Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 16

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/226204085

Students with Exceptionalities and the Peer Group Context of Bullying and
Victimization in Late Elementary School

Article  in  Journal of Child and Family Studies · April 2009


DOI: 10.1007/s10826-008-9214-1

CITATIONS READS

129 1,397

6 authors, including:

David B. Estell Matthew J Irvin


Indiana University Bloomington University of South Carolina
29 PUBLICATIONS   1,389 CITATIONS    60 PUBLICATIONS   1,227 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Daniel J. Boudah
East Carolina University
21 PUBLICATIONS   339 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Augmented Reality in Informal Science Learning View project

Invariance Testing Across Math and Science in Rural Place-Based View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Matthew J Irvin on 27 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150
DOI 10.1007/s10826-008-9214-1

ORIGINAL PAPER

Students with Exceptionalities and the Peer Group Context


of Bullying and Victimization in Late Elementary School
David B. Estell Æ Thomas W. Farmer Æ Matthew J. Irvin Æ Amity Crowther Æ
Patrick Akos Æ Daniel J. Boudah

Published online: 5 June 2008


Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2008

Abstract We examined bullying and victimization in 5th turn, were more likely to be bullied than students who
grade classrooms in relation to students’ education status associated with perceived-popular peers. Students with
and peer group membership. The sample consisted of 484 mild disabilities who had aggressive and perceived-popular
participants (258 girls, 226 boys), including 369 general associates had more peer nominations for bullying than
education students, 74 academically gifted students, and 41 all others. In contrast, students in general education with
students with mild disabilities. Students with mild dis- neither aggressive nor perceived-popular associates had the
abilities were more likely to be perceived as being bullies fewest peer nominations for bullying. We discuss impli-
by both teachers and peers. Teachers also rated students cations for research and intervention.
with mild disabilities significantly higher for being bullied
by peers. Academically gifted students were rated by Keywords Bullying  Victimization  Mild disabilities 
teachers as the lowest for both bullying and being bullied. Giftedness  Peer groups
Associating with aggressive or perceived-popular peers
increased the likelihood of being perceived as a bully.
Social isolates were more likely to be bullied than students Introduction
who did not associate with perceived-popular peers who, in
In recent years, several distinct lines of inquiry have helped
to inform current perspectives on bullying and aggression
D. B. Estell (&) in schools. This includes research on the social and
Department of Counseling and Educational Psychology, Indiana behavioral characteristics of youth who are at risk for
University, 201 N. Rose Avenue, Bloomington, IN 47401, USA bullying and victimization (Hodges and Perry 1999;
e-mail: destell@indiana.edu
Pellegrini et al. 1999; Schwartz 2000), developmental
T. W. Farmer patterns of victimization (Hanish and Guerra 2002;
Department of Educational Psychology, School Psychology, and Pellegrini and Bartini 2000; Smith et al. 2004; Troop-
Special Education, Pennsylvania State University, University Gordon and Ladd 2005), broader classroom social
Park, PA, USA
dynamics and aggression in the peer group (Farmer et al.
M. J. Irvin  A. Crowther 2002; O’Connell et al. 1999; Salmivalli et al. 1997) and
Center for Developmental Science, University of North Carolina linkages between bullying, victimization, and other school
at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC, USA adjustment factors (Glew et al. 2005; Schwartz et al.
2005). Collectively, these studies indicate that students
P. Akos
Human Development and Psychological Studies Area, School of involved in bullying and victimization are more likely to
Education, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel have academic and social adjustment problems, that social
Hill, NC, USA roles and peer group processes support bullying, and that
the late elementary school years are a time when classroom
D. J. Boudah
Department of Curriculum and Instruction, Special Education, social dynamics may be particularly important to bullying
East Carolina University, Greenville, NC, USA and victimization.

123
J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 137

Most research on bullying and victimization focuses on 2000; Farmer et al. 2002). In short, while aggression and
general school populations. Little work explicitly examines dominance are often related to being disliked, they can lead
the involvement of students with exceptionalities. In the to high social status and perceived popularity (Estell et al.
current climate of inclusion, students who receive excep- 2003).
tional children’s services tend to make up 10–20% or more Social dominance and influence in the social structure
of the public school population and are integrated at least also come into play in bullying. Although some bullies are
part of the school day with their general education peers. themselves aggressive victims, many others have high
While it is likely that many studies on bullying include social positions and are able to engage peers to support
students who receive exceptional services within their their behavior (Atlas and Pepler 1998; Salmivalli et al.
samples, these children are typically not identified and it is 1997). In fact, bullying episodes in elementary school tend
not possible to determine the degree to which they impact to involve several peers as onlookers, helpers, and
results. This is potentially an important oversight because encouragers (O’Connell et al. 1999). Although some chil-
students at the extremes of school functioning (students dren, usually girls, come to the aid of the victimized child,
with pronounced learning problems, and students achieving many youth appear to respond in ways that are aimed at
at advanced levels) may have differential social experi- protecting their status in the social structure including
ences that impact the degree to which they are involved currying favor with dominant bullies (Adler and Adler
in bullying relative to their general education peers 1995; Hawkins and Pepler 2001).
(Kaukiainen et al. 2002; Peterson and Ray 2006). Peer A recent study of the social dynamics of bullying in two
group affiliations are associated with students’ level of middle schools comprised almost exclusively of rural
involvement as bullies and victims (Estell et al. 2007; African American youth found that both bullies and vic-
Salmivalli et al. 1997), and students who are at the tims tended to have rejected sociometric status. However,
extremes of school functioning tend to have peer affiliation while bullies were well integrated into their peer groups,
patterns that are distinct from those of general education victims tended to be withdrawn and marginal in the social
students (Farmer and Hollowell 1994; Pearl et al. 1998). It structure (Estell et al. 2007). Bullies were heteroge-
is possible that exceptional students’ involvement in bul- neously dispersed in both popular and unpopular groups
lying and victimization is related to their peer group and aggressive and non-aggressive groups and they were
affiliations. Information along these lines could help clarify highly likely to be leaders of their peer group. Of particular
how individual and peer group level factors come together interest to us, students who were bullies and/or victims
to impact bullying and victimization in the classroom. tended to have behaviors (i.e., higher teacher ratings of
Peer dynamics are important part of development. In attention problems and hyperactivity) and social skills (i.e.,
late elementary school, children form hierarchical social lower rates of peer nominations of prosocial behavior) that
structures in which some individuals and peer groups are are typically associated with students with mild
more popular and central than others (Adler and Adler disabilities.
1996; Farmer and Rodkin 1996). Taunting, teasing, direct Exceptional students include such children with mild
confrontation, and physical attacks are forms of aggression disabilities as well as students who are identified as aca-
that children (particularly boys) use to demonstrate their demically gifted, though it is important to note that in our
prowess and to establish and protect their positions in the study both groups spend the majority of their school days
social structure (Adler and Adler 1996; Farmer 2000; in general education classrooms. For our purposes, the term
Pellegrini 1998; Rodkin and Hodges 2003). While physical students with mild disabilities refers to children with high
aggression is almost universally related to being disliked incidence disabilities (e.g., learning disabilities, mild
(i.e., having low social preference; Asher and Coie 1990), mental retardation, and mild emotional and behavioral
aggressive behavior has a close association with perceived disorders), academically gifted refers to high achieving
popularity (Rose et al. 2004), and social dominance— children who have been identified for academically gifted
while eventually adapting a more affiliative nature—often education programs, and general education students refers
begins with aggressive behavior (Pellegrini and Bartini to children who are not receiving special services to sup-
2000). Not only is aggression a common part of the daily port their learning needs. Students with exceptionalities
interpersonal dynamics in school, distinct subtypes of tend to have patterns of social behavior, peer acceptance,
aggressive youth can be differentiated in terms of popu- and peer affiliations that are distinct from their general
larity (Estell et al. 2002; Estell et al. 2003; Rodkin et al. education peers. While both academically gifted students
2000). Popular aggressive youth are more likely to asso- and students with mild disabilities differ from general
ciate with other popular and aggressive peers while education peers in terms of social functioning and social
unpopular aggressive youth are more likely to associate adjustment, they are also markedly different from each
with non-aggressive and unpopular peers (Bagwell et al. other.

123
138 J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150

Prior to diagnosis, students later identified as having Further, data from the National Longitudinal Study of
high incidence disabilities display social skill deficits and Adolescent Health, which included 1,301 adolescents with
have low social status (Vaughn et al. 1990; Walker et al. learning disabilities from a sample of 20,780, indicate that
1998). In preschool settings, students with mild develop- youth with learning disabilities were significantly more
mental delays are likely to have significant social likely than others to report involvement in violent behav-
interaction problems (Guralnick and Groom 1987) and iors (31% vs. 25% for boys; 20% vs. 11% for girls) and
these problems are likely to be sustained from the pre- were more likely to have witnessed or have been a victim
school to the early elementary school years (Guralnick of a violent act (Svetaz et al. 2000).
et al. 2006). In late childhood and early adolescence, stu- In contrast to students with mild disabilities, children
dents with mild disabilities tend to have problem social who are identified as academically gifted tend to have
behaviors, social skills difficulties, and are often not well higher levels of social functioning than their general edu-
accepted by their peers (Estell et al. 2008; Farmer et al. cation peers. Preuss and Dubow (2004) found that
1999; Frederickson and Furnham 2004; Gresham and academically gifted students were more likely to employ
MacMillan 1997; Sale and Carey 1995). In turn, they are active problem-solving approaches to social stressors, and
also more likely to be socially isolated (Kavale and Forness that being academically gifted moderated the impact of
1996; Pearl et al. 1998) and to report less social acceptance stressors on adjustment. Academically gifted students are
(Al-Yagon and Mikulincer 2004). rated by teachers as having highly positive levels of social
Although they are more likely to have social difficulties, skills (Janke and Lee 1991) and during the elementary
most students with mild disabilities are members of peer school years they are likely to experience high levels of
groups (Farmer and Farmer 1996; Pearl et al. 1998). social acceptance and low levels of peer rejection (Austin
However, the group membership of students with mild and Draper 1981; Luftig and Nichols 1990; Schneider et al.
disabilities may support social problems as they are more 1989). Academically gifted students are also more likely to
likely to affiliate with classmates with problematic inter- have prosocial or popular friends than their non-gifted
personal characteristics and less likely to associate with peers and less likely to be socially isolated (Farmer and
peers with prosocial characteristics (Farmer and Hollowell Rodkin 1996; Pearl et al. 1998; Schneider and Daniels
1994). Students with mild disabilities are also more likely 1992). On the whole, while they may face considerable
to develop social roles that support aggression or that stress due to their own high expectations (Moon 2004),
makes them the target of the aggressive behavior of others gifted children are largely socially skilled, use effective
(Evans and Eder 1993; Farmer and Farmer 1996; Farmer social coping mechanisms, are socially integrated with
and Rodkin 1996). In addition, students with mild dis- prosocial peers, and are well adjusted. These factors tend to
abilities who associate with antisocial peers tend to hold be negatively related to victimization, and may also make
these peers in high esteem (Rodkin et al. 2006). involvement with deviant peers and a subsequent increase
These social functioning challenges may place students in bullying behavior less likely.
with mild disabilities at greater risk for involvement with In summary, research on peer group dynamics and the
bullying as both targets and aggressors. Problems with social relations of exceptional students comes together to
social functioning, including social information processing suggest that students with mild disabilities may be more
difficulties, social skills deficits, and social isolation are all likely to be bullies and victims and they may have social
risk factors for being victimized by peers (Fox and Boulton characteristics and peer affiliation patterns that exacerbate
2006). Likewise, elementary students who are chronically their risk for involvement in bullying. In contrast, aca-
victimized are more likely to experience academic prob- demically gifted students may have social characteristics
lems and internalizing difficulties that may place them at- and affiliation patterns that protect against bullying and
risk of later learning and behavior problems (Hodges et al. victimization. Little work, however, has examined the
1999; Schwartz et al. 2005). In contrast, affiliations with relationship between educational status (i.e., academically
aggressive and deviant peers tend to be associated with a gifted, general education, mild disabilities) and involve-
greater likelihood of bullying others (Estell et al. 2007). ment in bullying, and less has looked at how this may
Collectively, these findings suggest that children with mild interact with associations with aggressive and/or popular
disabilities may be more likely to be involved in bullying peers. Information along these lines may help clarify peer
as aggressors and/or targets than their typically achieving group dynamics that contribute to bullying and victimiza-
peers. Consistent with this view, extant data suggest that tion in the classroom. Toward this end, we sought to
youth with mild disabilities are more likely to be victims examine differences among academically gifted students,
than are their typically-achieving counterparts and they students with mild disabilities, and general education stu-
may be at increased risk of being bullies and bully-victims dents on ratings of victimization and bullying in relation to
(Kaukiainen et al. 2002; Nazuboka and Smith 1993). their peer associations.

123
J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 139

Four research questions guided our work. Our first have the lowest levels of bullying. We further hypothesized
research question focused on the relationship between peer that students with special needs and neither aggressive nor
nominated social characteristics and involvement in bul- popular associates would have the highest victimization
lying. Are teachers’ and peers’ perceptions of bullying and ratings while gifted students with popular and non-
victimization related to peer nominations on key social aggressive associates would have the lowest levels of
factors (i.e., aggression, positive behavior, internalizing, victimization.
social prominence, social preference)? We hypothesized
that peer nominations of aggression would be positively
related, and social preference negatively related to both Method
teacher ratings and peer nominations of bullying. We fur-
ther hypothesized that victimization would be negatively We employed a multi-source survey design that included
related to peer nominated popularity and social preference, teacher- and peer-report measures as well as school record
and positively related to internalizing. Our second research information. This included peer reports to identify groups
question examined whether peers perceived classmates and to examine how bullying and education status relate to
differently as a function of their education status. Do a variety of other behaviors. We relied on teacher reports to
academically gifted students, general education students, classify the participants’ associates, and used both peer and
and students with mild disabilities differ in the nominations teacher reports to examine involvement in bullying.
they receive from peers? We hypothesized that gifted stu- Finally, we used school record information to determine
dents would be viewed as popular, well-liked, having the education status of the participants.
positive behavior, and neither aggressive nor high on
internalizing, while students with special needs would be Participants
viewed by peers as aggressive, high on internalizing, and
low on social preference, popularity, and positive behavior. Participants came from eleven elementary schools in two
Our third research question investigated the relationship school districts. Both districts were located in a state in the
between education status and associations with teacher- Southeastern United States. One district served a small
rated aggressive and popular peers. Is education status metropolitan area and the other served a rural county. We
differentially linked to peer group membership? We obtained parent consent and student assent for 69% of the
hypothesized that gifted students would be least likely and 701 students from 35 classrooms across the schools. Class
students with special needs the most likely to have size ranged from 18 to 27, with an average of 20 students
aggressive friends. We also hypothesized that gifted stu- per classroom. An average of nearly 14 of these 20 students
dents would be most likely and students with special needs participated in the present study. The sample consisted of
least likely to have popular friends. Our fourth research 484 fifth graders (258 girls, 226 boys), including 369
question explored the combined effect of education status (76.2%; 196 girls and 173 boys) general education stu-
and group types on involvement in bullying. Is bullying dents, 74 (15.3%; 50 girls and 24 boys) academically gifted
and victimization for students in different education status students, and 41 (8.5%; 12 girls and 29 boys) students with
categories moderated by membership in aggressive and high incidence disabilities (e.g., learning disabilities, mild
popular peer groups? We hypothesized that students with mental retardation, emotional and behavioral disorders; see
special needs who have aggressive but not popular asso- Table 1). While 10–20% of students typically qualify as
ciates would have the highest levels of bullying, and gifted having special needs, we examined only those students
students with popular but not aggressive associates would with special needs who spent the majority of their day in

Table 1 Sample characteristics


Girls Boys Total
by education classification and
gender Not in special education 196 (53.1%) 173 (46.9%) 369 (76.2%)
Academic gift 51 (68.0%) 24 (32.0%) 75 (15.5%)
Learning disability 3 (20.0%) 12 (80.0%) 15 (3.1%)
Emotional handicap/emotional behavior disability 1 (20.0%) 4 (80.0%) 5 (1.0%)
Mild mental retardation 2 (33.3%) 4 (66.7%) 6 (1.2%)
Speech impairment 2 (33.3%) 4 (66.7%) 6 (1.2%)
Other health impairment 0 (0.0%) 4 (100.0%) 4 (0.8%)
Unspecified special need 1 (100.0%) 0 (0.0%) 1 (0.2%)
504 plan 2 (66.7%) 1 (33.3%) 3 (0.6%)

123
140 J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150

general education classrooms. This reduced the number of Ethnicity and special education status were related
children with special needs in the sample to the previously (v2(6,n=483) = 17.74, p \ .01). Among Caucasians, 70.4%
mentioned 8.5%. These percentages reflect the distribution (188/267) were in general education, 8.2% (22/267) had
of these populations within the school districts where this special needs, and 21.3% (57/267) were academically gif-
research was conducted and were also consistent with state ted. Eighty-three percent (165/198) African Americans
level rates of having mild special needs and giftedness. were in general education, 8.6% (17/198) had special
All students who were in the top 15% of their class in needs, and 8.1% (16/198) were academically gifted. All
achievement were considered academically gifted by the three Latino/a students were in general education. Finally,
schools. Students in the mild disability sample were iden- 80.0% (12/15) of individuals from other ethnicities were in
tified by local school assessment procedures that reflected general education, 13.3% (2/15) had special needs, and
federal definitions and guidelines for learning disabled, 6.7% (1/15) were academically gifted.
behavioral disordered, health disability, noncategorical
disability, mild mental retardation, and speech disability. Procedures
The different definitions are not provided because all stu-
dents with disabilities were collapsed into a single category We used group administration procedures when collecting
(mild disabilities). We did this for three reasons. First, the survey data. Before the administration of the survey, we
preliminary analyses indicated a high level of similarity assured participants their answers would be kept confi-
between the different special education classifications for dential, asked them to protect the confidentiality of their
teacher-, peer-, and self-assessed characteristics. However, responses, and told them that they could stop participating
variability in the sample sizes for different categories, at any time. We consulted teachers prior to administration
together with the very small sample size of some catego- about typical accommodations needed by students for
ries, could have led us to misinterpret the results. exams, and made parallel accommodations where needed.
Collapsing students into a single mild disabilities category During the survey, one administrator read the instructions
protected against inaccurate comparisons across disability and questions aloud, while additional administrators pro-
areas. vided mobile monitoring and assistance as needed.
Second, during the current climate of serving and Teachers also completed rating forms on each participant
identifying special education students based on service during the group administration. Non-participants were
needs (e.g., mild and moderate need, high incidence ser- told by their teachers to work on class assignments during
vice needs) rather than categorical approaches, there is the data collection.
considerable variability across states in terms of how For all peer nomination measures, the probe focused on
categorical classifications are operationalized. By focusing the classroom level (i.e., participants were told that they
on students with mild disabilities who were included in could only nominate peers in their classroom). We did this
general education classrooms for most of the school day, because the sample was in elementary school and there was
we avoid the issue of variability in categorical classifi- minimal interaction among students in different classes. All
cations. Third, our aim was not to identify characteristics peer nominations were made from free recall (i.e., we did
associated with specific disabilities. Rather, our goal was not provide class rosters).
to explore the relationship between bullying involvement
and students with disabilities in general. Further, this Measures
approach has been used in other studies of the peer
relations of students with disabilities in general education Social Cognitive Maps (SCM)
classrooms (e.g., Farmer et al. 1999; Sale and Carey
1995; Rodkin et al. 2006). For this measure, participants were asked ‘‘Are there some
Because we focused on the social relations of students kids in your class who hang around together a lot? Who are
with disabilities in inclusive settings, we recruited only they?’’ Following the procedures developed by Cairns and
those students who spent more than 50% of the school day colleagues (e.g., Cairns et al. 1985), participants were
in general education classrooms to participate. Therefore, instructed to list, from free recall, as many groups as they
all participants, regardless of education status, were could think of in their class. In addition, they were asked to
included in general education classrooms for all or most of circle the individual or individuals who were the leaders of
the school day, and all classroom assessments refer to each group, if the group had a leader.
inclusive rooms. Fifty-five percent (267/483) of our par- To identify distinct groups within the classroom social
ticipants were Caucasian, 41% (198/483) were African network, we analyzed the SCM data following the proce-
American, 1% (3/483) were Latino(a), and 3% (15/483) dures outlined by Cairns and colleagues (Cairns and Cairns
were of another ethnicity. 1994; Cairns et al. 1995a). SCM procedures have been

123
J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 141

used extensively in research on school social networks in a test-retest r across the factors are .81 for girls and .87 for
variety of populations including a mixed-race urban and boys, and 1-year coefficients are moderately strong (i.e.,
rural sample (Cairns et al. 1988), a mixed-race suburban .40–.50; Cairns et al. 1995b). ICS-T has been shown to
sample (Rodkin et al. 2000), inner-city African Americans have convergent validity with direct observations, grades,
(Estell et al. 2002; Xie et al. 1999), Chinese students school discipline reports, and peer nominations, and pre-
(Leung 1996), and students with special needs (Farmer and dictive validity for early school dropout, teen parenthood,
Hollowell, 1994). Three week test-retest reliability coeffi- and criminal arrest in early adulthood, (Cairns and Cairns
cients indicate high short-term stability of children’s peer 1994; Cairns et al. 1995b). This measure has also been
groups (i.e., 90% of groups maintain a majority of their used extensively in past work on issues pertaining to
members over this period; Cairns et al. 1995a). Further, the popularity and aggression (Cairns et al. 1988; Estell et al.
results of SCM analyses match observed affiliations 2002, 2007; Farmer and Rodkin 1996; Rodkin et al. 2000).
(Cairns and Cairns 1994; Cairns et al. 1985).
We identified a total of 45 primarily-male groups with a Peer Social Preference
range of 2–9 members, and 29 male isolates. The mean
group size for boys was 6.26. A total of 61 female groups Participants were asked to nominate up to three classmates
were identified with a range of 2–9 members, and there from a list of participants they liked the most and up to
were 14 female isolates. The mean group size for girls was three they liked the least, and social preference was cal-
5.23. These means and ranges are consistent with those of culated following the criteria described in Asher and Coie
other studies of late elementary and middle school peer (1990). Specifically, each participant’s number of nomi-
groups (Bagwell et al. 2000; Cairns et al. 1988; Farmer nations for being most liked and least liked were
and Hollowell 1994; Xie et al. 1999). standardized within class, and social preference score was
calculated by subtracting their liked-least z-score from
Teacher Ratings of Social Adaptation their liked-most z-score.

Teachers rated participants on a number of items relating to Peer Interpersonal Assessments


social adaptation. These items were displayed in Likert-
type seven-point formats. The present study utilized three We used peer interpersonal assessments to determine
of these items: ‘‘bullied by peers,’’ ‘‘bullies peers,’’ classmates’ perceptions of peers’ social and behavioral
‘‘manipulates friendships.’’ The two items relating to bul- characteristics. Students were asked to nominate, from free
lying (as aggressor or target) were used as dependent recall, up to three classmates who best fit descriptors for
variables, and the ‘‘manipulates friendships’’ was included several items. They were told during the testing procedures
in the ICS-T factor analysis (described below) as a measure that they could nominate the same person for more than
of relational aggression. one item, they did not need to fill all three blanks if they
did not know three people who fit an item, they could
ICS-T bypass an item if they felt that they did not know anyone at
all who fit that particular item, and that they could nomi-
Teachers rated participants on the Interpersonal Compe- nate themselves. For analyses using these items, however,
tence Scale—Teacher (ICS-T; Cairns et al. 1995b), an 18- all self-nominations were removed from consideration,
item instrument which asks teachers to rate children on a making these peer-nominations exclusively.
variety of characteristics relating to aggressiveness, popu- Past studies using these measures have indicated 3-week
larity, and academic competence. The items are displayed test-retest reliability with individual items ranged from .72
in Likert-type, seven-point scales. These scales are to .93. These items are identical with or similar to peer
anchored by frequency (i.e., often vs. never fights, argues, assessments used by other investigators (e.g., Coie et al.
etc.) or degree (very popular vs. not popular; has lots of 1982; Masten et al. 1985), and past work has found strong
friends vs. has very few friends). These 18 items load onto evidence of reliability and validity in diverse samples (Coie
six sub-scales, though for the present study, only an et al. 1982; Masten et al.1985). We divided the total num-
aggression factor (‘‘argues,’’ ‘‘gets in trouble,’’ and ber of nominations participants received for each peer
‘‘fights’’ from the ICS-T, with ‘‘manipulates friendships’’ assessment item by the total number of possible nominators
from the teacher ratings described above added; a = .84) (i.e., all participants in the class). Because the denominator
and the popularity factor (‘‘popular with boys,’’ ‘‘popular was the total number of participants in each class, the
with girls,’’ ‘‘has lots of friends,’’ a = .81) were examined. resulting proportions were in many cases quite small. In
Past research has shown that 3-week test-retest reliability order to make mean differences clearer, we linearly trans-
coefficients are moderately high (i.e., .80–.92), median formed these proportions by multiplying them by 1,000.

123
142 J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150

Table 2 Peer nomination loadings for varimax-rotated component Peer-group types were a function of the proportion and
matrix number of aggressive and popular members in the group.
Component For the example of aggression, we classified a participant
as having aggressive associates if at least 50% of their
Aggression Positive Social Internalizing
behav. prom. SCM-derived peer group members were classified as
aggressive or, in larger groups, if they had at least three
Disruptive .90 -.04 -.02 -.02 aggressive associates. We used identical criteria to classify
Starts fights .86 -.06 .24 .06 groups by the proportion and/or number of popular
Gets in .82 -.11 .14 .02 members.
trouble
Starts rumors .76 .07 .25 -.04
Cooperative -.06 .88 .25 .12 Results
Good student .01 .85 .12 -.01
Friendly -.09 .86 .11 .17 We present out results in four major sections, corre-
Athletic .26 -.00 .74 .06 sponding to our four research questions. The first section
Cool .25 .24 .83 -.10 uses correlation analyses to examine how peer-nominated
Popular .02 .31 .78 -.06 interpersonal characteristics relate to bullying and victim-
Acts shy -.07 .21 .02 .84 ization. The second section uses ANOVAs to examine the
Sad .08 .01 -.07 .86 relationship between educational status and peer-nomi-
Note: all loadings in excess of .4 are bolded nated characteristics. The third section uses v2 analyses to
investigate the relationship between education status and
aggressive and popular groups. The fourth and final section
We ran a principal components analysis with a Varimax employs MANOVAs with follow-up ANOVAs and Tu-
rotation on 12 items (‘‘bully’’ and ‘‘picked on’’ were key’s tests to explore the combined effect of education
excluded from the factor analysis and instead used as status and group types on bullying and victimization.
dependent variables). Four factors had eigenvalues in excess
of 1.0, and a scree plot indicated that a four-factor solution Peer Behavioral Nominations and Bullying and
provided the best fit to the data, as there was a major change Victimization
in slope between the fourth and fifth factors. This four-
component solution accounted for 75.6% of the variance in We calculated correlations within classrooms and then
the items. As seen in the rotated loading matrix in Table 2, combined them for the following analyses. Due to the
all items loaded on their corresponding component in excess number of correlations run, we only considered those sig-
of .74, and no item cross-loaded on another component nificant at less than the .0025 level (.05/20) as significant.
greater than .31. The resulting factors were aggression Teacher ratings of ‘‘bullies’’ were positively related to peer
(Cronbach’s a = .87; consists of ‘‘disruptive,’’ ‘‘starts nominated aggression (r(454) = .50, p \ .001) and promi-
fights,’’ ‘‘gets in trouble,’’ and ‘‘starts rumors’’), positive nence (r(454) = .21, p \ .001) factors, negatively related to
behavior (a = .85; consists of ‘‘cooperative,’’ ‘‘good stu- the peer nominated positive behavior factor (r(454) = -.17,
dent,’’ and ‘‘friendly’’), social promience (a = .76; consists p \ .001) and social preference (r(451) = -.28, p \ .001),
of ‘‘athletic,’’ ‘‘cool,’’ and ‘‘popular’’), and internalizing and unrelated to the peer nominated internalizing factor
(a = .66; consists of ‘‘acts shy’’ and ‘‘sad’’). (r(454) = -.01, p = .86). Peer nominations for ‘‘bully’’
were positively related to the peer aggression factor
Peer-group Types (r(480) = .89, p \ .001) and the prominence factor
(r(480) = .40, p \ .001), negatively related to social pref-
We classified the peer groups identified by the SCM erence (r(477) = -.24, p \ .001), and unrelated to the peer
analysis according to the aggression and popularity level of positive behavior (r(480) = -.08, p = .08) and internaliz-
their constituent members. Consistent with past work ing (r(480) = .08, p = .10) factors.
examining peer-group characteristics (Farmer et al. 2002), Teacher ratings of ‘‘bullied’’ were negatively related to
we calculated within-sex and -class z-scores for the ICS-T social preference (r(452) = -.17, p \ .001), but unrelated
aggression and popularity factors. We classified a partici- to aggression (r(455) = .06, p = .19), internalizing
pant as popular or aggressive if their z-score was greater (r(455) = .28, p = .05), positive behavior (r(455) = -.13,
than or equal to ?.50. This resulted in 32.2% (146/454) of p = .01), or prominence (r(455) = -.11, p = .05). Peer
participants being identified as popular and 31.0% (140/ nominations of ‘‘picked on’’ were positively related to
452) as aggressive. aggression (r(480) = .29, p \ .001) and internalizing

123
J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 143

(r(480) = .81, p \ .001), negatively related to social pref- to 35.0% (14/40) of students with mild disabilities and
erence (r(477) = -.37, p \ .001), and unrelated to positive 20.5% (15/73) of academically gifted students. Education
behavior (r(480) = .04, p = .44) or prominence (r(480) = status was also not related to teacher-rated popularity status
-.03, p = .55). (v2(2,n=454) = 4.00, p = .14). Among children in general
These results indicate that both teachers and students education, 30.6% (104/340) were classified as popular, as
viewed bullying as related to aggression and social prom- were 27.5% (11/40) of those with mild disabilities and
inence, but also associated with being disliked (i.e., low 41.8% (31/74) of academically gifted participants.
social preference). Teachers also saw bullying as related to Our analyses for peer associations suggested that there
low levels of prosocial behaviors. Both students and were differences in peer group affiliations as a function of
teachers viewed being the target of bullying as related to education status. The effect for aggressive associates was
being disliked, and students further saw it as related to not significant (v2(2,n=465) = 5.30, p = .07). The differences
being aggressive and high in internalizing behavior. for education status were pronounced for popular associ-
ates (v2(2,n=465) = 39.37, p \ .001). A lower proportion (57/
Education Status and Peer Behavioral Nominations 350, or 16.3%) of participants in general education had
popular associates than expected by chance (Fisher’s Exact
Table 3 shows the means and standard deviations of the Probability \ .001), and a higher proportion (36/74, or
peer-nomination factors and social preference by educa- 48.6%) of academically gifted students had popular asso-
tional classification. Education status was not related to ciates (Fisher’s Exact Probability \ .001). The proportion
either the social prominence (F(2,477) = 1.04, p = .35; of students with mild disabilities (6/41, or 14.6%) was not
g2p = .004) or aggression (F(2,477) = 0.79, p = .45; significantly different from chance (Fisher’s Exact
g2p = .003) factors. It was related to peer-nominated pro- Probability = .19).
social behavior (F(2,477) = 7.13, p \ .001; g2p = .029), These results show that students in general education,
internalizing (F(2,477) = 8.43, p \ .001; g2p = .034), and those who are gifted, and those who have mild disabilities
social preference (F(2,474) = 4.61, p \ .01; g2p = .019). were all equally likely to be considered aggressive or
Post-hoc Tukey’s tests indicated that academically gifted popular, and are equally likely to have aggressive associ-
students had more positive behavior nominations than ates. Gifted students were, however, more likely to have
general education students or students with mild disabili- popular associates.
ties. Students with mild disabilities had higher internalizing
nominations than general education or academically gifted Bullying and Victimization by Education Status and
students. Finally, academically gifted students had signifi- Group Types
cantly higher social preference scores than students with
mild disabilities. Bullying by Education Status and Group Types

Education Status, Behavioral Classification, and Peer To examine the potential interaction of groups and edu-
Associates cation classifications on bullying, we ran a MANOVA with
education classification, popular associates, and aggressive
Education status was not significantly related to the tea- associates as independent variables and peer-nominations
cher-rated aggressive classification (v2(2,n=452) = 4.51, and teacher-ratings of being a bully as the dependent
p = .11). For students in general education, 32.7% (111/ variables. The MANOVA indicated that there were main
339) were rated by teachers as aggressive. This compared effects of education status (Wilks’ K = 0.93,

Table 3 Peer-nominated characteristics by education status


Variable Education status
General education Mild disabilities Academically gifted
M SD M SD M SD

Social prominence 82.06 94.43 89.45 143.21 100.00 92.77


Aggression 56.73 102.65 76.34 137.69 51.27 85.08
Positive behavior 92.75A 107.29 70.39A 96.28 141.07B 136.23
Internalizing 52.64A 73.32 96.87B 121.03 34.89A 62.22
Social preference 0.11AB 1.45 -0.31A 1.61 0.56B 1.69
Different superscripts indicate significant differences (p \ .01)

123
144 J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150

F(4,884) = 8.55, p \ .001; g2p = .053), aggressive-associ- 070), and popular associates (F(1,443) = 18.74, p \ .001;
ates (Wilks’ K = 0.93, F(2,442) = 16.93, p \ .001; g2p = .041). There were also two-way interactions of edu-
g2p = .071), popular-associates (Wilks’ K = 0.96, cation status by aggressive associates (F(2,443) = 8.96,
F(2,442) = 9.57, p \ .001; g2p = .041), education status by p \ .001; g2p = .039) and popular associates (F(2,443) =
aggressive associates (Wilks’ K = 0.95, F(4,884) = 5.57, 9.48, p \ .001; g2p = .041). Finally, the three-way interac-
p \ .001; g2p = .048), education status by popular associ- tion of education status by aggressive group by popularity
ates (Wilks’ K = 0.94, F(4,884) = 6.42, p \ .001; group was significant (F(1,443) = 21.42, p \ .001; g2p =
g2p = .043), and the three-way interaction of education .046).
status by aggressive associates by popular associates Post-hoc tests indicated that the main effect of education
(Wilks’ K = 0.94, F(2,442) = 14.59, p \ .001; g2p = .062). status was due to individuals with mild disabilities
To probe these multivariate effects, we ran follow-up (M = 200.97, SE = 29.51) having more peer nominations
ANOVAs for each dependent. For teacher ratings of for bullying than students in general education
‘‘bullies’’ we found main effects for education status (M = 56.64, SE = 9.25) or academically gifted students
(F(2,443) = 4.79, p \ .01; g2p = .021), and aggressive (M = 50.89, SE = 14.17). The main effect of aggressive
associates (F(1,443) = 9.05, p \ .01; g2p = .020), but no associates was due to those with aggressive associates
main effect of popular associates (F(1,443) = 1.49, p = .22; (M = 190.19, SE = 23.65) having more nominations for
g2p = .003), and no two-way interactive effects of educa- bullying than those without aggressive associates
tion status by either aggressive associates (F(2,443) = 0.12, (M = 38.69, SE = 9.33). Similarly, the main effect of
p = .88; g2p = .001) or popular associates (F(2,443) = 3.01, popular associates was due to those with popular associates
p = .05; g2p = .013), and no three-way interaction of edu- (M = 162.74, SE = 22.89) having more nominations for
cation status by aggressive group by popularity group bullying than those without popular associates (M = 61.57,
(F(1,443) = 0.35, p = .55; g2p = .001). SE = 10.56).
Post-hoc tests indicated that the main effect of education Figures 1 and 2 show the two-way interactions of edu-
status was due to students with mild disabilities (M = 4.16, cation status by aggressive associates and education status
SE = 0.54) having significantly higher teacher ratings of by popular associates, respectively. Post-hoc tests indicate
being bullies than those in general education (M = 3.15, that students with mild disabilities and aggressive associ-
SE = 0.17) who in turn had higher ratings of being bullies ates had more peer nominations of being bullies than all
than academically gifted students (M = 2.14, SE = 0.26). other cells. Individuals in general education and aggressive
The main effect of aggressive associates was due to those associates had significantly more peer nominations for
with aggressive associates (M = 4.21, SE = 0.43) having being bullies than all those without aggressive associates.
higher ratings for being bullies than those without Finally, academically gifted students with aggressive
aggressive associates (M = 2.43, SE = 0.17). associates had more peer nominations for bullying than
In summary, teacher ratings of bullying were highest academically gifted students and general education stu-
among students with mild disabilities and lowest among dents without aggressive associates.
gifted students. Further, all students with aggressive asso- Post-hoc tests for the education status by popular asso-
ciates had higher teacher ratings of bullying. ciates interaction indicated that students with mild
For peer-nominations of ‘‘bully’’ there were main effects disabilities and popular associates had more peer nomina-
for education status (F(1,443) = 11.59, p \ .001; g2p = .050), tions for being bullies than all others. Students with mild
aggressive associates (F(1,443) = 33.53, p \ .001; g2p =. disabilities without popular associates had significantly

Fig. 1 Peer nominations of 700


‘‘bully’’ by education status and
aggressive affiliations 600
Peer "Bully" Nominations

General Education Students


500 Students With Mild Disabilities
Academically Gifted Students
400 D

300

200
C BC
100 A AB A
0
Agg. Assoc. Not Agg. Assoc.

123
J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 145

Fig. 2 Peer nominations of 700


‘‘bully’’ by education status and
600 General Education Students

Peer "Bully" Nominations


popular affiliations
Students With Mild Disabilities
500
Academically Gifted Students
400 C

300

200
AB AB B
100 A AB

0
Pop. Assoc. Not Pop. Assoc.

Fig. 3 Peer-nominations of 700


‘‘bully’’ by education status, F
General Education Students
Peer "Bully" Nominations

aggressive and popular 600


affiliations Students With Mild Disabilities
500 Academically Gifted Students

400

300

200

E E
100 DE CD
AB AB AB BCD ABC
A
0
Agg. & Pop. Assoc. Agg. & Not Pop. Assoc. Not Agg. & Pop Assoc. Not Agg. & Not Pop Assoc.

more peer nominations for being bullies than those in associations related to fewer nominations for bullying in
general education with popular associates and academi- gifted and general education students. Finally, these vary-
cally gifted students with popular associates. ing types of associations exacerbated each others’ effects:
Figure 3 shows the three-way interaction of education students with mild disabilities who belonged to groups of
status by aggressive associates by popular associates. Post- both aggressive and popular peers had extremely high
hoc tests indicated that individuals with mild disabilities numbers of peer nominations for bullying.
who had aggressive and popular associates had more peer
nominations for bullying than all others. On the other end Victimization by Education Status and Group Types
of the spectrum, individuals in general education with
neither aggressive nor popular associates had the lowest We ran parallel analyses to those for bullying for victim-
number of nominations. These individuals significantly ization, with the inclusion of isolates as a third group type.
differed from all participants with aggressive associates as We ran a MANOVA with education classification, popular
well as students with mild disabilities with neither associates, and aggressive associates as independent vari-
aggressive nor popular associates. ables and peer-nominations of being picked on and teacher-
In summary, peer nominations for bullying were highest ratings of being bullied as the dependent variables. This
among students with mild disabilities, students with MANOVA indicated that there was a main effect of edu-
aggressive associates, and students with popular associates. cation status (Wilks’ K = 0.95, F(4,886) = 5.92, p \ .001;
But these relationships were more complicated. While g2p = .037), aggressive associates (Wilks’ K = 0.97,
having aggressive associates was related to higher nomi- F(2,443) = 4.66, p \ .01; g2p = .020) and popular associates
nations for bullying for all groups, the effect was especially (Wilks’ K = 0.96, F(2,443) = 4.68, p \ .01; g2p = .021).
evident among students with mild disabilities, and much There were no interactive effects for education status by
less marked in gifted students. A more pronounced dif- either aggressive associates (Wilks’ K = 1.00, F(4,880) =
ference occured with popular associates. While having 0.59, p = .67; g2p = .005) or popular associates (Wilks’
popular associates was related to higher nominations for K = 0.98, F(4,880) = 0.31, p = .87; g2p = .002), and no
bullying in students with mild disabilities, popular significant three-way interaction of education status by

123
146 J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150

aggressive associates by popular associates (Wilks’ (M = 181.04, SE = 41.59) having higher numbers of
K = 1.00, F(2,440) = 0.89, p = .41; g2p = .004). nominations for being picked on than those with
We probed these effects with follow-up ANOVAs for (M = 47.41, SE = 9.67) or without (M = 67.99,
each dependent variable. For teacher ratings of ‘‘bullied’’ SE = 7.34) popular associates. In short, isolates had high
there was a main effect of education status (F(2,441) = 7.62, numbers of peer nominations for being bullied.
p \ .001; g2p = .033), aggressive associates (F(1,441) =
4.09, p \ .05; g2p = .019), and popular associates
(F(1,441) = 5.55, p \ .01; g2p = .022). There were no Discussion
interactive effects for education status by either aggressive
associates (F(2,441) = 1.04, p = .35; g2p = .005) or popular The social dynamics of bullying and victimization involves
associates (F(2,441) = 0.45, p = .64; g2p = .002), and no the interplay between the interpersonal characteristics of
significant three-way interaction of education status by individual students and the characteristics of the peer
aggressive associates by popular associates (F(1,441) = groups in which they are embedded (Rodkin and Hodges
1.78, p = .18; g2p = .004). Post-hoc tests indicated that the 2003; Salmivalli et al. 1997). Consistent with prior
main effect of education status was due to significant dif- research with late elementary students (e.g., Adler and
ferences among all three groups of students: those with Adler 1995; Hodges and Perry 1999; Pellegrini et al.
mild disabilities (M = 3.10, SE = 0.29) had the highest 1999), our results indicate that bullying was positively
ratings for being bullied, followed by those in general associated with peer nominated aggression and—more
education (M = 2.44 SE = 0.08), while academically gif- modestly—social prominence, and negatively related to
ted participants were the lowest (M = 1.34, SE = 0.07). peer social preference and peer nominations of positive
The main effect of aggressive associates was due to isolates behavior. Victimization was positively related to peer
(M = 3.37, SE = 0.32) having higher ratings of being nominations for internalizing and modestly related to
bullied than participants with (M = 2.47, SE = 0.14) or aggressive behavior (positively) and social preference
without (M = 2.15, SE = 0.08) aggressive associates. (negatively). Further, while the effect sizes were small,
Finally, the main effect of popular associates was due to students who associated with aggressive peers and students
isolates (M = 3.37, SE = 0.32) having higher ratings of who associated with popular peers were more likely to be
being bullied than those without popular associates identified as bullies. Students who were isolated were more
(M = 2.35, SE = 0.08) and those with popular associates likely to be bullied than students were in non-popular
(M = 1.77, SE = 0.12). Students without popular associ- groups, who, in turn, were more likely to be bullied than
ates had significantly higher ratings of being bullied than peers who were in popular groups.
those with popular associates. Our findings help extend current viewpoints by showing
In summary, teachers rated students with mild disabili- that academically gifted students, general education stu-
ties as highest in being bullied, and gifted students lowest. dents, and students with mild disabilities are differentially
Further, while isolates had high ratings for being bullied, involved in bullying. Further, such involvement is moder-
those with popular associates had very low ratings for ated in part by their peer group membership. In our sample
being bullied. of 5th graders, students with mild disabilities were more
For peer nominations of ‘‘picked on’’ there were sig- likely to be viewed by peers as being bullies than
nificant main effects of aggressive associates (F(1,441) = were academically gifted and general education students.
5.33, p \ .01; g2p = .023), and popular associates Teachers also perceived students with mild disabilities as
(F(1,441) = 5.47, p \ .01; g2p = .022). The main effects of being more likely to be bullies and victims of bullying than
education status (F(2,441) = 2.17, p = .12; g2p = .010) was were general education students. General education stu-
non-significant. The interactions of education status by dents were, in turn, more likely to be rated by teachers as
aggressive associates (F(2,441 = 0.22, p = .80; g2p = .001), bullies and as being bullied than were academically gifted
education status by popular associates (F(2,441 = 0.17, students. In addition, students with mild disabilities who
p = .84; g2p = .001), and education status by aggressive associated with aggressive and popular peers were more
associates by popular associates (F(1,441 = 0.04, p = .84; likely to be bullies than all other students. In contrast,
g2p = .000) were also all non-significant. Post-hoc tests general education students who were not in popular or
indicated that the main effect of aggressive associates was aggressive groups were least likely to be nominated by
due to isolates (M = 181.04, SE = 41.59) having higher peers as bullies.
numbers of nominations for being picked on than those The importance of our findings comes into focus when
with (M = 75.92, SE = 12.91) or without (M = 59.68, considered in light of ethnographic research on social
SE = 6.87) aggressive associates. Similarly, the main dynamics and survey research on social networks and
effect of popular associates was due to isolates bullying participant roles. Ethnographic studies (e.g., Adler

123
J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 147

and Adler 1995; Evans and Eder 1993) describe early risks of students with mild disabilities. Such students are
adolescent social dynamics as being a process where stu- susceptible to both bullying and being victimized and may
dents are constantly vying for social positions and for also develop peer affiliations that further support their risk
social identities that protect against being victimized and for involvement in bullying. Current efforts in these areas
excluded by others. In such a context, students who are may need to be modified to account for individual needs.
social isolates are particularly vulnerable to being targeted Third, there is a need to develop inclusion strategies that
by bullies. Students who are themselves socially vulnerable are responsive to the differential social needs and risks of
(e.g., students with mild disabilities) are likely to seek out students with exceptionalities and that focus on creating
the approval of popular peers and to bully others as a way classroom social contexts that reduce bullying by enhanc-
to promote their own social positions. Further, by targeting ing the social opportunities and social positions of all
peers who are vulnerable, bullies appear to use aggressive students.
strategies as a way to consolidate the support of others in While our findings are compelling, our study has several
their peer group (Salmivalli et al. 1997). In contrast, stu- limitations that must be considered. First, it should be
dents in middle-level peer groups (e.g., groups that are not noted that despite their significance, the effect sizes for all
prominent or peripheral in the social structure) are more the relationships discussed above are small—ranging from
focused on school activities than their social prominence .02 to .07. These effect sizes may be due in part to the
and are less likely to be involved in bullying (Adler and complexity of the models being fitted—inclusion of three
Adler 1996). main effects in addition to two-two-way and one-three-way
When examined from this backdrop, our results have interactions greatly reduces the variability that any one
important implications for understanding bullying and component of the model can explain. Beyond that, how-
victimization in late elementary classrooms. As students ever, this speaks to the complexity of the processes leading
jockey for position in their classroom social network, they to bullying and victimization: while exceptionalities and
may seek to reduce their own social vulnerability by bul- peer relations are important, many other factors are at
lying others and by associating with popular and aggressive work. This leads directly to the second major limitation.
peers. Because they are socially vulnerable (Frederickson There are a number of variables that could be confounds
and Furnham 2004; Gresham and MacMillan 1997; Sale that we could not include in the study. Family structure and
and Carey 1995), students with mild disabilities may be socio-economic status, for example, tend to be related to
susceptible to being bullied and may compensate by bul- the need for special education services and academic
lying others and by affiliating with peers who support this giftedness, and also have demonstrated relationships with
behavior. On the other hand, academically gifted students social outcomes. These variables could be driving many of
tend to be fairly well accepted in late elementary school the relationships we found and may be contributing to the
(Austin and Draper 1981; Schneider et al. 1989) and may variance unexplained by the our model.
focus more on academics than social prominence. There- A third key limitation was that the sample only included
fore, while they tend to be socially prominent and associate fifth grade students. While this was consistent with our goal
with peers who teachers view as being popular, academi- of focusing on classroom social dynamics during late ele-
cally gifted students do not appear to be vulnerable to being mentary school and the emerging transition to early
bullied and seem to be relatively less likely to be involved adolescence, it is not appropriate to generalize our findings
in bullying. Nonetheless, our findings suggest that general to early elementary school or to middle school. Additional
education students who are not in popular or aggressive research is needed that addresses our aims in samples of
groups—i.e., those who are not supported in aggression by both younger and older students. Fourth, the sample of
aggressive peers and are not fighting to maintain a prom- students with disabilities was small and students with dif-
inent position in the social hierarchy—may be the least ferent types of mild disabilities were aggregated together.
likely to be involved in bullying. Although our sample was small in statistical terms, it is
Our study has important implications for the develop- fairly large relative to many studies that focus on the social
ment of bullying prevention programs and for the inclusion relations of students with mild disabilities. Because of
of students with exceptionalities in general education issues of critical mass (i.e., only a few students with mild
classrooms. First, there is a need for teachers to be aware disabilities per classroom), it was necessary to have very
that membership in both aggressive peer groups and pop- large samples to study this population. This made it nec-
ular peer groups may place students at risk for bullying essary to collapse students with different high incidence
others. Teachers may then be able to utilize seating charts disabilities into a single category. While this is not an
and group activities to encourage such students to form appropriate practice when the aim is to generate knowledge
associations with prosocial peers. Second, there is a need about a particular disability category, it does demonstrate
for anti-bullying efforts that focus specifically on the social the need to include a focus on students with disabilities in

123
148 J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150

bullying and victimization research (which was one of our suggest that some pairings of students may be advanta-
key aims). There is clearly a significant need for large scale geous while others may be highly detrimental. There is a
studies that have sufficiently large samples of students with critical need to scientifically generate new information in
different disabilities. this area that can ultimately be used to help guide peer
Finally, our investigation did not examine gender dif- support practices and anti-bullying efforts.
ferences. It is likely that boys and girls are involved in
bullying and victimization in different ways. Girls are more Acknowledgments This research was supported by grants
H325C020106 and H324C040230 from the Office of Special Edu-
likely to be involved with relational forms of bullying and cation Programs of the Department of Education to Thomas W.
aggression while boys tend toward physical forms of Farmer (Principal Investigator). The views expressed in this article
harassment (Crick et al. 1996). These forms of aggression are ours and do not represent the granting agency.
have differential relationships with both social skills
(Farmer 2000) and social functioning with peers (Cillessen
and Mayeux 2004).This is especially important in light of References
the higher incidence of mild disabilities among boys and
Adler, P. A., & Adler, P. (1995). Dynamics of inclusion and exclusion
the higher rate of giftedness among girls. The relationships in preadolescent cliques. Social Psychology Quarterly, 58, 145–
between exceptionalities, bullying, and popularity may be 162.
in many ways driven by these gender differences. Unfor- Adler, P. A., & Adler, P. (1996). Preadolescent clique stratification
tunately, it was not possible to examine gender differences and the hierarchy of identity. Sociological Inquiry, 66, 111–142.
Al-Yagon, M., & Mikulincer, M. (2004). Patterns of close relation-
among students with exceptionalities because of our small ships and socioemotional and academic adjustment among
sample sizes. As a result, our measures also collapsed school-age children with learning disabilities. Learning Disabil-
across forms of aggression (i.e., included both physical and ities Research and Practice, 19, 12–19.
relational aggression). While the resulting measures of Asher, S. R., & Coie, J. (1990). Peer rejection in childhood. New
York: Cambridge University Press.
internal consistency were adequate, there may be differ- Atlas, R. S., & Pepler, D. J. (1998). Observations of bullying in the
ential relationships between exceptionalities, social classroom. Journal of Educational Research, 92, 86–99.
functioning, and different forms of aggression. Research Austin, A. B., & Draper, D. C. (1981). Peer relationships of
with larger samples is needed to explore gender differences the academically gifted: A review. Gifted Child Quarterly, 25,
129–133.
across different special education categories as well as Bagwell, C. L., Coie, J. D., Terry, R. A., & Lochman, J. E. (2000).
within general education populations. Such work with Peer clique participation and social status in preadolescence.
larger samples may be able to disentangle the effects of Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 46, 280–305.
exceptionalities on bullying and social functioning behav- Cairns, R. B., & Cairns, B. D. (1994). Lifelines and risks: Pathways of
youth in our time. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
ior from those associated with gender and forms of Press.
aggression. Cairns, R. B., Cairns, B. D., Neckerman, H. J., Gest, S. J., & Gariépy,
In conclusion, our study indicates that students with J.-L. (1988). Social networks and aggressive behavior: Peer
mild disabilities, academically gifted students, and general support or peer rejection? Developmental Psychology, 24,
815–823.
education students are differentially at-risk for involvement Cairns, R. B., Leung, M.-C., Buchanan, L., & Cairns, B. D. (1995a).
in bullying and victimization. Further, membership in both Friendships and social networks in childhood and adolescence:
aggressive and popular peer groups supports bullying, Fluidity, reliability, and interrelations. Child Development, 66,
while membership in popular groups appears to protect 1330–1345.
Cairns, R. B., Leung, M.-C., Gest, S. D., & Cairns, B. D. (1995b). A
against victimization. While our work has several limita- brief method for assessing social development: Structure,
tions, it does provide clear evidence to suggest a need for reliability, stability, and developmental validity of the Interper-
research that focuses more specifically on exceptional sonal Competence Scale. Behavioral Research and Therapy, 33,
populations in bullying research. In addition, the finding 725–736.
Cairns, R. B., Perrin, J. E., & Cairns, B. D. (1985). Social structure ad
that membership in popular peer groups may support bul- social cognition in early adolescence: Affiliative patterns.
lying but protect against victimization is highly Journal of Early Adolescence, 5, 339–355.
provocative and warrants more intensive examination. Cillessen, A. N. H., & Mayeux, L. (2004). From Censure to
Finally, our results suggest there is a need to carefully reinforcement: Developmental changes in the association
between aggression and social status. Aggression & Violent
consider and explore the social vulnerability and bullying Behavior, 75, 147–163.
involvement of students with exceptionalities in the Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Coppotelli, H. (1982). Dimensions and
development of anti-bullying interventions. While there is types of social status: A cross-age perspective. Developmental
currently a strong focus on inclusion and on the use of peer Psychology, 18, 557–570.
Crick, N. R., Bigbee, M. A., & Howes, C. (1996). Gender differences
support strategies (i.e., cooperative learning, peer tutoring) in children’s normative beliefs about aggression: How do I hurt
to address the instructional needs of students with mild thee? Let me count the ways. Child Development, 67, 1003–
disabilities in general education classrooms, our findings 1014.

123
J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 149

Estell, D. B., Cairns, R. B., Farmer, T. W., & Cairns, B. D. (2002). of young children with mild developmental delays. Child
Aggression in inner-city early elementary classrooms: Individual Development, 77, 312–324.
and peer group configurations. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48, Hanish, L. D., & Guerra, N. G. (2002). A longitudinal analysis of
52–76. patterns of adjustment following peer victimization. Develop-
Estell, D. B., Farmer, T. W., & Cairns, B. D. (2007). Bullies and ment and Psychopathology, 14, 69–89.
victims in rural African American youth: Individual character- Hawkins, D. L., & Pepler, D. J. (2001). Naturalistic observations
istics and social network placement. Aggressive Behavior, 33, of peer interventions in bullying. Social Development, 10,
145–159. 512–527.
Estell, D. B., Farmer, T. W., Pearl, R., Van Acker, R., & Rodkin, P. C. Hodges, E. V. E., Boivin, M., Vitaro, F., & Bukowski, W. M. (1999).
(2003). Heterogeneity in the relationship between popularity and The power of friendship: Protection against an escalating cycle
aggression: Individual, group, and classroom influences. In W. of peer victimization. Developmental Psychology, 35, 94–101.
Damon (Series Ed.), S. C. Peck, & R. W. Roeser (Vol. Eds.), Hodges, E. V. E., & Perry, D. G. (1999). Personal and interpersonal
New directions for child and adolescent development: Vol. 101. antecedents and consequences of victimization by peers. Journal
Person-centered approaches to studying human development in of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 677–685.
context (pp. 75–85). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Janke, R. W., & Lee, K. (1991). Social skills ratings of exceptional
Estell, D. B., Jones, M. H., Pearl, R., Van Acker, R., Farmer, T. W., & students. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 9, 54–66.
Rodkin, P. C. (2008). Peer groups, popularity, and social Kaukiainen, A., Salmivalli, C., Lagerspetz, K., Tamminen, M.,
preference: Trajectories of social functioning among students Vauras, M., Maki, H., et al. (2002). Learning difficulties, social
with and without learning disabilities. Journal of Learning intelligence, and self-concept: Connections to bully-victim
Disabilities, 41, 5–14. problems. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 43, 269–278.
Evans, C., & Eder, D. (1993). ‘No exit’: Processes of social isolation Kavale, K. A., & Forness, S. R. (1996). Social skill deficits and
in the middle school. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 22, learning disabilities: A meta-analysis. Journal of Learning
139–170. Disabilities, 29, 226–237.
Farmer, T. W. (2000). The social dynamics of aggressive and Leung, M.-C. (1996). Social networks and self enhancement in
disruptive behavior in school: Implications for behavioral Chinese children: A comparison of self reports and peer reports
consultation. Journal of Education and Behavioral Consultation, of group membership. Social Development, 2, 146–157.
21, 194–208. Luftig, R. L., & Nichols, M. L. (1990). Assessing the social status of
Farmer, T. W., & Farmer, E. M. Z. (1996). Social relationships of gifted students by their age peers. Gifted Child Quarterly, 34,
students with exceptionalities in mainstream classrooms: Social 111–115.
networks and homophily. Exceptional Children, 62, 431–450. Masten, A. S., Morison, P., & Pellegrini, D. S. (1985). A revised class
Farmer, T. W., & Hollowell, J. H. (1994). Social networks in play method of peer assessment. Developmental Psychology, 21,
mainstream classrooms: Social affiliations and behavioral char- 523–533.
acteristics of students with EBD. Journal of Emotional and Moon, S. M. (Ed.). (2004). Social/emotional issues, underachieve-
Behavioral Disorders, 2, 143–155. ment, and counseling of gifted and talented students. Thousand
Farmer, T. W., Leung, M.-C., Pearl, R., Rodkin, P. C., Cadwallader, Oaks, CA: Corwin Press.
T. W., & Van Acker, R. (2002). Deviant or diverse groups? The Nazuboka, D., & Smith, P. K. (1993). Sociometric status and social
peer affiliations of aggressive elementary students. Journal of behavior of children with and without learning difficulties.
Educational Psychology, 94, 611–620. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disci-
Farmer, T. W., & Rodkin, P. C. (1996). Antisocial and prosocial plines, 34, 1435–1448.
correlates of classroom social positions: The social network O’Connell, P., Pepler, D., & Craig, W. (1999). Peer involvement in
centrality perspective. Social Development, 5, 174–188. bullying: Insights and challenges for intervention. Journal of
Farmer, T. W., Rodkin, P. C., Pearl, R., & Van Acker, R. M. (1999). Adolescence, 22, 437–452.
Teacher-assessed behavioral configurations, peer assessments, Pearl, R., Farmer, T. W., Van Acker, R., Rodkin, P., Bost, K. K., Coe,
and self-concepts of elementary students with mild disabilities. M., et al. (1998). The social integration of students with mild
Journal of Special Education, 33, 66–80. disabilities in general education classrooms: Peer group mem-
Fox, C. L., & Boulton, M. J. (2006). Friendship as a moderator of the bership and peer-assessed social behavior. The Elementary
relationship between social skills problems and peer victimiza- School Journal, 99, 167–185.
tion. Aggressive Behavior, 32, 110–121. Pellegrini, A. D. (1998). Bullies and victims in school: A review of
Frederickson, N. L., & Furnham, A. F. (2004). Peer-assessed and call for research. Journal of Applied Developmental
behavioural characteristics and sociometric rejection: Differ- Psychology, 19, 165–176.
ences between pupils who have moderate learning difficulties Pellegrini, A. D., & Bartini, M. (2000). A longitudinal study of
and their mainstream peers. British Journal of Educational bullying, victimization, and peer affiliation during the transition
Psychology, 74, 391–410. from primary school to middle school. American Educational
Glew, G. M., Fan, M. Y., Katon, W., Rivara, F. P., & Kernic, M. A. Research Journal, 37, 699–725.
(2005). Bullying, psychosocial adjustment, and academic per- Pellegrini, A. D., Bartini, M., & Brooks, F. (1999). School bullies,
formance in elementary school. Archives of Pediatrics & victims, and aggressive victims: Factors relating to group
Adolescent Medicine, 159, 1026–1031. affiliation and victimization in early adolescence. Journal of
Gresham, F. M., & MacMillan, D. L. (1997). Social competence and Educational Psychology, 91, 216–224.
affective characteristics of students with mild disabilities. Peterson, J. S., & Ray, K. E. (2006). Bullying and the gifted: Victims,
Review of Educational Research, 67, 377–415. perpetrators, prevalences and effects. Gifted Child Quarterly, 50,
Guralnick, M. J., & Groom, J. M. (1987). The peer relations of mildly 148–168.
delayed and nonhandicapped preschool children in mainstrea- Preuss, L. J., & Dubow, E. F. (2004). A comparison between
med playgroups. Child Development, 58, 1556–1572. intellectually gifted and typical children in their coping
Guralnick, M. J., Hammond, M. A., Connor, R. T., & Neville, B. responses to a school and a peer stressor. Roeper Review, 26,
(2006). Stability, change, and correlates of the peer relationships 105–111.

123
150 J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150

Rodkin, P. C., Farmer, T. W., Van Acker, R., Pearl, R., Thompson, J. Schwartz, D., Gorman, A. H., Nakamoto, J., & Toblin, R. L. (2005).
H., & Fedora, P. (2006). Who do students with mild disabilities Victimization in the peer group and children’s academic
nominate as cool in inclusive general education classrooms? functioning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 425–435.
Journal of School Psychology, 44, 67–84. Smith, P. K., Talamelli, L., Cowie, H., Naylor, P., & Chauhan, P.
Rodkin, P. C., Farmer, T. W., Pearl, R., & Van Acker, R. M. (2000). (2004). Profiles of non-victims, escaped victims, continuing
Heterogeneity of popular boys: Antisocial and prosocial config- victims and new victims of school bullying. British Journal of
urations. Developmental Psychology, 36, 14–24. Educational Psychology, 74, 565–581.
Rodkin, P. C., & Hodges, E. V. E. (2003). Bullies and victims in the Svetaz, M. V., Ireland, M., & Blum, R. (2000). Adolescents with
peer ecology: Four questions for psychologists and school learning disabilities: Risk and protective factors associated with
professionals. School Psychology Review, 32, 384–400. emotional well-being: Findings from the National Longitudinal
Rose, A. J., Swenson, L. P., & Waller, E. M. (2004). Overt and Study of Adolescent Health. Journal of Adolescent Health, 27,
relational aggression and perceived popularity: Developmental 340–348.
differences in concurrent and prospective relations. Develop- Troop-Gordon, W., & Ladd, G. W. (2005). Trajectories of peer
mental Psychology, 40, 378–387. victimization and perceptions of the self and schoolmates:
Sale, P., & Carey, D. M. (1995). The sociometric status of students Precursors to internalizing and externalizing problems. Child
with disabilities in a full-inclusion school. Exceptional Children, Development, 76, 1072–1091.
62, 6–19. Vaughn, S., Hogan, A., Kouzekanani, K., & Shapiro, S. (1990). Peer
Salmivalli, C., Huttunen, A., & Lagerspetz, K. M. J. (1997). Peer acceptance, self-perceptions, and social skills of learning
networks and bullying in schools. Scandinavian Journal of disabled students prior to identification. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 38, 305–312. Psychology, 82, 101–106.
Schneider, B., & Daniels, T. (1992). Peer acceptance and social play Walker, H. M., Kavanagh, K., Stiller, B., Golly, A., Severson, H. H.,
of gifted kindergarten children. Exceptionality, 3, 17–29. & Feil, E. G. (1998). First step to success: An early intervention
Schneider, B. H., Clegg, M. R., Byrne, B. M., Ledingham, J. E., & approach for preventing school antisocial behavior. Journal of
Crombie, G. (1989). Social relations of gifted children as a Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 6, 66–80.
function of age and school program. Journal of Educational Xie, H., Cairns, R. B., & Cairns, B. D. (1999). Social networks and
Psychology, 81, 48–56. configurations in inner-city schools: Aggression, popularity, and
Schwartz, D. (2000). Subtypes of victims and aggressors in children’s implications for students with EBD. Journal of Emotional and
peer groups. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 181–192. Behavioral Disorders, 7, 147–155.

123

View publication stats

You might also like