2003psychaging Differencescgnon CG

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 18

Psychology and Aging Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

2003, Vol. 18, No. 2, 250 –267 0882-7974/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.18.2.250

Differences Between Caregivers and Noncaregivers in Psychological


Health and Physical Health: A Meta-Analysis

Martin Pinquart Silvia Sörensen


Friedrich Schiller University University of Rochester

Providing care for a frail older adult has been described as a stressful experience that may erode
psychological well-being and physical health of caregivers. In this meta-analysis, the authors integrated
findings from 84 articles on differences between caregivers and noncaregivers in perceived stress,
depression, general subjective well-being, physical health, and self-efficacy. The largest differences were
found with regard to depression (g ⫽ .58), stress (g ⫽ .55), self-efficacy (g ⫽ .54), and general subjective
well-being (g ⫽ ⫺.40). Differences in the levels of physical health in favor of noncaregivers were
statistically significant, but small (g ⫽ .18). However, larger differences were found between dementia
caregivers and noncaregivers than between heterogeneous samples of caregivers and noncaregivers.
Differences were also influenced by the quality of the study, relationship of caregiver to the care
recipient, gender, and mean age of caregivers.

In the United States, care services to the elderly are provided prevalence of poor psychological and physical health of caregiv-
primarily by informal helpers, such as spouses and adult children ers. For example, whereas Vitaliano, Russo, Scanlan, and Greeno
(Stone, Cafferata, & Sangl, 1987). Providing care to an elderly (1996) and Bodnar and Kiecolt-Glaser (1994) reported higher
relative often restricts the personal life, social life, and employ- levels of depression in caregivers than noncaregivers, Haley et al.
ment of the caregiver. For example, caregivers may have less time (1995) and Loomis and Booth (1995) did not find significant
to spend with friends, to fulfill other family obligations, or to differences. Similar contradictions are found for physical health
pursue leisure pursuits (e.g., Gilleard, Gilleard, Gledhill, & Whit- (e.g., Rose-Rego, Strauss, & Smyth, 1998, vs. Barnes & Patrick,
tick, 1984; Kosberg & Cairl, 1986; Zarit, Reever, & Bach- 2000; Haley et al., 1995).
Petersen, 1980). Furthermore, caregivers are often faced with There are both methodological and conceptual reasons for these
difficult caregiving tasks (e.g., Steinmetz, 1988) and behavior inconsistencies. First, there are sampling issues with caregiver
problems of demented care recipients, such as verbal and physical research. Many studies on caregiving are based on nonrepresenta-
aggression and confusion (Teri et al., 1992). Because the progres- tive samples that overrepresent distressed caregivers (Schulz et al.,
sion of the care receivers’ illnesses and care needs are difficult to 1995, 1997). Thus, caregivers might not experience significantly
foresee, care receivers face increased uncertainty (e.g., Poulshok & higher levels of distress than the general population, if they were
Deimling, 1984). sampled differently. On the other hand, Schulz et al. (1997) have
A substantial literature shows that providing care to an older also suggested that more representative studies may underestimate
family member is associated with increased psychological distress
caregiver stress because of how they define caregivers: In some
(e.g., Donaldson, Tarrier, & Burns, 1998; Schulz, O’Brien, Book-
studies a caregiver is defined as a person sharing the household
wala, & Fleissner, 1995). For example, up to 48% of dementia
with an impaired family member without clarifying whether the
caregivers have been identified as being at risk for psychiatric
respondent provides care or not.
symptomatology (Brodarty & Hadzi-Pavlovic, 1990; Draper, Pou-
Second, many studies have large random sampling errors due to
los, Cole, Poulos, & Ehrlich, 1992). However, many caregivers are
small sample sizes. If the magnitude of population effect sizes is
able to cope quite well with their role. They experience few
low to medium, then the effects may not be detectable in these
symptoms of distress and often report positive gain from the
experience (e.g., Kramer, 1997; Schulz et al., 1997). studies (e.g., Rosenthal, 1991).
The inconsistencies in studies comparing caregivers and non- Third, caregivers often derive benefits from the caregiving ex-
caregivers make it difficult to draw clear conclusions about the perience, such as increased closeness to the family member being
cared for, and satisfaction at fulfilling one’s duty (for overview,
Kramer, 1997). Thus, caregivers may not be disproportionately
stressed compared to noncaregivers, unless the caregiving situation
Martin Pinquart, Department of Developmental Psychology, Friedrich is unusually stressful and there are no positive aspects to their
Schiller University, Jena, Germany; Silvia Sörensen, University of Roch-
caregiving experience.
ester Medical Center.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Martin Fourth, caregivers to older adults with dementia may be con-
Pinquart, Department of Developmental Psychology, Friedrich Schiller siderably more distressed than caregivers to other older adults
University, Am Steiger 3 Haus 1, D-07743 Jena, Germany. E-mail: (e.g., Clipp & George, 1993). However, these groups are rarely
Martin.Pinquart@rz.uni-jena.de differentiated in studies of caregiving (e.g., Cattanach & Tebes,

250
DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CAREGIVERS 251

1991; Teel & Press, 1999; Wallsten, 2000), thus influencing ob- caregiving (e.g., Donaldson et al., 1998; Kinney, Stephens, Franks,
served differences between caregivers and noncaregivers. & Norris, 1995; Wallhagen, 1992–1993). In contrast, influences of
Finally, research on the protection of psychological well-being caregiving on depression, general well-being, and health may be
and of a positive self-concept in adulthood has shown a consider- more indirect and mediated through caregiver stress (e.g., Yates,
able resilience of the self (Brandtstädter & Greve, 1994). Thus, Tennstedt, & Chang, 2000). Second, depression, general subjective
many caregivers may cope well with their role without showing well-being, and physical health are less situation-specific than
negative effects on psychological or physical health (e.g., Garity, stress; they are influenced by many experiences beyond the care-
1997). giver role, such as other social roles, socioeconomic status, the
Given the inconsistent findings in the literature and the potential quality of social relations, health promoting habits, personality,
reasons why many caregivers may, in fact, not experience more and even genetic factors (e.g., Blazer, 1993; Hooker, Monahan,
psychological distress than noncaregivers, a systematic integration Bowman, Frazier, & Shifren, 1998; Hooker, Monahan, Shifren, &
of these findings would be helpful at this time. Meta-analysis is an Hutchinson, 1992; Huck & Armer, 1996; Pinquart, 1998).
ideal tool to do this.
In this article, we focus on three research questions. In our first Moderators of Differences Between Caregivers and
research question, we ask whether caregivers and noncaregivers Noncaregivers
differ in psychological and physical health, specifically in their In the third research question, we asked whether the size of
levels of perceived stress, depression, general subjective well- observed differences between caregivers and noncaregivers is
being, physical health, and self-efficacy. In our second research moderated by caregiver characteristics (the nature of the care
question, we ask which aspects of psychological health and phys- receiver’s illness, the relationship to the care receiver, caregiver’s
ical health evidence the largest differences between caregivers and age and gender), and sample characteristics (e.g., representative-
noncaregivers. In the third research question, we analyze whether ness of the sample). Other aspects of the caregiving situation, such
observed differences between caregivers and noncaregivers are as the amount and duration of care provision, could not be included
influenced by moderator variables, such as caring for demented in the meta-analysis because of insufficient data.
older adults versus caring for physically impaired older adults. Caregiver characteristics. Caring for demented older adults
may be more stressful than caring for physically frail older adults,
Hypotheses because of five problems specific to dementia caregiving: (a)
dementia-related behavioral problems, disorientation, and shifts in
Differences Between Caregivers and Noncaregivers personality; (b) the increased need for supervision and the associ-
ated lack of spare time; (c) the isolation of the caregiver due to the
To compare caregivers to noncaregivers, we focus on variables care receiver’s behavior problems; (d) the limited ability of care
that can be meaningfully assessed in both groups, namely, on receivers to express gratitude and the associated reduction in
perceived general symptoms of stress not specific to caregiving, uplifts of caregiving; and (e) the progressive deterioration of the
depression, general subjective well-being, physical health, and care receiver, which reduces or eliminates visible positive long-
self-efficacy, but not stress measures unique to caregivers, such as term effects of caregivers’ engagement (e.g., Clipp & George,
caregiver burden, perceived economic costs, or social costs. 1993; Ory et al., 2000). On the basis of these considerations, we
Difficult caregiving tasks (e.g., Steinmetz, 1988), restrictions in expected larger differences in psychological and physical health
personal life (e.g., Kosberg & Cairl, 1986), and illness-specific between caregivers and noncaregivers for studies with dementia
problems of the care receiver (e.g., dementia care recipients’ caregivers than for studies of caregivers for nondemented older
behavior disturbances; Teri et al., 1992) may impair caregivers’ adults and studies that include both caregivers for demented el-
psychological and physical health. However, this negative effect derly and other caregivers.
may be reduced by positive aspects of caregiving (Kramer, 1997), Also, we expected larger differences between caregivers and
psychological resilience, and effective coping (Garity, 1997). Nev- noncaregivers in psychological and physical health for spouses
ertheless, not all caregivers may have the necessary skills to cope than for other relatives (e.g., adult children), for women than for
well with the caregiving demands (Lutzky & Knight, 1994), and men, and for older samples as compared with younger samples.
very severe stressors may exceed individual coping resources and Spousal caregivers were expected to show higher levels of distress
make it difficult to find positive aspects of providing support (e.g., than other caregivers because (a) they are more likely to have
Faßmann & Grillenberger, 1996; Ory, Yee, Tennstedt, & Schulz, age-associated illnesses and disabilities (Schneider, Murray, Ban-
2000). On the basis of these considerations, we expected with erjee, & Mann, 1999); (b) they provide up to four times the amount
regard to our first research question that caregivers would have, on of care provided by nonspousal family caregivers (Tennstedt,
average, significantly lower levels of psychological and physical McKinlay, & Sullivan, 1988); and (c) they are more likely than
health than matched controls, but that the effect sizes would be other caregivers to report a lack of alternative roles and social
small to medium.1 activities outside the home, which might function as buffers
In the second research question, we investigated which variables against caregiver stress (Barber & Pasley, 1994).
show the largest differences between caregivers and noncaregiv- We expected women to evidence greater psychological distress
ers. We expected larger differences between caregivers and non- than men because women (a) provide more caregiving assistance
caregivers for stress and self-efficacy than for depression, general
subjective well-being, and physical health for two reasons. First,
caregiver stress, difficult-to-manage situations, and loss of control 1
According to J. Cohen (1992), effect sizes of g ⫽ .20 are interpreted as
over one’s life have been described as direct negative effects of small, g ⫽ .50 as medium, and g ⫽ .80 and above as large.
252 PINQUART AND SÖRENSEN

in general and personal care tasks in particular; (b) are more likely trols, and for studies that were published in peer-
to assume the primary caregiver role, whereas men often become reviewed journals.
secondary caregivers; (c) are less likely to obtain informal and
formal assistance with caregiving; (d) are more likely to experi-
Method
ence social pressure to become caregivers, whereas male caregiv-
ers are more likely to feel that they have chosen to assume the Sample
caregiving role; (e) are more likely to stay in the caregiver role
even if it becomes very stressful; and (f) tend to have fewer coping Studies were identified from the developmental and gerontological lit-
resources, such as internal locus of control (e.g., Miller & Cafasso, erature through electronic databases {e.g., PsycINFO, Medline, Psyndex;
1992; Yee & Schulz, 2000). search terms: [(caregiving, caregiver, carer, or support provider) and non-
Psychological and physical health is more likely to be compro- caregivers and (elderly or old age)]}, browsing through library shelves, and
looking up references found in other articles. Criteria for inclusion of
mised for caregivers than noncaregivers in older samples: Com-
studies in the meta-analysis were as follows:
pared with younger adults, older adults (a) have lower levels of
1. A sample of informal caregivers of older adults is compared to a
psychological, physical, and financial resources (e.g., Baltes & sample of noncaregivers with regard to perceived stress, depression, gen-
Mayer, 1999); (b) have fewer stress-buffering roles and activities eral subjective well-being, physical health, or self-efficacy.2
due to age-associated losses of social roles (e.g., Barber & Pasley, 2. Differences between caregivers and noncaregivers can be converted to
1994); and (c) may be less likely to use formal support (Hooker et standard deviation units.
al., 1998), for example, because of a lack of knowledge about 3. The studies are written in English, French, German, or Russian.
available sources. Ninety-seven studies met these criteria, but about 15% of the total
Quality of the study. We expected that greater differences in number of publications surveyed had to be eliminated, for the most part
because they reported insufficient information about the magnitude of the
psychological health and physical health between caregivers and
relationship (zero-order effect sizes) between variables. After exclusion of
noncaregivers would be found in nonrepresentative samples than
such studies, we were able to include 84 studies in the meta-analysis. Most
in representative samples: Negative effects of caregiving may be of these were from English-language journals; only one German study and
overestimated for samples of highly distressed caregivers, such as one French-language study could be used. The majority of articles were
people who seek help (Schulz et al., 1995, 1997). In addition, from the Gerontologist (11); others were from Psychology and Aging (7),
population-based studies with representative samples may under- the Journal of Aging and Health (4), the Journal of Gerontology (4), the
estimate effects of caregiving because they do not directly assess Journal of Abnormal Psychology (4), Psychosomatic Medicine (4), Family
whether the respondent provides care, but rather they define care- Relations (2), the Journal of the American Geriatrics Society (2), and other
giving as the provision of a minimum level of support to an older journals and books (35). An additional 11 studies were taken from presen-
tations at conventions or dissertations. The studies were published or
adult, or even as merely living with an impaired family member
presented between 1987 and the summer of 2002. Additional information
(Schulz et al., 1997).
about the studies is provided in Figures 1 through 5 in the Appendix.
In addition, we expected the effect of caregiving to be larger in We entered the year of publication; the numbers of caregivers and
studies that do not ensure demographic equivalence of caregivers noncaregivers; the representativeness of the samples (representative ⫽ 1,
and controls: For example, women and individuals with lower nonrepresentative ⫽ 0); the sociodemographic equivalence of the caregiv-
socioeconomic status (SES) are more likely to become caregivers ers and noncaregivers (1 ⫽ equivalent/statistical control for nonequiva-
(Gage & Kinney, 1995; Haley, Levine, Brown, Berry, & Hughes, lence, 0 ⫽ equivalence not tested/no statistical control for differences
1987; Yee & Schulz, 2000) and to report poor psychological and between caregivers and noncaregivers); the quality of the source (1 ⫽
physical health, irrespective of caregiving (Pinquart, 1998). Fur- peer-reviewed journal, 0 ⫽ others); the impairment of the care receiver
(3 ⫽ dementia, 2 ⫽ combined sample of demented and nondemented care
thermore, we expected larger differences between caregivers and
receivers, 1 ⫽ nondemented care receivers, e.g., cancer); the measurement
noncaregivers in published, peer-reviewed studies than in unpub-
of stress, depression, subjective well-being, physical health, and self-
lished studies or chapters because nonsignificant results are less
likely to be published (the file-drawer problem; Rosenthal, 1991).
In sum, we expected 2
An alternative method might be to compute comparisons of health
measures from caregiver studies with health measures from age-matched
1. that caregivers would report higher levels of stress and noncaregiving community studies. Although this procedure would enable
depression, and lower levels of subjective well-being, us to include more studies in the present meta-analysis, it would have
physical health, and self-efficacy than noncaregivers; severe limitations: First, noncaregiving studies differ considerably in psy-
chological outcomes (e.g., average level of depression; Yee & Schulz,
2. that differences between caregivers and noncaregivers 2000). Thus, it would be difficult to decide which study might be the best
would be larger with regard to stress and self-efficacy empirical base for a comparison, even after controlling for differences in
than in the other measures; the distribution of age and gender of caregivers and noncaregivers. Second,
community samples may differ from caregiver samples in other character-
3. that larger differences between caregivers and noncare- istics that are related to psychological outcomes, such as SES. We would
not be able to statistically control for such differences in a meta-analysis
givers would emerge for studies with dementia caregivers
without having access to the complete data files. Third, some respondents
than with other caregivers, for spouses than for other from community samples may be caregivers thus biasing a comparison
relatives, for women than for men, and for older samples with caregiver studies. Therefore, we decided to focus our analysis on
than for younger samples, for nonrepresentative samples empirical studies that compared caregivers and noncaregivers and, in most
than for representative samples, for studies that do not cases, controlled for differences in sociodemographic variables between
ensure demographic equivalence of caregivers and con- caregivers and noncaregivers.
DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CAREGIVERS 253

efficacy; and the size of differences between caregivers and noncaregivers To test the influence of several moderators simultaneously, we used
with regard to these variables. weighted ordinary least squares regression analyses, following the ap-
proach outlined by Raudenbush (1994).
Measures
Perceived stress was most often assessed with the Perceived Stress Scale Results
(S. Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983; 6 studies), single-item indica-
Most of the studies included in this analysis were exclusively
tors (2 studies), and other scales (14 studies). Depression was most often
assessed with the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (Hamilton, 1967; 15 focused on caregivers of demented elderly (63.3%); other studies
studies), the Center for Epidemiological Studies—Depression Scale investigated other forms of illness (e.g., cancer, stroke; 4.7%) and
(CES–D; Radloff, 1977; 13 studies), the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck a mix of caregivers of older adults with physical and cognitive
& Steer, 1987; 11 studies), clinical interviews (4 studies), and other scales impairments (32%). Eighty-six percent of the studies tested for
(13 studies). Caregivers’ subjective well-being was most often assessed demographic differences between caregivers and control-group
with life-satisfaction scales (e.g., Life Satisfaction Index; Neugarten, Havi- members: In 68% of all studies, there were no significant differ-
ghurst, & Tobin, 1961; 11 studies), positive affect scales (e.g., Positive and ences or the authors controlled for these differences. Seven studies
Negative Affect Schedule; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988; 9 studies), did not control for emerging between-groups differences in SES
single-item indicators on happiness (5 studies), and other scales (9 studies).
(six studies reported lower SES in caregivers, and one study
Health was assessed with single-item indicators on subjective health (18
reported higher SES in caregivers) and age (age of caregivers was
studies), symptom checklists (17 studies), and items on the frequency of
medication use and the number of hospitalizations (5 studies). Self-efficacy higher in six studies and lower in one study). Because of the
was assessed with self-efficacy scales (e.g., Pearlin & Schooler, 1978; 4 limited number of available studies for the present meta-analysis,
studies), single-item indicators (2 studies), and sum-scales on perceived we decided not to exclude these studies from our analysis but
control over one’s life (3 studies). rather to statistically control for the impact of nonequivalence of
caregivers and controls.
Statistical Integration of the Findings The respondents (caregivers and controls) had a mean age
of 62.5 years (SD ⫽ 8.6 years). Almost 72% were women. Most of
Two common groups of statistical procedures are used in meta-analysis: the respondents were spouses (58.5%) and adult children (35.8%).
fixed- and random-effect models (e.g., Hedges & Vevea, 1998). If a
Seventy-eight percent had completed high school. About 14% of
common effect size is hypothesized for all studies, then fixed-effects
models are appropriate. If effect sizes vary systematically between studies
the respondents were members of ethnic minorities.
according to moderator variables (e.g., whether caregivers for demented Seventy-one percent of the caregivers shared the household with
and nondemented older adults are assessed), then random-effect models are the care receiver. About 47% of the caregivers were employed full
more appropriate because fixed-effect models can lead to inflated Type I time or part time. The caregivers have been providing care on
errors. Thus, the random-effect model was used, primarily based on pro- average for about 55 months (SD ⫽ 22) and spent on average 42.9
cedures outlined by Hedges and Vevea (1998) and Raudenbush (1994). hr per week (SD ⫽ 24.3) doing caregiving tasks. The care recip-
We computed effect sizes for each study as the difference in psycho- ients’ age was, on average, 75.6 years (SD ⫽ 4.4 years). About half
logical/physical health between caregivers and noncaregivers divided by (49.5%) of the care receivers were women.
the pooled standard deviation. Effect sizes were also derived from t values,
F values, and exact p values. In cases where the direction of differences
between caregivers and noncaregivers but no effect size was reported, we Effects of Caregiver Status
used vote counts to estimate the effect size, as suggested by Bushman and
Wang (1996). This procedure enabled us to include two additional studies First we tested the effect of caregiver status on psychological
in the present meta-analysis. If in one study effect sizes were reported for health and physical health. As hypothesized, caregivers had higher
more than one subsample (e.g., caregivers for cognitively impaired older levels of stress and depression as well as lower levels of subjective
adults and for physically impaired adults), then separate effect sizes were well-being, physical health, and self-efficacy than noncaregivers
computed for these subsamples rather than computing an average effect (Table 1). Caregiver status explained 0.8% (physical health)
size for the whole sample. The effect-size estimates were adjusted for to 7.8% (depression) of the variance of the dependent variables.
biases due to overestimation of the population effect size (common for The size of differences between caregivers and noncaregivers did
small samples), based on Hedges (1981). Confidence intervals that in-
not vary for self-rated depression versus clinician-rated depression,
cluded 95% of the effects were computed for each effect size.
The homogeneity of effect sizes was tested by using the homogeneity
or for subjective health versus objective health, as measured by
statistic Q, which is distributed approximately as chi-square with k – 1 dfs, symptom checklists and medication use.
where k is the number of effect sizes. In the case of heterogeneous effect
sizes, we estimated the between-studies variance of the effects, based on Which Outcomes Show the Largest Effects?
Hedges and Vevea (1998).
Studies were weighted by the reciprocal of the sum of the between- With regard to our second research question, we had expected
studies variance component and the random sampling error of the study. If larger differences between caregivers and noncaregivers for stress
more than one outcome was provided for a sample with regard to one group and self-efficacy than for depression, subjective well-being, and
of outcome measures (e.g., clinician-rated depression and self-rated de-
physical health. The results confirm our expectations only with
pression were assessed), then we weighted studies with more than one
outcome measure, as suggested by Rosenthal (1991).
regard to physical health. Differences between caregivers and
The significance of the mean effect size was tested by dividing the noncaregivers were significantly smaller for physical health than
weighted mean effect size by the estimation of the standard deviation. for the other outcome variables (see Table 1). The strongest
Differences between two conditions were interpreted as significant when negative effects of caregiving were observed for clinician-rated
the 95% confidence intervals did not overlap. depression.
254 PINQUART AND SÖRENSEN

Table 1
Differences in Psychological Health and Physical Health Between Caregivers and Noncaregivers

CI
Significance
Lower Upper of the mean
Variable k Ncg Nncg g limit limit QW difference

Stress 30 2,696 4,578 0.55 0.41 0.69 182.68*** 7.58***


Depression 81 6,237 14,383 0.58 0.46 0.70 482.59*** 9.16***
Self-rated 57 5,463 11,856 0.51 0.40 0.62 387.96*** 9.03***
Clinician rated 23 1,176 2,507 0.66 0.49 0.84 84.81*** 7.55***
Low SWB 48 4,263 17,063 0.40 0.32 0.48 177.67*** 9.14***
Poor health 66 6,716 24,597 0.18 0.13 0.23 133.78*** 6.93***
Subjective 25 2,981 12,157 0.20 0.12 0.28 65.99*** 4.43***
Objective 39 3,655 12,405 0.15 0.09 0.22 66.48** 4.79***
Low self-efficacy 14 898 1,092 0.54 0.31 0.77 58.88*** 4.68***

Note. To make the table easier to read, we coded all dependent variables in a way that positive coefficients
indicate larger impairments of caregivers compared with noncaregivers. k ⫽ number of effect sizes; Ncg ⫽
number of caregivers; Nncg ⫽ number of noncaregivers; g ⫽ mean difference (measured in standard deviation
units): Positive values indicate higher levels of psychological and physical distress of caregivers than non-
caregivers; CI ⫽ confidence interval of the mean effect size; QW ⫽ Significant values indicate heterogeneity of
the effect size; SWB ⫽ subjective well-being.
** p ⬍ .01. *** p ⬍ .001.

Moderators of Differences Between Caregivers and studies on dementia caregiving used nonrepresentative samples,
Noncaregivers whereas only 46% of comparisons of combined samples of de-
mentia caregivers and other caregivers with noncaregivers used
In the third research question, we explored whether differences nonrepresentative samples. Thus, we also tested whether the mod-
between caregivers and noncaregivers would vary by caregiver erator effects described above would remain significant after sta-
characteristics and study characteristics. We first compared studies tistically controlling for other moderators. Weighted ordinary least
with dementia caregivers, a mix of caregivers of demented and squares regression analyses were computed that included the ill-
nondemented older adults, and studies that focused on caregivers
ness of the care recipient (1 ⫽ dementia, 0 ⫽ nondementia/mix of
of physically frail elderly adults (e.g., cancer, Parkinson’s Dis-
demented and nondemented care receivers), the percentage of
ease). Unfortunately, only six studies were found for the latter
spousal caregivers, the percentage of female caregivers, the mean
group. Differences in favor of noncaregivers were significantly
age of the caregivers, the representativeness of the sample (1 ⫽
larger in studies with only dementia caregivers compared with
yes, 0 ⫽ no), the demographic equivalence of caregivers and
studies in which caregivers of demented and nondemented older
controls (1 ⫽ yes, 0 ⫽ no), and the quality of the source of
adults were combined for all outcomes except depression (Table
publication (1 ⫽ peer reviewed journal, 0 ⫽ others) as independent
2). However, although they were smaller, the differences between
variables. The dependent variables were stress, depression, sub-
caregivers and noncaregivers were still significant in the combined
group. Caregivers of nondemented older adults reported higher jective well-being, physical health, and self-efficacy. As shown in
levels of stress and lower levels of subjective well-being and Table 4, the four univariate effects for dementia caregiving were
physical health than noncaregivers, but there were no significant replicated in the multivariate analysis. In addition, we found a
differences in depression and self-efficacy. marginal effect indicating that the tendency of caregivers to be
We further analyzed whether the observed differences between more depressed than noncaregivers was more pronounced for
caregivers and noncaregivers would be larger in spouses than in dementia caregivers than for mixed caregivers.
adult children. Because only a small number of studies were Consistent with the univariate analysis, we found that differ-
available for homogeneous samples of spouses or adult children, ences between caregivers and noncaregivers were more pro-
our results have to be interpreted with caution. Differences in nounced the more spousal caregivers were in the sample. In
perceived stress and depression between caregivers and noncare- addition, we found significant effects of caregivers’ gender and
givers were significantly larger in samples consisting of spouses age: In samples with a higher percentage of female respondents,
than in samples of adult children (Table 3). We also found larger caregivers were more impaired than noncaregivers with regard to
differences between caregivers and noncaregivers in nonrepresen- all outcome variables except for stress. Furthermore, caregivers
tative samples than in representative samples with regard to all were more likely to report higher levels of depression and lower
variables except stress. However, outcome variables did not differ levels of self-efficacy in older than in younger samples. However,
for studies that used demographically equivalent samples of care- younger caregivers were more likely to report increased stress than
givers and noncaregivers versus those that did not (see Table 3). older caregivers.
The moderators may not be statistically independent. For exam- The quality of the study influenced the size of observed differ-
ple, 67% of caregivers for demented older adults were spouses, ences between caregivers and noncaregivers: Smaller differences
compared with 43% of caregivers from other studies; and 93% of in four out of five outcome variables were revealed in represen-
DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CAREGIVERS 255

Table 2
Moderating Effects of the Type of Illness of the Care Recipient on Differences in Psychological
and Physical Health of Caregivers and Noncaregivers

CI
Significance
Lower Upper of the mean
Variable k Ncg Nncg g limit limit Qw difference

Stress
Dementia CG 14 728 831 0.85*** 0.58 1.12 74.94*** Dementia CG ⬎ mixed CG
Mixed CG 13 1,802 3,630 0.32*** 0.16 0.48 69.51***
Nondementia CG 3 166 117 0.47*** 0.23 0.71 1.95
Depression
Dementia CG 60 2,933 3,874 0.65*** 0.56 0.74 159.66***
Mixed CG 18 3,160 10,383 0.23*** 0.19 0.27 688.36***
Nondementia CG 3 144 126 0.27 ⫺0.08 0.62 3.61
Low SWB
Dementia CG 25 1,645 2,469 0.55*** 0.43 0.67 70.77*** Dementia CG, nondementia
Mixed CG 20 2,422 14,456 0.20*** 0.11 0.29 49.35*** CG ⬎ mixed CG
Nondementia CG 3 196 138 0.56*** 0.33 0.78 0.86
Poor health
Dementia CG 34 2,249 2,238 0.26*** 0.19 0.34 46.21* Dementia CG ⬎ mixed CG
Mixed CG 28 4,212 22,174 0.08** 0.02 0.14 57.47***
Nondementia CG 4 255 185 0.31** 0.12 0.50 4.29
Low self-efficacy
Dementia CG 7 371 351 0.80*** 0.53 1.07 18.29* Dementia CG ⬎ mixed CG
Mixed CG 6 497 708 0.19* 0.03 0.35 6.25
Nondementia CG 1 30 33 0.45 ⫺0.05 0.95 —

Note. To make the table easier to read, we coded all dependent variables in a way that positive coefficients
indicate larger impairments of caregivers compared with noncaregivers. The dash indicates that the heterogeneity
of effect sizes could not be computed, because only one study was available. k ⫽ number of effect sizes; Ncg ⫽
number of caregivers; Nncg ⫽ number of noncaregivers; g ⫽ mean difference (measured in standard deviation
units): Positive values indicate higher levels of psychological and physical distress of caregivers than non-
caregivers; CI ⫽ confidence interval of the mean effect size; QW ⫽ Significant values indicate heterogeneity of
the effect size; CG ⫽ caregiver; SWB ⫽ subjective well-being.
* p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01. *** p ⬍ .001.

tative samples than in nonrepresentative samples. In addition, caregiving (Schulz et al., 1997), probably because of below-
studies that controlled for the demographic equivalence of care- average involvement in caregiving (e.g., Beery et al., 1997), care
givers and noncaregivers revealed smaller differences with regard providers’ receipt of stress-buffering support (e.g., Barusch &
to subjective well-being and physical health. Furthermore, we Spaid, 1989), positive aspects of caregiving (such as feeling use-
found larger differences between caregivers and noncaregivers in ful; e.g., Kramer, 1997), psychological resiliency (Garity, 1997),
studies that were published in peer-reviewed journals than in other or because caregivers—similar to other adults—may not be willing
studies with regard to stress, depression, subjective well-being and to admit negative feelings to the researcher (Diener, Sandvik, &
self-efficacy. However, the differences with regard to physical Larsen, 1985). Second, negative effects of caregiving may also
health were smaller (see Table 4). have been underestimated in comparative studies of caregivers and
noncaregivers because caregiving-specific stressors were not as-
Discussion sessed. Third, the size of the observed effects may have been
The present meta-analysis suggests that caregivers fare worse reduced because of large random sampling error due to small
than noncaregivers with respect to five indicators of psychological sample sizes and imperfect reliability of the measures. Because the
and physical health. Caregivers are more stressed, depressed, and reliability of measures was not reported in many studies, we were
have lower levels of subjective well-being, physical health, and not able to correct for this source of bias in our meta-analysis.
self-efficacy than noncaregivers. However, caregiver status by Differences between caregivers and noncaregivers were greater
itself explains less than 8% of the variance of the dependent for stress, self-efficacy, depression, and subjective well-being than
variables. Although caring for an impaired relative has been de- for physical health. This may be the case because stress indicates
scribed as a very stressful experience, the size of the differences immediate negative effects of caregiving (e.g., Donaldson et al.,
between caregivers and noncaregivers in stress, depression, sub- 1998; Kinney et al., 1995), and because it is difficult to maintain
jective well-being, physical health, and self-efficacy is medium to one’s sense of competence and confidence when caregiving tasks
small according to J. Cohen’s (1992) guidelines. There are three are difficult to manage and when one has little control over
possible explanations for this finding. First, a substantial propor- symptoms of the care receiver. The above-average effect of care-
tion of caregivers report very low levels of burden and other giving on depression may reflect symptoms of fatigue due to
negative effects of caregiving or even no aversive effects of chronic stress (Teel & Press, 1999), psychological responses to the
256

Table 3
Moderating Effects of Type of Relationship to the Care Recipient, Representativeness of the Sample on Differences Between Caregivers’ and Noncaregivers’ Psychological
and Physical Health, and Sociodemographic Equivalence of Caregivers and Noncaregivers

CI CI

Lower Upper Lower Upper Contrast


Variable k Ncg Nncg g limit limit QW k Ncg Nncg g limit limit QW effect

Spouses Adult children


Stress 8 654 823 1.05*** 0.60 1.50 78.31*** 8 830 1,449 0.23*** 0.13 0.33 7.53 Sign.
Depression 43 2,398 2,645 0.69*** 0.54 0.84 238.66*** 8 814 634 0.29*** 0.10 0.48 16.33* Sign.
Low SWB 22 1,375 2,127 0.43*** 0.29 0.57 61.45*** 4 499 940 0.16 ⫺0.03 0.35 9.28* —
Poor health 25 1,905 2,034 0.24*** 0.16 0.32 31.51 6 595 581 0.10 ⫺0.13 0.33 14.54* —
Low self-efficacy 7 519 398 0.69*** 0.40 0.98 25.66*** 4 239 140 0.24* 0.03 0.45 2.67 —

Representative samples Nonrepresentative samples


Stress 8 1,429 3,564 0.37*** 0.20 0.54 43.56*** 22 1,267 1,014 0.68*** 0.45 0.90 128.63*** —
Depression 12 2,563 9,888 0.26*** 0.03 0.49 209.86*** 69 3,674 4,495 0.64*** 0.56 0.72 182.06*** Sign.
Low SWB 11 1,789 13,787 0.14** 0.04 0.24 30.21*** 37 2,474 3,276 0.51*** 0.41 0.61 87.89*** Sign.
Poor health 20 3,852 21,759 0.09** 0.02 0.16 49.98*** 46 2,864 2,839 0.23*** 0.16 0.30 65.61* Sign.
Low self-efficacy 3 349 649 0.18 ⫺0.06 0.40 4.47 11 549 443 0.66*** 0.42 0.90 30.72*** Sign.
PINQUART AND SÖRENSEN

Equivalent samples Equivalence not tested/nonequivalent samples


Stress 16 1,405 2,426 0.64*** 0.43 0.85 108.08*** 14 1,291 2,152 0.47*** 0.27 0.67 71.32*** —
Depression 65 5,155 12,618 0.56*** 0.45 0.67 426.90*** 16 1,082 1,765 0.63*** 0.46 0.80 43.64*** —
Low SWB 32 2,913 14,489 0.36*** 0.28 0.44 118.22*** 16 1,350 2,574 0.43*** 0.27 0.59 55.13*** —
Poor health 47 5,368 22,937 0.14*** 0.09 0.20 97.15*** 19 1,348 1,660 0.24*** 0.13 0.34 34.74** —
Low self-efficacy 14 898 1,092 0.54*** 0.31 0.77 58.88*** 0

Note. Dashes indicate that there were no significant contrast effects. k ⫽ number of effect sizes; Ncg ⫽ number of caregivers; Nncg ⫽ number of noncaregivers; g ⫽ mean difference (measured in
standard deviation units): Positive values indicate higher levels of psychological and physical distress of caregivers than noncaregivers; CI ⫽ confidence interval of the effect size; QW ⫽ significant
values indicate heterogeneity of the effect size; Sign. ⫽ significant; SWB ⫽ subjective well-being.
* p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01. *** p ⬍ .001.
DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CAREGIVERS 257

Table 4
Predictors of Differences in Caregivers’ and Noncaregivers’ Psychological and Physical Health (Weighted Ordinary Least Squares
Regression Analysis)

Stress Depression Low SWB Poor physical health Low self-efficacy

Variable B ␤ B ␤ B ␤ B ␤ B ␤

Dementia CGsa 0.32*** 0.30 0.08 0.09† 0.11*** 0.17 0.15*** 0.33 0.18* 0.23
Spousal CGs (%) 0.01*** 0.55 0.00 0.11 ⫺0.00 ⫺0.02 0.002*** 0.27 0.01* 0.53
Female CGs (%) ⫺0.00 ⫺0.09 0.002* 0.11 0.002*** 0.14 0.002*** 0.19 0.003* 0.18
M age of CGs ⫺0.01* 0.30 0.01*** 0.26 0.00 0.09 0.00 0.02 0.07*** 1.05
Representative sampleb ⫺0.18* ⫺0.16 ⫺0.23*** ⫺0.23 ⫺0.27*** ⫺0.38 ⫺0.02 ⫺0.04 ⫺0.33*** ⫺0.37
Demographic equivalence of CGs
and controls ⫺0.03 ⫺0.03 ⫺0.05 ⫺0.05 ⫺0.09*** ⫺0.13 ⫺0.10*** ⫺0.20 — —
Quality of sourcec 0.22* 0.09 0.08* 0.09 0.24*** 0.28 ⫺0.06*** ⫺0.11 0.54*** 0.49
Constant 1.13* ⫺0.40 ⫺0.06 ⫺0.01 ⫺4.02**
R2 0.35 0.26 0.43 0.24 0.72

Note. Dependent variable is the difference between caregivers’ (CGs) and noncaregivers’ psychological and physical distress. Higher positive values
indicate higher distress of CGs than non-CGs. B ⫽ unstandardized regression coefficient; ␤ ⫽ standardized regression coefficient; R2 ⫽ explained variance
by the moderators; SWB ⫽ subjective well-being. Dashes indicate that no effect size could be computed for demographic equivalence, because this variable
did not vary between studies.
a
1 ⫽ yes, 0 ⫽ no/mix of CG. b 1 ⫽ yes, 0 ⫽ no. c 1 ⫽ peer-reviewed journal, 0 ⫽ other.
† p ⬍ .10. * p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01. *** p ⬍ .001.

lack of control over the caregiving demands (Miller, Campbell, spouse increases the risk of poor health, even if no care is provided
Farran, & Kaufman, 1995), and because in some studies depres- (e.g., Bookwala & Schulz, 1996). Future studies that compare
sion was measured by clinician ratings, which are less susceptible caregiving spouses and adult children should control for these
to dissimulation of symptoms than self-ratings (Eaton, Neufeld, factors.
Chen, & Cai, 2000). Differences between caregivers and noncaregivers were larger
in samples with a high percentage of female participants: Women
Effects of Sample Characteristics tend to provide more personal and instrumental care than men
(Miller & Cafasso, 1992; Yee & Schulz, 2000). In addition, they
Although caregivers were in worse psychological and physical are more likely to report impaired well-being than men because of
health than noncaregivers, these effects were moderated by sample their greater sensitivity to negative feelings, greater willingness to
characteristics. Differences between caregivers and noncaregivers report negative feelings, and less effective coping styles (Lutzky &
were considerably larger in dementia caregivers than in studies Knight, 1994).
that included a combination of caregivers for demented and non- We also identified moderating effects of age on differences
demented older adults. Probably because of the small number of between caregivers and noncaregivers, but these were somewhat
studies that compared nondementia caregivers to noncaregivers, inconsistent. Differences between caregivers and noncaregivers
we were not able to detect significant differences to studies on were larger for depression and self-efficacy in the older than in the
dementia caregiving. According to J. Cohen’s (1992) guidelines, younger samples, thus indicating that older adults have fewer
differences between dementia caregivers and noncaregivers for coping resources (e.g., Baltes & Mayer, 1999). However, differ-
stress and self-efficacy can be described as being large and differ- ences in stress were larger for younger than for older samples,
ences in depression and subjective well-being as being medium, probably because younger caregivers may have more competing
thus supporting the notion that caregiving for demented older roles to fulfill (e.g., work and family responsibilities), thus increas-
adults is especially stressful (e.g., Ory et al., 2000). ing the risk that caregiving demands exceed the individual re-
The difference in stress and depression between caregivers and sources (e.g., Moen, Robison, & Dempster-McClean, 1995).
noncaregivers was greater for spouses than for adult children.
Spousal caregivers have higher levels of objective burden than
Effects of the Quality of the Studies
adult child caregivers and fewer psychological and physical re-
sources to cope with stressors, due to age-associated losses and Although it has been suggested that differences between care-
declines. In fact, after statistically controlling for care receiver’s givers and noncaregivers may be larger in nonrepresentative sam-
illness and caregiver age in a multivariate analysis, the negative ples than in representative samples, previous qualitative reviews of
effect of caregiving was larger in samples with more spouses only caregiving research were not able to test for this difference (e.g.,
for stress, physical health, and self-efficacy. Note that greater Schulz et al., 1995). In the present meta-analysis, we were able to
impairments of spousal caregivers’ health compared with adult show that the tendency of caregivers to report lower levels of
children may not exclusively reflect above-average negative ef- psychological and physical health than noncaregivers is signifi-
fects of caregiving. Because married couples are likely to have cantly larger in nonrepresentative samples than in representative
similar risks for poor health outcomes as a result of selection, samples. Many caregiver studies recruit convenience samples
lifestyle factors, and access to health care, having a disabled through self-help organizations or service providers and are, there-
258 PINQUART AND SÖRENSEN

fore, likely to overrepresent distressed caregivers (see also Schulz study because other potential moderators cannot be controlled for
et al., 1995). In fact, in the subsample of studies that reported the and because only very few studies on nondementia caregivers were
average number of caregiving hours per week, we found higher available for making statistical inferences.
caregiving demands in nonrepresentative than in representative Despite these caveats, several conclusions can be drawn from
samples (36.7 hr/week vs. 26.8 hr/week), t(13720) ⫽ 27.96, p ⬍ the present study. First, although caregivers have higher levels of
.001. stress and depression and lower levels of subjective well-being,
Schulz et al. (1997) have also suggested that representative physical health, and self-efficacy than noncaregivers, most differ-
studies may underestimate caregiver stress because they define ences are small to medium, and the size of difference varies by
caregivers as individuals sharing a household with an impaired study characteristics. However, caring for demented relatives is
family member without assessing whether the respondent provides consistently associated with considerably higher stress levels and a
care. We ensured that this was not the case in our analysis by high risk for poor psychological and physical health. Therefore,
excluding studies on family members who shared the household caregivers for demented persons should be considered for psycho-
with ill older adults without providing support. social interventions.
After controlling for other moderators, we found smaller differ- Second, because the largest differences between caregivers and
ences between caregivers and noncaregivers in subjective well- noncaregivers were found with regard to depression, stress, and
being and physical health in studies that controlled for the demo- self-efficacy, the main points of an intervention should be to
graphic equivalence of caregivers and noncaregivers. This reduce the objective amount of care they need to provide and give
indicates that uncontrolled demographic differences between care- them more freedom in their time use (e.g., by providing flexible
givers and noncaregivers may inflate observed differences in psy- respite; Zarit, Stephens, Townsend, & Greene, 1998), to increase
chological and physical health. For example, in some studies caregivers’ ability to control the caregiving experience (e.g.,
caregivers were older or had lower SES than noncaregivers. How- increasing skills to manage behavior disturbances by means
ever, uncontrolled demographic differences did not exert a consis- of problem-solving and self-management therapy; Gallagher-
tent effect: In some studies there were stronger demographic risk Thompson et al., 2000), and to reduce negative thoughts that might
factors for low psychological and physical health in control-group contribute to stress and depression (e.g., Chang, 1999). A recent
members than in caregivers. These inconsistencies may have con-
meta-analysis by Sörensen, Pinquart, and Duberstein (2002)
tributed to a reduction in the difference between caregivers and
showed that psychotherapeutic interventions improved caregivers’
control-group members for depression and stress.
stress, depression, and self-efficacy by about one third of a stan-
For four out of five outcome variables, differences between
dard deviation, and that respite or adult day care had similar effects
caregivers and noncaregivers were larger in studies that were
on the stress reduction.
published in peer-reviewed journals. This can be interpreted in
Third, with regard to research methodology, we have concluded
terms of the file drawer problem: Significant results are more
that results from studies that use nonrepresentative samples of
likely to be published than nonsignificant results (Rosenthal,
caregivers have to be interpreted with caution because most of the
1991). The opposite was found with regard to physical health,
studies exclusively focus on highly distressed caregivers and are
probably based on some published large studies that found low
therefore likely to overestimate the negative effects of caregiving.
caregiving effects on physical health (e.g., Schulz et al., 1997).
Future epidemiological studies on physical and mental impairment
in old age may be used to get representative caregiver samples that
Limitations and Conclusions would enable less biased estimates of impact of caregiving. Sim-
A few caveats need to be considered regarding the present study. ilarly, studies that compare caregivers with noncaregivers must
First, we focused on five outcome variables related to stress, assess the equivalence of the samples with regard to important
well-being, and physical health. Other outcome variables, such as demographic variables, such as age and SES.
the level of anxiety, the level of social integration, or leisure time Fourth, because most studies report neither the levels of care
activities, should also be considered. Unfortunately, we did not receivers’ impairments in activities of daily living and instrumen-
have access to a sufficient number of studies that compared care- tal activities of daily living, nor the amount of care provision, we
givers and noncaregivers with regard to these variables. Second, were not able to include these variables in our meta-analysis. To
only a small number of studies that compared caregivers of phys- understand moderators on the impact of caregiving, we recom-
ically frail, nondemented older adults with noncaregivers were mend that future studies provide more descriptive information with
available. Thus, the results regarding this group have to be inter- regard to these and other potential moderator variables of caregiv-
preted with caution. Third, in the present meta-analysis we focused ing outcomes.
on only seven moderator variables. Other variables may influence Fifth, because we were only able to locate a very small number
observed psychological differences between caregivers and non- of studies that compared homogeneous samples of caregivers for
caregivers as well, for example, SES and the duration of caregiv- nondemented older adults to noncaregivers, we recommend more
ing. Because most of the studies reviewed here did not provide research in this field. Finally, we encourage research that compares
information on these variables, we were not able to include them. caregivers and noncaregivers with regard to other outcome vari-
Fourth, random-effect models may slightly overestimate the actual ables that could not be included in the present meta-analysis, such
between-studies variability of effect sizes and make the identifi- as future time perspective or social integration. These variables
cation of moderators more difficult (e.g., Hedges & Vevea, 1998). would add to the understanding of caregiver stress by further
Nevertheless, this more conservative approach is appropriate for probing the initial vulnerability of particular groups or types of
low information situations (Overton, 1998). Such is the case in our caregivers and enabling more tailored interventions.
DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CAREGIVERS 259

References standardized mean differences and vote counts to estimate the popula-
tion standardized mean difference in fixed effect models. Psychological
References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the Methods, 1, 66 – 80.
meta-analysis. *Cacioppo, J. T., Burleson, M. H., Poehlmann, K. M., Malarkey, W. B., et
*Acton, G. J. (2002). Health-promoting self-care in family caregivers. al. (2000). Autonomic and neuroendocrinic responses to mild psycho-
Western Journal of Nursing Research, 24(1), 73– 88. logical stressors: Effects of chronic stress on older women. Annals of
*Allen-Holmes, L. M., Markides, K., & Chiriboga, D. (2000). Physical Behavioral Medicine, 22, 140 –148.
health and service use utilization among elderly Hispanic caregivers *Carnwath, T. C., & Johnson, D. A. (1987). Psychiatric morbidity among
and non-caregivers. Paper presented at the 53rd annual scientific meet- spouses of patients with stroke. British Medical Journal, 294, 409 – 411.
ing of the Gerontological Society of America, Washington, DC. *Cattanach, L., & Tebes, J. K. (1991). The nature of elder impairment and
*Almberg, B., Jansson, W., Grafström, M., & Winblad, B. (1998). Differ- its impact on family caregivers’ health and psychosocial functioning.
ences between and within genders in caregiving strain: A comparison Gerontologist, 31, 246 –255.
between caregivers of demented and non-caregivers of non-demented Chang, B. L. (1999). Cognitive-behavioral intervention for homebound
elderly people. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 28, 849 – 858. caregivers of persons with dementia. Nursing Research, 48, 173–182.
Baltes, P. B., & Mayer, K. U. (Eds.). (1999). The Berlin Aging Study: Clipp, E. C., & George, L. K. (1993). Dementia and cancer: A comparison
Aging from 70 to 100. New York: Cambridge University Press. of spouse caregivers. Gerontologist, 33, 534 –541.
Barber, C. E., & Pasley, B. K. (1994). Family care of Alzheimer’s patients: Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159.
The role of gender and generational relationship on caregiver outcomes. Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of
Journal of Applied Gerontology, 14, 172–192. perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24, 385–396.
*Barnes, K. A., Hicks, P., & Kirk, M. (2001, November). Caregiving *Dautzenberg, M. G., Diedriks, J. P., Philipsen, H., & Tan, F. E. (1999).
among older husbands. Paper presented at the 54th annual meeting of Multigenerational caregiving and well-being: Distress in middle-aged
the Gerontological Society of America, Chicago. daughters providing assistance to elderly parents. Women and Health,
*Barnes, K. A., & Patrick, J. H. (2000). Effects of caregiving on older 29(4), 57–74.
husbands. Paper presented at the 53rd annual scientific meeting of the Diener, E., Sandvik, E., & Larsen, R. J. (1985). Age and sex differences for
Gerontological Society of America, Washington, DC. emotional intensity. Developmental Psychology, 21, 542–546.
Barusch, A. S., & Spaid, W. M. (1989). Gender differences in caregiving: Donaldson, C., Tarrier, N., & Burns, A. (1998). Determinants of carer
Why do wives report greater burden. Gerontologist, 29, 667– 676. stress in Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psy-
*Bauer, M. E., Vedhara, K., Perks, P., Wilcock, G. K., Lightman, S. L., & chiatry, 13, 248 –256.
Shanks, N. (2000). Chronic stress in caregivers of dementia patients is Draper, B. M., Poulos, C. J., Cole, A. D., Poulos, R. G., & Ehrlich, F.
associated with reduced lymphocyte sensitivity to glucocorticoids. Jour- (1992). A comparison of caregivers for elderly stroke and dementia
nal of Neuroimmunology, 103, 84 –92. victims. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 40, 896 –901.
*Baumgarten, M., Battista, R. N., Infante-Rivard, C., Hanley, J. A., *Dura, J. R., Haywood-Niler, E., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1990). Spousal
Becker, R., & Gauthier, S. (1992). The psychological and physical caregivers of persons with Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease
health of family members caring for an elderly person with dementia. dementia. Gerontologist, 30, 332–336.
Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 45, 61–70. *Dura, J. R., Stukenberg, K. W., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1990). Chronic
Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1987). Beck Depression Inventory—Manual. stress and depressive disorders in older adults. Journal of Abnormal
San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Psychology, 99, 284 –290.
Beery, L. C., Prigerson, H. G., Bierhals, A. J., et al. (1997). Traumatic
*Dura, J. R., Stukenberg, K. W., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1991). Anxiety
grief, depression and caregiving in elderly spouses of the terminal ill.
and depressive disorders in adult children caring for demented parents.
OMEGA, 35, 261–279.
Psychology and Aging, 6, 467– 473.
*Bertram, H. (1995). Das Individuum und seine Familie: Lebensformen,
Eaton, W., Neufeld, K., Chen, L. S., & Cai, G. (2000). A comparison of
Familienbeziehungen und Lebensereignisse im Erwachsenenalter [The
self-report and clinical diagnostic interviews for depression: Diagnostic
individual and his/her family: Living arrangements, family relations, and
Interview Schedule and Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuro-
life-events in adulthood]. Opladen, Germany: Leske & Budrich.
psychiatry in the Baltimore Epidemiologic Catchment Area follow-up.
Blazer, D. G. (1993). Depression in late life (2nd ed.). St. Louis, MO:
Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 217–222.
Mosby.
Faßmann, H., & Grillenberger, R. (1996). Burnout bei Pflegepersonen von
*Bodnar, J. C., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1994). Caregiver depression after
bereavement: Chronic stress isn’t over when it’s over. Psychology and Schwerpflegebedürftigen [Burnout in caregivers of the severely ill].
Aging, 9, 372–380. Report Psychologie, 21, 788 –798.
Bookwala, J., & Schulz, R. (1996). Spousal similarity in subjective well- *France, A. C., & Alpher, V. (1995). Structural analysis of social behavior
being: The cardiovascular health study. Psychology and Aging, 11, and perceptions of caregiving. Journal of Psychology, 129, 375–388.
582–590. *Fuller-Jonap, F., & Haley, W. W. (1995). Mental and physical health of
*Bougard, L. L. (1995). Existential growth resulting from giving care to a male caregivers of a spouse with Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Aging
spouse with Alzheimer’s disease. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, and Health, 7, 99 –118.
Arizona State University. *Gage, M. J., & Kinney, J. M. (1995). They aren’t for everyone: The
Brandtstädter, J., & Greve, W. (1994). The aging self: Stabilizing and impact of support group participation on caregivers’ well-being. Clinical
protective processes. Developmental Review, 14, 52– 80. Gerontologist, 16(2), 21–34.
Brodarty, H., & Hadzi-Pavlovic, D. (1990). Psychosocial effects on carers *Gallagher-Thompson, D., DalCanto, P. G., Jacob, T., & Thompson, L. W.
of living with persons with dementia. Australian and New Zealand (2001). A comparison of marital interaction patterns between couples in
Journal of Psychiatry, 24, 351–361. which the husband does or does not have Alzheimer’s disease. Journals
*Brody, E. M., Hoffman, C., Kleban, M. H., & Schoonover, C. B. (1989). of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sci-
Caregiving daughters and their local siblings: Perceptions, strains, and ences, 56, S140-S150.
interactions. Gerontologist, 29, 529 –538. Gallagher-Thompson, D., Lovett, S., Rose, J., McKibbin, C., Coon, D.,
Bushman, B. J., & Wang, M. C. (1996). A procedure for combining sample Futterman, A., & Thompson, L. W. (2000). Impact of psychoeducational
260 PINQUART AND SÖRENSEN

interventions on distressed family caregivers. Journal of Clinical Gero- pituitary-adrenal cortical function, and sympathetic tone. Annals of
psychology, 6, 91–110. Behavioral Medicine, 19, 83–90.
Garity, J. (1997). Stress, learning style, resilience factors, and ways of *Jutras, S., & Lavoie, J. -P. (1995). Living with an impaired elderly person:
coping in Alzheimer family caregivers. American Journal of Alzheimer’s The informal caregiver’s physical and mental health. Journal of Aging
Disease, 12, 171–178. and Health, 7, 46 –73.
*Gaynor, S. E. (1988). The effects of home care on elderly female care- *Karlin, N. J., & Retzlaff, P. D. (1995). Psychopathology in caregivers of
givers. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Rush University. the chronically ill: Personality and clinical syndromes. Hospice Journal,
Gilleard, C. J., Gilleard, K., Gledhill, K., & Whittick, J. (1984). Caring for 10(3), 55– 61.
the mentally infirm at home: A survey of the supporters. Journal of *Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Dura, J. R., Speicher, C. E., Trask, J., & Glaser, R.
Epidemiology and Community Health, 38, 319 –325. (1991). Spousal caregivers of dementia victims: Longitudinal changes in
*Glaser, R., Sheridan, J., Malarkey, W. B., MacCallum, R. C., & Kiecolt- immunity and health. Psychosomatic Medicine, 53, 345–362.
Glaser, J. K. (2000). Chronic stress modulates the immune response to *Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Glaser, R., Shuttleworth, E. C., Dyer, C. S.,
a pneumococcal pneumonia vaccine. Psychosomatic Medicine, 62, 804 – Ogrocki, P., & Speicher, C. E. (1987). Chronic stress and immunity in
807. family caregivers of Alzheimer’s disease victims. Psychosomatic Med-
*Golden-Kreutz, D. M. (1993). Social networks, support perception, and icine, 49, 523–535.
depressive symptomatology in dementia family caregivers and noncar- *King, A., Oka, R. A., & Young, D. R. (1994). Ambulatory blood pressure
egivers. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Ohio State University. and heart rate responses to the stress of work and caregiving in older
*Grafström, M., Fratiglioni, L., Sandman, P. -O., & Winblad, B. (1992). women. Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and
Health and social consequences for relatives of demented and non- Medical Sciences, 49, M239-M245.
demented elderly: A population-based study. Journal of Clinical Epide- *Kinney, J. M., & Stephens, M. A. (1989). Hassles and uplifts of giving
miology, 45, 861– 870. care to a family member with dementia. Psychology and Aging, 4,
*Grant, I., Adler, K. A., Patterson, T. L., Dimsdale, J. E., Ziegler, M. G., 402– 408.
& Irwin, M. R. (2002). Health consequences of Alzheimer’s caregiving Kinney, J. M., Stephens, M. P., Franks, M. M., & Norris, V. K. (1995).
transitions: Effects of placement and bereavement. Psychosomatic Med- Stress and satisfaction of family caregivers to older stroke patients.
icine, 64, 477– 486. Journal of Applied Gerontology, 14, 3–21.
*Haley, W. E., Levine, E. G., Brown, S. L., Berry, J. W., & Hughes, G. H. Kosberg, J. I., & Cairl, R. E. (1986). The Cost of Care Index: A case
(1987). Psychological, social, and health consequences of caring for a management tool for screening informal care providers. Gerontolo-
relative with senile dementia. Journal of the American Geriatrics Soci- gist, 26, 273–285.
ety, 35, 405– 411. Kramer, B. J. (1997). Gain in the caregiving experience: Where are we?
*Haley, W. E., West, C. A., Wadley, V. G., Ford, G. R., White, F. A., What next? Gerontologist, 37, 218 –232.
Barrett, J. J., et al. (1995). Psychosocial, social, and health impact of *Kramer, B. J., & Lambert, J. D. (1999). Caregiving as a life course
caregiving: A comparison of Black and White dementia family caregiv- transition among older husbands: A prospective study. Gerontolo-
ers and noncaregivers. Psychology and Aging, 10, 540 –552. gist, 39, 658 – 667.
Hamilton, M. (1967). Development of a rating scale for primary depressive *Lawton, M. P., Moss, M., Hoffman, C., & Perkinson, M. (2000). Two
illness. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 6, 278 –296. transitions in daughters’ caregiving careers. Gerontologist, 40, 437– 448.
Hedges, L. V. (1981). Distribution theory for Glass’s estimator of effect *Lee, J. A. (1997). Balancing elder care responsibilities and work: Two
size and related estimators. Journal of Educational Statistics, 6, 107– empirical studies. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 2, 220 –
128. 228.
Hedges, L. V., & Vevea, J. L. (1998). Fixed- and random-effects models in *Lee, J. A., Walker, M., & Shoup, R. (2001). Balancing elder care
meta-analysis. Psychological Methods, 3, 486 –504. responsibilities and work: The impact on emotional health. Journal of
Hooker, K., Monahan, D. J., Bowman, S. R., Frazier, L. D., & Shifren, K. Business and Psychology, 16, 277–289.
(1998). Personality counts a lot: Predictors of mental and physical health *Levesque, L., Cossette, S., & Ducharme, F. (1993). La santé mentale de
of spouse caregivers in two disease groups. Journals of Gerontology. conjoints âgés Quebécois: Comparaison entre aidants naturels et non
Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 53, P73-P85. aidants [The mental health of elderly spouses in Quebec: A comparison
Hooker, K., Monahan, D., Shifren, K., & Hutchinson, C. (1992). Mental of caregivers and noncaregivers]. Canadian Journal of Community Men-
and physical health of spouse caregivers: The role of personality. Psy- tal Health, 12, 111–125.
chology and Aging, 7, 367–375. *Lichtenberg, P. A., Manning, C. A., & Turkheimer, E. (1992). Memory
*Hoyert, D. L., & Seltzer, M. M. (1992). Factors related to the well-being dysfunction in depressed spousal caregivers. Clinical Gerontologist, 12,
and life activities of family caregivers. Family Relations, 41, 74 – 81. 77– 80.
*Hubley, A. M., Hemmingway, D., Michalos, A. C., & Zumbo, B. D. *Loomis, L. S., & Booth, A. (1995). Multigenerational caregiving and
(2001). Quality of life and health of caregivers and non-caregivers who well-being: The myth of the beleaguered sandwich generation. Journal
are 65 years or older. Paper presented at the 17th world congress of the of Family Issues, 16, 131–148.
International Association of Gerontology, Vancouver, British Columbia, *Lorensini, S., & Bates, G. W. (1997). The health, psychological and social
Canada. consequences of caring for a person with dementia. Australian Journal
Huck, D. M., & Armer, J. M. (1996). Health perceptions and health- on Aging, 16, 198 –202.
promoting behaviors among elderly Catholic nuns. Family and Commu- *Lutgendorf, S. K., Garand, L., Buckwalter, K. C., Reimer, T. T., Hong,
nity Health, 18, 81–91. S.-Y., & Lubaroff, D. M. (1999). Life stress, mood disturbance, and
*Irwin, M., Brown, M., Patterson, T., Hauger, R., Mascovich, A., & Grant, elevated Interleukin-6 in healthy older women. Journals of Gerontology.
I. (1991). Neuropeptide Y and natural killer cell activity: Findings in Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 54, M434-M439.
depression and Alzheimer caregiver stress. FASEB Journal, 5, 3100 – Lutzky, S. M., & Knight, B. G. (1994). Explaining gender differences in
3107. caregiver distress: The roles of emotional attentiveness and coping
*Irwin, M., Hauger, R., Patterson, T. L., Semple, S., Ziegler, M., & Grant, styles. Psychology and Aging, 9, 513–519.
I. (1997). Alzheimer caregiver stress: Basal natural killer cell activity, *McGuire, L., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Glaser, R. (2002). Depressive
DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CAREGIVERS 261

symptoms and lymphocyte proliferation in older adults. Journal of *Robinson, K. M., & Kaye, J. (1994). The relationship between spiritual
Abnormal Psychology, 111, 192–197. perspective, social support, and depression in caregiving and noncare-
*McMillan, S. C., & Mahon, M. (1994). The impact of hospice services on giving wives. Scholary Inquiry for Nursing Practice, 8, 375–389.
the quality of life of primary caregivers. Oncology Nursing Forum, 21, *Robinson-Whelen, S., Kim, C., McCallum, R. C., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K.
1189 –1195. (1997). Distinguishing optimism from pessimism in older adults: Is it
Miller, B., & Cafasso, L. (1992). Gender differences in caregiving: Fact or more important to be optimistic or not to be pessimistic? Journal of
artifact? Gerontologist, 32, 498 –507. Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1345–1353.
Miller, B., Campbell, R. T., Farran, C. J., & Kaufman, J. E. (1995). Race, *Robinson-Whelen, S., Tada, Y., McCallum, R. C., McGuire, L., &
control, mastery, and caregiver distress. Journals of Gerontology. Series Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (2001). Long-term caregiving: What happens when
B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 50, S374-S382. it ends? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 573–584.
Moen, P., Robison, J., & Dempster-McClean, D. (1995). Caregiving and *Rose-Rego, S. K., Strauss, M. E., & Smyth, K. A. (1998). Differences in
women’s well-being: A life course approach. Journal of Health and the perceived well-being of wives and husbands caring for persons with
Social Behavior, 36, 259 –273. Alzheimer’s disease. Gerontologist, 38, 224 –230.
*Morris, D. L., Wykle, M. L., & Lu, J. (2001, November). A comparison Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research. Bev-
of self-care practices on the health of elderly caregivers and non- erly Hills, CA: Sage.
caregivers. Paper presented at the 54th annual meeting of the Geronto- *Roth, D. L., Haley, W. E., Owen, J. E., Clay, O. J., & Goode, K. T.
logical Society of America, Chicago. (2001). Latent growth models of the longitudinal effects of dementia
*Mort, J. R., Gaspar, P. M., Juffer, D. I., & Kovarna, M. B. (1996). caregiving: A comparison of African American and White family care-
Comparison of psychotropic agent use among rural elderly caregivers givers. Psychology and Aging, 16, 427– 436.
and noncaregivers. Annals of Pharmacotherapy, 30, 583–585. *Russo, J., Vitaliano, P. P., Brewer, D. D., Katon, W., & Becker, J. (1995).
Neugarten, B. L., Havighurst, R. J., & Tobin, S. S. (1961). The measure- Psychiatric disorders in spouse caregivers of care recipients with Alz-
ment of life satisfaction. Journal of Gerontology, 16, 134 –143. heimer’s disease and matched controls: A diathesis-stress model of
Ory, M. G., Yee, J. L., Tennstedt, S. L., & Schulz, R. (2000). The extent psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 197–204.
and impact of dementia care: Unique challenges experienced by family *Scanlan, J. M., Vitaliano, P. P., Zhang, J., Savage, M., & Ochs, H. D.
caregivers. In R. Schulz (Ed.), Handbook of dementia caregiving: (2001). Lymphocyte proliferation is associated with gender, caregiving,
Evidence-based interventions for family caregivers (pp. 1–32). New
and psychosocial variables in older adults. Journal of Behavioral Med-
York: Springer.
icine, 24, 537–559.
Overton, R. C. (1998). A comparison of fixed-effects and mixed (random-
*Scharlach, A., & Runkle, C. (1994). Health conditions and service utili-
effects) models for meta-analysis: Tests for moderator variable effects.
zation of adults with elder care responsibilities. Journal of Aging and
Psychological Methods, 3, 354 –379.
Health, 6, 336 –352.
*Owens, S. J., & Swensen, C. H. (2000, August). Loneliness in older adult
Schneider, J., Murray, J., Banerjee, S., & Mann, A. (1999). EUROCARE:
spousal caregivers. Paper presented at the 108th Annual Convention of
A cross-national study of co-resident spouse carers for people with
the American Psychological Association, Washington, DC.
Alzheimer’s disease: I. Factors associated with carer burden. Interna-
Pearlin, L., & Schooler, C. (1978). The structure of coping. Journal of
tional Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 14, 651– 661.
Health & Social Behavior, 19, 2–21.
*Schulz, R., Beach, S. R., Lind, B., Martire, L. M., Zdaniuk, B., Hirsch, C.,
*Perez-Cahill, D. (1998). The impact of multiple caregiving roles on
et al. (2001). Involvement in caregiving and adjustment to death of a
well-being: A longitudinal study of middle-aged adults. Unpublished
spouse. Journal of the American Medical Association, 27, 3123–3129.
doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts.
*Schulz, R., Newsom, J., Mittelmark, M., Burton, L., Hirsch, C., &
*Picot, S. J., Zauszniewski, J. A., Debanne, S. M., & Holston, E. C. (1999).
Mood and blood pressure response in Black female caregivers and Jackson, S. (1997). Health effects of caregiving: The caregiver health
noncaregivers. Nursing Research, 48, 150 –161. effects study: An ancillary study of the Cardiovascular Health Study.
*Picot, S. J., Zauszniewski, J. A., & Delgado, C. (1997). Cardiovascular Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 19, 110 –116.
response of African American female caregivers. Journal of National Schulz, R., O’Brien, A. T., Bookwala, J., & Fleissner, K. (1995). Psychi-
Black Nurses Association, 9, 3–21. atric and physical morbidity effects of dementia caregiving: Prevalence,
Pinquart, M. (1998). Das Selbstkonzept im Seniorenalter [Self-concept in correlates, and causes. Gerontologist, 35, 771–791.
old age]. Weinheim, Germany: Beltz. *Seltzer, M. M., & Li, W. L. (2000). The dynamics of caregiving: Tran-
Poulshok, S. W., & Deimling, G. T. (1984). Families caring for elders in sitions during a three-year prospective study. Gerontologist, 40, 165–
residence: Issues in the measurement of burden. Journal of Gerontol- 178.
ogy, 39, 230 –239. *Shaw, W. S., Patterson, T. L., Semple, S. J., Grant, I., Yu, E. S., Zhang,
*Pruchno, R. A., & Potashnik, S. L. (1989). Caregiving spouses: Physical M., et al. (1997). A cross-cultural validation of coping strategies and
and mental health in perspective. Journal of the American Geriatrics their associations with caregiving distress. Gerontologist, 37, 490 –504.
Society, 37, 697–705. *Shaw, W. S., Patterson, T. L., Semple, S. J., Ho, S., Irwin, M. R., Hauger,
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for R. L., & Grant, I. (1997). Longitudinal analysis of multiple indicators of
research in the general population. Journal of Applied Psychological health decline among spousal caregivers. Annals of Behavioral Medi-
Measurement, 1, 385– 401. cine, 19, 101–109.
Raudenbush, S. W. (1994). Random effect models. In H. Cooper & L. V. *Sistler, A. B., & Blanchard-Fields, F. (1993). Being in control: A note on
Hedges (Eds.), Handbook of research synthesis (pp. 301–321). New differences between caregiving and noncaregiving spouses. Journal of
York: Sage. Psychology, 127, 537–542.
*Reese, D. R., Gross, A. M., Smalley, D. L., & Messer, S. C. (1994). Sörensen, S., Pinquart, M., & Duberstein, P. (2002). How effective are
Caregivers of Alzheimer’s disease and stroke patients: Immunological interventions with caregivers? An updated meta-analysis. Gerontolo-
and psychological considerations. Gerontologist, 34, 534 –540. gist, 42, 356 –372.
*Richardson, R. C., & Sistler, A. B. (1999). The well-being of elderly Steinmetz, S. K. (1988). Elder abuse by family caregivers: Processes and
Black caregivers and noncaregivers: A preliminary study. Journal of intervention stragegies. Contemporary Family Therapy: An Interna-
Gerontological Social Work, 31, 109 –117. tional Journal, 10, 256 –271.
262 PINQUART AND SÖRENSEN

Stone, R. G., Cafferata, L., & Sangl, J. (1987). Caregivers of the frail caregivers’ and noncaregivers’ perceptions of stress in daily experiences.
elderly: A national profile. Gerontologist, 27, 616 – 626. Journal of Community Psychology, 18, 228 –238.
*Strawbridge, W. J., Wallhagen, M. I., Shema, S. J., & Kaplan, G. A. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and vali-
(1997). New burdens or more of the same? Comparing grandparent, dation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS
spouse, and adult-child caregivers. Gerontologist, 37, 505–510. scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.
*Teel, C. S., & Press, A. N. (1999). Fatigue among elders in caregiving and *Wells, Y. D., & Kendig, H. L. (1997). Health and well-being of spouse
noncaregiving roles. Western Journal of Nursing Research, 21, 498 – caregivers and the widowed. Gerontologist, 37, 666 – 674.
520. *Wright, D. L., & Aquilino, W. S. (1998). Influence of emotional support
Tennstedt, S. L., McKinlay, J. B., & Sullivan, L. M. (1988, November). exchange in marriage on caregiving wives’ burden and marital satisfac-
Informal care for frail elders: The role of secondary characteristics. tion. Family Relations, 47, 195–204.
Paper presented at the annual scientific meeting of the Gerontological *Wright, L. K. (1994). AD spousal caregivers: Longitudinal changes in
Society of America, San Francisco. health, depression, and coping. Journal of Gerontological Nursing,
Teri, L., Truax, P., Logsdon, R., Uomoto, J., Zarit, S., & Vitaliano, P. P.
20(10), 33– 48.
(1992). Assessment of behavioral problems in dementia: The revised
Yates, M. E., Tennstedt, S., & Chang, B. -H. (2000). Contributors to and
Memory and Behavior Problems Checklist. Psychology and Aging, 7,
mediators of psychological well-being for informal caregivers. Journals
622– 631.
of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sci-
*Vitaliano, P. P., Russo, J., & Niaura, R. (1995). Plasma lipids and their
ences, 54, P12-P22.
relationship with psychological factors in older adults. Journals of
Yee, J. L., & Schulz, R. (2000). Gender differences in psychiatric morbid-
Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 50,
P18-P24. ity among family caregivers: A review and analysis. Gerontologist, 40,
*Vitaliano, P. P., Russo, J., Scanlan, J. M., & Greeno, C. G. (1996). Weight 147–164.
changes in caregivers of Alzheimer’s care recipients: Psychobehavioral *Yonan, C. A., Hebel, J. R., & Baumgarten, M. (2000, November).
predictors. Psychology and Aging, 11, 155–163. Depression in bereaved caregivers: A five-year prospective study. Paper
*Vitaliano, P. P., Russo, J., Young, H. M., Becker, J., & Maiuro, R. D. presented at the 53rd annual meeting of the Gerontological Society of
(1991). The screen for caregiver burden. Gerontologist, 31, 76 – 83. America, Washington, DC.
Wallhagen, M. I. (1992–1993). Perceived control and adaptation in elder Zarit, S. H., Reever, K. E., & Bach-Petersen, J. (1980). Relatives of the
caregivers: Development of an explanatory model. International Journal impaired elderly: Correlates of feelings of burden. Gerontologist, 20,
of Aging and Human Development, 36, 219 –237. 649 – 655.
*Wallsten, S. M. (2000). Effects of caregiving, gender, and race on the Zarit, S. H., Stephens, M. -A., Townsend, A., & Greene, R. (1998). Stress
health, mutuality, and social supports of older couples. Journal of Aging reduction for family caregivers: Effects of adult day care use. Journals
and Health, 12, 90 –111. of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sci-
*Wallsten, S. M., & Snyder, S. S. (1990). A comparison of elderly family ences, 53, S267-S277.

Appendix

Effect Sizes and 95% Confidence Intervals of Single Studies Ordered by the Size of the Effects

The articles by Vitaliano, Russo, and Niaura (1995) and Vitaliano et al. gstress/gdep/gswb/ghealth/gself-efficacy ⫽ differences in perceived stress/depres-
(1996) are, in part, based on the same data set, but they report results for sion/subjective well-being/health/self-efficacy between caregivers and
different outcome variables or points of measurement. The same applies to noncaregivers; C.I. ⫽ confidence interval. (a) dementia caregivers; (b)
Dura, Haywood-Niler, and Kiecolt-Glaser (1990); Dura, Stukenberg, and caregivers for frail elderly; (c) Parkinson caregivers; (d) caregivers for
Kiecolt-Glaser (1990); Kiecolt-Glaser et al. (1991); and Schulz et al. cancer patients; (e) clinician-rated depression (Hamilton Rating scale); (f)
(1997; Schulz et al., 2001), as well as Grant et al. (2002); Irwin et al. self-rated depression; (g) clinical interview-based diagnosis of depression;
(1991); Shaw, Patterson, Semple, Grant, et al. (1997); and Shaw, Patterson, (h) White caregivers; (i) Black caregivers; (j) life satisfaction; (k) positive
Semple, Ho, et al. (1997). Differences in the levels of stress between affect; (l) subjective health; (m) objective health; (n) male caregivers; (o)
caregivers and noncaregivers that were reported in different articles were female caregivers; (p) first time of measurement; (q) second time of
only coded once. If more than one measure of a dependent variable was measurement; (r) third time of measurement; (s) spouses; (t) adult children;
used (e.g., self-rated and clinician-rated depression), we report both effect (u) United States caregivers; (v) Chinese caregivers; (w) stroke caregivers;
sizes. Ncg ⫽ number of caregivers; Nnoncg ⫽ number of noncaregivers; (x) first study; (y) second study.
DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CAREGIVERS 263

Figure A1. Differences in stress between caregivers and noncaregivers (effect-sizes g, and 95% confidence
intervals).

(Appendix continues)
264 PINQUART AND SÖRENSEN

Figure A2. Differences in depression between caregivers and noncaregivers (effect-sizes g, and 95% confi-
dence intervals).
DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CAREGIVERS 265

Figure A3. Differences in subjective well-being between caregivers and noncaregivers (effect-sizes g, and 95%
confidence intervals).

(Appendix continues)
266 PINQUART AND SÖRENSEN

Figure A4. Differences in physical health between caregivers and noncaregivers (effect-sizes g, and 95%
confidence intervals).
DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CAREGIVERS 267

Figure A5. Differences in self-efficacy between caregivers and noncaregivers (effect-sizes g, and 95%
confidence intervals).

Received July 24, 2001


Revision received August 22, 2002
Accepted September 3, 2002 䡲

You might also like