of comparing, contrasting, and as-
a special (temporary)reduction or regular is high- similating the psychological price with the internal
lightedin our priceinformationprocessingmodel. Such referencepriceand otheritems' prices(see Sherif 1963)
perceptualcategorizationwill precede and influence may influence the long-term storage of the psycho-
any evaluative categorizationof the acceptability of logical price. This effect is reflected in the feedback
the price (see Cohen and Basu 1987). As the attitu- loops in the lower half of the model in Figure 1. For
dinal categorizationof price will influence behavior, example, if the price is contrasted,viewed to be very
the initial perceptionof the price as special or regular low, and hence evaluated positively, it may be re-
is likely to have a vital role in determiningindividual memberedbetter. Hence, in the aggregate, a special
behavior and aggregatedemand. price may be recalled better than a regularprice. If
The coding of an item's price as regularor special the past reference price is not replaced but instead
may or may not requirethe processing of the item's merely adjusted by the most recent price paid, as
actualprice. A featureof the item in an advertisement Winer's and Raman and Bass' mathematicalmodels
or flier, a special supermarket aisle displayof the item, suggest, exact recall of price last paid will be poor,
or a special shelf price tag all may enable a shopper butapproximaterecallof pricelast paidshouldbe good.
to conclude that the item is selling at a "special"price The reasonis thatafterassimilationof the psychologi-

PriceKnowledge
andSearchof Supermarket
Shoppers
/43

This content downloaded from 138.251.14.35 on Tue, 26 May 2015 20:04:21 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
FIGURE 1
Conceptual Model of Price Encoding and Knowledge

ObjectiveAdvertising

ObjectivePrice QbjectivePoint-of-Purchase
label

\-,T /
Attentionto ObjectivePrice
* Checkedprice
* Recognizedpricelabel

.,I

'1 Enco&
Encod
ing andCategorization
of ObjectivePrice
.II ^. . Encodedit as special(reduced)priceor regular x.--..

,------------------- Storageof PsychologicalPrice


* Pricerecallaccuracy -:-;;, -------:-. - ----
* Speedof response
* Pricedifferencerecallaccuracy --- -

-S, AttitudeTowardPsychologicalPriceandCategorization
of PriceImage
* Brandpriceimage
* Storepriceimage

- ... Integration
of PsychologicalPricewithOtherPriceInformation

V
Volume Comparisions
/
BrandandStoreComparisions

Purchase

44 / Journalof Marketing,
July1990

This content downloaded from 138.251.14.35 on Tue, 26 May 2015 20:04:21 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
cal price shoppers have only the resulting reference cial price should have greater price accuracy. Several
price available to estimate the item's price. The mem- of the quantitative models of the effect of price pro-
ory trace of the actual price paid will decay quickly motions on purchase behavior also assume that the
and be lost because all of its useful information has amount of the price reduction is noted by the shopper.
been incorporated into updating the reference price. The measures we expect to be affected are price
However, because the psychological price was assim- checking of the chosen brand, price checking of other
ilated rather than contrasted, the reference price should brands and sizes, use of unit price information, rec-
be close to the actual price paid. ognition of the presence of the distinctive price special
When an item is offered at a special price, the dif- label, time spent at the point of choice, and price re-
ference between the recalled regular price and the cur- call accuracy. Hence:
rent special price also may be computed. Accurate re-
Hi: Shopperswho buy a special-priceditemdo moreprice
call of this price difference requires the accurate noting
checkingand have greaterprice recall accuracythan
of both the objective regular price and special prices shopperswho buy a regular-priced item.
(at least in the information environment in our study)
and an accurate computation of the difference. The Frequent buyers have a greater economic stake in
measurement of price difference recall accuracy in- purchasing the product and thus should be more price
dicates how sensitively shoppers process the amount sensitive. They also have been exposed more fre-
of a price reduction. If the amount of a price reduction quently and recently to category prices so their inter-
is an importantdeterminantof behavior, that fact should nal adaptation price level will be narrower, reflecting
be reflected in accurate recall of the price difference. their superior current knowledge of category prices (see
This measure is also relevant to Thaler's (1985) trans- Sawyer and Dickson 1984). Thus they will be more
action utility theory, which argues that shoppers de- discriminating and sensitive to price (Lichtenstein,
rive utility from buying an item at a bargain price. Bloch, and Black 19188). Hence:
The amount of this utility is a direct function of the H2: Frequentbuyersof a productcategorydo moreprice
difference between the perceived regular and special checkingand have greaterprice recall accuracythan
prices. shopperswho are less frequentbuyersof the product
Point-of-purchase price information obviously is category.
used to help decide whether to purchase the product.
It follows from Stigler's (1961) cost/benefit the-
It also may lead to an evaluation of the price image
of the item and the store. An item's psychological price ory of search that some search activities should be
substitutes rather than complements. For example, time
may be compared with the price of other brands and
of other sizes of the same brand if the benefit of such spent searching for price specials in the store adver-
tising prior to shopping should reduce the incremental
price comparisons is expected to exceed the effort (see benefit of further search at the point of purchase. If
Blattberg, Eppen, and Lieberman 1981; Stigler 1961). the purchase decision is made while reading the ad-
In the following section we develop several hy-
vertising, there will be little further search other than
potheses based on the preceding conceptualization of some confirmatory checking of the special price. Hence:
information processing at the point of purchase and
several economic theories of search. H3: Shoppers who note the item is at a special price in
store advertising spend less time at the point of pur-
chase, do less price checking, and have lower price
recall accuracy.
Hypotheses
The signal of a special price at the point of choice In the conceptual framework depicted in Figure 1,
may trigger some shoppers to switch among sizes and objective price information may affect relative price
brands. Such switching may be preceded by more ac- judgments and evaluations (see Zeithaml 1982). Both
tive price search and value-for-money analysis at the the fact that the item is actually on special and the
point of choice. The presence of an unusual signal at perception that the item is on special may have such
the point of choice may also precipitate an increase an effect. Hence:
in price search among regular buyers of the brand.
This notion is based on the classic behavioral premise H4: The presence and recognition of a special price influ-
ence the price image of both the brand and the store.
that anything out of the usual in a choice environment
will arouse attention and lead to an increase in search. According to Stigler's cost/benefit search theory,
Thaler's (1985) theory of transaction utility requires label recognition should be higher than price recall
the noting of the bargain price so it can be compared accuracy because it takes less effort to find and rec-
with the external or internal standard price. If Thaler's ognize the presence of a distinctive special price point-
theory holds in grocery shopping, it provides another of-purchase label than to note the actual price. The
reason why shoppers who purchase an item at a spe- incremental value of encoding the special price after

PriceKnowledgeand Searchof SupermarketShoppers/45

This content downloaded from 138.251.14.35 on Tue, 26 May 2015 20:04:21 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
noting that the item is on special may also be low for with the title "Cost CutterBonus Buy" and a picture
many shoppers. Hence: of a pair of scissors symbolizing the price reduction
beside the standardprice label. The special label pre-
H5: The accuracy of the recognition of the item's regular
versus special price status is higher than the accuracy sented the new special price and unit price but did not
of actual price recall. give the amountof the pricereduction.The "CostCutter
Bonus Buy" phraseand the scissors symbol had been
Accordingto the cost/benefit theoryof search,one used for several years in the supermarket'snewspaper
of the majorreasons for noting price is to assess price and TV advertising. Most shopperswere expected to
variabilityby comparingprices across differentbrands at least recognize the slogan and symbol because the
and stores. Therefore in-store price checking should
supermarkethad a large marketshare and advertised
be related to recency of interstoreprice search (see
heavily. The shopperwas shown a cardwith the three
Figure 1). There may also be a general price sensi- differentshelf labels and asked to identify the correct
tivity shoppingtraitthat links price comparisonshop- pricing label for the chosen item in a recognitiontest
ping between brandswith price comparisonshopping and then explain the meaningand significanceof each
between stores. Hence: label. The full interview included several other ques-
H6: Interstore price vigilance is related positively to in- tions and took four to five minutes. On completion
store price vigilance. the interviewerrecordedthe actual price of the item,
whetherit was on special, and the amountof any price
reduction.
Method Four stores in a large supermarketchain were in-
Observers with clipboards were stationed in super- cluded in the study. Initially, two stores were chosen
markets at the point of purchasefor particularitems because they served communities with similar de-
and were instructedto give the appearanceof under- mographicprofiles representativeof the entirearea(of
taking some stocktakingactivity, a common sight in more than 1 million people). The majora priori dif-
a supermarket.On each tenth minute of the hour the ference of interest was that one store used both item
observerrecordedthe behaviorof the first shopperto and shelf pricing, but the other had just abandoned
choose a brandof the target product. This procedure item pricing and provided only shelf pricing.' After
involved timing the intervalbetween when the shop- completingthe interviewsin these stores, we received
pers turnedtheir attentionto the display and when the permission to extend the study to include two other
item was placed in the shopping cart. The numberof stores. To assess the generalizabilityof the resultsfrom
different brandsand sizes physically touched and in- the two demographically typicalstores, we chose stores
spected, including the chosen item, also was noted. representing two extremes: a store in an upscale res-
The shopperwas interceptedimmediatelyafterthe idential white suburband a downtown innercitystore
item was placed in the cart. The interviewerpointed that served a poor, less educated, primarily black
to a universitylapel label and said, "Excuse me, I am community. The latter interviews were deliberately
from University. May I ask you a few ques- undertakenin late January,the time of the year and
tions about the item you just chose? In return, as a month (accordingto store managers)when the store's
token of appreciation,I would like to give you this patronswould be most financially strappedand there-
one dollar bill." The refusal rate was less than 1%. fore presumably most motivated to be price con-
The interviewerthen asked: "Off the top of your head, scious.2
without checking, can you tell me what the price is Four products were chosen after discussion with
of the [productname] you just chose?" Thus shoppers the supermarketchain's regional merchandisingand
were asked to recall the price of the item within 30 advertisingmanagers.They were selected to represent
seconds of selecting the item and placing it in the relatively low (coffee and toothpaste)and high (mar-
shopping cart. garine and cold cereal) turnoverproducts and infre-
Awarenessof whetherthe price of the selecteditem quently (cold cereal and toothpaste) and frequently
was regularor special was assessedin two ways. First, (margarineand coffee) price-promotedproductgroups.
the shopper was asked whether the price of the item An attemptwas made to gathera sample that was rep-
was lower than the usual price (i.e., on special). The
second measureof awarenessof the item's price status
involved shelf-label recognition. The supermarketin 'The study results determined that the item pricing was not missed
the study used a standardprice label that had black by the shoppers and had little, if any, effect on point-of-purchase
shopping behavior and price perception. This result is contrary to pre-
printon white with the unit price presentedin a shaded dictions of shoppers before item prices were dropped (Harrisand Mills
gray box. The label also contained the brand name, 1980).
2The shoppers in this store were significantly less accurate in their
weight, and SKU (stock-keepingunit) code. A special price estimates than the shoppers at the other three stores (see Dickson
price was signaled by placing a bright yellow label and Sawyer 1986b).

46 / Journalof Marketing,July 1990

This content downloaded from 138.251.14.35 on Tue, 26 May 2015 20:04:21 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
resentativeof shopperbehavior by undertakingmore prices between supermarkets.Shoppers who claimed
observations and interviews at peak shopping times not to have checked prices were most likely to explain
(such as Thursdaynight, Friday night, and Saturday either that "this was a case where price was not that
morning). Fifty interviews for each of the four prod- important"(67.8%) or they "already knew the ap-
ucts were conducted in each of the four stores. The proximateprice"(40.0%). Otherreasonsincluded"too
total numberof shoppersinterviewed was 802. rushed"(11.9%), "used a coupon" (10.1%), and "al-
Price recall accuracy was calculated by the fol- ready knew the exact price" (6.9%).
lowing formula (see Zeithaml 1984).
Price Recall Accuracy
objective price - recalled price We expected price recall accuracyto be much higher
price recall accuracy =
objectiveprice in this study than in past studies in which the shopper
was questionedminutes, hours, and even days (rather
This average price recall accuracy measured the
thanseconds) aftermakingthe purchasechoice. How-
percentagedeviationof the recalledprice from the ob- ever, it was not. One in five of the shoppers(21.1%)
jective price, whether high or low. Price knowledge did not even offer a price estimate; they seemed to
bias was calculated in the same way, but was mea-
have no idea of the price of the item they had chosen.
sured as an algebraic difference ratherthan an abso-
Less than half (47.1%) were able to state the correct
lute difference. It establishedwhetherthere was a sys-
exact price (55.6% gave a price within 5% of the ob-
tematicdirectionalbias in the subjects'price estimates.
jective price), and 31.8% gave a price estimate that
Analyses of varianceand covariancewere used to was inaccurate.Price estimates of those who were in-
analyzethe metricdependentvariables(e.g., time spent accuratewere off by an average of 30 cents (about a
at point of purchase). The nonmetric categorical re-
15%error).Finally, there was a systematicbias in the
sponses (e.g, correctlabel recognition)were analyzed incorrectestimates;the recalled price was on average
with chi squareand loglinearmodel tests. The average
10 cents lower (4.2%) than the actual price.
statistical power of the tests (probabilityof rejecting
The amount of the price reduction was not dis-
a false null hypothesis) was generally high because
the sample size for most of the analyses was between played when an item was offered at a special price in
the studiedstore environment.The shopperhad to see
600 and 800.
the special price and calculate the differencefrom the
normal price that was also displayed. Consequently,
it is not surprisingthatthe accuracyof knowledgeabout
Results the amount of the price reduction was considerably
Several general statistics provide interestingperspec- lower (13%) than the price recall accuracy (49.1%).
tives on what occurredat the point of purchase. The Only 41.9% of special shoppers gave an estimate of
average time between arrivingat and departingfrom the amount of the price reduction, and their average
the productcategorydisplay was less than 12 seconds. price reductionestimate was off by 47%.
About 42% of shoppers spent five seconds or less;
25% spent more than 15 seconds. In 85% of the pur- Purchases at Regular and Special Prices
chases only the chosen brandwas handled, and 90% Table 1 contraststhe behavior and price perceptions
of the shoppers physically inspected only one size. of the 232 shopperswho chose an item with a special
These time and price comparisonresultsare consistent price with those of the 570 shoppers who chose an
with those of Hoyer (1984). item with a regular price. Consistent with HI, the
Consistent with the short time spent at the point shoppersbuying an item at a special price claimed to
of purchase,the numberof shopperswho reportedprice undertakemore search and price checking. They also
checkingwas not high. Slightlymorethanhalf (57.9%) more frequentlychecked price to make a brandchoice
of the shoppersclaimed to have checked the price of and to decide whetheror not to buy from the product
the chosen item and fewer than one in four (21.6%) category. However, inconsistentwith Hi, they did not
reportedchecking the price of an alternativebrand. spend more time at the point of purchase, nor were
Several differentreasons were given for checking the their price estimates more accurate.In fact, a system-
price of the chosen item. The most common un- atic underestimationbias is found for the special price
promptedreasongiven by shopperswho checkedprice that is not present for a regular price. On average,
was "simple habit" (44%), followed by price check- shopperswho purchaseda special-priceditem and gave
ing to "help make a brandchoice" (31.7%), "decide an inaccurateprice estimatebelievedthatthe pricethey
how much to buy" (28.7%), and "decide whether to were paying was 10% lower than the actual special
buy or not buy from the category" (21.5%). Only price, whereas the price estimate bias was negligible
13.9% said they checked prices to "rememberuntil for the counterpartshoppers who purchasedan item
next time" and 12.8% checked the price to compare at its regularprice.

PriceKnowledgeand Searchof SupermarketShoppers/ 47

This content downloaded from 138.251.14.35 on Tue, 26 May 2015 20:04:21 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
TABLE 1
Reported Shopping Behavior, Price Knowledge, and Price Perceptions of Shoppers
Buying at Special and Regular Prices
Actual Price Status (%)
Regular Special
(n = 570) (n = 232)
Behavior
Reported noticing choice in ads 3.1 17.1b
Reported checking price at point of choice 54.0 67.5b
Reasons for checking price
Habit 46.3 39.6
To choose the brand 26.4 42.2c
To decide how much to buy 28.4 29.9
To decide whether to buy or not 18.2 27.9c
To compare prices between stores 15.0 8.4
Reported checking price of other brands 18.1 30.7c
Reported using unit price information
To compare brands 4.4 9.5b
To compare sizes 8.4 8.6
Made supermarket price comparisons
Within last week 20.7a 29.7b
Within last month 19.1 15.5
Sometime in the past 15.3 20.3
Never 44.9 34.5
Price Knowledge and Perceptions
Reduced perception
Special price 11.1a 49.1b
Usual price 50.0 26.7
Not aware/not sure 38.9 24.1
Price estimate of chosen item was
No estimate 22.8 16.8C
For those who estimated a price, price estimate of chosen item was
Exactly right 60.0 59.1
Wrong 40.0 40.9
Relative brand price perception of selected item
Lower 19.5w 30.2b
About the same 53.5 50.7
Higher 19.5 13.8
Don't know 7.5 5.2
Store's prices in comparison with those of other stores
Lower 36.0 36.2
About the same 46.3 40.1
Sometimes higher, sometimes lower 7.7 14.7c
Higher 3.9 4.7
Don't know 6.1 4.3
Average price recall accuracy 5.2 6.0
Average price estimate bias
All shoppers -.6 -4.1
Shoppers giving incorrect price estimate -1.5 -10.0o
test of differencebetweentwo distributions;all others are a test of differencebetweentwo proportionsor means.
aSignificance
bStatisticallysignificant at p < .005.
cStatisticallysignificant at p < .05.

Behavior of Frequent Shoppers physical inspection of different sizes of the product,


reported reasons for checking and not
H2 is not supported. The more frequent buyers of the and reported checkin and not checking
check
e Thee buyers
price. ers whoo purchased once
one a month
oh or less
product category did not undertake more price check-
often spent more tFme at the po of puhase 14.2
ing and were not more accurate in their price knowl-
edge.3 However, frequency of purchase is related to vsp 10e8 secondsi F2795 3s46, p <w05) and in-
the amount of time spent at the point of choice, the spected more sizes than the shoppers who purchased
the product three or more times a month (1.20 vs.
3The shoppers in this store were significantly less accurate in their
1.05 sizes,
sizes F2795 = 12.86,
F2,795 1286 p << .001).
01) The more fre-
price estimates than the shoppers at the other three stores (see Dickson quent buyers of an item were more likely to check
and Sawyer 1986b). prices to compare with other stores' prices (17.7% vs.

48 / Journalof Marketing,July 1990

This content downloaded from 138.251.14.35 on Tue, 26 May 2015 20:04:21 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
8.4% for the less frequentbuyers; X2= 6.77, 2 d.f., Price Status Recognition
p < .05). The less frequent buyers more often gave We expected that shoppers would be more likely to
the use of a coupon as a reason for not checking the
recognize correctlywhetherthe item was at a regular
item's price thandid the more frequentbuyers (16.1% or special price than to recall the price accurately.In
vs. 5.6%: X2(d.f.) = 7.64, p < .05).
fact, only 53.6% recognized the correct shelf price
label for the chosen item and only 49.7% were cor-
Effect of Noting Item In-Store Advertising rectlyawareof price status.These percentagesare only
a few percentagepoints higherthanthe 47.1% for ac-
The use of the supermarket'sweekly advertisingwas curate recall of the price, and the difference is not
not related to behavior at the point of purchase, con-
statisticallysignificant. H5 is not supported.
trary to the prediction of H3. Shoppers who claimed
they had noted the item in the advertisingdid not spend General Price Vigilance
less time at the point of purchase or engage in less As predicted by H6, there is evidence of significant
price checking or physical inspection of other brands positive relationships between whether the shopper
and sizes. The hypothesisthatdifferentsourcesof price checked the price of the selected item and within-store
informationare used as substitutesis not supported.
price search (X2 = 46.3, 3 d.f., p < .0001) and be-
tween-store search (X2 = 27.2, 3 d.f., p < .0001).
Effect of Price Special on Brand and Store The shopperswho claimed to have checked the price
Price Image of the chosen item and those who checked the price
of other brands were both more likely to have com-
Interestingly,93.1% of shoppersoffered an answer to pared prices recently between supermarkets.For ex-
the question, "Comparedto other brands of similar
ample, 53.1% of those who did not check the price
size, is the item you just chose a high-priced,average- of the selected item could not rememberever making
priced, or low-priced brand?" Consistent with the pricecomparisonsbetweenstoresversus33.8%of those
propositionthat the qualitativecoding of the relative who did claim to have checked the price of the se-
price of an item may be an easier task for the shopper lected item. Hence H6 is supported.
thanthe coding of the item's actualprice (see Zeithaml
1984), this percentageis significantlygreaterthan the
78.9% of shopperswho could provide an actual price Discussion
estimate (z = 4.53, p < .001). When presentedto the marketingexecutives of lead-
H4 predictsthatboth the presenceof a special price ing packaged goods companies and interestedacade-
and the perceptionof a special price are relatedto the micians, our findings have been received with sur-
price image of the brandand the store. A relationship prise and concern. It is not that practitioners or
between the presence of a special price and brand im- academiciansbelieve that grocery shoppersmake de-
age is observed, but no importantrelationshipis found cisions on the basis of perfect information;what is
for store image. Table 1 indicates that a special price surprisingis just how imperfectinformationattention
was accompaniedby more frequentperceptionthat the and retentionare at the very point of purchase. The
chosen brandwas low priced (30.2% vs. 19.5%). For fact is that less than half of the shopperscould recall
store price perceptions, the only statistically signifi- the price of the item they hadjust placed in theirshop-
cant difference for a purchaseat a special price is the ping basket, and less than half were aware they had
percentage of shoppers who believed that the super- selected an item that was selling at a reduced price.
market'sprices were sometimes lower and sometimes Only a small minorityof those who bought a special
higher than competitors' prices. knew both its price and the amount of the price re-
Given that half of the special shoppers were not duction.
even aware they had purchased the item at a lower The average percentage error in shoppers' price
than normal price, we wondered about the relation- estimates of the item they had just chosen may not
ship between the perception that the chosen item was seem large until it is consideredwithin the price en-
reducedin price (ratherthan the reality) and the price vironment.The shopperswho gave an incorrectprice
image of the brand and store. The results are consis- estimatewere off by an averageof 30 cents above and
tent with H4. When the shopperperceived the item to below the average observed selling price in the study
be at a special price, the relative price image of both of $2.00. The average range in actual prices across
the brand (X2 = 79.5, 6 d.f., p < .0001) and store the alternativebrandsof a given size, as estimatedby
(X2= 22.2, 8 d.f., p < .01) was lower. For example, interviewersfrom shelf checks, was in most instances
35% of shopperswho perceivedthe price to be special smaller than the range of the price estimates. That is,
categorizedthe price of the brandboughtas lower than for an averageprice of $2.00, the lowest pricedbrand
averageversus 20.5% of those who perceivedthe price was rarelyless than$1.70 and the highestpricedbrand
to be a regularprice. rarelymorethan$2.30. This fmdingsuggeststhatsome

PriceKnowledgeand Searchof SupermarketShoppers/ 49

This content downloaded from 138.251.14.35 on Tue, 26 May 2015 20:04:21 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
of the shoppers had a price in mind for the chosen point of purchaseappearto be conspicuous, they seem
item that was not only inaccurate,but higher or lower not to communicatetheir message to the many shop-
than all of the actual prices in the brandchoice set. pers who undertakeminimal search and information
Given that we asked shoppers to recall prices of processing at the point of purchase. Our observation
items that had just been selected and placed in their of low use of price andprice statusinformationis sim-
shopping carts, it seems unlikely that shoppersnoted ilarto thatfor otherpoint-of-purchaseinformation(e.g.,
the price and forgot it in the short intervening time. Achabal et al. 1987; Russo et al. 1986).
However, despiteour convictionthatquestioningabout Better promotion and identificationof price spe-
price immediately after the item had been placed in cials might benefit both the consumer who wants the
the shopping cart would result in higher accuracy of best deals and the manufacturerwho wants to increase
price knowledge, a comparisonof the price recall ac- the impact of its promotions. Shoppers who claimed
curacy found in our study with that found in previous to have checked price while makingtheirchoice were
studies revealed that the results are not very different much more likely to recall exactly the price of their
(see Dickson and Sawyer 1986b). In fact, the per- chosen item than shoppers who claimed to have not
centage recall is slightly lower in our study than that checkedprice(70.7%vs. 17.3%).They werealso much
in several other studies. After discussing the limita- more likely to perceive correctlythat the chosen item
tions of our research, we explore its managerialand was at a special price (60.4% vs. 25.7%) and rec-
theoreticalimplications. ognize the special price label (73.6% vs. 38.5%).
The majorproblemis thatonly 58.9% even claimed
Research Limitations to have checked the price. Hutchinson(1989) found
thattemporaryprice specials thatreceive no otherpro-
Thoughwe believe our methodis an improvementover motional aid such as displays or advertisingfeatures
those used in previous studies of price search and
are less profitablethan maintainingregularprices. One
knowledge (Dickson and Sawyer 1986a), our study
has many limitations. We studied only four product explanationis thatmany shopperswill not notice either
the special price statusor the amountof the price dis-
categories in four stores of one majornational super- count unless their attentionis drawnby somethingbe-
marketchain in one largeMSA. We also reliedheavily
on self-reportdata. Though observation was used to yond a mere shelf label. Futureprice researchshould
measuresearchamong brandsand sizes, we could not test ways of increasingshopperprice checking at the
use observationsto corroborateself-reportsaboutprice point of purchase.Studiesof methodsof calling greater
attention to prices, such as different labels or shelf
checking and knowledge. Also, though pretests in-
dicated high reliability, we did not assess the reli- talkers, shelf space allocation, video carts, or dis-
ability of our observersin the study itself because we plays, should include measuresof theireffect on price
used only one observerper shopper. We also did not checking and recall, as well as sales, to gain a fuller
ascertain whether shoppers in different demographic understandingof the sales results (see Quelch and
Cannon-Bonventre 1983; Wilkinson, Mason, and
categoriesdifferedin theirbehavioror knowledge. Fi-
nally, we did not obtainindependentinformationabout Paksoy 1982).
external store factors such as the amountof advertis- The preceding suggestions are based on the as-
ing, advertisedprice specials, or distributionof cou- sumption that the manufacturerwants to improve
communicationof its special price. In the long term,
pons. a manufacturermay preferto have its loyal consumers
As in previous studies, we used open-endedrecall
to measure price knowledge. Monroe, Powell, and not notice when the item is on special. Loyal cus-
tomers who are unawareof the amountthat could be
Choudhury(1986) argue that recognition and not re- saved at a special price or are not even attentive to
call is the appropriatemeasure of the effects of inci-
dental as well as intentionallearning of prices. Rec- whether the item is at a special price may buy more
often at the regularprice. The issue is a tradeoff be-
ognition measures may result in higher estimates of
tween the benefit of using a better promoted special
price knowledge. We did use a simple recognitiontest
of the price label, and correct label recognition was to attract nonloyal shoppers and the forgone profit
no better than price recall. We consequently find it marginfrommakingregular,loyal shoppersmoreaware
of and sensitive to price promotions (see McAlister
unlikely that price recognition would be high at the
and Lattin 1983).
point of purchase.
Our results are consistent with the notion that a
Managerial Implications coupon promotion may be much more effective and
efficient thana point-of-purchasespecial promotionin
Apparently,many reducedprices at the point of pur- reachingthe price-sensitivesegment (see Narasimhan
chase are not noticed. Even though currentmerchan- 1984). With coupons a manufacturergives away mar-
dising practices used to identify price specials at the gin only to shoppers who are aware and appreciate

50 / Journalof Marketing,July 1990

This content downloaded from 138.251.14.35 on Tue, 26 May 2015 20:04:21 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
that the coupon is giving them a price break. This istence and form of internalreferenceprices (includ-
selective margin cut to coupon users contrasts with ing the updatingof the reference price) and identify
point-of-purchaseprice specials that give everyone a the situations and ways in which such price percep-
price breakwhetheror not they are aware of it or care tions are used by shoppers at the point of purchase
aboutit. However, the manufacturermust balancethis would be informative.
potential savings to nonusers of coupons against the Our study provides a static depiction of what is
likely greatercosts of distributingthe coupons. clearly a dynamic process. Much remains unknown
Anotherimplication of our findings is that it may aboutthe distributionof price-checkingbehavioracross
be better for both manufacturerand retailerto reduce differentgrocery shoppersand differentshoppingtrips
the amountof the price reductionon grocery specials by the same shopper. Shoppers may initially check
and invest more in promotingthe special, particularly many prices in deciding what brand to buy at a su-
at the point of purchase. Given that the perceived permarketand then only occasionally recheck prices
presenceof a price reductionenhancesthe price image to ascertainany changes. The issue here is the level
of the brand and store, it may also be advisable to of discontinuityin consumerprice sensitivity. For ex-
have more frequentbut shallowerprice reductions(see ample, we found that aboutone in five shopperscom-
Alba and Marmorstein1987; Buyukkurt1986). pared prices among brands. This result could be pro-
duced by a segment of careful shoppers who always
Theoretical Implications
compareprices on all the items they buy or by a larger
The underestimationbias we observed in the special numberof shopperswho infrequentlycompareprices.
shoppers'priceestimatessupportsGaudagniandLittle's However, the fact that segmentationstudies based on
(1983) finding that a price promotion has a positive consumers' claimed buying habits and attitudeshave
relationshipwith choice beyond that due to the total found a similar percentageof price-comparisonshop-
price and the amountof the price reduction.The prime pers in several productclasses (Calantoneand Sawyer
effect of a special price may be as a "good buy" signal 1978; Freedman1989; Tigert 1988) suggests that the
(Inman, McAlister, and Hoyer, in press). The very formerpossibility is more likely. What is clear is that
low accuracyof estimates of the amountof reductions shoppersare very heterogeneousin terms of their at-
made available by special prices suggests that few tention and reaction to price and price promotions.
shoppers compute the amount of the price reduction Though our research emphasizes the number of
and/or most of those who do are very inaccurate.This shopperswho did not check or know the price of cho-
result raises a question about the applicability of sen items, the fact is that nearly half of the shoppers
Thaler's (1985) theoryof transactionutility to grocery did know the price. Researchis needed to learn more
shopping,because the amountof the utilityin Thaler's about these price-vigilant shoppers. Presumablythis
theoryis based on the differencebetweenregularprice segment's behavioris a prime driverof the price sen-
and speical price. At a minimum, this result suggests sitivity observed in aggregate scanner data studies
that transactionutility is based on the perceived dif- (Tellis 1988). Other shopperswho use nonpricecues
ference between regularand special price and not nec- at the point of purchaseto identify a promotionalprice
essarily the actual difference. If transactionutility is also contributeto the observed aggregate price sen-
derived from grocery shopping as Thaler suggests, it sitivity.
is often based on a gross miscalculationof the price Though more research obviously is needed for a
reduction. full understandingof supermarketshoppers' infor-
We did not attemptto operationalizeand measure mationprocessing at the point of purchase,the results
internalreferenceprices. However, the results for one of our study provide some common-sense explana-
question in our study are at least tangentiallyrelevant tions. Shoppersdo not always check or know the price
to the issue of whetherreferenceprices commonly ex- for several reasons: a lack of time, a belief that the
ist or are spontaneouslyutilized by shoppers. Imme- price is satisfactoryor not very different from when
diately after the price recall question, shoppers who it was last checked, brand loyalty or habitualrepeat
gave a price were asked, "How did you know the purchasing,difficulty in finding the shelf price label
price?". Of 802 shoppers, 169 or 21.1% would not of the item, or the belief that the limited total savings
even offer a price; 415 or 51.7% claimed to have re- from checking prices is not worth the time or effort.
lied on the point-of-purchase(shelf or item) price in- However, the degree of error in shoppers' infor-
formation;and only 218 or 27.2% reliedon what might mation processing of special prices is surprising.We
be interpretedas an internal reference price. These found only limited evidence that special-price-item
findings suggest that reference prices are either not purchasingwas relatedto more careful shopping, and
coded as precise quantitativeprices in the shoppers' special-price-itembuyers were no more accuratethan
minds or not retrievedvery accurately(if at all) from shopperswho boughtat a regularprice. Shoppersseem
memory. Furtherresearchto measuredirectly the ex- to use the presence of a special price as an indicator

PriceKnowledgeand Searchof SupermarketShoppers/ 51

This content downloaded from 138.251.14.35 on Tue, 26 May 2015 20:04:21 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
of a good deal. However, they overestimate the sav- rate. Clearly, much available information about the
ings from these price discounts. This may be one rea- prices of purchased and nonpurchaseditems is not being
son for the sales effects of a price promotion beyond processed. Future research may offer a clearer under-
the amount of the price reduction (Gaudagni and Little standing of what information is utilized by shoppers
1983). Moreover, shoppers' assessments of whether at the point of purchase and how manufacturer and
the item is at a special price are remarkably inaccu- retailer tactics can influence shoppers' decisions.

REFERENCES
Achabal, Dale D., Shelby H. McIntyre, Cherryl H. Bell, and Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
Nancy Tucker (1987), "The Effect of Nutrition P-O-P signs Harris, Brian F. and Michael K. Mills (1980), "The Impact
on Consumer Attitudes and Behavior," Journal of Retail- of Item Price Removal on Grocery Shopping Behavior,"
ing, 63 (Spring), 9-24. Journal of Retailing, 56 (Winter), 73-93.
Alba, Joseph W. and Howard Marmorstein (1987), "The Ef- Helson, Harry (1964), Adaptation-Level Theory. New York:
fects of Frequency Knowledge on Consumer Decision Harper & Row Publishers, Inc.
Making," Journal of Consumer Research, 14 (June), 14- Hoyer, Wayne D. (1984), "An Examination of Consumer
25. Decision Making for a Common Repeat Purchase Pro-
Allen, J. W., G. D. Harrell, and M. D. Hutt (1976), Price duct," Journal of Consumer Research, 11 (December),
Awareness Study. Washington, DC: The Food Marketing 822-9.
Institute. Hutchinson, J. Wesley (1989), "Profit Oriented Graph Paper:
Blattberg, Robert, Gary D. Eppen, and Joshua Lieberman Promotional Strategies Jointly Optimal for Manufacturers
(1981), "A Theoretical and Empirical Evaluation of Price and Retailers," presented at University of Florida Market-
Deals for Consumer Nondurables," Journal of Marketing, ing Colloquium Series (September 1).
45 (Winter), 116-29. Inman, J. Jeffrey, Leigh McAlister, and Wayne D. Hoyer (in
Buyukkurt, B. Kemal (1986), "Integrationof Serially Sampled press), "Promotion Signal: Proxy for a Price Cut?"Journal
Price Information: Modeling and Some Findings," Journal of Consumer Research.
of Consumer Research, 13 (December), 357-73. Jacoby, Jacob and Jerry C. Olson (1977), "Consumer Re-
Calantone, Roger and Alan G. Sawyer (1978), "The Stability sponse to Price: An Attitudinal, Information Processing
of Benefit Segments," Journal of Marketing Research, 15 Perspective," in Moving Ahead with Attitude Research, Y.
(August), 395-404. Wind and B. Greenberg, eds. Chicago: American Market-
Cohen, Joel B. and Kunal Basu (1987), "Alternative Models ing Association.
of Categorization: Toward a Contingent Processing Frame- Kahnemann, Daniel and Amos Tversky (1979), "Prospect
work," Journal of Consumer Research, 13 (March), 455- Theory: An Analysis of Decision Under Risk," Econome-
72. trica, 47, 263-91.
Conover, Jerry N. (1986), "The Accuracy of Price Knowl- Lichtenstein, Donald R., Peter H. Bloch, and William C. Black
edge: Issues in Research Methodology," in Advances in (1988), "Correlates of Price Acceptability," Journal of
Consumer Research, Vol. 13, Richard J. Lutz, ed. Ann Consumer Research, 15 (September), 243-52.
Arbor, MI: Association for Consumer Research, 589-93. McAlister, Leigh and James M. Lattin (1983), "Identifying
Dickson, Peter R. and Alan G. Sawyer (1986a), "Methods to Substitute and Complementary Relationships Revealed by
Research Shoppers' Knowledge of SupermarketPrices," in Consumer Variety Seeking Behavior," Working Paper No.
Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 13, Richard J. Lutz, 1487-83, Sloan School of Management, Massachusetts In-
ed. Ann Arbor, MI: Association for Consumer Research, stitute of Technology.
584-8. Monroe, Kent B. (1971), "Measuring Price Thresholds by
and (1986b), "Point-of-Purchase Behav- Psychophysics and Latitudes of Acceptance," Journal of
ior and Price Perceptions of SupermarketShoppers," work- Marketing Research, 8 (November), 460-4.
ing paper. Cambridge, MA: Marketing Science Institute. (1973), "Buyers' Subjective Perceptions of Price,"
Farley, John U., ed. (1985) Measuring and Evaluating Sales Journal of Marketing Research, 10 (February), 70-80.
Promotions to the Trade and to Consumers, Report No. and Susan M. Petroshius (1981), "Buyers' Percep-
85-113. Cambridge, MA: Marketing Science Institute. tions of Price: An Update of the Evidence," in Perspectives
Freedman, Alix M. (1989), "Most Consumers Shun Luxuries, in Consumer Behavior, 3rd ed., Harold H. Kassarjian and
Seek Few Frills But Better Service," Wall Street Journal Thomas S. Robertson, eds. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman
(September 19), B1. and Company, 43-55.
Gabor, Andre and C. W. J. Granger (1961), "On the Price , Christine P. Powell, and Pravat K. Choudhury
Consciousness of Consumers," Applied Statistics, 10 (No- (1986), "Recall Versus Recognition as a Measure of Price
vember), 170-88. Awareness," Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 13,
Gaudagni, Peter M. and John D. C. Little (1983), "A Logit Richard J. Lutz, ed. Ann Arbor, MI: Association for Con-
Model of Brand Choice Calibratedon Scanner Data," Mar- sumer Research, 594-9.
keting Science, 2 (Summer), 203-38. Nagle, Thomas T. (1987), The Strategy and Tactics of Pric-
Gurumurthy,K. and John D. C. Little (1987), "A Pricing Model ing: A Guide to Profitable Decision Making. Englewood
Based on Perception Theories and Its Testing on Scanner Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc.
Panel Data," working paper, Sloan School of Management, Narasimhan, Chakravarti (1984), "A Price Discrimination

52 / Journalof Marketing,July 1990

This content downloaded from 138.251.14.35 on Tue, 26 May 2015 20:04:21 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Theory of Coupons," Marketing Science, 3 (Spring), 128- Journal of Marketing Research, 25 (November), 231-41.
47. Thaler, Richard (1985), "Mental Accounting and Consumer
Progressive Grocer (1964), "How Much Do Consumers Know Choice," Marketing Science, 4 (Summer), 199-214.
About Retail Prices?" (February), 104-6. Tigert, Douglas J. (1988), "Winners and Losers in Ready-to-
(1975), "Thenand Now: Shopping Behavior 10 Years Wear Merchandising," presented to 11th World Conference
Apart" (October), 37-41. on Retailing (October 4).
(1981), "What Shoppers Want Now" (September), Wilkinson, J. B., J. Barry Mason, and Christie H. Paksoy
136-7. (1982), "Assessing the Impact of Short-Term Supermarket
Quelch, John A. and KristinaCannon-Bonventre(1983), "Better Strategy Variables," Journal of Marketing Research, 19
Marketing at the Point of Purchase," Harvard Business Re- (February), 72-80.
view, 61 (November-December), 162-9. Winer, Russell S. (1986), "A Reference Price Model of Brand
Raman, Kalyan and Frank M. Bass (1989), "A General Test Choice for Frequently Purchased Products," Journal of
of Reference Price Theory in the Presence of Threshold Ef- Consumer Research, 13 (September), 250-6.
fects," unpublished working paper, University of Florida. (1987), "Behavioral Perspectives on Pricing: Buy-
Russo, J. Edward, Richard Staelin, Catherine A. Nolan, Gary ers' Subjective Perceptions of Price Revisited," in Issues
J. Russell, and Barbara L. Metcalf (1986), "Nutrition In- in Pricing: Theory and Research, T. M. Divinney, ed.
formation in the Supermarket," Journal of Consumer Re- Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
search, 13 (June), 48-70. Zbytniewski, Jo Ann (1979), "How Do Shoppers Choose a
Sawyer, Alan G. and Peter R. Dickson (1984), "Psychological Supermarket," Progressive Grocer (August), 105-7.
Perspectives on Consumer Response to Sales Promotion," Zeithaml, Valarie A. (1982), "Consumer Response to Instore
in Research on Sales Promotions: Collected Papers, Kath- Price Information Environments," Journal of Consumer
erine E. Jocz, ed. Cambridge, MA: Marketing Science In- Research, 8 (March), 357-69.
stitute, 1-21. (1984), "Issues in Conceptualizing and Measuring
Sherif, Carolyn W. (1963), "Social Categorization as a Func- Consumer Response to Price," in Advances in Consumer
tion of Latitude of Acceptance and Series Range," Journal Research, Vol. 11, Thomas C. Kinnear, ed. Provo, UT:
of Abnormal Psychology, 67 (August), 148-56. Association for Consumer Research, 612-16.
Stigler, George J. (1961), "The Economics of Information," (1988), "Consumer Perceptions of Price, Quality,
Journal of Political Economy, 69 (June), 213-25. and Value: A Means-End Model and Synthesis of Evi-
Tellis, Gerald J. (1988), "The Price Elasticity of Selective De- dence," Journal of Marketing, 52 (July), 2-22.
mand: A Meta-Analysis of Econometric Models of Sales,"
Reprint No. JM543103

AMERICANMARKETINGASSOCIATION
REPRINT AND PERMISSION POLICIES AND PROCEDURESFOR JOURNALARTICLES
The AmericanMarketingAssociation is eager to serve you in the sharingof the valuableinformationfound in AMA's journalarticles.

REPRINTS
If you wish to orderREPRINTS of any article, write to the AmericanMarketingAssociation, ReprintsDepartment,250 S. WackerDrive, Chicago,
IL 60606. Call (312) 648-0536 or Fax (312) 993-7540.
The prices below apply to each title ordered.Multiple titles may be combinedto reachthe minimumorderamountof $15.00.

Each reproductionwill be providedwith an attachedcover, and reproducedon 20# matte paper stock. Orders must be prepaid by check or credit
card. NO REFUNDS OR EXCHANGES.

REPRINT PRICES (minimumorderis $15.00)


1-14 reproductionsof the same article or an assortment $1.50 each
of articles.
15-99 reproductionsof the same article $1.25 each
100-500 reproductionsof the same article $1.00 each
>500 reproductionsof the same article call for quote
ALL PRICESINCLUDESHIPPING
ANDHANDLINGCHARGES

Also, a single copy reprintor an orderof less than 50 copies may be obtainedfrom UniversityMicrofilmsInternational,300 N. Zeeb Road, Ann
Arbor,MI 48106. Articles are priced prepaidat $17.50 plus $2.25 for each additionalcopy of the same article. Those who have a standingaccount
with UMI pay $7.50 per articleplus $2.25 for each additionalcopy of the same article. Completeissues are obtainableat $35.00 each.
PERMISSIONS
AMA would be proudto grant PERMISSIONFOR THEREPRODUCTIONof ?
copyrightedmaterials.To obtainpermission
to reproducemultiple copies from AMA publications,contactthe American
MarketingAssociation,PermissionsDepartment.
Underthe 'fairuse' provision of the CopyrightLaw, effective January,1978,
anyonemay make a photocopy of a copyrighted
work for their own use without seeking permission. AMERICAN
To secure permissionto reproduceone's own works in quantity, A44RKETING
please contact the PermissionsDepartment. ASOCIATION

PriceKnowledgeand Searchof SupermarketShoppers/ 53

This content downloaded from 138.251.14.35 on Tue, 26 May 2015 20:04:21 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like