Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

DOI: 10.

1093/brain/awh043 Brain (2004), 127, 330±339

The basal ganglia and inhibitory mechanisms in


response selection: evidence from subliminal
priming of motor responses in Parkinson's disease
Ellen Seiss1 and Peter Praamstra1,2
1Behavioural Brain Sciences Centre and 2Department of Correspondence to: P. Praamstra, Behavioural Brain
Clinical Neurology, Queen Elizabeth Hospital, University Sciences Centre, University of Birmingham, Birmingham
of Birmingham, Birmingham, UK B15 2TT, UK E- mail: p.praamstra@bham.ac.uk

Summary

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on September 24, 2015


Subliminal response priming was used to investigate sal of prime±target compatibility effects compared with
inhibitory control processes relevant to response selec- the ISI = 0 ms task. This reversal implied that there
tion impairments in Parkinson's disease. Using a back- was a so-called negative compatibility effect with faster
ward masking technique, covert activation of left- or responses and fewer errors when prime and target
right-hand responses was induced without subjects con- arrows pointed in opposite directions than when they
sciously perceiving the stimuli (right- or left-pointing required the same response. This negative compatibility
arrows). The masked priming stimuli were followed by effect turned into a positive compatibility effect in
visible arrow stimuli, instructing for a left- or right- Parkinson's disease patients, while age-matched controls
hand response, at a delay (interstimulus interval, ISI) of produced intermediate values. Together, these results
0 or 100 ms. Motor cortex activation was recorded by support the view that response selection involves com-
means of the electroencephalographic lateralized readi- petitive, mutually inhibitory interactions between
ness potential (LRP). Parkinson's disease patients (n = response alternatives, in¯uenced by basal ganglia±thala-
12) were compared with age-matched controls (n = 12) mocortical mechanisms. As indicated by the reduced
and young controls (n = 10). In young controls, the ISI inhibition of partially activated responses, Parkinson's
= 100 ms task effectively invoked inhibition of the sub- disease and, to a lesser degree, normal ageing affect the
liminally primed responses, as demonstrated by a rever- ef®ciency of these inhibitory interactions.

Keywords: basal ganglia; Parkinson's disease; subliminal priming; inhibition; lateralized readiness potential

Abbreviations: ERP = event-related potential; LRP = lateralized readiness potential; ISI = interstimulus interval;
RT = reaction time; TMS = transcranial magnetic stimulation; UPDRS = Uni®ed Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale
Received July 15, 2003. Revised September 15, 2003. Accepted September 18, 2003. Advanced Access publication November 25, 2003

Introduction
Effects of basal ganglia dysfunction are often conceived in relevant lines of evidence. First, transcranial magnetic
terms of an altered balance of facilitatory and inhibitory stimulation (TMS) studies have shown reduced motor
in¯uences on the frontal cortex (Alexander et al., 1990). In cortex inhibition in Parkinson's disease (Ridding et al.,
the motor domain, excess of movement in Huntington's 1995; Strafella et al., 2000). Secondly, functional imaging
disease and restriction of movement in Parkinson's disease studies have yielded evidence for overactivity of motor
are typically explained by disinhibition and overinhibition cortical areas in Parkinson's disease (Sabatini et al., 2000;
of thalamocortical projection neurons, respectively, result- Haslinger et al., 2001; Turner et al., 2003; but see
ing in overexcitable versus depressed neural activity in Buhmann et al., 2003). Finally, investigations in the MPTP
motor areas of the cortex (Albin et al., 1989; DeLong, (1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine)-lesioned
1990). However, on the basis of various sources of monkey indicate that motor cortex neuronal activity is
evidence that have emerged in recent years, this model characterized by abnormal ®ring patterns rather than a
may need revision, at least where Parkinson's disease is decrease in activity (Goldberg et al., 2002; see also Doudet
concerned. Goldberg et al. (2002) emphasized three et al., 1990; Watts and Mandir, 1992).

Brain Vol. 127 No. 2 ã Guarantors of Brain 2003; all rights reserved
Response selection in Parkinson's disease 331

prime±target compatibility turns into inhibition when a delay


is introduced between the presentation of prime and target.
They proposed that an initial covert response tendency,
induced by the prime stimulus, is checked automatically by a
self-inhibition process intrinsic to the motor system (Eimer,
1999; Schlaghecken and Eimer, 2002). Depending on the
interval between prime and target, the inhibition phase may
coincide with the preparation of the required response and
thus reverse normal priming effects, yielding a so-called
negative compatibility effect, both in reaction times and in
error rates. Importantly, the prime-induced motor activation
and the subsequent inhibition phase have distinct correlates in
movement-related LRP recordings (Eimer and Schlaghecken,
1998).
The combined behavioural and electrophysiological evalu-
ation of inhibitory motor processes elicited by subliminal

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on September 24, 2015


Fig. 1 Trial structure for the ISI = 0 ms task (A) and the ISI = 100 stimuli may provide more direct evidence for de®cient
ms task (B). inhibitory control processes in Parkinson's disease than has
been available hitherto from con¯ict tasks. Based on the
evidence from the latter type of task, discussed above, the
prediction is that the negative compatibility effect is attenu-
Electroencephalographic event-related potential (ERP) ated in Parkinson's disease. Of note, Aron et al. (2003)
studies have also contributed to a reconsideration of the predicted an increased negative compatibility effect in
view that motor cortical activity is depressed in Parkinson's Parkinson's disease, based on subliminal response priming
disease. While changes in the distribution of movement- results in Huntington's disease.
related potentials are often interpreted in terms of compen-
satory activation of lateral premotor areas (Dick et al., 1989;
Cunnington et al., 1995), Praamstra et al. (1996) acknow- Methods
ledged that a loss of speci®city and impaired focusing of
Participants
neural activity might produce such changes. Furthermore, The investigation included three experimental groups: Parkinson's
recordings of the movement-related lateralized readiness disease patients (n = 12), age-matched controls (n = 12) and young
potential (LRP) during so-called interference or con¯ict tasks participants (n = 10). All participants gave informed consent and the
have shown that voluntary response activation in Parkinson's investigation was approved by the South Birmingham Research
disease patients is susceptible to interference by inappropriate Ethics Committee. The Parkinson's disease group consisted of nine
motor cortex activation evoked by irrelevant information men and three women (mean age 6 SD, 60 6 8 years). All patients
(Praamstra et al., 1998; Praamstra and Plat, 2001). Such were on dopaminergic medication and had moderate disease
®ndings do not readily conform to the notion of thalamocor- severity. All patients but one were right-handed, as determined
with the Edinburgh Handedness Questionnaire (Old®eld, 1971).
tical underactivation, and we have proposed that they re¯ect a
Motor symptoms were assessed using the Uni®ed Parkinson's
disinhibition of the motor cortex that may be related to the Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS; Lang and Fahn, 1989). The median
de®cient motor cortical inhibition revealed by TMS investi- score on the motor subsection was 29 (range 24±36). The
gations (Ridding et al., 1995). investigation and the UPDRS rating were performed after overnight
The present investigation sought to further delineate the withdrawal from medication (>12 h). In view of the known visual
extent to which de®cient inhibitory control is responsible for contrast sensitivity changes in Parkinson's disease (Bodis-Wollner,
response selection impairments in Parkinson's disease. For 1987; Tebartz van Elst, 1997; Pieri et al., 2000), contrast sensitivity
this purpose, we exploited a recently discovered feature of was determined using the Pelli±Robson chart (Pelli et al., 1988).
subliminal priming, namely self-inhibition. It is well-known Nine men and three women (age 58 6 7 years) formed the age-
that masked visual stimuli, the conscious perception of which matched control group. The young control group consisted of ®ve
men and ®ve women (age 27 6 5 years). All control subjects were
is prevented by a short presentation duration and backward
right-handed and without a history of neurological or psychiatric
masking, can nevertheless in¯uence behaviour (e.g. Taylor diseases.
and McCloskey, 1990; Neumann and Klotz, 1994). For
instance, a masked arrow (the `prime' stimulus) pointing to
the left speeds up the response to a subsequently presented Stimuli and procedures
visible arrow stimulus (the `target' stimulus) when that arrow Figure 1 gives a schematic representation of the trial structure. Each
points to the left, but induces a delay when that arrow is trial started with a ®xation cross that was presented for 1500 ms and
directed to the right. Eimer and Schlaghecken (1998) made disappeared 200 ms before the appearance of the prime stimulus.
the important discovery that the expected facilitation by The prime consisted of arrows positioned to the left and to the right
332 E. Seiss and P. Praamstra

of the screen centre and pointing in the same direction, i.e. either left (EOG) derivations. EEG and EOG signals were ampli®ed with a
or right. The prime stimuli were presented for 32 ms and were bandpass of 0.16±128 Hz by BioSemi Active-One ampli®ers and
immediately followed by masking stimuli consisting of random lines sampled at 512 Hz.
displayed for a duration of 100 ms. The stimulus that conveyed the The continuous EEG recordings were segmented off-line in
response instruction, i.e. the target, consisted of arrows identical to epochs measuring from 100 ms before prime onset to 700 ms after
the prime stimuli in shape and size, and was presented at the centre target onset. Individual trials containing artefacts were rejected
of the screen for a duration of 100 ms. The target appeared either at before averaging, using a threshold of 675 mV. The EEG data of two
the same time as the mask or after a delay of 100 ms. Thus, the Parkinson's disease patients and two age-matched controls were
interstimulus interval (ISI) between prime and target was either 0 ms excluded from the ERP analysis because of a poor signal-to-noise
(ISI = 0 ms task) or 100 ms (ISI = 100 ms task). The next trial started ratio. The behavioural data were used.
300 ms after target offset. LRPs were calculated for each condition separately, in order to
The stimuli were presented on a computer monitor in black on a separate movement-related EEG activity from overlapping stimulus-
grey background. Left- and right-pointing double arrows (<< and >>, related activity (cf. Praamstra et al., 1998). Activity at electrode sites
size 0.6° 3 0.4°) were used as prime and target stimuli. Masking ipsilateral to the responding hand was subtracted from the activity at
stimuli consisted of 20 randomly drawn lines on a virtual grid (0.8° contralateral electrode sites, yielding difference waveforms for left-
3 0.5°). The lines were newly drawn in each trial. The ®xation cross and right-hand responses. Subsequently, these difference waveforms
had a size of 0.2° 3 0.2°. The ®xation cross and target stimuli were averaged across response sides to obtain LRPs. Lateralized

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on September 24, 2015


appeared at the centre of the screen, whereas prime and mask stimuli movement-related activity, i.e. the LRP, is typically measured at
were presented at a distance of 0.8° to the left and right from the electrodes C3 and C4 overlying the hand area of the motor cortex. To
centre. The experiment was performed in a dimly lit room, with the improve the signal-to-noise ratio of the LRP we pooled the C3 and
computer screen placed 100 cm in front of the participant's eyes in C4 electrodes with four adjacent electrodes (FCC3h/4h, C1/2,
the centre of the horizontal straight-ahead line of sight.
CCP5h/6h and CP3/4). In this paper, EEG data analyses mainly
The experiment consisted of eight experimental blocks of 128
concern amplitude and latency measurements of the LRP. The
trials each, divided into four consecutive blocks for the ISI = 0 ms
amplitude of the prime-induced response activation was measured in
task and four blocks for the ISI = 100 ms task. Each task was
a time window of 8 ms around the individual peak latency (baseline
preceded by a practice block of 32 trials. The order of the two tasks
to peak). The amplitude of the subsequent inhibition phase was
was balanced across subjects. Each block contained an equal number
analysed relative to the amplitude of the preceding prime-induced
of trials with a compatible and an incompatible prime±target
activation (peak to peak) (time window around peak, 20 ms).
relation, presented in a pseudorandomized order. Participants
Force signals were recorded and stored with the EEG data.
responded with the hand corresponding to the direction of the target
Response latencies for each trial were determined with a force onset
arrow and the instruction emphasized response speed. Left- and
right-hand responses were recorded using hand grip force detection algorithm. When both response hands were activated, a
measurements. trial was counted as error whenever the wrong response channel was
At the end of the experiment, masking ef®ciency was evaluated activated earlier than the correct response and reached an amplitude
by means of a staircase procedure determining the individual prime >10% of the force amplitude on the correct side.
identi®cation thresholds. Subjects remained sitting in front of the
computer screen, where a ®xation cross was presented for the
duration of the entire block, which consisted of 126 trials. Prime and
mask stimuli were presented in the same way as in the main Data analyses
experiment. Participants had to identify the direction of the arrows For the statistical analysis of behavioural [force onset (reaction time,
and to respond with the corresponding hand. They were instructed to RT), error rates] and electrophysiological data (amplitudes and
respond spontaneously when they thought they could not identify the latencies), analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were performed with the
arrow direction. The number of the random lines in the mask was within-subject factors Compatibility (compatible versus incompat-
adjusted from trial to trial according to the performance of the ible), prime±target ISI (0 versus 100 ms) and the between-subjects
participant. A ®xed-step, 1-up/2-down procedure was employed: one factor Group (Parkinson's disease patients, age-matched controls,
random line was added in trials following correct responses and two young control subjects). Within-subject effects were corrected for
lines were removed after incorrect responses. The block started with non-sphericity using the Huynh±Feldt correction. Signi®cant effects
the presentation of an unmasked prime (zero line mask). With this were further analysed using post hoc tests (t tests for paired and
procedure, identi®cation performance converges on a 66% correct independent samples) and the critical a level was adjusted with the
level (Kaernbach, 1991). Bonferroni correction. Planned comparisons were used to compare
the magnitude of compatibility effects between Parkinson's disease
patients and age-matched controls. The hypothesis of a positive
Data acquisition and preprocessing correlation between disease severity and reduction of the negative
The EEG was recorded continuously with Ag/AgCl electrodes from compatibility effect was evaluated (one-tailed) using Spearman's
82 scalp electrodes relative to a linked mastoids reference. The rank coef®cients.
electrodes were placed according to the International 10±10 Pelli±Robson scores of contrast sensitivity for binocular vision
electrode system, with additional electrodes at electrode sites were obtained from a subset of the participants consisting of 11
FCC3h, FCC4h, FCC5h, FCC6h, CCP5h and CCP6h (American Parkinson's disease patients, nine age-matched controls and 10
Electroencephalographic Society, 1994; Oostenveld and Praamstra, young controls (Pelli et al., 1988). The ranking scores were
2001), using a carefully positioned nylon cap. Eye movements were compared between groups using non-parametric tests (Kruskal±
monitored by bipolar horizontal and vertical electro-oculogram Wallis test, Mann±Whitney test).
Response selection in Parkinson's disease 333

Results matched controls (55 ms), followed by young controls


Behavioural data (41 ms). While differences across the three groups were
An omnibus ANOVA was applied to the collective reaction expressed in a signi®cant Group 3 Compatibility effect
time data set from the task with prime±target ISI = 0 ms and [F(2,31) = 4.4, P = 0.021], the difference between
the task with ISI = 100 ms. This analysis showed that the Parkinson's disease and age-matched control groups was
groups had priming effects in the same direction at ISI = 0 ms. not signi®cant.
At ISI = 100 ms, by contrast, there were marked differences The error pattern matched the RT results, with more errors
between the groups, with priming effects in opposite direc- when the prime±target relation was incompatible than when it
tions, as will be explained below. This was expressed in a was compatible [F(1,31) = 35.0, P < 0.01]. Moreover, the
signi®cant ISI 3 Group 3 Compatibility interaction for susceptibility to interference by an incompatible prime
reaction times [F(2,31) = 3.8, P = 0.034] and for error rates differed between the groups, with especially Parkinson's
[F(2,31) = 4.0, P = 0.029]. This expected result focuses our disease patients generating a high error rate in the incompat-
main interest on the ISI = 100 ms task. For clarity of ible condition [Group 3 Compatibility interaction, F(2,31) =
exposition the two ISI tasks are further presented separately, 4.7, P = 0.016]. Furthermore, the compatibility effect in terms
except for the overall RTs per group, which were 381 6 60 of error rate was larger in Parkinson's disease patients than in
aged controls [t(13.5) = 2.7; P = 0.018]. Disease severity in

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on September 24, 2015


ms (young controls), 435 6 39 ms (age-matched controls)
and 437 6 57 ms (Parkinson's disease). These differences the Parkinson's disease group, measured by the UPDRS, did
yielded a signi®cant main effect of Group [F(2,31) = 3.9, not show any correlation with compatibility effects in
P = 0.032]. Of note, while force onsets (i.e. reaction times) reaction time or error rate.
were comparable for Parkinson's disease patients and aged
controls, patients demonstrated a slower rate of force
generation [t(12.1) = 3.8; P = 0.002], as quanti®ed by the ISI = 100 ms task
time from force onset to peak force. Here, in contrast to the previous task, the direction of priming
effects differed between the groups [Group 3 Compatibility
interaction, F(2,31) = 20.0, P < 0.01]. The young control
group demonstrated a performance characterized by a
ISI = 0 ms task negative compatibility effect, with faster responses when
Responses were faster when the target arrows were preceded there was an incompatible prime±target relation than when
by arrows that pointed in the same direction (compatible the relation was compatible, yielding an incompatible±
prime) than when they were preceded by arrows instructing compatible difference of ±31 ms (Fig. 2B). In the other
the opposite hand (incompatible prime). This was manifested participant groups, by contrast, the compatibility effect had a
in a main effect of Compatibility [F(1,31) = 293.2, positive sign, measuring +8 ms in aged controls and +25 ms
P < 0.001]. The size of this compatibility effect (Fig. 2A) in Parkinson's disease patients. One-tailed planned compari-
was largest in Parkinson's disease (64 ms), followed by age- son of the compatibility effect in Parkinson's disease patients

Fig. 2 Reaction times and error rates for the ISI = 0 ms task (A) and the ISI = 100 ms task (B).
334 E. Seiss and P. Praamstra

and aged controls con®rmed a signi®cant difference [t(22) = This was expressed in a signi®cant Group 3 Compatibility
1.9; P = 0.034]. A further analysis evaluated the correlation interaction [F(2,31) = 12.0, P < 0.01]. One-tailed planned
between Parkinson's disease severity, as expressed in the comparison of the compatibility effects of Parkinson's
UPDRS motor subscale, and the size of the compatibility disease patients and aged controls con®rmed a signi®cant
effect in the Parkinson's disease group, yielding a positive difference [t(12.9) = 3.0; P = 0.005]. In contrast to the
correlation (r = 0.54, P = 0.034). That is, more severely compatibility effect for RTs, the compatibility effect for
affected Parkinson's disease patients had a more positive errors did not show a signi®cant correlation with Parkinson's
compatibility effect. disease severity.
Like the RTs, the errors showed opposite patterns in young
controls and Parkinson's disease patients, the aged controls
demonstrating an intermediate behaviour (Fig. 2B). Thus, the
young control group had more errors in the compatible Movement-related potentials
condition, whereas Parkinson's disease patients had more in ISI = 0 ms task
the incompatible condition, with approximately equal error Stimulus-locked LRP waveforms are presented in Fig. 3A.
rates in the two conditions for the age-matched control group. Recall that the LRP measures differential activation of the

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on September 24, 2015


right and left motor cortices. Hence, the most relevant feature
in the waveforms is the transient activation of the incorrect
response in the incompatible condition, manifested in the
brief dip below the baseline just preceding the activation of
the correct response. The amplitude of the incorrect acti-
vation was measured both in absolute terms and scaled
relative to the magnitude of the correct response activation, to
take into account differences in LRP amplitude between the
groups. The ®rst analysis showed no difference between the
groups [F(2,27) < 1]. However, scaled amplitudes differed
between groups [F(2,27) = 3.9, P = 0.034]. Scaled amplitudes
were larger in Parkinson's disease patients than in age-
matched controls [t(12.6) = 2.5; P = 0.029], which ®ts the
slightly larger size compatibility effect of patients in terms of
reaction time.
Peak latencies of the incorrect, prime-induced response
activation are presented in Table 1, showing differences
clearly determined by age, i.e. markedly longer latencies for
the Parkinson's disease group and the age-matched control
group than for the young controls. Statistical evaluation
con®rmed a signi®cant difference between the groups
Fig. 3 Lateralized readiness potentials for the ISI = 0 ms task (A) [F(2,27) = 3.4, P = 0.05]. Note that the transient incorrect
and the ISI = 100 ms task (B). The time scale is aligned at target response activation in the LRP, as pictured in the grand
onset (0 ms) with the onset of the prime stimulus (indicated by average of Fig. 3A, was less well de®ned, i.e. smeared in
arrowheads) at ±32 ms (ISI = 0 ms task) and ±132 ms (ISI = 100
ms task). Note the grey bar indicating temporary activation of the
time, in the Parkinson's disease patients and aged controls
incorrect response, due to inhibition of the prime-induced motor compared with the young controls, presumably related to
cortex activation. latency variability.

Table 1 Latency and amplitude of prime-induced motor cortex activation


ISI Compatibility Group Latency(ms) Amplitude(mV)

0 ms Incompatible Parkinson's disease 270 6 45 1.4 6 0.4


Aged controls 279 6 33 1.4 6 0.6
Young controls 240 6 24 1.5 6 0.5
100 ms Compatible Parkinson's disease 303 6 33 ±0.9 6 0.6
Aged controls 303 6 49 ±0.9 6 0.5
Young controls 240 6 31 ±1.0 6 0.6
100 ms Incompatible Parkinson's disease 315 6 33 1.2 6 0.5
Aged controls 308 6 40 1.4 6 0.4
Young controls 248 6 35 1.1 6 0.4
Response selection in Parkinson's disease 335

mV (young controls), 1.1 6 1.0 mV (age-matched controls)


and 0.9 6 0.4 mV (Parkinson's disease). This gave a
signi®cant main effect of Group [F(2,27) = 6.1, P < 0.01],
due to stronger inhibition in the young controls relative to the
other groups.
The differences in the LRP between young controls on the
one hand and age-matched controls and Parkinson's disease
groups on the other hand are consistent with and clarify the
mechanism behind the attenuation of the negative compati-
bility effect in the latter groups. Note, however, that the
waveforms as shown in Fig. 3B are markedly affected by
interindividual latency differences of especially the initial
prime-induced motor cortex activation. Moreover, the latency
difference of this activation between groups (Table 1) was
unexpected and might play a role in the attenuation of the
negative compatibility effect in the older participant groups.

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on September 24, 2015


To facilitate the evaluation of how this delay might have
affected performance, the LRPs were realigned at the peak of
the prime-induced motor cortex activation, as determined for
Fig. 4 Lateralized readiness potentials for the ISI = 100 ms task
realigned at the peak latency of the prime-induced response
each participant. The realigned waveforms are displayed in
activation. Response times relative to this latency are represented Fig. 4. The superimposed waveforms illustrate, con®rming
below the waveforms. The left grey bar marks the prime-induced the analysis reported above, that the delayed prime-induced
motor cortex activation, while the right bar marks the subsequent motor cortex activation in the Parkinson's disease and age-
inhibition. Solid lines and arrowheads indicate the compatible matched control groups was not reduced in amplitude. The
condition; dashed lines and open arrow heads indicate the
incompatible condition.
superimposition of waveforms also emphasizes the attenu-
ation of the self-inhibition phase in the LRP of the compatible
condition in these groups.
ISI = 100 ms task
Stimulus-locked LRPs are shown in Fig. 3B. The waveforms
are characterized by initial de¯ections due to prime-induced
Control measures
The ef®cacy of masking was measured with a staircase
motor cortex activation, most clearly illustrated in the
procedure. The thresholds, expressed as the mean number of
waveforms of the young control group. Uniquely for this
lines required in the mask to prevent conscious perception,
group, the initial activation by compatible prime stimuli
were 8.6 6 4.5 lines for the Parkinson's disease patients, 6.9
(continuous line) was followed by activation of the incorrect
6 3 lines for the aged controls, and 7.1 6 3 lines for the
response before the correct response was reactivated and
young controls. These values were not signi®cantly different
executed. The phase of incorrect response activation follow-
between the groups [F(2,31) < 1] and were well below the
ing a compatible prime represents the self-inhibition process
number of lines, i.e. 20, used for masking in the main
that was the main interest of this study. In the aged control experiment. Hence, performance differences between the
and Parkinson's disease groups there was no incorrect groups are not likely to be due to differences in perceptual
response activation in the same time window, indicating awareness of the prime stimuli, which seem adequately
that self-inhibition of the prime-induced activation was weak. masked for all three groups.
Note that although the self-inhibition of prime-induced motor Contrast sensitivity differed between the groups [c2(2) =
cortex activation stands out as a separate LRP de¯ection in 9.7; P < 0.01]. It was lower in the aged control group (median
the compatible condition only, it is presumably equally strong 1.65, range 0.3) than in the young controls (median 1.95,
in the incompatible condition, in which it coincides with range 0.3; Z = 3.1, P < 0.01). The contrast sensitivity
voluntary response activation and drives the RT advantage. measured in the Parkinson's disease patients was intermedi-
Table 1 shows the peak amplitude and latencies of the ate between aged and young controls (median 1.8, range 0.3)
prime-induced motor cortex activation. There was no differ- and did not differ signi®cantly from the values in either of
ence in amplitude between groups [F(2,27) < 1]. The latency, these groups.
by contrast, was signi®cantly different between groups
[F(2,27) = 10.2, P < 0.01], with shorter latencies for the
young controls than for the aged controls and Parkinson's Discussion
disease patients. The self-inhibition phase in the compatible The present study investigated response selection impair-
condition was quanti®ed relative to the preceding prime- ments in Parkinson's disease, using a masked priming
induced activation, yielding peak-to-peak values of 2.1 6 1.0 paradigm. In the ISI = 100 ms task that we used, covert
336 E. Seiss and P. Praamstra

response activation induced by a subliminal stimulus is target were separated by an ISI of 100 ms, produced the
normally subject to inhibition (Eimer and Schlaghecken, desired effect in young control subjects, with a negative
1998). We hypothesized that this inhibition would be reduced compatibility effect size of ±31 ms. Moreover, the recorded
in Parkinson's disease. The results con®rm our hypothesis, LRP demonstrated a robust initial prime-induced response
both in terms of response times and in terms of response activation followed by an inhibition phase that, in the
errors. Compared with young control subjects, healthy elderly compatible condition, temporarily shifted the balance of
subjects also showed a reduced inhibition of covert response activation between left and right motor cortex towards an
activation. As we will explain below on the basis of our activation of the incorrect response (Fig. 3). Presumably, a
electrophysiological data, this age effect may be partly due to similar yet invisible inhibition phase coincided with the
an age-related delay in the latency of prime-induced response voluntary activation of the correct response in the incompat-
activation, extraneous to the inhibitory de®cits the task was ible condition. Not only the Parkinson's disease patients but
designed to measure. also the aged controls showed a markedly different pattern,
characterized by a positive instead of negative compatibility
effect. Notwithstanding this age-related attenuation of the
Negative compatibility effects negative compatibility effect, there were signi®cant differ-
In masked priming experiments, response activation induced ences between Parkinson's disease patients and aged controls

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on September 24, 2015


by a subliminal stimulus supports or con¯icts with the that supported our hypothesis of an inhibitory de®cit in
response demanded by the imperative stimulus, very similar Parkinson's disease. Compared with the negative compati-
to the effects of ¯anker (in)compatibility and spatial bility effect in young controls (±31 ms), there was a more
stimulus±response (in)compatibility, previously examined positive compatibility effect in patients compared with
in Parkinson's disease (Brown et al., 1993; Cope et al., controls (25 versus 8 ms), re¯ecting a pattern in which the
1996; Praamstra et al., 1998; Lee et al., 1999; Praamstra and behaviour of aged controls is intermediate between that of
Plat, 2001). What, then, is the added value of examining the young controls and Parkinson's disease patients. This pattern
negative compatibility effect? The application of con¯ict was also re¯ected in the error analysis. Aged controls had
tasks in patients with movement disorders is often reported as equal numbers of errors in the compatible and incompatible
testing visual attention de®cits (Georgiou et al., 1995; Cope conditions, whereas Parkinson's disease patients made more
et al., 1996; Lee et al., 1999). We explained the impaired errors in the incompatible condition (Fig. 2). The amplitude
performance of Parkinson's disease patients on such tasks, by of the inhibitory phase of the LRP was not sensitive to the
contrast, in terms of inhibitory de®cits within the motor group difference, most likely due to overlap with voluntary
system that compromise the selection between competing response activation.
responses (Praamstra et al., 1998; Praamstra and Plat, 2001). In contrast to the task with 100 ms ISI, with an ISI of 0 ms
Such an explanation would be further strengthened when the direction of compatibility effects was the same across the
Parkinson's disease patients exhibited a reduction of the groups, reaction times being always faster in the compatible
negative compatibility effect following subliminal response than in the incompatible condition. The 9 ms difference in the
priming. One obvious reason for this is that the irrelevant size of the compatibility effect between Parkinson's disease
information is presented subliminally. Hence, impaired patients and aged controls (64 versus 55 ms) was in the
performance cannot be attributed to an inability to ignore expected direction, but not signi®cant. However, compatibil-
irrelevant information. Of greater import, however, is that an ity effects in error rates were signi®cantly different between
evaluation of the negative compatibility effect provides direct these groups. In addition, the size of the incorrect LRP
information on inhibitory processes, within the motor system, activation was larger in patients than in aged controls.
that come into play when a response is activated but does not Together, these results in the ISI = 0 ms task resemble those
reach threshold (Eimer and Schlaghecken, 1998; obtained with visible instead of subliminally presented
Schlaghecken and Eimer, 2002). An analogy with TMS ¯ankers (Praamstra et al., 1998), on which our hypothesis
studies is suggested, in which the `double-pulse paradigm' of an inhibitory de®cit was partly based.
involves the application of a conditioning stimulus to the
motor cortex, followed by a test stimulus (Kujirai et al.,
1993). A conditioning stimulus of subthreshold intensity, i.e.
not evoking an EMG response, does activate inhibitory Age-related attenuation of the negative
circuits that reduce the response to the suprathreshold test compatibility effect
stimulus. This inhibition is reduced in Parkinson's disease Reduction of the negative compatibility effect from ±31 ms in
(Ridding et al., 1995; Strafella et al., 2000), and a similar young controls to +8 and +25 ms in aged controls and
reduction of the inhibition underlying the negative compati- Parkinson's disease patients, respectively, suggests a signi®-
bility effect would support the possibility that the relevant cant effect of age. While age increases the susceptibility to
inhibitory circuits have a role in response selection. interference in con¯ict tasks, commonly attributed to a
Turning now to the results, the task designed to elicit a reduced ability to inhibit prepotent response tendencies
negative compatibility effect, i.e. the task where prime and (Zeef et al., 1996; Nieuwenhuis et al., 2000), the present
Response selection in Parkinson's disease 337

investigation identi®es another cause of age differences in relevance of the negative compatibility effect, obtained with
masked priming. Prime-induced motor cortex activation in subliminal priming, is its demonstration that the resolution of
the 100 ms ISI task peaked some 60 ms later in the aged response con¯ict is assisted by active inhibition of partially
controls and the Parkinson's disease patients than in the activated responses. To capture this inhibition electrophysio-
young controls. As a result, the prime-induced activation logically, using the LRP, response alternatives need to be
interacted with the target induced activation at a later time mapped onto opposite hands. The negative compatibility
point compared with young controls. Hence, the age-related effect, however, is also obtained with the responses de®ned
attenuation of the negative compatibility effect may partly on one hand (Eimer and Schlaghecken, 1998), showing that it
depend on differences in timing of automatic and voluntary is not peculiar to interhemispheric interaction. Presumably,
response activation. This does not exclude an age-related the underlying inhibition is not unique either to partial
decline of inhibitory function, as tested with masked priming. response activation in subliminal priming. More likely, it
Interestingly, inhibitory circuits probed with the TMS paired- re¯ects inhibitory interactions whose normal operation
pulse technique decline with age (Peinemann et al., 2001). subserves the selection between alternative responses.
Note that the incorrect prime-induced motor cortex acti- Consistent with such a view, inhibitory interactions support-
vation in the ISI = 0 ms task, elicited by incompatible primes, ing the selection and suppression of competing responses
also peaked later in aged controls and patients than in young have been identi®ed in premotor areas and in the basal

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on September 24, 2015


controls, although the delay was smaller than in the ISI = 100 ganglia (Schall et al., 1995; Basso and Wurtz, 2002; Cisek
ms task. Latency and amplitude differences (Table 1) may be and Kalaska, 2002).
due to the fact that, with ISI = 0 ms, the prime occurs closer in As stated earlier, it is tempting to hypothesize a relation
time to the target, with effects of temporal attention (Miniussi between the de®cient inhibition found in con¯ict tasks and
et al., 1999) on motor cortex excitability leading to a higher- subliminal priming and the impaired cortical inhibition
amplitude prime-induced response. On the other hand, the demonstrated with TMS in Parkinson's disease (Ridding
close succession of prime and target in the ISI = 0 ms task et al., 1995). However, such a link remains to be demon-
makes the delayed prime-induced activation prone to trunca- strated and it is relevant to point out that different TMS
tion by the target-elicited voluntary response activation, thus indices of cortical inhibition, i.e. the silent period, short-
explaining the smaller delay relative to the ISI = 100 ms task interval intracortical inhibition and long-interval cortical
for the aged participants. inhibition, probably measure different inhibitory mechanisms
The marked prolongation in the latency of prime-induced (Sanger et al., 2001). Nonetheless, these measures of cortical
motor cortex activation with age is a novel and intriguing inhibition are in¯uenced by basal ganglia disease and are
®nding, especially since the delay does not seem to dissipate responsive to pharmacotherapy and surgical interventions for
the activation. If it proves replicable, it will require incorp- Parkinson's disease (Ridding et al., 1995; Ziemann et al.,
oration in theories of masked response priming, which does 1997; Chen et al., 2001; Cunic et al., 2002). The identi®ca-
not seem straightforward. Masked response priming is often tion, by means of functional MRI, of the caudate nucleus and
regarded as evidence for the existence of direct perceptuo- thalamus as structures involved in the inhibitory control
motor links, possibly mediated by dorsal visual stream underlying the negative compatibility effect (Aron et al.,
circuitry (Neumann and Klotz, 1994; Eimer and 2003) does not, therefore, exclude a relation between this
Schlaghecken, 1998). It is hardly likely that this pathway is effect and impaired cortical inhibition measured with TMS.
abnormally vulnerable to the effects of ageing. Our ®nding Caution regarding such a relation is, on the other hand,
may be more easily accommodated by accounts of masked suggested by the ®nding that repetitive TMS of the motor
priming in terms of retinotectal connections (e.g. Morris et al., cortex does not affect the negative compatibility effect
1999). But, to our knowledge, there is no evidence either for (Schlaghecken et al., 2003).
disproportionate ageing of retinotectal pathways. Aron et al. (2003) investigated inhibitory control processes
in subliminal priming in Huntington's disease, expecting to
®nd a reduction of the negative compatibility effect. The
results did not demonstrate an abnormal performance for the
Functional anatomical locus and comparison patients as a group, however, as the majority of patients
with Huntington's disease showed an exaggerated negative compatibility effect while
Our prediction of a reduced negative compatibility effect in only a smaller subgroup demonstrated the predicted attenu-
Parkinson's disease rested on previous work showing ation. The latter subgroup had higher chorea scores than the
enhanced susceptibility to interference in response con¯ict former and the authors proposed that the patients with an
tasks, perhaps related to de®cient cortical inhibition enhanced negative compatibility effect had an akinetic±rigid
(Praamstra and Plat, 2001). The con®rmation of this predic- variant of Huntington's disease with features resembling
tion supports an inference of de®cient response selection in Parkinson's disease. Hence, this reconstruction entails a
Parkinson's disease and implicates relatively low-level motor prediction regarding Parkinson's disease that is discon®rmed
processes rather than cognitive striatofrontal processes rele- by the present results. As to what may have caused the
vant to interference control (Cope et al., 1996). The key bimodal pattern in the behaviour of the Huntington's disease
338 E. Seiss and P. Praamstra

group, one possibility relates to the latency of the prime- References


induced response activation relative to the voluntary response Albin RL, Young AB, Penney JB. The functional anatomy of basal ganglia
disorders. Trends Neurosci 1989; 12: 366±75.
activation. As suggested by the present investigation, the Alexander GE, Crutcher MD, DeLong MR. Basal ganglia±thalamocortical
relative timing of these activations is probably an important circuits: parallel substrates for motor, oculomotor, `prefrontal', and
factor in driving the direction and size of compatibility `limbic' functions. Prog Brain Res 1990; 85: 119±46.
effects. The variable extent of extrastriatal pathology in American Electroencephalographic Society. Guideline 13: guidelines for
Huntington's disease patients (Rosas et al., 2003), in standard electrode position nomenclature. J Clin Neurophysiol 1994; 11:
111±3.
combination with slow and variable voluntary response
Aron AR, Schlaghecken F, Fletcher PC, Bullmore ET, Eimer M, Barker R,
activation (Georgiou et al., 1995), may have affected the et al. Inhibition of subliminally primed responses is mediated by the
timing in a way that produced the bimodal behavioural caudate and thalamus: evidence from functional MRI and Huntington's
pattern. disease. Brain 2003; 126: 713±23.
Basso MA, Wurtz RH. Neuronal activity in substantia nigra pars reticulata
during target selection. J Neurosci 2002; 22: 1883±94.
Bodis-Wollner I, Marx MS, Mitra S, Bobak P, Mylin L, Yahr M. Visual
Conclusions dysfunction in Parkinson's disease: loss in spatiotemporal contrast
The present results extend previous evidence for impaired sensitivity. Brain 1987; 110: 1675±98.
response inhibition in Parkinson's disease. The experimental

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on September 24, 2015


Boraud T, Bezard E, Bioulac B, Gross CE. From single extracellular unit
approach, using subliminal priming to induce a competition recording in experimental and human Parkinsonism to the development of
between two response alternatives, provides arguments for a functional concept of the role played by the basal ganglia in motor
control. Prog Neurobiol 2002; 66: 265±83.
impaired inhibition at a fairly low level in the central motor
Brown RG, Jahanshahi M, Marsden CD. Response choice in Parkinson's
system (Eimer, 1999), possibly related to the de®cient disease: the effects of uncertainty and stimulus±response compatibility.
inhibitory mechanisms revealed by paired-pulse TMS. The Brain 1993; 116: 869±85.
function of the circuitry underlying self-inhibition of a Buhmann C, Glauche V, StuÈrenburg HJ, Oechsner M, Weiller C, BuÈchel C.
partially activated response goes evidently beyond the Pharmacologically modulated fMRIÐcortical responsiveness to levodopa
arti®cial experimental situation created here. As indicated in drug-naive hemiparkinsonian patients. Brain 2003; 126: 451±61.
Cisek P, Kalaska JF. Simultaneous encoding of multiple potential reach
by the lateralization of the recorded movement-related
directions in dorsal premotor cortex. J Neurophysiol 2002; 87: 1149±54.
potentials, inhibition of a partially activated response auto- Chen R, Garg RR, Lozano AM, Lang AE. Effects of internal globus pallidus
matically produces activation of the alternative response. stimulation on motor cortex excitability. Neurology 2001; 56: 716±23.
Hence, we infer that the relevant circuitry mediates inhibitory Cope MT, Georgiou N, Bradshaw JL, Iansek R, Phillips JG. Simon effect
interactions between competing response alternatives and has and attention in Parkinson's disease: a comparison with Huntington's
a role in response selection. The rapid alternation of LRP disease and Tourette's syndrome. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1996; 18:
276±90.
de¯ections in young control subjects demonstrates that the Cunic D, Roshan L, Khan FI, Lozano AM, Lang AE, Chen R. Effects of
inhibitory circuitry is ®nely tuned to admit the release of only subthalamic nucleus stimulation on motor cortex excitability in
one response. Similar selectivity is required in natural Parkinson's disease. Neurology 2002; 58: 1665±72.
movement repertoires involving, for instance, bimanual Cunnington C, Iansek R, Bradshaw JL, Phillips JG. Movement-related
coordination or sequencing of movements. The reduced potentials in Parkinson's disease: presence and predictability of temporal
and spatial cues. Brain 1995; 118: 935±50.
inhibition in Parkinson's disease and, to a lesser degree, aged
DeLong MR. Primate models of movement disorders of basal ganglia origin.
controls, is manifested here as impaired control of partial Trends Neurosci 1990; 13: 281±5.
response activation, leading to a marked change of prime± Dick JPR, Rothwell JC, Day BL, Cantello R, Buruma O, Gioux M, et al. The
target compatibility effects both in response times and error Bereitschaftspotential is abnormal in Parkinson's disease. Brain 1989;
pattern. In line with current views on basal ganglia function 112: 233±44.
(Mink, 1996; Boraud et al., 2002), we propose that this Doudet DJ, Gross C, Arluison M, Bioulac B. Modi®cations of precentral
cortex discharge and EMG activity in monkeys with MPTP-induced
re¯ects the loss of spatial and temporal selectivity in the
lesions of DA nigral neurons. Exp Brain Res 1990; 80: 177±88.
operation of basal ganglia±thalamocortical mechanisms that Eimer M. Facilitatory and inhibitory effects of masked prime stimuli on
in¯uence the selection and suppression of competing motor activation and behavioural performance. Acta Psychol (Amst)
responses. 1999; 101: 293±313.
Eimer M, Schlaghecken F. Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation:
behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept
Perform 1998; 24: 1737±47.
Acknowledgements Georgiou N, Bradshaw JL, Phillips JG, Bradshaw JA, Chiu E. The Simon
The authors wish to thank the patients and volunteers for their effect and attention de®cits in Gilles de la Tourette's syndrome and
participation in this study, N. Roach, G. Barbieri and C. Huntington's disease. Brain 1995; 118: 1305±18.
Hesse, who provided technical and programming support, F. Goldberg JA, Boraud T, Maraton S, Haber SN, Vaadia E, Bergman H.
Schlaghecken for advice and for making available the Enhanced synchrony among primary motor cortex neurons in the 1-
methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine primate model of Parkinson's
staircase program for testing masking ef®ciency, and M.
disease. J Neurosci 2002; 22: 4638±53.
Eimer, F. Schlaghecken and H. Siebner, who provided Haslinger B, Erhard P, Kampfe N, Boecker H, Rummeny E, Schwaiger M,
helpful comments on an earlier draft. The investigation was et al. Event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging in Parkinson's
supported by the MRC. disease before and after levodopa. Brain 2001; 124: 558±70.
Response selection in Parkinson's disease 339

Kaernbach C. Simple adaptative testing with the weighted up±down method. Praamstra P, Stegeman DF, Cools AR, Horstink MWIM. Reliance on
Percept Psychophys 1991; 9: 227±9. external cues in movement initiation in Parkinson's disease: evidence
Kujirai T, Caramia MD, Rothwell JC, Day BL, Thompson PD, Ferbert A, from movement related brain potentials. Brain 1998; 121: 167±77.
et al. Corticocortical inhibition in human motor cortex. J Physiol 1993; Ridding MC, Inzelberg R, Rothwell JC. Changes in excitability of motor
471: 501±19. cortical circuitry in patients with Parkinson's disease. Ann Neurol 1995;
Lang AET, Fahn AS. Assessment of Parkinson's disease. In: Munsat TL, 37: 181±8.
editor. Quanti®cation of neurological de®cit. Boston: Butterworth; 1989. Rosas HD, Koroshetz WJ, Chen YI, Skeuse C, Vangel M, Cudkowicz ME,
p. 285±309. et al. Evidence for more widespread cerebral pathology in early HD: an
Lee SS, Wild K, Hollnagel C, Grafman J. Selective visual attention in MRI-based morphometric analysis. Neurology 2003; 60: 1615±20.
patients with frontal lobe lesions or Parkinson's disease. Sabatini U, Boulanouar K, Fabre N, Martin F, Carel C, Colonnese C, et al.
Neuropsychologia 1999; 37: 595±604. Cortical motor reorganization in akinetic patients with Parkinson's
Miniussi C, Wilding EL, Coull JT, Nobre AC. Orienting attention in time. disease: a functional fMRI study. Brain 2000; 123: 394±403.
Modulation of brain potentials. Brain 1999; 122: 1507±18. Sanger TD, Garg RR, Chen R. Interactions between two different inhibitory
Mink JW. The basal ganglia: focused selection and inhibition of competing systems in the human motor cortex. J Physiol 2001; 530: 307±17.
motor programs. Prog Neurobiol 1996; 50: 381±425. Schall JD, Hanes DP, Thompson KG, King DJ. Saccade target selection in
Morris JS, Ohman A, Dolan RJ. A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala frontal eye ®eld of macaque. I. Visual and premovement activation. J
mediating `unseen' fear. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1999; 96: 1680±5. Neurosci 1995; 15: 6905±18.
Neumann O, Klotz W. Motor responses to nonreportable, masked stimuli: Schlaghecken F, Eimer M. Motor activation with and without inhibition:
where is the limit of direct parameter speci®cation? In: Umilta C, evidence for a threshold mechanism in motor control. Percept Psychophys

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on September 24, 2015


Moscovitch M, editors. Attention and performance. XV: conscious and 2002; 64: 148±62.
nonconscious information processing. Cambridge (MA): MIT Press; Schlaghecken F, MuÈnchau A, Bloem B, Rothwell JC, Eimer M. Slow
1994. p. 123±50. frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation affects reaction
Nieuwenhuis S, Ridderinkhof KR, de Jong R, Kok A, van der Molen MW. times, but not prime effects, in a masked priming task. Clin Neurophysiol
Inhibitory inef®ciency and failures of intention activation: age-related 2003; 114: 1272±7.
decline in the control of saccadic eye movements. Psychol Aging 2000; Strafella AP, Valzania F, Nassetti SA, Tropeani A, Bisulli A, Santangelo M,
15: 635±47. et al. Effects of chronic levodopa and pergolide treatment on cortical
Old®eld RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh excitability in patients with Parkinson's disease: a transcranial magnetic
inventory. Neuropsychologia 1971; 9: 97±113. stimulation study. Clin Neurophysiol 2000; 111: 1198±202.
Oostenveld R, Praamstra P. The ®ve percent electrode system for high- Taylor JL, McCloskey DI. Triggering of preprogrammed movements as
resolution EEG and ERP measurements. Clin Neurophysiol 2001; 112: reactions to masked stimuli. J Neurophysiol 1990; 63: 439±46.
713±9. Tebartz van Elst L, Greenlee MW, Foley JM, LuÈcking CH. Contrast
Peinemann A, Lehner C, Conrad B, Siebner HR. Age-related decrease in detection, discrimination and adaptation in patients with Parkinson's
paired-pulse intracortical inhibition in the human primary motor cortex. disease and multiple system atrophy. Brain 1997; 120: 2219±28.
Neurosci Lett 2001; 313: 33±6. Turner RS, Grafton ST, McIntosh AR, DeLong MR, Hoffman JM. The
Pelli D, Robson JG, Wilkins AJ. Designing a new letter chart for measuring functional anatomy of parkinsonian bradykinesia. Neuroimage 2003; 19:
contrast sensitivity. Clin Vis Res 1988; 2: 187±99. 163±79.
Pieri V, Diederich NJ, Raman R, Goetz CG. Decreased color discrimination Watts RL, Mandir AS. The role of motor cortex in pathophysiology of
and contrast sensitivity in Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Sci 2000; 172: 7± voluntary movement de®cits associated with parkinsonism. Neurol Clin
11. 1992; 10: 451±69.
Praamstra P, Plat FM. Failed suppression of direct visuomotor activation in Zeef EJ, Sonke CJ, Kok A, Buiten MM, Kenemans JL. Perceptual factors
Parkinson's disease. J Cogn Neurosci 2001; 13: 31±43. affecting age-related differences in focused attention: performance and
Praamstra P, Meyer AS, Cools AR, Horstink MWIM, Stegeman DF. psychophysiological analyses. Psychophysiology 1996; 33: 555±65.
Movement preparation in Parkinson's disease: time course and Ziemann U, Tergau F, Bruns D, Baudewig J, Paulus W. Changes in human
distribution of movement-related potentials in a movement precueing motor cortex excitability induced by dopaminergic and anti-dopaminergic
task. Brain 1996; 119: 1689±704. drugs. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1997; 105: 430±7.

You might also like