Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

An Examination of Consumer Decision

Making for a Common Repeat


Purchase Product
WAYNE D. HOYER*

Despite the large amount of theory and research on consumer choice, current
understanding is still at a less than desirable level-especially in the cases where
involvement with or importance of the choice is low and the product is purchased
frequently. The present paper provides a view of decision making based on the
notion that consumers are not motivated to engage in a great deal of in-store
decIsion making at the time of purchase when the product is purchased repeatedly
and is relatively unimportant. As a result, consumers tend to apply very simple
choice rules or tactics that provide a satisfactory choice while allowing a quick
and effortless decision. An empirical test of this proposition is provided and
implications are discussed.

I n recent years, a considerable amount of effort has


been devoted to understanding the processes by
which consumers arrive at some type of decision
tions which elicit a considerable degree of cognitive
effort and commitment.
For example, the widely employed Fishbein model
(usually a purchase). A large portion of this research (Fishbein 1963, 1967) was developed to understand
has focused on two basic aspects: information acqui~ social attitudes of major importance to individuals.
sition (e.g., Bettman and Park 1980; Jacoby 1977; Some authors, however, have suggested that many
Russo and Rosen 1975) and information integration common product decisions may not be that important
(e.g., Ryan and Bonfield 1975; Wilkie and Pessemier or involving to consumers (e.g., Hupfer and Gardner
1973; Wright 1975). Despite the attention this topic 197 I; Kassarjian 1978). One must therefore question
has received, a large portion of the variance in con~ whether these models are accurate representations of
sumer choice has yet to be explained (Kassarjian how decisions are made for a variety of decision
1978). Although broad generalizations can be made contexts. Indeed, Wright (1975) states that certain
regarding choice processes, our ability to predict and decision strategies require a considerable degree of
understand this behavior is still at a less than desirable cognitive effort which the consumer may be unwilling
level. The present paper is an attempt to account for to expend. Thus, when examining these decision con~
some of this unexplained variance. texts, a driving issue is the extent to which consumers
engage in processing that is consistent with traditional
models of consumer choice (e.g., compensatory or
CONSUMER DECISION PROCESSES noncompensatory models), or whether some other
Although a number of specific explanations for the form of processing occurs.
shortcomings of consumer decision research can be Second, much of the research on consumer decision
generated, the present paper focuses on two major making has focused on cognitive processing that occurs
aspects. First, researchers have tended to apply decision immediately prior to the act of purchase (or selection).
models which have been borrowed from other areas Yet many decisions are made repeatedly or frequently
of inquiry (e.g., social psychology, economics, and over time and thus involve continuous-as opposed
cognitive psychology). The problem is that these mod~ to discrete-processing (Hogarth 1981). In these in~
els were developed to understand processes in situa~ stances, consumers may rely not only on previously
acquired product information stored in memory, but
also on judgments of brand satisfaction or dissatisfac~
• Wayne D. Hoyer is Assistant Professor in the Department of tion which occur in the post-purchase evaluation (or
Marketing Administration, University of Texas at Austin, TX usage) stage of the decision process. In addition,
787 I 2. Thanks are extended to Rohit Deshpande, Mark Alpert, consumers may engage in other types of processing
and Julia Brown for comments on an earlier draft of this paper.
outside the immediate choice context. For example,
822
© JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH. Vol. J J • December J984

Copyright © 2001 All Rights Reserved


COMMON PRODUCT DECISION MAKING 823

consumers may decide to choose a product based on best"), affect ("buy the most pleasing brand"), or
knowledge from an advertisement or because of a normative factors ("buy the same brand my mother
coupon acquisition. Again, one must question whether buys;" Deshpande et al. 1982). It is important to note
traditional models of consumer choice adequately that these tactics are even more simple and involve
explain processing in these types of situations. less effort than many of the heuristics already suggested
in the consumer literature (Bettman 1979; Engel and
AN ALTERNATIVE VIEW Blackwell 1983).
The notion of decision heuristics is not new to
In light of these two issues, Deshpande et al. (1982) consumer research. However, a choice tactic approach
proposed a view which attempts to describe consumer differs distinctly from previous models in the process
decision making in situations that involve repeated of tactic development. Most of the previous approaches
purchases over time and that can be typically consid- assume that cognitive evaluation of the evoked set is
ered as low in importance or involvement. This per- performed prior to each choice (i.e., a discrete process;
spective is based on the notion that "when purchase Hogarth 1981); but a choice tactic view assumes that
behavior is preceded by a choice process, it is likely since purchase (or choice) occurs fairly often, evalua-
to be very limited" (Olshavsky and Granbois 1979, tion occurs over a repeated number of trials (or
p. 99). It assumes that the major goal in repetitive purchase occasions). This focus on processing over
and relatively unimportant decisions is not to make trials distinguishes the present approach from more
an "optimal" choice but, rather, to make a satisfactory traditional simplifying heuristics (e.g., the lexicographic
choice while minimizing cognitive effort. It therefore model).
postulates that consumers optimize time and effort as In the process of tactic development, initial choices
opposed to consequences (Einhorn and Hogarth 1981). may be either haphazard, based on the modeling of
This emphasis on effort is the result of three major others (e.g., "buy what my friends or parents buy"),
factors: or constructed at the time of the choice (Bettman and
1. These decisions are not important enough to indi- Zins 1977). Also, the familiarity which results from
viduals and do not involve a degree of risk large passive exposure to advertisements may play an im-
enough to warrant significant decision making effort. portant role in the early stages of choice tactic devel-
opment. Over time, however, consumers refine their
2. Consumers have made these decisions numerous tactics until a satisfactory decision can be made with
times in the past and thus do not need to expend a
very little effort. For example, a consumer might
great deal of decision making effort.
initially employ the tactic "buy the cheapest brand."
3. A typical shopping trip involves numerous decisions, If this results in a satisfactory choice, this tactic will
and it is likely that the consumer does not want to have an increased probability of being employed on
expend a great deal of time and effort on anyone the next purchase occasion. Alternatively, consumers
decision. might purchase the same brand on subsequent occa-
Preliminary support for the concept of reduced cog- sions to minimize the cognitive effort associated with
nitive effort in these types of situations is provided in checking all the prices. If an unsatisfactory choice
a study of Deshpande and Hoyer (1983), in which it results, the consumer might then employ a more
was found that consumers exerted considerably less refined or stringent rule, such as "buy the cheapest
effort in choosing peanut butter than in choosing a national brand," or use another tactic altogether.
pair of running shoes and an automobile. Consistent It is recognized that brand choice may occur as the
with this notion are Wright's (1975) findings that result of a brand evaluation process. But in contrast
certain decision strategies-especially those which to the traditional view, which assumes that this eval-
"optimize" the decision-require a considerable degree uation is developed at the moment of choice, the
of cognitive effort, and that in some cases, a strategy choice tactic view suggests that an evaluation may
may be chosen because of its "simplifying" potential. occur if the product is perceived to be satisfactory (or
Thus it is postulated that consumers employ very unsatisfactory) when it is used (i.e., at the post-
simple choice heuristics or tactics when making repeat purchase evaluation stage). Thus the consumer still
decisions which are relatively low in importance or does not engage in extensive processing; rather, s/he
involvement. I These tactics are rules of thumb which relies on a simple judgment which is acquired over
allow consumers to make a very quick and effortless time.
decision. For example, tactics could be related to price It should also be mentioned that two familiar con-
(e.g., "buy the cheapest brand" or "buy the brand on cepts in the consumer behavior literature-habitual
sale"), performance ("buy the brand which works the purchase and brand loyalty-are easily incorporated
into this framework. Habitual purchase would consist
of the continued use of the tactic "buy the same brand
1 It must be noted that consumers may also apply these very
I bought last time." As Jacoby and Kyner (1973)
simple rules in very complex and involving tasks, but discussion point out, habitual purchase is not necessarily the
here is limited to the low-involvement situation. result of a strong positive brand evaluation. The

Copyright © 2001 All Rights Reserved


824 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

habitual purchaser does not engage in repeat purchase the basis of verbal responses) may not agree with what
because of a strong preference for the brand; rather, they actually do (under direct observation).
repeat purchasing represents a convenient way of One data collection technique which may partially
reducing cognitive effort. overcome these problems is that of direct, in-store
Brand loyalty involves a preference for the brand. observation. Wells and Lo Scuito (1966) suggest that
This preference probably results from a post-purchase this technique provides a more accurate view of con-
evaluation of product performance in which the con- sumer behavior because it allows the consumer to
sumer perceives that the brand satisfies needs better behave without as much interference from the re-
than do existing alternatives. Thus, habitual and brand- searcher and because it does not rely on the consumer's
loyal consumers possess different motivations for the ability to interpret a question or remember an event.
same behavior, and any effort to change these behaviors In the present study, consumers were unobtrusively
would involve a different strategy (e.g., altering habit observed while making a repetitive, low-involvement
vs. changing brand preference). purchase. This was done to gain a clearer picture of
The basic point is that-through a series of trials how the choice process proceeds in a natural, in-store
which involve either positive, negative, or neutral environment. Particularly important was an exami-
experiences-the post-purchase evaluation will stabi- nation of the degree of decision effort (measured in
lize and consumers will come to possess a set of very terms of the amount of information search and time
simple decision heuristics. That is, consumers acquire taken). Then, by supplementing natural observation
a set of choice tactics which are learned over time. with a simple questioning technique, an attempt was
These tactics make it unnecessary for the consumer made to determine the type of choice strategy consu-
to engage in extended cognitive processing each time mers used while making their decisions.
a decision is made; all that is required is the application
of the simple rule of thumb. This enables the consumer METHOD
to minimize cognitive effort and still make a satisfac-
tory choice. Support for this view is provided by Subjects
Hogarth (1981), who states that in continuous contexts, A total of 120 subjects was interviewed in three
decision makers apply simple heuristics and "adjust" major chain grocery stores in a metropolitan area.
these rules on the basis of outcome feedback. These stores were selected to provide a typical sample
of consumers living in the city (in terms of social
EXAMINING CHOICE TACTICS class, age, and race). Subjects were interviewed in
three major parts of the day (morning, afternoon, and
The main thrust of the present study is that choice evening) across all three stores to reduce biases due
tactics make it unnecessary to engage in extended to shopping environment and time period (Wells and
cognitive effort in any given purchase situation. If Lo Scuito 1966). Every consumer who entered a
decision making is examined at anyone point in time, supermarket aisle (when the experimenter was free)
the process is very likely to be very limited-involving was approached immediately after having selected a
only the application of a choice tactic. The purpose brand of laundry detergent. All age categories (over
of the present study is to examine both the degree 18) were represented: the 25-34-year-old group was
and the type of processing that occurs when consumers the most common (35 percent), followed by 45-54
are making a choice for a common, frequently pur- (19 percent) and 35-44 (18 percent). About 80 percent
chased, and typically low-involvement product. In of the subjects were female, 72 percent were white, 16
examining these issues, however, an important meth- percent were black, and 10 percent were Hispanic.
odological issue arises. The subjects had been purchasing laundry detergent
Many of the previous studies examining the con- from one to 61 years, with a mean of about 19 years.
sumer choice process have employed methodologies The number of brands each consumer had purchased
which are obtrusive. In some instances, this obtrusive- ranged from one to 15, with a mean of about five.
ness occurs because the measurement technique struc- Each potential subject was offered a 25-cent coupon
tures the problem for the consumer. (Examples include good on any brand of laundry detergent. This appears
compensatory models and conjoint measurement.) to have been an adequate incentive, since only JO of
Given that decision makers are highly responsive to the 130 consumers approached declined to participate.
task structure variables (Bettman 1979), there is a
potential for the "true" decision process to be obscured. Choice Task
Further, with many of the decision process techniques,
consumers are well aware that their behavior is being Laundry detergent was selected as the choice task
observed, and this may alter their responses due to for several reasons. 2 First, detergent is clearly a repeat
reactivity of measurement (Cook and Campbell 1979).
In addition, many techniques rely heavily on detailed 2 The examination of only one product category poses limitations
verbal reports, and as Wells and Lo Scuito (1966) and on the ability to draw conclusions. This fact will be discussed at
Jacoby (1977) report, what consumers say they do (on more length in a later section.

-------~-------"'c::-o::::py-:::r;;:;ig~ht;:-;©i"\c~2001 All Rights Reserved


COMMON PRODUCT DECISION MAKING 825

purchase product that appears to be similar to generally coupon for the brand they purchased in return for
recognized low-involvement products (e.g., Lastovicka participating in the study. A 12-question instrument
and Gardner 1979; Rothschild 1979). Second, deter- was then administered to assess several aspects of
gent was found to possess a very low level of perceived consumers' laundry detergent purchases. Choice tactics
risk in a pre-test. Third, there is a wide variety of used by consumers were measured two ways. First,
brands available, which might enhance the number consumers were asked to tell the interviewer why they
and variety of tactics that consumers could employ. made their laundry detergent purchase. Specifically,
Fourth, large laundry detergent displays are common consumers were asked, "Can you tell me why you
in supermarkets. It would be difficult to observe search selected the brand you have chosen?" This open-
behavior in front of a very small display. ended question was intended to elicit an immediate
post-decision response concerning the way the chosen
Assessment of the Search brand had been selected. This question was strictly
and Decision Process "free response" with an absence of probes, to allow
consumers to describe their choice process in their
Consumers were carefully observed by a single own terms. It was felt that more detailed questioning
experimenter (who was naive to the purpose of the would have been too obtrusive.
study) as they were making their laundry detergent
Later in the interview, subjects were presented with
purchase. 3 The observer appeared to be a store em-
a list of 10 possible choice tactics from which they
ployee who was taking inventory. Since the observer
were asked to select the one that best represented the
stood behind and slightly to the side of the shopper
way their decision had been made that day. The
as the choice was being made, consumers appeared to
selection of these tactics was based on data acquired
be unaware that they were being observed while they
in an earlier study. Multiple measures of the choice
were searching. The very large size of the laundry
tactic variable were included to provide greater con-
detergent display greatly facilitated the recording of
information search behavior, since fairly substantial fidence in reported findings. Thus the purpose of such
movement was required to examine different brands. closed questions was to provide an additional "check"
on the type of tactic employed.
An observation chart was developed which mirrored
Other variables of interest were product class ex-
the laundry detergent display in each store and which
perience, brand loyalty, perceived influence of in-store
was used to record each subject's behavior as s/he
factors, perceived influence of out-of-store factors and
made a brand selection. A coding system was also
developed which allowed the interviewer to record the demographics. Product class experience was mea~ured
search process easily. When a package was examined with two questions. Subjects were asked how long
a line was drawn next to that brand and size packag~ they had been purchasing laundry detergent and how
on the chart. If the package was picked up, a circle many brands they had purchased. Brand experience
was drawn around the line. An "X" was made through was also measured by asking how long the subject had
the circle for the package selected for purchase. The been purchasing the brand s/he had selected on the
amount of time (in seconds) spent examining each day of the interview. Both of these variables were
package was placed next to the other markings. If a measured on seven-point scales ranging from I (0-3
shelf tag was examined, a circle was made along the years) to 7 (over 40 years). Both the repeated purchase
line representing the shelf at the point where the tag and the affective aspects of brand loyalty were mea-
was located. sured by asking subjects to rate their frequency of
The major dependent variables of interest included purchasing the chosen brand (on a six-point scale
the number of packages examined, the total amount ranging from always purchased to never purchased)
of time taken, the time taken for the chosen brand, and the strength of preference for that brand (on a
the number of cross-brand comparisons (i.e., exam- five-point scale ranging from very strong preference
ining different brands), the number of within-brand to very weak preference).
comparisons (i.e., examining different sizes within The perceived influence of in-store situational factors
brands), and the number of shelf tags examined. The was assessed on several three-point scales that ranged
latter four measures represent rough approximations from a lot of attention to no attention. Subjects were
given the difficulty of acquiring detailed in-store ob~ asked how much attention they paid to sale signs,
servations of in-store brand choice behaviors. shelf tags, end-of-aisle and point-of-purchase displays,
and package information other than the price. In
Assessment of Choice Tactics addition, as mentioned previously, attention to in-
store factors was coded as part of the observational
Upon completion of choice, consumers were ap- data.
proached by the experimenter and offered a 25-cent Two questions determined the influence of factors
outside the store. Influence of advertising on subjects'
. It should be mentioned that the observer was carefully trained
J laundry detergent decisions was measured on a three-
In the conducting of the experiment and also engaged in live point scale ranging from a lot of influence to no
"practice" observations before actual data were collected. influence. It is recognized that this measure is some-

Copyright © 2001 All Rights Reserved


826 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

TABLE 1
SUMMARY OF SEARCH DATA

Frequency distribution proportions

Variable Mean Median 0 2 3 4 5

Number of packages
examined 1.42 1.19 .00 .72 .18 .07 .02 .02
Number of packages
picked up 1.23 1.10 .00 .83 .13 .02 .01 .01
Number of across-brand
comparisons .45 .17 .74 .17 .01 .07 .02 .00
Number of within-brand
comparisons .07 .03 .95 .03 .02 .00 .00 .00
Number of shelf tags
examined .13 .06 .89 .08 .03 .00 .00 .00

Frequency distribution seconds

1-10 11-20 21-30 31-40 40

Time per brand 7.72 4.77 .86 .10 .03 .01 .00
Total time 13.16 8.49 .75 .10 .08 .01 .06

NOTE N = 120

what simplistic; however, the focus of the present store pre-purchase evaluation was almost nonexistent.
study was not a detailed examination of advertising This is further supported by the finding that extremely
influence. Subjects were also asked if they had a few across-brand and within-brand (i.e., size) compar-
coupon for the brand they had selected. isons were made (X = 0.45 and 0.07, respectively).
The final questions covered demographic variables Further, these means are as high as they are because
such as family size, education, and age. The interviewer of several outliers. Finally, only a very small number
also observed whether the subject was shopping alone of shelf tags was examined.
or with others, as well as the subject's sex and race. Consumers took an average of 13 seconds from the
The interviewer read each question to the subject. time they entered the aisle to complete their in-store
Subjects were given cards with the possible answers decision. Given that the laundry detergent display
to the multiple-choice questions in order to speed up spans an entire aisle, it takes consumers some amount
the interview process and to aid subjects in answering of time to arrive in the physical proximity of their
the questions. The interview generally took three to desired brand. When one considers that the time taken
four minutes to complete. to locate the brand is included in the overall time
estimate, it is obvious that the typical consumer is
RESULTS making an extremely quick decision with only a
minimal degree of cognitive effort in the store envi-
Information Search ronment. This finding is given further support by the
fact that a majority of the time taken (median = 4.77)
The first area of interest concerned the extent of was devoted to the chosen brand.
information search and the amount of time taken In summary, the data from the present study present
while making a laundry detergent choice. Table I clear evidence that a majority of consumers engage in
presents summary data for the observation-based very little pre-purchase deliberation when making a
search categorization. Consumers examined a very laundry detergent purchase. Given this, the next ques-
small number of packages (X = 1.42, median = 1.19). tion concerns the nature of this brief decision process.
According to the frequency distribution, 72 percent
of the consumers looked at only one package, and
only II percent looked at more than two. An even Consumer Usage of Choice Tactics
lower number of packages was actually picked up (X
= 1.23, median = 1.10): 83 percent of the consumers Immediately following their choices, consumers were
picked up only one package, and only 4 percent approached and asked how they had made their pur-
picked up more than two. chases. If consumers use simplified heuristics, this
Given that consumers must examine at least one should be reflected in their free responses to the open-
package in order to make a choice, it is clear that in- ended question.

Copyright © 2001 All Rights Reserved


COMMON PRODUCT DECISION MAKING 827

TABLE 2 (n = 20), and "because of the price" or "it is cheapest"


(n = 18). Further, many of the other responses are
RESPONSES TO OPEN-ENDED CHOICE TACTIC QUESTION
quite similar (e.g., "cleans better" and "cleaner whites"
Total number of
are similar to "it works well").
Response consumers Percentage Based on the judgments of two separate individuals,
responses were categorized into five major groups.
Price tactics 27 22.5 The two judges exhibited an agreement rate of 93
Cheapest 18 15.0 percent in the categorizations. Discrepancies were
Coupon 5 4.2
Use less so costs less 3 2.5 discussed and resolved by the experimenters. Based
On sale 0.8 on these categorizations, the following groups emerged:
price tactics (n = 27), affect or "I like it" tactics (n
Affect tactics 24 20.0
16.7 = 24), performance tactics (n = 34), normative tactics
Like it 20
Love it 4 3.3 (n = 13), and other tactics (n = 22). Thus it appears
that a large number of consumers based their decision
Performance tactics 34 28.3
Works well 14 11.7 on one of four major types of tactics. One group tends
Cleans clothes better 7 5.8 to purchase a brand based on price, another chooses
Cleaner whites 5 4.2 a brand that is perceived to perform well in fulfilling
Best brand 4 3.3 needs, another buys a brand simply because the brand
Other 4 3.3
"is liked," and still another bases its decision on the
Normative tactics 13 10.8 preferences of others.
Wife likes it 8 6.7 In addition to identifying the types of tactics em-
Mother bought 3 2.5
Girlfriend likes 1 0.8 ployed, this study sought to discover whether there
Husband likes 1 0.8 are meaningful differences between different types of
decision makers. For exploratory purposes only, a
Nonclassified 11 9.2
Multiple statements 11 9.2 discriminant analysis was performed to determine
whether other variables measured in the study could
Total 120 differentiate among the major categories of tactics.
NOTE 11 5ubtects gave multiple statements such as "works well and IS the Cheapest ..
Due to the sample size, only major demographic and
search variables were employed as predictors. Also,
while there are meaningful conceptual differences be-
In these responses, 91 percent of the 120 consumers tween affect and performance tactics, present data do
provided a simple, one-statement reason for their not permit a clear distinction between these two
choice. Of the 11 who did not, all provided a combi- groups (i.e., one could like the product because it
nation of two simple statements (e.g., it works well performs well). As a result, these two categories were
and has a low price). If one views these multiple combined. Thus differences between the price, affect/
statements as more refined or specific choice tactics, performance, and normative groups were examined.
even greater credence is given to the choice tactic Results indicate that a set of variables was able to
view. When compared with the closed question, the discriminate between the three groups (Wilk's A
degree of inter-question agreement was 72.5 percent. = 0.369, p < 0.0001). The results of group classification
However, most of the disagreement was due to the indicate that 81.6 percent of the cases were correctly
fact that only 10 tactics were listed for the closed classified. In addition, a jackknife procedure (Lach-
question and that, as a result, many of the idiosyncratic enbruch and Mickey 1968) which was employed to
responses were not included. This instrument was validate this finding resulted in a 73.5 percent correct
developed in the absence of knowledge about the classification. Both of these percentages are significantly
diversity of tactics that consumers would provide and better than chance (t = 7.41, P < 0.001 and t = 5.78,
is thus limited. Since it was unable to fully capture p < 0.001}.4
the richness of the choice process, only findings per- The seven variables responsible for discriminability
taining to the open-ended responses are reported. are presented in Table 3. To examine specific differ-
One alternative explanation for these results is that ences between groups, an examination of the univariate
consumers were unable or unmotivated to provide a tests was made. Differences between each specific
complete description of the choice process. However, group mean were tested by employing a Student
if one remembers that the typical choice time was
approximately 4-5 seconds, it is clear that consumers 4 As computed by the formula;
were not engaging in a more detailed in-store evalua-
tion process. P-1I"
t=--
Table 2 presents a summary of the answers to the S.
where
free response question. A variety of choice tactics
was employed. The most frequent responses were:
"because it works well" (n = 14), "because I like it"

Copyright © 2001 All Rights Reserved


828 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

TABLE 3 comparison, perceived attention to point-of-purchase


UNIVARIATE TESTS FOR DISCRIMINATING VARIABLES displays, packages, end-of-aisle displays, perceived ad
influence, coupon usage, family size, and age.
Means

Per-
DISCUSSION
formance- Nor- The data from the present study allow one important
Variable F Price Affect mative
conclusion: in the case ofa laundry detergent decision,
Sex (1 = F; 2 = M) 24.49" 1.19 1.07 1.77 consumers engage in remarkably little in-store decision
Brand loyalty (higher deliberation. This finding is consistent with the con-
scores = less clusion reached by Olshavsky and Granbois (1979)
loyalty) 19.92" 6.40 3.16 4.08 about other products.
Length of time This implies that when decision making occurs, a
purchasing laundry large portion of the process may occur outside the
detergent 3.54" 4.07 4.62 3.15 immediate in-store decision context (at least for de-
(1 = 1 year or less)
(7 = Over 40 years) tergents). In light of this, it is likely that a better
understanding of consumer behavior may be aided by
Perceived attention to further investigation of how consumers process infor-
sale signs 4.68 c 1.70 2.24 2.38
(1 = A lot of attention) mation continuously. In particular, research is needed
(3 = No attention) to determine how consumers come to apply different
types of choice tactics. For example, there are probably
Information search 4.46 c 1.52 1.76 1.92
(1 = Search) key reasons why some consumers employ price-related
(2 = No search) tactics while others base their decisions on a perfor-
mance judgment. This study suggests that a variety of
Examination of shelf
tags 2.94" .33 .09 .15
product-related experiences-such as product usage,
Length of time numerous exposures to advertisements, and influence
purchasing brand 3.22" 2.33 3.62 2.85 by others in a social environment (either by direct
pressure or by modeling)-may play an important
·p<0001.
• p < 005
role in the decision-making process .
'p < 0.01 It must be noted that the present study possesses
several limitations. First, it examines decision making
for only one product category. Before solid conclusions
Newman-Kuehls procedure. In particular, several spe- can be drawn, research is needed to make comparisons
cific differences are worth noting. not only with other repeatedly purchased products,
First, consumers employing price-related tactics dif- but also with important, less frequently purchased
fered in several aspects. They were less likely to be products. The latter comparison would serve as an
brand loyal and to have purchased a chosen brand for important means of differentiating the types of decision
a long period of time. Also, they were more likely to strategies employed in different choice situations.
engage in search, examine shelf tags, and state that Deshpande and Hoyer (1983) have already found that
they had paid greater attention to sale signs. Given consumers reported (from a retrospective question-
that search time was generally brief, it is likely that naire) using choice strategies which involved signifi-
these activities involved the search for price informa- cantly higher degrees of cognitive effort for a choice
tion, and hence that price-conscious consumers were that involved high risk (e.g., automobile) compared
no more likely to pay attention to information other to a choice that involved low risk (e.g., peanut butter).
than price than were other consumers. Second, this study examines decision making at
Affect performance tactic users are most strongly only one point in time. If decision making in many
characterized by a higher degree of brand loyalty and common choice contexts is truly of a continuous
longer experience in purchasing laundry detergent. nature, an attempt must be made to examine the
Also, this category displays a higher proportion of process over a repeated number of choice trials. We
females and a low level of attention to shelf tags. need information concerning the extent to which
Normative tactic users possessed the lowest level of choice tactic usage is either consistent or altered over
laundry detergent purchase experience and tended to time, as well as information on which factors may
show the lowest level of actual search (based on lead to the adoption of an alternative choice tactic.
observation), although they indicated paying greater The third limitation of this study is that it employed
attention to package information other than price. A only one observer. Future research of this nature
higher proportion of male consumers fall into this should use multiple observers in order to permit
category. reliability assessment.
The following variables did not discriminate between In summary, the present study has attempted to
groups: education, across-board and within-board extend work on consumer choice by demonstrating

Copyright © 2001 All Rights Reserved


COMMON PRODUCT DECISION MAKING 829

that consumers employ simple choice tactics when ___ (1967), "Attitude and Prediction of Behavior," in
making a low-involvement decision. This choice is Readings in Attitude Theory and Measurement, ed.
Martin Fishbein, New York: Wiley.
not necessarily habitual (Howard 1977), nor is it Hogarth, Robin M. (1981), "Beyond Discrete Biases: Func-
necessarily characterized by the total absence of any tional and Dysfunctional Aspects of Judgmental Heu-
pre-purchase deliberation (Olshavsky and Granbois ristics," Psychological Bulletin, 90 (2), 197-217.
1979); rather, this study suggests that choice is the Howard, John A. (1977), Consumer Behavior: Application
result of numerous experiences and evaluations which of Theory, New York: McGraw-Hill.
occur over a repeated number of trials (or purchases). Hupfer, Nancy T. and David M. Gardner (1971), "Differ-
Over these trials, consumers develop a set of simple ential Involvement with Products and Issues: An Ex-
choice tactics which permit a quick yet satisfactory ploratory Study," in Proceedings of the Second Annual
decision. While the present study does not provide Conference, ed. D. M. Gardner, College Park, MD:
full support for this notion, it represents an important Association for Consumer Research, 262-269.
Jacoby, Jacob (1977), "The Emerging Behavioral Process
first step in developing a more descriptive theory of
Technology in Consumer Decision Making Research,"
consumer choice. Research is now needed to test this in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 4, ed. William
view and to examine how situational and individual- D. Perreault, Chicago: Association for Consumer Re-
level variables interact in the usage of choice tactics search, 263-265.
over time. - - - and David B. Kyner (1973), "Brand Loyalty vs.
Repeat Purchase Behavior," Journal of Marketing Re-
[Received June 1983. Revised July 1984.] search, 10 (February), 1-9.
Kassarjian, Harold H. (1978), "Presidential Address, 1977:
REFERENCES Anthropomorphism and Parsimony," in Advances in
Consumer Research, Vol. 5, ed. H. Keith Hunt, Ann
Bettman, James R. (1979), An Information Processing Theory Arbor: Association for Consumer Research, xiii-xiv.
of Consumer Choice, Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Lachenbruch, Peter A. and M. Ray Mickey (1968), "Esti-
- - - and C. Whan Park (1980), "Implications of a mation of Error Rates in Discriminant Analysis," Tech-
Constructive View of Choice for Analysis of Protocol nometrics, 10 (February), I-II.
Data: A Coding Scheme for Elements of Choice Pro- Lastovicka, John L. and David M. Gardner (1979), "Com-
cesses," in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. VII, ponents of Involvement," in Attitude Research Plays
ed. Jerry C. Olson, Ann Arbor, MI: Association for for High Stakes, eds. J. Maloney and B. Silverman,
Consumer Research, 148-153. Chicago: American Marketing Association, 53-73.
- - - and Michael Zins (1977), "Constructive Processes Olshavsky, Richard W. and Donald H. Granbois (1979),
in Consumer Choice," Journal of Consumer Research, "Consumer Decision Making-Fact or Fiction?" Jour-
4 (September), 75-85. nal of Consumer Research, 6 (September), 93-100.
Cook, Thomas D. and Donald T. Campbell (1979), Quasi- Rothschild, Michael L. (1979), "Advertising Strategies for
Experrmentation: Design and Analysis Issues for Field High and Low Involvement Situations," in Attitude
Settings, Chicago: Rand-MCNally. Research Plays for Higher Stakes, eds. J. Maloney and
Deshpande, Rohit and Wayne D. Hoyer (1983), "Consumer B. Silverman, Chicago: American Marketing Associa-
Decision Making: Strategies, Cognitive Effort and Per- tion, 74-93.
ceived Risk," in /983 AMA Educators' Proceedings, Russo, Edward J. and Larry D. Rosen (1975), "An Eye
eds. P. E. Murphy et aI., Chicago: American Marketing Fixation Analysis of Multialternative Choice," Memory
Association, 88-91. & Cognition, 3 (May), 267-276.
- - - , Wayne D. Hoyer, and Scot Jeffries (1982), "Low Ryan, Michael J. and E. H. Bonfield (1975), "The Fishbein
Involvement Decision Processes: The Importance of Extended Model and Consumer Behavior," Journal of
Choice Tactics," in Marketing Theory: Philosophy of Consumer Research, 2 (September), 118-136.
Science Perspectives, eds. R. F. Bush and S. D. Hunt, Wells, William D. and Leonard A. Lo Scuito (1966), "Direct
Chicago: American Marketing Association, 155-158. Observation of Purchasing Behavior," Journal of Mar-
Einhorn, Hillel J. and Robin M. Hogarth (1981), "Behavioral keting Research, 3 (August), 227-233.
Decision Theory: Processes of Judgment and Choice," Wilkie, William L. and Edgar A. Pessemier (1973), "Issues
Annual Review of Psychology, 32, 53-58. in Marketing's Use of Multi-attribute Models," Journal
Engel, James F. and Roger D. Blackwell (1983), Consumer of Marketing Research, to (November), 428-441.
Behavior, 4th Edition, Chicago: Dryden Press. Wright, Peter L. (1975), "Consumer Choice Strategies: Sim-
Fishbein, Martin (1963), "An Investigation of the Relation- plifying vs. Optimizing," Journal of Marketing Research,
ships between Belief about an Object and the Attitude II (February), 60-67.
toward the Object," Human Relations, 16,233-240.

Co ri ht © 2001 All Ri hts Reserved

You might also like