Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

This article was downloaded by: [University of Central Florida]

On: 14 November 2014, At: 06:42


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Journal of Personality Assessment


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hjpa20

Construct Validity of the Relationship Profile Test:


Impact of Gender, Gender Role, and Gender Role
Stereotype
Robert F. Bornstein , Kimberly J. Geiselman , Heather A. Gallagher , H. Mei Ng , Erin E.
Hughes & Mary A. Languirand
Published online: 10 Jun 2010.

To cite this article: Robert F. Bornstein , Kimberly J. Geiselman , Heather A. Gallagher , H. Mei Ng , Erin E. Hughes & Mary
A. Languirand (2004) Construct Validity of the Relationship Profile Test: Impact of Gender, Gender Role, and Gender Role
Stereotype, Journal of Personality Assessment, 82:1, 104-113, DOI: 10.1207/s15327752jpa8201_15

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa8201_15

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and
are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for
any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of
the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY ASSESSMENT, 82(1), 104–113
Copyright © 2004, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Construct Validity of the Relationship Profile Test:


Impact of Gender, Gender Role,
and Gender Role Stereotype RPT
BORNSTEIN
GENDER EFFECTS
ET AL.

Robert F. Bornstein, Kimberly J. Geiselman, Heather A. Gallagher, H. Mei Ng,


Erin E. Hughes, and Mary A. Languirand
Department of Psychology
Downloaded by [University of Central Florida] at 06:42 14 November 2014

Gettysburg College

Research supports the construct validity of the Relationship Profile Test (RPT; Bornstein &
Languirand, 2003), a 30-item, self-report measure of destructive overdependence (DO), dys-
functional detachment (DD), and healthy dependency. In this investigation, we assessed the re-
lationships of gender, gender role, and gender role stereotype ratings to RPT subscale scores. In
Study 1, we replicated earlier patterns of gender differences in RPT scores, assessed
cross-sample consistency in gender difference effect sizes, and provided preliminary
nonclinical norms for the RPT subscales. Study 2 showed that—as expected—DO items are
perceived as stereotypically feminine, whereas DD items are perceived as stereotypically mas-
culine. In Study 3, we examined the relationships of RPT subscale scores to masculinity, femi-
ninity, and androgyny scores. We discuss the theoretical, empirical, and clinical implications of
these findings.

Virtually every theory of personality—cognitive and tion-appropriate affiliative behaviors (Birtchnell, 1987,
psychodynamic, behavioral and humanistic—attempts to ex- 1996). Detachment is associated with deficits in a broad array
plain the antecedents and consequences of individual differ- of interpersonal domains (e.g., social, sexual, occupational)
ences in relatedness. As Baumeister and Leary (1995) noted, and can result from an array of underlying factors, including
variations in interpersonal closeness–distance are not only early learning and socialization experiences that emphasize
central to theories of personality but also have implications self-sufficiency at the expense of social connectedness (Clark
for models of attachment, psychopathology, social develop- & Ladd, 2000; Colgan, 1987), intrapsychic conflicts regard-
ment, social cognition, stress and illness, and psychological ing closeness and intimacy (Birtchnell, 1996; Bornstein,
treatment. Although researchers disagree regarding whether 1998), and biologically based differences in temperament that
humans have a fundamental “need to relate” to other humans elicit detachment-promoting responses from parents and
or whether affiliative ties are a product of other, more basic peers (Coolidge, Thedy, & Jang, 2001).
motives (e.g., biological gratification), evidence leaves little Studies indicate that dependency—like detachment—is
doubt that people exhibit a variety of strategies designed to more complex than theoreticians initially believed, and sev-
attain some optimal level of closeness–distance (Ainsworth, eral researchers (e.g., Bornstein & Languirand, 2003; Cross,
1969, 1989; Birtchnell, 1987, 1996; Gurtman, 1992; Millon, Bacon, & Morris, 2000; Kobayashi, 1989) have distinguished
1996). Some people clearly value close contact with others destructive overdependence (DO; characterized by
and expend great effort to obtain it. Other people seem to find maladaptive, inflexible dependency) from healthy depend-
close contact aversive and expend great effort to avoid it. ency (HD; characterized by flexible, adaptive, help and sup-
Given the importance of interpersonal closeness–distance port seeking). DO stems in part from a sustained pattern of
for myriad domains within psychology, it is not surprising that overprotective and/or authoritarian parenting (Head, Baker,
researchers have devoted considerable energy to exploring the & Williamson, 1991), although certain infantile temperament
interpersonal dynamics of dependency and detachment variables (e.g., low “soothability”) may help elicit these
(Bornstein & Languirand, 2003; Bornstein et al., 2003). Con- parenting styles (Bornstein, 1993). Throughout life,
temporary definitions of detachment emphasize the person’s overdependent individuals exhibit a pattern of insecure, cling-
inability to cultivate social ties or exhibit situa- ing behavior that alienates potential caregivers and under-
RPT GENDER EFFECTS 105
mines the dependent person’s efforts to cultivate affiliative TABLE 1
ties (Blatt & Homann, 1992; Pincus & Gurtman, 1995). Components of DO, DD, and HD
In contrast to destructively overdependent persons, Component
healthy dependent individuals show considerable behavioral
flexibility and the ability to delay short-term gratification to Cognitive Emotional Motivational Behavioral
strengthen long-term supportive relationships (Bornstein, DO Perception of Fear of negative Desire to Clinginess;
1998; Hetherington, 1999). Studies indicate that HD is self as weak evaluation; maintain reassurance
rooted in (a) a history of exposure to authoritative parenting, and abandonment close ties to seeking;
which instills in the child a sense of confidence and ineffectual concerns caregivers/ helpless
authority self-
self-directedness (Lee & Robbins, 1995), and (b) consistent figures presentation
messages from parents and other authority figures that it is DD Perception Fear of being Desire to Social
acceptable to ask for support when needed (Clark & Ladd, others as hurt/over- maintain avoidance;
2000; Lang-Takac & Osterweil, 1992). Studies suggest that hurtful or whelmed distance from rigidly
untrustworthy others; need autonomous
when authoritative parenting is coupled with active encour-
for control self-
agement of situation-appropriate help-seeking, an individual presentation
Downloaded by [University of Central Florida] at 06:42 14 November 2014

is particularly likely to develop a healthy dependent person- HD Perception of Security in Desire for Autonomous
ality style (Kobayashi, 1989; Singelis, 1994; Wang, Bristol, self as intimacy; closeness/ functioning
Mowen, & Chakraborty, 2000).1 competent, confidence in intimacy in coupled
others as autonomy the context of with
Recent research on the links among DO, dysfunctional de- trustworthy autonomy situation-
tachment (DD), and HD has drawn primarily from Bornstein and appropriate
et al.’s (2003) four-component model, which conceptualizes self-reliance help-
DO, DD, and HD in terms of four core processes: cognitive, seeking
emotional, motivational, and behavioral. As Table 1 shows, Note. From “Construct Validity of the Relationship Profile Test: A
each style reflects a particular set of beliefs regarding the self Self-Report Measure of Dependency–Detachment,” by R. F. Bornstein et al.,
and other people, longstanding emotional responses to social 2003, Journal of Personality Assessment, 80, pp. 64–74. Copyright 2003 by
contact, one or more affiliative/isolating motives, and an ar- Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Reprinted with permission. DO =
destructive overdependence; DD = dysfunctional detachment; HD = healthy
ray of behaviors designed to attain some optimal level of in- dependency.
terpersonal closeness–distance. This four-component model *p < .05. **p < .01.
is consistent with findings regarding the interpersonal and
intrapersonal dynamics of DO, DD, and HD (Birtchnell,
1987; Lang-Takac & Osterweil, 1992; Millon, 1996; Rude & daily stressors or major life events (Bornstein, Geiselman,
Burnham, 1995) and provides a framework for conceptualiz- Eisenhart, & Languirand, 2002; Bornstein et al., 2003). The
ing change processes in dependent, detached, and healthy de- convergent and discriminant validity of each RPT subscale is
pendent psychotherapy patients (Beck & Freeman, 1990; supported by data showing that scores on these subscales
Blatt & Schichman, 1983; Overholser & Fine, 1994). show meaningful correlations with indexes of attachment
To facilitate research on the antecedents, correlates, and style, relatedness, alexithymia, affect regulation,
consequences of these three personality styles, Bornstein and self-concept, life satisfaction, and need for approval
Languirand (2003) developed the Relationship Profile Test (Bornstein et al., 2002, 2003).
(RPT), a 30-item, self-report measure of DO, DD, and HD. Initial construct validity studies (Bornstein et al., 2002,
Initial construct validity findings for the scale have been 2003) further indicate that women obtain significantly higher
promising: RPT subscale scores show acceptable internal scores than men do on the RPT DO and HD scales, which
consistency, predictable interscale intercorrelations, and suggests that women show higher levels than men of both
good retest reliability over 23- and 85-week intertest inter- maladaptive dependency and HD.2 Men obtain slightly
vals, even in participants who experience high numbers of higher scores than women do on the RPT DD scale, although
in two separate validation samples (Bornstein et al., 2002,
2003), these DD scale gender differences were not statisti-
1
Although healthy dependency is similar in certain ways to secure cally significant. Thus, women have higher levels of healthy
attachment, there are some important differences between these two
constructs. Secure attachment is associated with substantially
greater behavioral consistency than is healthy dependency, which
may be expressed very differently in different contexts (Bornstein,
2
Riggs, Hill, & Calabrese, 1996; Heiss, Berman, & Sperling, 1996; In both validation samples (Bornstein et al., 2002, 2003), DO
Pincus & Gurtman, 1995). Beyond these behavioral differences, and HD scores were inversely correlated in women (rs were –.35 and
studies confirm that healthy dependency scores are only modestly –.32, respectively) and in men (rs were –.24 and –.34, respectively).
related to scores on measures of secure attachment, with correlations Thus, the RPT DO and HD scales are tapping different constructs,
typically in the r = .30 to .40 range (Bornstein et al., 2003; Pincus & not overlapping aspects of the same trait constellation (for a discus-
Wilson, 2001). sion of this issue, see also Bornstein & Languirand, 2003).
106 BORNSTEIN ET AL.
and unhealthy dependency than men do, but women and men The RPT yields three 10-item subscale scores: (a) DO, (b)
report comparable levels of detachment. DD, and (c) HD. Sample items from the RPT subscales in-
Because gender and gender role help determine partici- clude “Being responsible for things makes me nervous”
pants’ responses to self-report dependency test items (DO), “Other people want too much from me” (DD), and “I
(Bornstein, 1995) and responses to items on measures of var- am comfortable asking for help” (HD).
ious dependency- and detachment-related constructs (e.g., RPT statements were derived from the theoretical and em-
avoidance, alexithymia, self-efficacy, need for approval; see pirical literature on overdependence, detachment, and HD.
Alden, 1989; Birtchnell, 1996; Colgan, 1987; O’Neill & Statements were written to tap the four components of each
Bornstein, 1996; Pincus & Gurtman, 1995; Pincus & Wilson, personality style (i.e., cognitive, emotional, motivational, be-
2001), continued exploration of the impact of these variables havioral) as well as other core features of the dimension in
on RPT subscale scores is warranted. This investigation con- question. Sources used to derive RPT items included Baillie
sists of three studies examining the relationships of gender, and Lampe (1998), Benjamin (1996), Birtchnell (1987,
gender role, and gender role stereotype ratings to RPT 1996), Bornstein (1993), Gurtman (1992), Kantor (1993),
scores. In Study 1, gender differences in RPT DO, DD, and Livesley (1995), Millon (1996), Overholser (1996), Paris
HD scores were assessed, and the magnitudes of these gender (1998), Pincus and Gurtman (1995), and Rude and Burnham
Downloaded by [University of Central Florida] at 06:42 14 November 2014

differences were compared to those obtained in earlier RPT (1995). In addition, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
validition samples. In Study 2, the degree to which RPT DO, Mental Disorders (4th ed. [DSM–IV]; American Psychiatric
DD, and HD items reflect stereotypically feminine or mascu- Association, 1994) dependent personality disorder (PD),
line behaviors was explored by asking participants to rate avoidant PD, and schizoid PD symptom criteria were used to
each RPT item on a 7-point masculinity–femininity dimen- develop certain DO and DD items. Preliminary construct va-
sion. In Study 3, correlations between RPT subscale scores lidity data for the RPT were provided by Bornstein et al.
and scores on the Bem (1974) Sex Role Inventory (BSRI) (2002, 2003); detailed information regarding item develop-
were assessed to evaluate the degree to which variance in the ment and subscale refinement was provided by Bornstein et
RPT subscales may be attributable to gender role effects. al. (2003).

Procedure. Participants completed the RPT in groups


STUDY 1: GENDER DIFFERENCES of 10 to 12. Testing sessions lasted approximately 15 min.
IN RPT SCORES
Results
A plethora of investigations have shown that women obtain
higher scores than men do on self-report—but not free re- The central results of Study 1 are summarized in Table 2,
sponse—measures of dependency (see Bornstein, 1995, for a which contrasts the mean DO, DD, and HD scores obtained
meta-analytic review). These patterns have also been ob- by these participants with those obtained by participants in
tained in studies involving the RPT (Bornstein et al., 2002, earlier RPT investigations. As Table 2 shows, women in our
2003). Consistent with previous results, women should ob- sample obtained significantly higher DO and HD scores than
tain significantly higher scores than men do on the DO and did men. These gender differences parallel closely those of
HD scales, whereas men should obtain slightly (but not sig- the earlier validation samples. Tests for heterogeneity of ef-
nificantly) higher scores than women do on the DD scale. A fect size (Rosenthal, 1991) confirmed that the gender differ-
test for heterogeneity of gender difference effect sizes across ence effect sizes in Table 2 did not differ across samples for
samples should indicate that these effect sizes do not differ any RPT subscale: Heterogeneity estimates (χ2 values) for
from those obtained in earlier investigations. the DO, DD, and HD subscales were 3.75, 0.85, and 5.07, re-
spectively (df = 2 throughout, all ps > .05).
Method Follow-up analyses examined the internal consistency of
each RPT subscale and intercorrelations among these
Participants. Participants were 92 women and 82 men subscale scores. Coefficient alphas for the DO, DD, and HD
enrolled in General Psychology classes at Gettysburg Col- scales were .85, .69, and .77, respectively. Because women
lege who took part in the study to fulfill a course requirement. and men produced highly similar patterns of intersubscale
Participants ranged in age from 18 to 22 years, with a mean of correlations, these were calculated for the combined sample
19.04 (SD = 0.85). No participants had taken part in earlier and were as follows: DO–DD, r(90) = .02, ns; DO–HD, r(90)
RPT studies. = –.23, p < .05; DD–HD, r(90) = –.43, p < .01.

Measure. The RPT is a rationally derived question- Discussion


naire that asks the participant to respond to 30
self-statements, each of which is rated on a 7-point scale The results of Study 1 support the construct validity of the
ranging from 1 (not at all true of me) to 7 (very true of me). RPT by demonstrating that across three separate samples of
RPT GENDER EFFECTS 107
TABLE 2 (DD), and .63 (HD). Cohen (1988) considered effect sizes of
Gender Differences in RPT Scores this magnitude to be small (DD), and medium (DO, HD).3
RPT Scale
Finally, internal consistency estimates and intersubscale
correlations in this sample paralleled closely those obtained
DO DD HD by Bornstein et al. (2003). Although coefficient alphas for
Sample M SD M SD M SD the RPT subscales were somewhat lower than the optimal in-
ternal consistency values described by Nunnally and
Bornstein, Geiselman, Eisenhart, & Languirand (2002)
Womena 27.14 5.90 29.74 6.33 37.77 6.02
Bernstein (1994), they are well within the range of those ob-
Menb 23.75 6.62 30.86 5.68 34.52 6.04 tained for other widely used self-report instruments (see
t 3.54** 1.18 2.27** Viswesvaran & Ones, 2000, for relevant meta-analytic re-
d 0.74 0.27 0.54
sults). Thus, our internal consistency estimates and
Bornstein et al. (2003) intersubscale correlations offer additional support for the
Womenc 26.97 7.05 29.84 5.78 38.13 5.53
Mend 23.69 6.11 30.58 5.37 34.60 4.64 psychometric soundness of the RPT.
t 2.77* 0.74 3.85**
d 0.49 0.14 0.67
Downloaded by [University of Central Florida] at 06:42 14 November 2014

This sample STUDY 2: GENDER ROLE STEREOTYPE


Womene 25.87 6.94 30.46 5.41 37.26 6.41 RATINGS OF RPT ITEMS
Menf 22.45 6.23 30.37 5.81 34.62 6.17
t 3.40** 0.11 2.01**
d 0.52 0.02 0.32 Several investigations have assessed the degree to which
Note. t values represent gender differences in each RPT scale. d values are scores on measures of personality traits and PDs are influ-
standardized effect size indexes derived from corresponding t values. RPT = enced by respondents’ gender role orientation (see Lindsay,
Relation Profile Test; DO = destructive overdependence; DD = Sankis, & Widiger, 2000; Morey, Warner, & Boggs, 2002;
dysfunctional detachment; HD = healthy dependency. Widiger & Spitzer, 1991). To evaluate the impact of gender
an = 50. bn = 40. cn = 75. dn = 55. en = 92. fn = 82.
role on DSM–IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994)
*p < .05. **p < .01.
PD symptom criteria, Sprock, Crosby, and Nielsen (2001)
asked college student participants to rate each PD criterion
on a 7-point masculinity–femininity scale ranging from 1
college students, small to moderate gender differences in (extremely masculine) to 7 (extremely feminine). In Study 2,
RPT subscale scores are obtained. These DO and HD gender we used a similar procedure, with RPT subscale items rated
differences are consistent with a priori expectations and with in lieu of PD symptoms.
previous research on gender differences in self-reported de- In line with Western norms regarding gender role-related
pendency (Bornstein, 1995). The very modest differences in behavior (Brems & Johnson, 1990; Cadbury, 1991; Wang &
DD scores are also consistent with our hypotheses and with Mowen, 1997) and previous research on relatedness and gen-
previous studies involving the RPT (Bornstein et al., 2002, der role (Bornstein, Bowers, & Bonner, 1996; Cross &
2003). Moreover, these small DD gender differences dovetail Madson, 1997; Sprock et al., 2001), we expected there to be
with DSM–IV (text revision [DSM–IV–TR]; American Psy- significant differences in mean masculinity–femininity rat-
chiatric Association, 2000) assertions regarding differential ings of DO, DD, and HD items. DO items should be rated as
gender base rates in detachment-related PDs. According to most “feminine,” whereas DD items should be rated as most
the DSM–IV–TR, women and men are diagnosed with “masculine,” and HD items should be rated as being neither
avoidant PD at equal rates, whereas schizoid PD “is diag- strongly masculine nor strongly feminine.
nosed slightly more often in males” (American Psychiatric
Association, 2000, p. 696).
Method
By pooling data across the three participant samples in Ta-
ble 2, it was possible to derive preliminary nonclinical norms
Participants. Participants were 43 women and 40 men
for the RPT scale based on a reasonably large combined
enrolled in General Psychology classes at Gettysburg Col-
group (N of women = 217, N of men = 177). For DO, women
lege who took part in the study to fulfill a course requirement.
obtained a mean score of 26.43 (SD = 6.45, range = 12 to 45),
Participants ranged in age from 18 to 22 years, with a mean of
whereas men obtained a mean of 23.01 (SD = 6.18, range =
19.93 (SD = 1.30). No participants had taken part in Study 1
10 to 43). For DD, women obtained a mean score of 30.01
or in earlier RPT studies.
(SD = 5.98, range = 16 to 44), whereas men obtained a mean
of 30.90 (SD = 5.65, range = 15 to 51). For HD, women ob-
tained a mean score of 37.84 (SD = 6.21, range = 20 to 50),
whereas men obtained a mean of 34.59 (SD = 5.79, range = 3
RPT subscale norms were calculated using sample size weighted
20 to 49). The standardized gender difference effect sizes means, so each participant sample in Table 2 contributed to the over-
(ds) associated with these norms are as follows: .77 (DO), .16 all mean in proportion to its size.
108 BORNSTEIN ET AL.
Measure. A modified version of the RPT was con- TABLE 3
structed with the following instructions used in place of stan- RPT Scale Gender Stereotype Ratings
dard RPT instructions: Gender Role Stereotype Rating

Listed below are a series of self-statements. Using the Women Men


RPT
scale alongside each statement, please rate that state- Scale M SD Range M SD Range
ment for masculinity–femininity. That is, if you think a
DO 49.58 4.88 37 to 63 50.88 5.60 35 to 70
given statement is stereotypically masculine, you’d DD 33.30 9.10 17 to 56 34.73 6.06 25 to 48
give it a low rating, like 1 or 2. If a given statement is HD 44.37 4.32 36 to 58 44.40 4.04 34 to 52
stereotypically feminine, you’d give it a high rating,
Note. N = 43 women and 40 men. Gender role stereotype rating scores have
like 6 or 7. If a statement seems neither masculine nor a potential range of 10 to 70, with higher scores reflecting greater perceived
feminine, but neutral with respect to gender role, you’d femininity in RPT test items. RPT = Relation Profile Test; DO = destructive
give that statement a middle rating of 4. overdependence; DD = dysfunctional detachment; HD = healthy
dependency.
Aside from this change, RPT items were presented in the
Downloaded by [University of Central Florida] at 06:42 14 November 2014

same order and using the same wording as in the standard


version of the test. The scale for the modified RPT ranged
from 1 (statement is stereotypically masculine) to 7 (state- Discussion
ment is stereotypically feminine).
The results of Study 2 confirm that items in the three RPT
Procedure. Participants completed the RPT in groups subscales differ with respect to the degree to which they re-
of 10 to 12. Testing sessions lasted approximately 15 min. flect stereotypically masculine or feminine behavior. DO
items were perceived as most stereotypically feminine,
Results whereas DD items were perceived as most stereotypically
masculine. These results parallel those obtained in studies of
The results of Study 2 are summarized in Table 3, which gender role and PD symptoms (e.g., Sprock et al., 2001) and
shows the mean gender role stereotype ratings for items are consistent with prevailing theoretical models of un-
within each RPT subscale. A 2 × 3 mixed analysis of variance healthy dependency (Bornstein, 1992, 1993), HD (Bornstein
revealed a significant main effect of RPT subscale on gender & Languirand, 2003), and detachment (Birtchnell, 1996;
role ratings, F(2, 162) = 149.66, p < .001. There was no main Kantor, 1993).
effect of participant gender on these ratings, F(1, 81) = 1.68, In the absence of data regarding the relationship of gender
ns, nor was there a Subscale × Participant Gender interaction, role orientation to RPT scores, it is difficult to know whether
F(2, 162) = 0.33, ns. the Study 2 gender role stereotype ratings reflect genuine dif-
To determine whether gender role stereotype ratings var- ferences in gender role orientation in participants who score
ied as expected across RPT subscales, a focused contrast high on the DO, DD, and HD scales or merely reflect differ-
analysis was conducted on scores collapsed across gender ences in raters’ perceptions of RPT item content. Study 3 was
using Equation 5.2 from Rosenthal, Rosnow, and Rubin designed to address this question.
(2000; with clarification from R. Rosenthal, personal com-
munication, June 30, 2003). This analysis yielded an
intersubscale gender role stereotype effect size (r) of .85, STUDY 3: RELATIONSHIPS OF RPT SCORES
confirming that—as the data in Table 3 suggest—gender role AND GENDER ROLE SCORES
stereotype ratings vary as expected across the DO, HD, and
DD subscales.4 Study 3 examined the relationship between RPT subscale
To further investigate differences in gender role stereo- scores and Bem (1974) BSRI scores in a mixed-sex sample of
type ratings across RPT subscales, three effect size estimates college students. Consistent with research on other
(ds) were calculated reflecting the magnitudes of self-report dependency and detachment tests (Benjamin,
intersubscale gender role stereotype rating differences. 1996; Bornstein, Bowers, et al., 1996; Gurtman, 1992;
These ds were as follows: DO–HD, 1.02; HD–DD, 1.10; and Pincus & Wilson, 2001), we expected to obtain statistically
DO–DD, 1.65. significant RPT–BSRI correlations as follows. RPT DO
scores should be positively correlated with BSRI femininity
scores and negatively correlated with BSRI masculinity
scores. DD scores should show the opposite patterns. In addi-
4
When gender role stereotype effect size estimates were calcu- tion, DO and DD scores should be inversely related to BSRI
lated separately for women and men, the corresponding rs were .82 androgyny (gender role balance/flexibility) scores. Because
and .89, respectively. studies show that HD is associated with cognitive and behav-
RPT GENDER EFFECTS 109
ioral flexibility across a broad array of domains (Bornstein & TABLE 4
Languirand, 2003), HD scores should show moderate posi- Relationships of BSRI Scores
to RPT Scale Scores
tive correlations with both femininity and masculinity scores
and with Bem’s (1974) index of androgyny. Correlation RPT Scale
With BSRI
Method Score DO DD HD

Masculinity
Participants. Participants were 84 women and 49 men Women –0.61** 0.08 0.41**
enrolled in General Psychology classes at Gettysburg Col- Men –0.61** 0.08 0.29*
Z 0.00 0.00 0.60
lege who took part in the study to fulfill a course requirement. Femininity
Participants ranged in age from 18 to 22 years, with a mean of Women 0.20 –0.24* 0.30**
19.26 (SD = 1.01). No participants had taken part in Studies 1 Men 0.11 –0.17 0.40*
or 2 or in earlier RPT studies. Z 0.45 0.41 0.50
Androgyny
Women –0.24* –0.33** 0.44**
Measures. The standard version of the RPT was used to Men –0.22 –0.37** 0.49**
Downloaded by [University of Central Florida] at 06:42 14 November 2014

assess DO, DD, and HD (see Study 1 Method section). Bem’s Z 0.06 0.23 0.28
(1974) BSRI was used to assess gender role. The BSRI is a Note. N = 84 women and 49 men. Z values are Z scores representing the
60-item questionnaire consisting of a series of personality magnitude of gender differences in RPT–gender-role correlations. Because
traits, each of which is rated by the participant for the degree to high androgyny scores reflect smaller masculinity–femininity differences, an
inverse correlation represents a positive relationship between androgyny and
which the trait listed in that item is characteristic of him or her.
RPT subscale score. To maintain consistency with the other correlations in
Ratings are made on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (never or Table 4, the signs of all androgyny correlations are reversed, so a positive
almost never true) to 7 (almost or almost always true). Twenty correlation reflects a positive RPT–androgyny relationship. RPT = Relation
BSRI items comprise the masculinity scale, 20 items comprise Profile Test; BSRI = Bem Sex Role Inventory; DO = destructive
the femininity scale, and the remaining 20 items serve as fill- overdependence; DD = dysfunctional detachment; HD = healthy dependency.
*p < .05. **p < .01.
ers. Typical items from the BSRI masculinity scale include
“Ambitious,” “Analytical,” and “Willing to take risks.” Typical
items from the femininity scale include “Affectionate,” “Com- scores. As Table 4 shows, in participants of both genders, DO
passionate,” and “Gentle.” scores were negatively correlated with masculinity scores
A review of literature examining the construct validity of and unrelated to femininity scores. DO scores were nega-
the BSRI as a measure of gender role orientation was pro- tively correlated with androgyny scores in women, but in
vided by Lenney (1991). Studies assessing the convergent men, this correlation was not significant. DD scores were un-
and discriminant validity of the BSRI have generally been related to masculinity scores in women and men, negatively
supportive, although some studies suggest that BSRI items correlated with femininity scores in women, and negatively
go beyond the straightforward assessment of gender roles, correlated with androgyny scores in women and men. HD
tapping other dimensions of personality (e.g., interpersonal scores were positively correlated with masculinity, feminin-
sensitivity, self-efficacy) as well as masculinity and feminin- ity, and androgyny scores in women and men.
ity (Brems & Johnson, 1990). Further information regarding these RPT–BSRI correla-
In addition to computing standard BSRI masculinity and tions may be obtained from the gender difference Z scores in
femininity scores, a separate androgyny score was calculated Table 4. These Z scores reflect the magnitude of the differ-
for each participant. Following the guidelines of Bem (1974) ence of each RPT–BSRI correlation across gender. As these
and Lenney (1991), androgyny was operationalized as the dif- Z scores show, on every dimension, women and men pro-
ference between a respondent’s masculinity and femininity duced comparable RPT–BSRI correlations (all Zs were
scores, with smaller differences reflecting greater androgyny nonsignificant). When the RPT–BSRI correlations in Table 4
(see Watson, Biderman, & Boyd, 1989, for a discussion of the were collapsed across gender, the following pooled correla-
interpersonal and intrapersonal dynamics of androgyny). tion coefficients were obtained (df = 131 throughout): For
masculinity scores, DO = –.61 (p < .01), DD = .08 (ns), and
Procedure. Participants completed the RPT and BSRI HD = .37 (p < .01); for femininity scores, DO = .17 (ns), DD
in groups of 10 to 12. Order of measures was counterbal- = –.21 (p < .05), and HD = .33 (p < .01); for androgyny
anced across participants. Testing sessions lasted approxi- scores, DO = .23 (p < .05), DD = .34 (p < .01), and HD = –.46
mately 30 min. (p < .01). Except for the DO–masculinity correlation (which
was –.61 in both women and men), all these RPT–gender role
Results correlations are in the small to medium range (Cohen, 1988).
Follow-up analyses examining the internal consistency of
The central results of Study 3 are presented in Table 4, which each RPT subscale and intercorrelations among the RPT
summarizes the correlations between RPT scores and BSRI subscale scores yielded patterns similar to those of Study 1.
110 BORNSTEIN ET AL.
Coefficient alphas for the DO, DD, and HD scales were .82, unaggressive, unambitious, unassertive, and unwilling to take
.64, and .74, respectively. RPT intersubscale correlations risks. Although there have been no investigations assessing
(collapsed across gender) were as follows: DO–DD, r(131) = the detachment–gender role link, these findings are generally
.07, ns; DO–HD, r(131) = –.40, p < .01; and DD–HD r(131) consistent with prevailing theoretical models of avoidance
= –.31, p < .01. and detachment (e.g., Benjamin, 1996; Birtchnell, 1996;
Kantor, 1993) and indicate that participants who score high on
Discussion the DD scale report low levels of femininity: They describe
themselves as relatively unempathic, unaffectionate, and in-
The results of Study 3 indicate that RPT subscale scores sensitive to the needs of others. Clearly, gender role orienta-
show the expected relationships to gender role scores. Ob- tion helps predict self-reported dependency and detachment
tained RPT–BSRI correlations were all in the predicted di- levels in adolescents and adults (see Bornstein, 1996, and
rection and were generally modest in magnitude. Although Kantor, 1993, for discussions of this process).5
the DO–masculinity correlation was larger than the other The finding that HD participants reported high levels of
RPT–gender role correlations, the magnitude of this correla- both masculinity and femininity as well as scored high on
tion (r = –.61 in participants of both genders) was virtually Bem’s (1974) index of androgyny is particularly interesting.
Downloaded by [University of Central Florida] at 06:42 14 November 2014

identical to the correlation between Interpersonal Depend- This pattern suggests that—as Bornstein and Languirand
ency Inventory (IDI) scores (Hirschfeld et al., 1977) and (2003) and others (e.g., Hetherington, 1999; Kobayashi,
BSRI masculinity scores obtained by Bornstein, Bowers, et 1989) argued—HD reflects a complex array of traits that en-
al. (1996). In the Bornstein, Bowers, et al. investigation, able the person to combine intimacy with autonomy and seek
IDI–masculinity correlations were –.64 in women and –.55 reassurance and support without feeling weak or helpless.
in men. The positive relationship between HD scores and androgyny
The positive relationship between androgyny and HD scores indicates that HD participants are capable of system-
score along with the inverse relationships between atically varying their self-presentation and social influence
androgyny and DO and DD scores offer additional support strategies across situation and setting, showing the flexibility
for the construct validity of the RPT subscales. As Bornstein in responding that is central to HD (see Bornstein et al., 2002,
and Languirand (2003) noted, HD should be associated with 2003; Hetherington, 1999; Lang-Takac & Osterweil, 1992;
role-related flexibility in myriad domains, including gender Pincus & Wilson, 2001).
role. Conversely, previous studies of DO and DD (Alden, Taken together, the results of these studies help tighten the
1989; Bornstein, 1992, 1993; Kantor, 1993; Millon, 1996) nomological net surrounding DO, DD, and HD. Table 5 sum-
suggest that these personality styles are associated with lack marizes the empirically established correlates of these three
of flexibility in social interaction and with rigidity in inter- personality styles, combining our results with those of earlier
personal coping and strategic self-presentation. investigations (Bornstein et al., 2002, 2003) and allowing for
some general statements regarding the intrapersonal and in-
terpersonal functioning of overdependent, detached, and
GENERAL DISCUSSION healthy dependent people. As Table 5 shows, the DO person
lacks self-confidence, fears rejection, reports few masculine
The results of Studies 1 to 3 support the construct validity of characteristics, has difficulty resisting external influence and
the RPT as a measure of DO, DD, and HD. The patterns of gen- modulating affect through internal means, and reports a gen-
der differences in this and earlier samples, coupled with the eralized dissatisfaction with life. The DD person distrusts
RPT subscale–gender role links obtained in Study 3, confirm others, is uncomfortable with closeness, feels socially iso-
that the DO, DD, and HD scales are related to gender and gen- lated, has difficulty verbalizing emotions, reports low levels
der role in theoretically meaningful ways. Although under- of both femininity and androgyny, and—like the DO per-
graduate raters judged DO items to be stereotypically femi- son—acknowledges a generalized dissatisfaction with life.
nine and DD items to be stereotypically masculine (Study 2), The picture that emerges of the HD person stands in stark
the RPT–gender role correlations in Study 3 were generally contrast to those of DO and DD individuals. As Table 5
modest in magnitude and indicated that—with the exception shows, the HD person has a strong identity, is comfortable
of the large DO–masculinity correlation—RPT subscale with closeness, has confidence in the dependability of others,
scores are not unduly influenced by gender role orientation. and has an interdependent self-concept that is firmly embed-
In addition to supporting the construct validity of the RPT, ded in their social and cultural milieu (see Cross et al., 2000;
these results have implications for understanding the interper- Wang & Mowen, 1997). In addition, the HD person is able to
sonal and intrapersonal dynamics of DO, DD, and HD. Con-
sistent with extant studies of interpersonal dependency (e.g.,
Sanfilipo, 1994; Watson et al., 1989), these findings indicate 5
Needless to say, detached participants did not literally describe
that participants who score high on the DO scale report low themselves as unsympathetic or insensitive but rated themselves low
levels of masculinity: They describe themselves as on BSRI traits such as sympathetic and sensitive.
RPT GENDER EFFECTS 111
TABLE 5 A key limitation of RPT construct validity evidence to
Correlates of DO, DD, and HD date concerns its normative base: Extant RPT data have been
Trait Correlate r
obtained exclusively from college student samples. Al-
though these data are useful in research settings and in cer-
DO Fear of abandonment/rejection .26a tain clinical contexts (e.g., college and university counseling
Discomfort with closeness .25a
Identity impairment .55a centers), an important task in RPT development involves as-
Susceptibility to external influence .55a sessing the generalizability of these results—as well as those
Affect dysregulation .27a of Bornstein et al. (2002, 2003)—to clinical populations. It
Lack of self-confidence/reliance on others .71b
Dissatisfaction with life .20b
will also be necessary to obtain clinical norms for the RPT
Low masculinity .61c subscales so the instrument can be used effectively in inpa-
DD Lack of confidence in others .41a tient and outpatient settings. Once such norms are obtained,
Discomfort with closeness .38a RPT scores can be used to facilitate treatment planning and
Disillusionment .37a assess changes in functioning during the course of therapy.
Separateness/isolation .27b
Dissatisfaction with life .26b Based on extant construct validity data, it is reasonable to ex-
Alexithymia .47b pect that successful treatment will result in decreases in DO
Downloaded by [University of Central Florida] at 06:42 14 November 2014

Low need for approval .31b and DD, along with increases in HD; unsuccessful treatment
Independent self-construal .21b
Low femininity .19c should not lead to significant changes in these domains (for
Low androgyny .34c related discussions, see Blatt & Ford, 1994, and Sundin,
HD Confidence in others .39a Armelius, & Nilsson, 1994).
Lack of anxiety regarding abandonment .29a As clinical norms are obtained for the RPT, it will be im-
Comfort with closeness .60a portant to assess links between RPT subscale score patterns
Strong identity .37a
Ability to modulate affect/reduce tension internally .27a and PD diagnoses. In this investigation (Studies 1 and 3) as
Satisfaction with life .46b well as earlier investigations (Bornstein et al., 2002, 2003),
Ability to verbalize feelings (low alexithymia) .48b DO and HD scores were found to be orthogonal in women
High need for approval .32b
Relational/interdependent self-construal .33b and men. It is possible that the accuracy of PD diagnoses
High masculinity .37c could be enhanced by examining differences in DO–HD pat-
High femininity .33c terns across PD categories that show high comorbidity but
High androgyny .46c
different underlying dynamics (e.g., narcissistic and border-
Note. rs represent the magnitude of the relationship between Relation line, dependent and avoidant). It might also be useful to ex-
Profile Test score and score on the relevant comparison measure. DO = amine differences in DO–HD patterns within certain PDs: As
destructive overdependence; DD = dysfunctional detachment; HD = healthy
dependency.
several researchers have noted (e.g., Bornstein, 2003;
aBornstein, Geiselman, Eisenhart, and Languirand (2002). bBornstein et al. Westen & Shedler, 1999), many PD categories are far more
(2003). cThis study. heterogenous than is commonly recognized with respect to
symptom patterns as well as associated behaviors. Research
examining within-category differences in dependency, de-
modulate anxiety through internal means, verbalize feelings tachment, and HD may increase the discriminant validity of
(including negative feelings), and show gender role flexibil- PD diagnoses and provide a foundation for more precise PD
ity along with an array of feminine and masculine traits. In descriptors and diagnostic criteria in future versions of the
contrast to DO and DD persons, the HD person reports high diagnostic manual.6
levels of life satisfaction. As with all self-report instruments, it will also be impor-
Although findings regarding the construct validity of the tant to assess the impact of participants’ self-presentation
RPT scale have been supportive, several questions remain strategies on RPT scores in future construct validity studies.
unaddressed. For example, no studies have examined the im- Preliminary findings in this area (Bornstein et al., 2003) indi-
pact of culture and ethnicity on RPT scores, although re- cate that HD scores were positively correlated r = .32) with
search in other areas suggests that these variables may affect scores on a widely used measure of need for approval
the dynamics of overdependence (Neki, 1976), detachment (NAPP; Crowne & Marlowe, 1964), whereas DD scores
(Birtchnell, 1996), and HD (Kobayashi, 1989). An important were negatively correlated with NAPP scores r = –.31). Ad-
next step in establishing the construct validity of the RPT
will be to assess cultural differences in RPT subscale scores
6
and explore subcultural differences in RPT patterns within a The orthogonality of unhealthy overdependence and HD is con-
given society. It is possible that as individuals raised in a sistent with certain models of adult attachment (e.g., Griffin &
sociocentric or individualistic culture become assimilated Bartholomew, 1994); research assessing the links between RPT
scores and attachment style in clinical participants will be useful in
into a society with contrasting norms and values, their DO, extending the utility of the RPT to other contexts and settings (for
DD, and HD scores will shift accordingly (see, e.g., Dwairy, preliminary findings regarding RPT-attachment style relationships
2002; Sato, 2001). in nonclinical participants, see Bornstein et al., 2002).
112 BORNSTEIN ET AL.
ditional research is needed to assess the impact of various Bornstein, R. F. (1992). The dependent personality: Developmental, social,
and clinical perspectives. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 3–23.
self-presentation and dissimulation/faking effects on RPT
Bornstein, R. F. (1993). The dependent personality. New York: Guilford.
scores in different contexts and settings. Bornstein, R. F. (1995). Sex differences in objective and projective depend-
Finally, it will be useful to examine the impact of situational ency tests: A meta-analytic review. Assessment, 2, 319–331.
variables (e.g., mood) on RPT scores. Although Bornstein et Bornstein, R. F. (1996). Beyond orality: Toward an object rela-
al. (2003) found that daily hassles and major life events did not tions/interactionist reconceptualization of the etiology and dynamics of
dependency. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 13, 177–203.
moderate the retest reliability of RPT scores over 23 and 85
Bornstein, R. F. (1998). Depathologizing dependency. Journal of Nervous
weeks, other more immediate influences might well alter par- and Mental Disease, 186, 67–73.
ticipants’ responses to RPT items. In this context, Bornstein, Bornstein, R. F. (2003). Behaviorally referenced experimentation and symp-
Rossner, Hill, and Stepanian (1994) found that instructional tom validation: A paradigm for 21st-century personality disorder re-
set affected participants’ responses to a self-report—but not a search. Journal of Personality Disorders, 17, 1–18.
Bornstein, R. F., Bowers, K. S., & Bonner, S. (1996). Relationships of objec-
free response—measure of interpersonal dependency.
tive and projective dependency scores to sex role orientation in college
Whether similar manipulations affect scores on the RPT students. Journal of Personality Assessment, 66, 555–568.
subscales is an issue that should now be addressed. Bornstein, R. F., Geiselman, K. J., Eisenhart, E. A., & Languirand, M. A.
(2002). Construct validity of the Relationship Profile Test: Links with at-
Downloaded by [University of Central Florida] at 06:42 14 November 2014

tachment, identity, relatedness, and affect. Assessment, 9, 373–381.


Bornstein, R. F., & Languirand, M. A. (2003). Healthy dependency. New
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
York: Newmarket Press.
Bornstein, R. F., Languirand, M. A., Geiselman, K. J., Creighton, J. A.,
This research was supported by National Institute of Mental West, M. A., Gallagher, H. A., et al. (2003). Construct validity of the Rela-
Health Grant MH63723–01A1 to Robert F. Bornstein. We tionship Profile Test: A self-report measure of dependency-detachment.
thank Robert Rosenthal for his statistical advice. Journal of Personality Assessment, 80, 64–74.
Bornstein, R. F., Riggs, J. M., Hill, E. L., & Calabrese, C. (1996). Activity,
passivity, self-denigration, and self-enhancement: Toward an
interactionist model of interpersonal dependency. Journal of Personality,
REFERENCES 64, 637–673.
Bornstein, R. F., Rossner, S. C., Hill, E. L., & Stepanian, M. L. (1994). Face
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1969). Object relations, dependency, and attachment: validity and fakability of objective and projective measures of depend-
A Theoretical review of the infant–mother relationship. Child Develop- ency. Journal of Personality Assessment, 63, 363–386.
ment, 40, 969–1025. Brems, C., & Johnson, M. E. (1990). Re-examination of the Bem Sex Role
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psy- Inventory: The Interpersonal BSRI. Journal of Personality Assessment,
chologist, 44, 709–716. 55, 484–498.
Alden, L. E. (1989). Short-term structured treatment for avoidant personal- Cadbury, S. (1991). The concept of dependence as developed by Birtchnell:
ity disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 756–764. A critical evaluation. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 64,
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical man- 237–251.
ual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Clark, K. E., & Ladd, G. W. (2000). Connectedness and autonomy sup-
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical man- port in parent–child relationships: Links to children’s socioemotional
ual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author. orientation and peer relationships. Developmental Psychology, 36,
Baille, A. J., & Lampe, L. A. (1998). Avoidant personality disorder: Empiri- 485–498.
cal support for DSM–IV revision. Journal of Personality Disorders, 12, Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd
23–30. ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for in- Colgan, P. (1987). Treatment of dependency disorders in men: Toward a bal-
terpersonal attachment as a fundamental human motivation. Psychologi- ance of identity and intimacy. Journal of Chemical Dependency Treat-
cal Bulletin, 117, 497–529. ment, 1, 205–227.
Beck, A. T., & Freeman, A. (1990). Cognitive therapy of the personality dis- Coolidge, F. L., Thedy, L. L., & Jang, K. J. (2001). Heritability of personal-
orders. New York: Guilford. ity disorders in childhood: A preliminary investigation. Journal of Per-
Bem, S. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of sonality Disorders, 15, 33–40.
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 155–162. Cross, S. E., Bacon, P. L., & Morris, M. L. (2000). The rela-
Benjamin, L. S. (1996). Interpersonal diagnosis and treatment of personal- tional-interdependent self-construal and relationships. Journal of Person-
ity disorders. New York: Guilford. ality and Social Psychology, 78, 791–808.
Birtchnell, J. (1987). Attachment-detachment, directiveness-receptiveness: Cross, S. E., & Madson, L. (1997). Models of the self: Self-construals and
A system for classifying interpersonal attitudes and behavior. British gender. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 5–37.
Journal of Medical Psychology, 60, 17–27. Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1964). The approval motive. New York:
Birtchnell, J. (1996). Detachment. In C. G. Costello (Ed.), Personality char- Wiley.
acteristics of the personality disordered (pp. 173–205). New York: Wiley. Dwairy, M. (2002). Foundations of psychosocial dynamic personality the-
Blatt, S. J., & Ford, R. Q. (1994). Therapeutic change: An object relations ory of collective people. Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 343–360.
perspective. New York: Plenum. Griffin, D., & Bartholomew, K. (1994). Models of the self and other: Funda-
Blatt, S. J., & Homann, E. (1992). Parent–child interaction in the etiology of de- mental dimensions underlying measures of adult attachment. Journal of
pendent and self-critical depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 12, 47–91. Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 430–445.
Blatt, S. J., & Schichman, S. (1983). Two primary configurations of Gurtman, M. B. (1992). Construct validity of interpersonal personality mea-
psychopathology. Psychoanalysis and Contemporary Thought, 6, sures: The interpersonal circumplex as a nomological net. Journal of Per-
187–254. sonality and Social Psychology, 63, 105–118.
RPT GENDER EFFECTS 113
Head, S. B., Baker, J. D., & Williamson, D. A. (1991). Family environment Pincus, A. L., & Wilson, K. R. (2001). Interpersonal variability in dependent
characteristics and dependent personality disorder. Journal of Personality personality. Journal of Personality, 69, 223–251.
Disorders, 5, 256–263. Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research (Rev.
Heiss, G. E., Berman, W. H., & Sperling, M. B. (1996). Five scales in search ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
of a construct: Exploring continued attachment to parents in college stu- Rosenthal, R., Rosnow, R. L., & Rubin, D. B. (2000). Contrasts and effect
dents. Journal of Personality Assessment, 67, 102–115. sizes in behavioral research: A correlational approach. New York: Cam-
Hetherington, A. (1999). The development of trust and dependency in an in- bridge University Press.
dividual through the lifespan and its influence on group dynamics. Social Rude, S. S., & Burnham, B. L. (1995). Connectedness and neediness: Fac-
Science International, 15, 1–10. tors of the DEQ and SAS dependency scales. Cognitive Therapy and Re-
Hirschfeld, R. M. A., Klerman, G. L., Gough, H. G., Barrett, J., Korchin, S. search, 19, 323–340.
J., & Chodoff, P. (1977). A measure of interpersonal dependency. Journal Sanfilipo, M. P. (1994). Masculinity, femininity, and subjective experiences
of Personality Assessment, 41, 610–618. of depression. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 50, 144–157.
Kantor, M. (1993). Distancing. Westport, CT: Praeger. Sato, T. (2001). Autonomy and relatedness in psychopathology and treat-
Kobayashi, J. S. (1989). Depathologizing dependency: Two perspectives. ment: A cross-cultural formulation. Genetic, Social, and General Psy-
Psychiatric Annals, 19, 653–658. chology Monographs, 127, 89–127.
Lang-Takac, E., & Osterweil, Z. (1992). Separateness and connectedness: Singelis, T. M. (1994). The measurement of independent and interdependent
Differences between the genders. Sex Roles, 27, 277–289. self-construals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 580–591.
Lee, R. M., & Robbins, S. B. (1995). Measuring belongingness: The Social Sprock, J., Crosby, J. P., & Nielsen, B. A. (2001). Effects of sex and sex roles
Downloaded by [University of Central Florida] at 06:42 14 November 2014

Connectedness and Social Assurance scales. Journal of Counseling Psy- on the perceived maladaptiveness of DSM–IV personality disorder symp-
chology, 42, 232–241. toms. Journal of Personality Disorders, 15, 41–59.
Lenney, E. (1991). Sex roles: The measurement of masculinity, femininity, Sundin, E., Armelius, B. C., & Nilsson, T. (1994). Reliability studies of
and androgyny. In J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman scales of psychological capacities: A new method to assess psychological
(Eds.), Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes (pp. change. Psychoanalysis and Contemporary Thought, 17, 591–615.
573–660). New York: Academic. Viswesvaran, C., & Ones, D. S. (2000). Measurement error in “Big Five Fac-
Lindsay, K. A., Sankis, L. M., & Widiger, T. A. (2000). Gender bias in tors” personality assessment: Reliability generalization across studies and
self-report personality disorder inventories. Journal of Personality Disor- measures. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 60, 224–235.
ders, 14, 218–232. Wang, C. L., Bristol, T., Mowen, J. C., & Chakraborty, G. (2000). Alterna-
Livesley, W. J. (Ed.). (1995). The DSM–IV personality disorders. New tive modes of self-construal: Dimensions of connectedness-separateness
York: Guilford. and advertising appeals to the cultural and gender-specific self. Journal of
Millon, T. (1996). Disorders of personality: DSM–IV and beyond. New Consumer Psychology, 9, 107–115.
York: Wiley. Wang, C. L., & Mowen, J. C. (1997). The separateness-connectedness
Morey, L. C., Warner, M. B., & Boggs, C. D. (2002). Gender bias in the per- self-schema. Psychology and Marketing, 14, 185–207.
sonality disorders criteria: An investigation of five bias indicators. Jour- Watson, P. J., Biderman, M. D., & Boyd, C. (1989). Androgeny as synthetic
nal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 24, 55–65. narcissism. Sex Roles, 21, 175–207.
Neki, J. S. (1976). An examination of the cultural relativism of dependence Westen, D., & Shedler, J. (1999). Revising and assessing Axis II: Toward an
as a dynamic of social and therapeutic relationships. British Journal of empirically based and clinically useful classification of personality disor-
Medical Psychology, 49, 1–10. ders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 273–285.
Nunnally, J. C., & Bernstein, I. H. (1994). Psychometric theory (3rd ed.). Widiger, T. A., & Spitzer, R. L. (1991). Sex bias in the diagnosis of personal-
New York: McGraw-Hill. ity disorders: Conceptual and methodological issues. Clinical Psychology
O’Neill, R. M., & Bornstein, R. F. (1996). Dependency and alexithymia in Review, 11, 1–22.
psychiatric inpatients. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 184,
302–306.
Robert F. Bornstein
Overholser, J. C. (1996). The dependent personality and interpersonal prob-
lems. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 184, 8–16. Department of Psychology
Overholser, J. C., & Fine, M. A. (1994). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of Box 407
excessive interpersonal dependency: A four-stage psychotherapy model. Gettysburg College
Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 8, 55–70. Gettysburg, PA 17325
Paris, J. (1998). Working with traits: Psychotherapy of personality disor-
E-mail: bbornste@gettysburg.edu
ders. Northvale, NJ: Aronson.
Pincus, A. L., & Gurtman, M. B. (1995). The three faces of interpersonal de-
pendency: Structural analysis of self-report dependency measures. Jour- Received April 16, 2003
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 744–758. Revised June 11, 2003

You might also like