The Role of Knowledge of Results in Performance

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Journal of Voice

Vol. 14, No. 2, pp. 137-145


© 2000 Singular Publishing Group

The Role of Knowledge of Results in Performance


and Learning of a Voice Motor Task

Kimberly Steinhauer and Judith Preston Grayhack


Department of Communication Science and Disorders
University of Pittsburgh
Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania

Summary: The relationship between the provision of Knowledge of Results


and the performance and learning of a voice motor task was examined. Thir-
ty adult subjects, randomly assigned to a 100%, 50%, or No Knowledge of
Results group, practiced a novel vowel nasalization task. Measures of accura-
cy and variability obtained during the practice session indicated influence of
knowledge of results schedule on the transient effects of motor performance.
Deviations from the nasalance target during the retention phase, 5 minutes lat-
er, and during a transfer phase, 24 hours later, indicated influence of knowl-
edge of results schedule on the permanent effects of motor learning. Collec-
tive results revealed that an increase in relative frequency of knowledge of
results led to a decrease in motor performance and learning of a vowel nasal-
ization task: Both accuracy and variability were degraded as knowledge of re-
sults increased, with those subjects in the 100% group exhibiting the poorest
scores. Key Words: Knowledge of Results--Practice--Nasalance

Singing teachers and voice therapists deal with im- relative frequency of KR on performance and learn-
proving human performance and facilitating learning ing of a vowel nasalization task as measured by a
through permanent behavioral changes. It is critical nasalance score.
to understand how variables such as task and prac- Feedback, sensory information about a move-
tice impact the acquisition of new motor patterns. ment, remains second in importance only to prac-
This investigation addresses the relationship be- tice as a variable influencing motor skill acquisi-
tween a practice strategy using Knowledge of Results tion.I, 2 Voice researchers have explored the
(KR) and motor acquisition and retention of a voice contribution of auditory feedback to voice motor
motor task, vowel nasalance. The purpose of the control, 3 but have been less attentive to the role of
study was to determine the effect of manipulating extrinsic, augmented feedback in voice motor skill
acquisition. 4 Due to the complexity of studying in-
Accepted for publication April 20, 1999. trinsic feedback, motor learning researchers have
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Kimberly concentrated on manipulating variables of extrinsic
Steinhauer, Department of Communication Science and Disor- feedback such as KR. 1,5-10 KR is verbal, or verbal-
ders, 4033 Forbes Tower, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh,
izable, terminal augmented feedback about the out-
PA 15260 USA
Presented at the Twenty-Seventh Annual Symposium: Care come or nature of the result in terms of the environ-
of the Professional Voice; June 2, 1998; Philadelphia, PA mental goal (eg, "The target was 80, your score was

137
138 KIMBERLY STEINHAUER AND JUDITH PRESTON GRAYHACK

72."). Relative frequency of KR refers to the per- crease in relative frequency of KR failed to produce
centage of trials in which KR is offered. a performance effect during acquisition.12, ~4
Within the confines of a motor learning experi- The alternative to the prescriptive theory of motor
ment, subjects practice on a selected motor task, and control and learning lies in the dynamical systems
then progress is documented as a function of prac- approach that describes humans as self-organizing
tice trial. Salmoni, Schmidt, and Walter advocated systems capable of movement through an emergent
distinguishing between KR effects on performance result of changes in coordinative structures governed
and KR effects on learning. 1 Performance effects by underlying physical, synergetic, or ecological
typically measured during the acquisition phase are principles. 15,16 In the emergent context, motor learn-
considered as temporary or transient changes. Learn- ing is viewed as the integration of new coordination
ing effects measured during retention and transfer patterns or synergies on the background of preexist-
phases are considered as permanent changes. ing patterns called intrinsic dynamics. 16-L8 Swinnen
Variables such as motivation and fatigue that ac- suggests that KR acts to either exploit or suppress
company the provision of KR during acquisition the learner's intrinsic dynamics during practice. 18
have been found to contaminate motor learning re- Although KR has been used to describe the "to be
sults; therefore, Salmoni et al 1 suggested measuring learned" pattern, ~9 specific predictions about the fre-
motor learning via a retention design in which all quency of KR have not been addressed due to the re-
groups are treated to a common level of the indepen- cent incorporation of motor learning into the dynam-
dent variable, usually no KR, following the acquisi- ical theories of motor control.
tion phase. In addition, transfer designs that include Regardless of the theoretical genesis, many studies
a shift of the task or target as a powerful indicator of have documented relationships between limb motor
motor learning were advocated. The most com- learning and relative frequency of KR; 20 however,
pelling results of studies of motor learning have been few investigations have examined the impact of
those in which retention and transfer tests were ad- practice with KR on voice production. Ferrand ex-
ministered after the performance effects dissipat- plored the relationship between KR and phonatory
e d - r a n g i n g from as little as 5 minutes to 5 days, or stability, as indexed by jitter and shimmer. 4 Two
5 months postacquisition.1 groups of subjects practiced sustaining a n / a / f o r 2
Theories of motor learning underscore different seconds. The KR group received information on
roles for KR during motor skill learning. Properties every trial about the waveform and jitter/shimmer
of motivation, guidance, and association have been values. A second group received no KR. Results of
examined as possible explanations for the function the transfer session, held 1 week after practice, re-
of KR during motor learning.l, 2,8 Schmidt's open- vealed no difference between groups for either shim-
loop schema theory and guidance hypothesis are mer or jitter measurements, indicating no relation-
founded on movement categories or schemas (ie, ship between providing KR and permanent changes
ball pitching or pitch elevation) that are governed by in phonatory stability.
a stored, generalized motor program (GMP) con- The disparate results within the limb and voice KR
taining specific parameters for that action. 2,H Stud- literature motivated this research project designed to
ies conducted within the context of Schmidt's theory determine the influence of KR on learning a voice
showed an inverse relationship between relative fre- motor task integral to optimal voice resonance. The
quency of KR and motor learning, regardless of the percept nasality is modulated to some degree by
KR schedule (ie, after alternating trials, after trial varying the size of the velopharyngeal port, as well
block, or faded throughout practice). Increases in as the entire oral cavity. Volitional modification of
relative frequency of KR resulted in degraded learn- nasality is a common goal among singers and speak-
ing effects during retention and transfer. 1,12-14 In ad- ers across most voice quality productions but novel
dition, increases in relative frequency of KR in- to voice producers when practiced in isolation, thus
creased motor performance during acquisition; 1 providing a unique task upon which to test voice
however, other investigations reported that an in- motor learning.

Journal of Voice, Vol. 14, No. 2, 2000


THE ROLE OF KNOWLEDGE OF RESULTS IN PERFORMANCE AND LEARNING OF A VOICE 139

Although proprioceptive innervation of the had participated in less than five private speak-
velopharyngeal sphincter is limited, 21 volitional con- ing/singing lessons. The subjects were healthy non-
trol of nasality has been documented.22, 23 Moon and smokers, experiencing no upper respiratory or sinus
Jones demonstrated dynamical properties of the infections, and free of alcohol and medications that
velopharynx through a study in which 10 subjects have the potential to compromise normal voice mo-
successfully produced b o t h / a / a n d / i / a t 50% and tor control. The subjects remained naive to the spe-
75% nasality using simultaneous phototransduction cific purposes of the experiment, had no previous ex-
to provide visual feedback. 22 Results showed that posure to the experimental vocal task, and were not
success at achieving the target was attributable to significantly different in their baseline performance
plasticity (learning) or flexibility (success without of the task.
learning), two mechanisms of dynamical motor
learning described by Folkins. 24 Plasticity referred Apparatus
to a restructuring of the physiological rule system The Kay Elemetrics Nasometer, a computer-assist-
that occurs during practice. Flexibility referred to an ed instrument, facilitated measurement of nasalance
existing rule system confined to parameters of the over time. The Nasometer is a noninvasive input de-
coordinative structure that accounts for success at a vice consisting of two directional microphones and a
novel task without practice. sound separator plate. The calculated nasalance
In singing instruction,25, 26 cleft palate rehabilita- score is a ratio of nasal to oral plus nasal acoustic en-
tion, 22 and resonant voice therapy 27 clients are asked ergy for each subject. KR as nasalance score ap-
to exploit nasality to alter articulation and resonance peared on the computer monitor. The dynamic range
properties of the vocal tract. Therefore, the aim of of the Nasometer is 50 dB, thus ensuring accurate
this study was to test the effects of manipulating the data collection on a wide range of voice intensities.
relative frequency of KR (100%, 50%, and no KR)
during learning of a vowel nasalization task, as esti- Task
mated by nasalance score. Results from the study
will be interpreted within the context of Schmidt's 11 Acquisition~Retention
schema theory predicting differential effects of KR The subjects were required to nasalize a sustained
on performance and learning and Folkin's 24 theory vowel ( / i / - - > / t / ) . To ensure stable and volitional
predicting a possibility of no effect of KR on perfor- control, each trial was 6 seconds in duration--2 sec-
mance and learning due to the dynamical properties onds of oral phonation before switching to nasal for
of coordinative structures. the final 4 seconds. Target nasalance score for the
oral vowel was between 0% and 40%, and the target
METHODS score for the nasalized vowel was 80%. Subjects
proceeded with the task only after achieving the ini-
Subjects tial target for the oral vowel.
Thirty subjects (20 women, 10 men) between the
ages of 18 and 40 years were selected from the stu- Transfer
dent population at the University of Pittsburgh for The transfer task required the subjects to perform
the study. All subjects were required to have a nega- the identical task Using a new vowel,/a/.
tive history for speech deficits, and to pass a hearing
screening at 20 dB SPL. The racial, gender, and eth- Experimental design and procedure
nic characteristics of the proposed subject popula-
tion reflected the demographics of Pittsburgh and the Acquisition
surrounding area. Thirty subjects were assigned randomly to three
Subjects were required to test within nasalance groups of feedback (100% KR, 50% KR, No KR).
norms for the Zoo Passage as measured by the Kay KR as percent nasalance was administered to the
Elemetrics Nasometer. All subjects were native subjects via a computer monitor. Subjects in the
speakers of English and vocally untrained, that is, 100% KR group received immediate information af-

Journal of Voice, Vol. 14, No. 2, 2000


140 KIMBERLY STEINHAUER AND JUDITH PRESTON GRAYHACK

ter every trial. The 50% KR group received immedi- cy and bias in the directional measures of average
ate information after every other trial. The No KR magnitude of response:
group was not permitted to view the monitor.
During initiation of the session, the experimenter
provided instructions and demonstrations involving
• (x i - T)
/ C E / = i=l _
the task and apparatus. Using the nasometer, sub- 12
jects practiced on four trials using a different vowel,
/if, and a different nasalance target, 65%. All sub- Variable error (VE) is the variability of the subject
jects received information in the form of nasalance about the mean response and is a measure of intra-
score and received a countdown to prepare, initiate, individual response consistency:
and change the vocalization. Motor performance
was examined during the acquisition phase consist-
ing of 5 blocks of 8 trials. All subjects filled the 2- t ~ (Xi _ .~)2
VE = i=1
second intertrial interval with counting by twos or
threes in order to control for mental rehearsing.
The acquisition data were analyzed in a 3 × 5
Retention~transfer (Group × Block) ANOVA with repeated measures
Learning was examined during a retention and on the last factor. For both retention and transfer
transfer phase in which all subjects moved to a com- tests, the 16 trials were collapsed into one block and
mon level of the independent variable, No KR. The analyzed in univariate ANOVAs (group as the inde-
immediate retention phase, 5 minutes following ac- pendent variable). Alpha was set at the .05 level, and
quisition, consisted of 2 blocks of 8 trials using the appropriate post hoc tests were performed to elicit
identical task. The transfer phase, 1 day after acqui- specific group or block differences.
sition, consisted of 2 blocks of 8 trials using the
transfer vowel/a/. RESULTS

Data collection and statistical considerations Group mean and standard deviation data for the
The effect of the independent variable, KR relative dependent measures are listed in Table 1. The depen-
frequency, was measured using the dependent vari- dent measures as a function of trial block on a per-
able, nasalance score. The central 2 seconds of the formance curve depict group differences in Figure 1
total nasal utterance (4 seconds in duration) were (/CE/), and Figure 2 (VE). The Pearson product mo-
used for analysis to ensure stability of the target ment correlation for accuracy measures AE and
nasalized vowel. Performance effects were measured /CE/, r = .98, indicated that the measures were re-
dundant; therefore, o n l y / C E / i s reported as a mea-
during the acquisition phase, and learning effects
were measured during retention and transfer phases. sure of accuracy and bias that is independent of
Three dependent measures sensitive to changes in V E ) 2 One outlier with scores consistently 2 stan-
accuracy and variability 2 were calculated. Absolute dard deviations from the mean was discarded from
error (AE) is the average absolute deviation between the No KR group.
the subject's responses and the target: Acquisition
Results for the/CE/analysis showed a significant

AE - i=l
Ixi- sl group difference F(2,104 ) = 4.70, P<.05 (Figure 1).
Post hoc Student-Newman-Keuls multiple compar-
rt
isons revealed significant differences between 100%
KR (M = 14.08, SD = 15.18) and 50% KR (M =
Absolute constant error (/CE/) is the average error 4.96, SD = 4.09), and between 100% and no KR (M
in responding to the target and represents the accura- = 6.19, SD = 5.21) groups, P<.05. There were no

Journal of Voice, Vol. 14, No. 2, 2000


THE ROLE OF KNOWLEDGE OF RESULTS I N PERFORMANCE AND LEARNING O F A VOICE 141

T A B L E 1. Group Means and Standard Deviatons in %


Nasalance for Feedback Group

Acquisition Retention Transfer

100% KR
/CE/ 14.08 (15.18)* 8.61 (6.58) 25.20 (16.82)*
VE 7.70 (6.76) 7.10 (4.90)* 6.94 (4.04)
50% KR
/CE/ 4.96 (4.09)* 5.63 (4.15) 12.20 (5.64)*
VE 5.77 (5.81) 3.80 (2.40)* 6.13 (3.48)
No KR
/CE/ 6.19 (5.21)* 6.19 (5.21) 14.57 (7.55)
VE 5.29 (5.54) 3.60 (0.70)* 6.95 (6.89)
Mean (Standard Deviation) *P <.05

/CE/

30
25
~ 20 ¢ N o KR
,,~ 15 --m-- 50% KR
~ 10 ~100%KR
5
0 I t I I I I I I
0 1 2 3 4 5 ret trans
Block
F I G 1. Group mean five-block absolute constant error (/CE/) scores in % nasalance for
acquisition (Block 1-5), retention (ret), and transfer (trans).

VE

12
10
8 ; N o KR

tU 6 -- 50% KR
>
4 100% KR
2
0 I I I I I I I I
0 1 2 3 4 5 ret trans
Block
F I G 2. Group mean five-block variable error (VE) scores in % nasalance for acquisition
(Block 1-5), retention (ret), and transfer (trans).

Journal of Voice, Vol. 14, No. 2, 2000


142 K I MB E R LY STEINHAUER AND JUDITH PRESTON GRAYHACK

other main effects or interactions. The group differ- DISCUSSION


ences in/CE/indicate that accuracy to the nasalance
The aim of the present study was to explore the ef-
target was enhanced by decreasing the relative fre-
fect of relative frequency of KR on the motor perfor-
quency or withholding KR, with 100% KR leading
mance and learning of a vowel nasalization task. The
to detrimental decreases in accuracy.
results showed that an increase in relative frequency
Results for the VE analysis showed significant
of KR led to a decrease in both motor performance
block differences across KR conditions in the acqui-
and learning of a vowel nasalization task; however,
sition phase, F(4,104 ) = 2.62, P<.05 (Figure 2). Post
the results revealed a curious departure from the
hoc Student-Newman-Keuls multiple comparisons
limb motor learning literature in that No KR proved
revealed significant differences between Block 1
as beneficial as 50% KR.
(100% K R M = 11.09, SD = 8.27; 50% KR M =
The group differences in target accuracy (/CE/)
7.17, SD = 6.27; No KR M = 8.03, SD = 9.19) and
during the acquisition phase indicate that tempo-
Block 5, (100% KR M = 6.51, SD = 3.75; 50% KR
rary effects of motor performance were enhanced
M = 3.50, SD = 1.21; No KR M = 3.68, SD = 1.63),
by decreasing the relative frequency to 50% or
P<05. There were no other main effects or interac-
withholding KR, with 100% KR leading to detri-
tions. The block difference in VE indicates that vari-
mental decreases in motor performance of a vowel
ability about the subject's nasalance mean was de-
nasalization task. These results are inconsistent with
creased by practice from Block 1 to Block 5.
past studies testing Schmidt's schema theory ~ that
Retention predict increased motor performance with increased
Differences in /CE/ measures were not great KR; however, the results of this study are consistent
enough to elicit significant results; however, results with more recent studies in which increased KR de-
for the VE analysis showed significant feedback dif- creased or exhibited no effect on motor perfor-
mance.12,14
ferences in the retention phase, F(2,26) = 3.62, P<.05
Similar group differences in target accuracy (/CE/)
(Figure 3). Post hoc Dunnett's multiple comparisons
revealed significant differences between 100% KR were observed in the transfer phase, indicating that
( M = 7.10, SD = 4.90) and 50% KR ( M = 3.80, SD = permanent effects of motor learning were enhanced
2.40) and between 100% KR and no KR (M = 3.60, by decreasing the relative frequency to 50% KR,
SD = .70) groups, P<.05. The group differences in
with 100% KR leading to detrimental decreases in
motor learning of a vowel nasalization task. These
VE indicate that variability about the subject's
nasalance mean was reduced by decreasing the rela- results are consistent with Schmidt's schema theo-
ry 11 (and collaborative studies) that predict increased
tive frequency or withholding KR, with 100% KR
motor learning with decreased relative frequency of
leading to detrimental increases in variability.
KR.1,12-14,20
Transfer Results of the variability measure (VE) exhibited a
Results of the/CE/analysis indicated significant different pattern for acquisition and retention. The
group differences for the Transfer phase F(2,26/ -- block difference between Block 1 and Block 5 dur-
4.06, P<.05 (Figure 2). Post hoc Student-Newman- ing acquisition reveals that practice, not amount of
Keuls multiple comparisons revealed significant dif- KR, was the most important indicator for reducing
ferences between 100% KR (M = 25.2, SD = 16.82) variability during motor performance of a vowel
and 50% KR (M = 12.2, SD = 5.64) groups, P<.05. nasalization task. The group difference during reten-
The group differences for/CE/indicate that accura- tion reveals that amount of KR was the most impor-
cy to the nasalance target was enhanced by decreas- tant indicator for reducing variability in motor learn-
ing the relative frequency or withholding KR, with ing: Decreasing the relative frequency to 50% or
100% KR leading to detrimental decreases in accu- withholding KR enhanced motor learning, with
racy. No significant differences were observed for 100% KR leading to detrimental decreases in motor
VE measures. learning of a vowel nasalization task.

Journal of Voice, Vol. 14, No. 2, 2000


THE ROLE OF KNOWLEDGE OF RESULTS IN PERFORMANCE AND LEARNING OF A VOICE 143

Often overlooked in favor of accuracy measures, trol. 15,24 Moon and Jones reported evidence of suc-
reductions in variability are of paramount impor- cess without learning as subjects achieved the novel
tance as indicators of enhanced subject stability, thus task of velopharyngeal closure on first attempts. 22 In
motor learning.Z, 19 All subjects in this study became the context of Folkins, 24 they attributed this success
more consistent with practice during acquisition, but as compatible with the function of forced-variation
during retention, an indicator of motor learning, flexibility during motor learning involving coordina-
those subjects who received 50% or no KR were tive structures. Folkins contends "when a rule sys-
more consistent than subjects receiving 100% KR. tem is set up through a coordinative structure, it is
As stated previously in the discussion of accuracy not necessary to go through an adaptation process to
during transfer, the variability results of this study find novel solutions fitting the coordinative struc-
are consistent with those conducted within the con- ture."24(pl12)
text of Schmidt's schema theory of motor learning.11 In the present study, it is possible that the target of
The following explanations for the present result, 80% lies in the optimal range of nasalance for the
that 100% KR degrades both accuracy and variabili- production of the vowel / 7/, rendering KR futile for
ty of motor performance and learning, have been successful completion of the task; however, too much
postulated:2°,29, 30 (1) KR may become so integrated information, as in the case of 100% KR, was so pow-
into the task that performance may be disrupted erful that it interfered with the optimal movement of
when KR is withdrawn. (2) Increased KR as extrin- the nasalization coordinative structure during both
sic feedback may prevent subjects from attending to performance and learning phases. Similar results
their own intrinsic feedback. Subjects may overfacil- were obtained during experiments in which subjects
itate planning of the next movement and negate en- were provided with erroneous KR that overrode cor-
gaging in self-generated retrieval operations. (3) A rect sensory feedback to diminish target accuracy. 6
high-frequency KR schedule may lead subjects to Remaining in the dynamical context, it is possible
produce excessive response variability during prac- to explain the decreased accuracy and increased vari-
tice or maladaptive short-term corrections, thus pre- ability of producing the v o w e l / a / a t 80% nasalance
venting the development of a stable movement rep- during the transfer phase (Figure 1). Moon and Jones
resentation or motor program. reported that the v o w e l / a / w a s more accurately pro-
Maladaptive corrections occur in situations where duced at 50% closure than the vowel/i/. 22 Although
the motor system is successful at obtaining the target the acoustic nasalance score is once removed from
but not capable of exact precision due to neuromus- closure percents of phototransduction, it appears as
cular variability. Perhaps the target of 80% nasalance if the vowel/a/is more optimally nasalized at a low-
was too precise for the subjects to master, and a er percent than the vowel/i/. These results also are
range of 75% to 85% nasalance would have been consistent with acoustic and perceptual data showing
more accessible. The maladaptive correction theory characteristic patterns of nasality on the basis of
could also explain why the jitter and shimmer values tongue height for the v o w e l s / i / a n d / a / ) ~,32 Due to
in Ferrand remained unresponsive to differences in tongue height, the nasalization o f / i / r e q u i r e d less
the provision of KR. 4 coupling with the nasal cavity than that required for
Although findings that 100% KR leads to de- a nasalized production of the vowel/a/.
creased motor learning are common, 20 less common Finally, a second explanation for the similar be-
are results similar to the present study in which a havior of the No KR to the 50% K R group lies in
group receiving No KR performed as well as a group methodological limitations of the study. The sample
receiving information. Results of accuracy measures could have been too small to elicit the group differ-
in acquisition (Figure 1) and variability measures ences. It is possible that auditory feedback provided
during retention (Figure 2) reveal that groups receiv- enough information to guide the learners to the cor-
ing 50% and No KR performed similarly. rect target; however, control of the auditory feedback
An explanation for the similarity of 50% and No loop was maintained by using only those subjects
KR groups lies in dynamical theories of motor con- who did not receive formal voice or singing training.

Journal of Voice, Vol. 14, No. 2, 2000


144 KIMBERLY STEINHAUER AND JUDITH PRESTON GRAYHACK

Also although group assignment was randomized, it 7. Feltz DL, Landers DM, Becker BJ. A revised meta-analy-
is possible that the more proficient subjects were as- sis of the mental practice literature on motor skill learning.
In: Druckman D, Swet J, eds. Enhancing Human Perfor-
signed to the No KR and 50% KR groups.
mance: Issues, Theories, and Techniques. Washington,
DC: National Academy Press; 1988:61-101.
CONCLUSIONS 8. Lee TD, Swinnen SR Serrien DJ. Cognitive effort and mo-
tor learning. Quest. 1994;46:328-344.
Collective results indicate a relationship between 9. Lee TD, White MA, Carnahan H. On the role of knowl-
relative frequency of KR and motor performance edge of results in motor learning: exploring the guidance
and learning of a vowel nasalization task. An in- hypothesis. J Motor Behav. 1990;22:191-208.
crease in relative frequency of KR to 100% degraded 10. Wulf G, Horstmann G, Choi B. Does mental practice work
both motor performance and learning as indexed by like physical practice without information feedback? Res
Q Exerc Sport. 1995;66:262-267.
accuracy and variability measures. In acquisition, re-
11. Schmidt RA. A schema theory of discrete motor skill
tention, and transfer, both accuracy (/CE/) and vari- learning. Psychol Rev. 1975;82(4):225-260.
ability (VE) diminished as KR increased, with 100% 12. Winstein CJ, Schmidt RA. Reduced frequency of knowl-
KR groups exhibiting the poorest scores; however, edge of results enhances motor skill learning. J Exp Psy-
there were minimal or no differences between chol. 1990;16:677-691.
groups receiving 50% or No KR. The detrimental ef- 13. Wulf G. The learning of generalized motor programs and
motor schemata: Effects of KR relative frequency and
fects of providing a high frequency of KR have been
contextual interference. J Hum Movement Stud. 1992;23:
documented and are consistent within the context of 53-76.
Schmidt's schema theory.20, % Success at executing a 14. Wulf G, Schmidt RA. The learning of generalized motor
voice motor goal with minimal or no KR has been programs: Reducing the relative frequency of knowledge
documented and is consistent within the context of of results enhances memory. J Exp Psychol. 1989;15:748-
dynamical systems theory.22, 24 Further study incor- 757.
15. Kelso JA, Tuller B. A dynamical basis for action systems.
porating auditory masking of trained and untrained
In: Gazzaniga M, ed. Handbook of Cognitive Neuro-
subjects could provide additional evidence for the science. New York, NY: Plenum Press; 1984:321-356.
power of KR during voice motor skill acquisition. 16. Wallace SA. Dynamic pattern perspective of rhythmic
Also, future research to more discretely examine the movement: an introduction. In: Zelaznik HN, ed. Ad-
theoretical issues of the prescriptive and dynamical vances in Motor Learning and Control. Champaign, Ill:
Human Kinetics; 1996:155-194.
systems as they pertain to KR and voice motor con-
17. Newell KM. Motor skill acquisition. Ann Rev Psychol.
trol is warranted. 1991;42:213-237.
18. Swinnen SR Information feedback for motor skill learn-
REFERENCES ing: a review. In: Zelaznik HN, ed. Advances in Motor
Learning and Control. Champaign, Ill: Human Kinetics;
1. Salmoni AW, Schmidt RA, Walter CB. Knowledge of re- 1996:37-66.
sults and motor learning: A review and critical reappraisal.
19. Schoner G, Zanone PG, Kelso JAS. Learning as change of
Psychological Bull, 1984;95:355-386.
coordination dynamics: Theory and experiment. J Motor
2. Schmidt RA. Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral
Behav. 1992;24:29-48.
Emphasis. 2nd ed. Champaign, Ill: Human Kinetics; 1988.
20. Schmidt RA, Bjork RA. New conceptualizations of prac-
3. Buruett TA, Freedland MB, Larson CR, Hain TC. Voice
tice: Common principles in three paradigms suggest new
FO responses to manipulations in pitch feedback. JAcoust
Soc Am. 1998;103:3139-3152. concepts for training. Psychol Sci. 1992;3(4):207-217.
4. Ferrand CT. Effects of practice with and without knowl- 21. Kuehn DR Lemme ML, Banmgartner JM. Neural Bases of
edge of results on jitter and shimmer levels in normally Speech, Hearing and Language. Boston, Mass: College-
speaking women. J Voice. 1995;9:419-423. Hill Press; 1989.
5. Bennet DM, Simmons RW. Effects of precision of knowl- 22. Moon JB, Jones DL. Motor control of velopharyngeal
edge of results on acquisition and retention of a simple structures during vowel production. Cleft Palate-Cranio-
motor skill. Percept Motor Skills. 1984;58:785-786. fac J. 1991;28:267-273.
6. Buckers MJ, Magill RA. The role of task experience and 23. Moon JB, Smith AE, Folkins JW, Lemke JG, Gartlan M.
prior knowledge for detecting invalid augmented feedback Coordination of velopharyngeal muscle activity during po-
while learning a motor skill. Q J Exp Psychol. 1995;48A: sitioning of the soft palate. Cleft Palate-Craniofac J. 1994;
84-97. 31:45-55.

Journal of Voice, Vol. 14, No. 2, 2000


THE ROLE OF KNOWLEDGE OF RESULTS IN PERFORMANCE AND LEARNING OF A VOICE 145

24. Folkins JW. Issues in speech motor control and their rela- mach GE, eds. Tutorials in Motor Neuroscience. Nether-
tion to the speech of individuals with cleft palate. Cleft lands: Kluwer Academic Publishers; 1991.
Palate J. 1985;22(2):106-122. 29. Lee TD, Maraj BKV. Effects of bandwidth goals and
25. Austin SE Movement of the velum during speech and singing bandwidth knowledge of results on motor learning. Res Q
in classically trained singers. J Voice. 1997;11(2):212-221. Exer Sport. 1994;65(3):244-249.
26. Vennard W. Singing: The Mechanism and the Technic. 30. Wulf G, Lee TD, Schmidt RA. Reducing knowledge of re-
New York, NY: Carl Fischer; 1967. sults about relative versus absolute timing: differential ef-
27. Verdolini-Marston K, Burke MK, Lessac A, Glaze L, fects on learning. JMotorBehav. 1994;26:362-369.
Caldwell E. Preliminary study of two methods of treat- 31. House AS, Stevens KN. Analog studies of the nasalization
ment for laryngeal nodules. J Voice. 1995;9:74-85. of vowels. J Speech Hearing Disord. 1956;21:218-232.
28. Schmidt RA. Frequent augmented feedback can degrade 32. Fant G. Acoustic Theory of Speech Production. The
learning: evidence and interpretations. In: Requin J, Stel- Hague: Mouton; 1960.

Journal of Voice, Vol. 14, No. 2, 2000

You might also like