Clinical Research Articles

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

With Standard and Helmut Hildebrandt, PhD 3,4

den et alNeurorehabilitation and Neural Repair © The Author(s) 2010


Reprints and permission: http://www. sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

NNRXXX10.1177/1545968311425927Möd

Occupational
Clinical Research Articles
Therapy During Abstract

Inpatient Stroke
A Randomized Rehabilitation
Controlled Trial Neurorehabilitation and

Comparing 2 Neural Repair


26(5) 463–469
Claudia Mödden, MSC 1, Marion
Interventions for
© The Author(s) 2012
Reprints and permission: http://www.
Behrens, MSC 2, sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI:

Visual Field Loss Iris Damke1, Norbert Eilers,


10.1177/1545968311425927 http://nnr.sagepub.com

MD1, Andreas Kastrup, MD 3,

Background and Purpose. Compensatory and restorative treatments have been developed to improve visual field defects
after stroke. However, no controlled trials have compared these interventions with standard occupational therapy (OT).
Methods. A total of 45 stroke participants with visual field defect admitted for inpatient rehabilitation were randomized to
restorative computerized training (RT) using computer-based stimulation of border areas of their visual field defects or to
a computer-based compensatory therapy (CT) teaching a visual search strategy. OT, in which different compensation
strategies were used to train for activities of daily living, served as standard treatment for the active control group. Each
treatment group received 15 single sessions of 30 minutes distributed over 3 weeks. The primary outcome measures
were visual field expansion for RT, visual search performance for CT, and reading performance for both treatments.
Visual conjunction search, alertness, and the Barthel Index were secondary outcomes. Results. Compared with OT, CT
resulted in a better visual search performance, and RT did not result in a larger expansion of the visual field. Intragroup
pre–post comparisons demonstrated that CT improved all defined outcome parameters and RT several, whereas OT only
improved one. Conclusions. CT improved functional deficits after visual field loss compared with standard OT and may
be the intervention of choice during inpatient rehabilitation. A larger trial that includes lesion location in the analysis is
recommended.

Keywords
visual field defect, stroke, compensatory treatment, restitution training
Two main methods of rehabilitation have been proposed.
Restorative computerized training (RT) is based on visual
detection tasks without execution of saccades, aiming for
Introduction
direct restoration of a portion of the visual field by stimulat
Visual field deficits affect approximately 8% to 25% of all ing preserved neuronal representations of the visual system.
Compensatory therapy (CT) trains strategies to make more
stroke survivors.1-3 These deficits may have debilitating
efficient and systematic saccades into the hemianopic field
effects on mobility, visuospatial orientation, higher percep
to
tual or attentional functions, and reading. Although visual
indirectly expand the usable field of view. Several investiga
rehabilitative interventions suggest that homonymous
tions found that RT may mildly reduce visual field deficits,
hemianopia can be treated to some extent, complete restitu
on average, by less than 2°.6-11 One study did not find any
tion is rarely observed.3-5
reduc tion.12 CT also may enhance compensation for visual
3
field defects to improve functional outcome.13-21 To date, Klinik für Neurologie, Bremen, Germany
4
only 1 study has directly compared RT with CT. In 2 Universität Oldenburg, Oldenburg, Germany
groups of chronic brain-damaged patients with unilateral Corresponding Author:
field defects, Helmut Hildebrandt, Klinik für Neurologie, Züricher Str 40,
Bremen 28325, Germany
Email: helmut.hildebrandt@uni-oldenburg.de
1
Rehabilitationszentrum Oldenburg, Oldenburg,
Germany 2Hedon Klinik Lingen, Lingen, Germany
464 Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair 26(5)

Assessed for eligibility (n = 47)

Excluded (n = 0)
Not meeting inclusion criteria
(n = 0)
Declined to participate (n = 0)

Randomized (n = 45 + 2)
analysis: n = 0 (n = 15)
Allocated to intervention All patients received the
CT (n = 15 + 1) allocated intervention.
One patient was unable to get
Allocated to intervention RT the allocated intervention
(n = 15 + 1) due to health impairment and
One patient was unable to get was replaced by next patient.
the allocated intervention
due to health impairment and Analyzed (n = 15)
was replaced by next patient. Excluded from
analysis: n = 0
Analyzed (n = 15)
Excluded from
analysis: n = 0
Analyzed (n = 15)
Excluded from Allocated to intervention OT

Figure 1. Flow chart of recruitment and retention. Abbreviations: RT, restorative computerized training; CT, compensatory therapy;
OT, occupational therapy.

Roth et al16 showed that CT was superior to RT by Patients


decreasing search time at the follow-up investigation and
by improving eye movement strategies. A total of 45 consecutive admitted rehabilitation inpatients
A recent systematic Cochrane review argues for further with homonymous hemianopia with a posterior cerebral
studies to compare compensatory and restitutive interven artery stroke participated in this prospective randomized
con trolled, single-blind, single-center treatment study.
tions with a placebo control or usual care.22 According to
Patients
this review, there is only limited evidence for
compensatory treat ment and no evidence that restitutive with visual neglect, eye-movement disorders, neuropsycho
training improves out comes for hemianopic patients. In logical disorders like aphasia, dysexecutive syndromes,
addition, almost all studies focused on chronic patients and memory deficits, or higher order motor impairments like
included patients with hemi anopia that was a consequence apraxia were excluded. All patients gave written informed
of various organic causes. A systematic treatment offered consent. The study was approved by the ethics committee
early in the recovery process may be more effective than in of the Carl von Ossietzky University of Oldenburg.
the late phase, and results may differ for patients with
stroke and traumatic brain injury. Therefore, the aim of this Study Design
prospective, randomized controlled trial was to compare
the efficacies of CT and RT with that of standard Patients were randomly assigned to receive either CT, RT,
occupational therapy (OT) on visual field defects and on or OT. Randomization by throwing dice and allocation
visual search during inpatient stroke rehabilitation. took place before starting with the initial assessment of
neuropsy chological tests (Figure 1). All patients were
recruited and assigned to treatment groups by a
Methods neuropsychologist. The same neuropsychologist also tested
the patients before (time point t1) and after (time point t2)
the treatment and was not blinded to the type of training.
The training itself was per formed by a psychological Clinical Investigations
assistant or by the occupational therapists not involved in At t1 and t2, all patients underwent neuropsychological
the study, and they provided the test results at t1 and t2. tests, including visual scanning, an attention test, and a
All participants moreover got inpa tient standard perimetry
rehabilitation treatment encompassing physio therapy,
speech therapy, and health education.
Mödden et al 465
geneously but were clustered in the blind side. Thus, the
exploration in the hemianopic field was further promoted.
test from the Test Battery of Attentional Performance,23 the The patients had to respond (by pressing a key) when the
latter having a sensitivity and specificity for visual field targeted icon was perceived in the circle. The program con
defects similar to the Goldmann perimetry.24 For these tests, tained several difficulty levels. In levels 1 to 20, all lines
head movements were prevented by a chin rest in front of were completely filled with symbols, whereas there were
the monitor. Participants were instructed not to move their omissions in the rows of symbols in levels 21 to 30 to
eyes and were monitored by the examiner. For assessment increase the difficulty.
of visual exploration, we used the search tasks of the Occupational therapy. After a standardized assessment
Behavioural Inattention Test, which include line, stars, and of daily living activities, the therapy consisted of
letter cancella tion tests.25 Reading ability was assessed by individually adapted stimulation of daily activity tasks to
the standardized texts of the Wechsler Memory Test.26 compensate via eye-, head-, and body movements. These
Independence in activities of daily living was scored by compensation strategies included aspects of spatial and
nurses with the German Extended Barthel Index (EBI).27 body perception, searching or arranging objects, pen and
paper searching task, reading maps or newspapers, and
self-care activities. The participant was instructed to
Procedures
perform systematic eye movements toward the lost visual
The daily duration of each session for all groups was 30 field. The interventions were carried out in the treatment
minutes (also OT was standardized on 30-minute sessions), rooms, on the wards, in a kitchen or a bathroom, outside in
and each participant received a total of 15 sessions. The the park, or in a supermar ket. Patients receiving RT and
participants in both PC-based therapy groups were seated CT did not receive OT in the context of their standard
60 cm away from the screen (19-in. monitor) and had to rehabilitation treatment.
perform the tasks binocularly. As during testing, the head
was fixed by a chin rest. The sessions were always con
trolled by the assistant to make sure that the instructions Statistical Analysis
were followed. Based on the distribution of the visual field test of the TAP,
Restitution therapy. A therapy-integrated perimeter pro we defined a minimum reduction of 3 omissions, which cor
gram (provided by Teltra company) created the exact responds to an expansion of 1° over the whole visual field
measurement of the individual visual field border. Using (upper and lower quadrant) as a meaningful effect. Previous
that measurement, a series of colored targets appeared on a work has shown that the standard deviation of visual field
blue screen anywhere at 1 of 10 positions on the border testing after posterior brain artery stroke is about 5.9.28,29
line. A randomly presented first fixation target (a rotating For a given α and β error of .05, this leads to 15 patients per
arrow) announced the second stimulus target in the hemi group. Based on the results of the same studies, we know
anopic border zone (basic principle of covert attention that the standard deviation for omissions in the visual scan
shift). The patients were instructed to respond (by press ing ning test of TAP is 9.97. 28,29 Because the search array of the
a key) to each stimulus target (colored and flickering scanning test is symmetrically organized in 5 lines and 5
frames, beams, and spots) as soon it was perceived. The columns, we defined a reduction of 5 omissions (ie, of 1
program contained no adaptive difficulty levels. Eye contralesional column) as a clinically meaningful end point.
movements were not allowed, and this was controlled by For the already mentioned error level, this also leads to a
the assistant. value of 15 patients per group. We were not aware of previ
Compensatory therapy. The “Exploration” task (from ous relevant studies for the Behavioural Inattention Test.25
RehaCom, provided by HASOMED GmbH, Magdeburg, We therefore decided that 15 patients per group should be
Germany) was adapted individually according to the side of included in our study. To compare the effects of the treat
the hemianopia. On a dark background, different bright ments, we subtracted the performance of the patients before
stimuli arranged in rows and columns were presented. A and after the treatment. To analyze the group differences,
ring (diameter of 2 cm) moved line by line (interlaced) on a we performed Mann-Whitney U tests for these difference
matrix unit over the field. The participant was instructed to scores comparing RT/OT, CT/OT, and RT/CT. For the sec
follow the ring (starting point to an outmost fixation in the ondary outcome parameters, the significance level was set
blind side) by eye movements and to identify a critical tar to P < .0166 (P < .05/3 domains: attention, conjunction
geted icon. The targets were not always distributed homo search, and activities of daily living) because of multiple
testing. Intragroup comparisons of treatment effects were stroke. At baseline, the 3 groups did not differ in demo
done using the Friedman test. graphic and neuropsychological measures. Demographic
data are summarized in Table 1.
All patients were highly motivated to improve the visual
Results performance, and no one dropped out because of problems
Patients were recruited on average about 4 weeks after their
466 Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair 26(5)

Table 1. Summary of Demographic, Lesion, and Impairment Data of the 3 Treatment Groups a

Restitution Training Compensatory Training Occupational Therapy

Sex Male: 10, Female: 5 Male: 9; Female: 6 Male: 7; Female: 8


Primary stroke localization Left: 7 (o = 3; tm = 3;ph = Left: 5 (o = 2;to = 2;tm = 1) Left: 5 (to = 1; tm = 1; ph = 1)
(reported by the 1) Right: 8 (o = 4; to = 2; Right: 10 (o = 4; to = 1; tm = Right: 10 (o = 4; to = 2; tm =
neuroradiologists) tm = 2) 4; ph = 1) 4)
QA)
Visual field defect (hemianopia: HA, quadrantanopia: HA: 10; QA: 5 HA: 12; QA: 3 HA: 10; QA: 5
(number in parentheses => than 2° sparing 3/15 (2/15) Patients with less
Visual field sparing in hemianopicsame number after the 3/15 (2/15) Patients with less than 2° sparing
patients before treatment treatment) than 2° sparing plus 1 (0)
3/15 (2/15) Patients with less patient with no sparing

Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD


Age, y 58.3 11.4 57.1 8.3 59.0 11.1 Time since onset, wk 4.7 4.9 4.3 Cancellation tasks, BIT; omissions 15.3 23.3 6.5 7.4
5.4 7.2 Reading, omissions 1.0 2.5 1.6 2.7 0.9 1.1 TAP Alertness without cueing, ms 304.2 80.8 383.7 205.2 308.1 58.6 TAP
Conjunction Search, omissions 9.1 9.0 10.7 6.7 10.3 5.6 Extended Barthel Index 59.2 7.0 59.3 5.0 59.9 3.4
Abbreviations: o, occipital; to, temporo-occipital; tm, temporomedial; ph, parahippocampal; SD, standard deviation; BIT, Behavioural Inattention Test;
TAP, Testing Battery for Attention Performance.
a
No significant difference between groups at baseline.

Table 2. Significant Improvement per Training Group (Only Intragroup Pre/Post Comparisons)

Restitutional Training Compensatory Treatment Occupational Treatment


Yes (3.9 ± 4.9)a,b Yes (2.9 ± 4.0)a No (1.3 ± 4.7)a
Visual field enlargement (TAP,Visual Field Assessment)

Cancellation tasks of the BIT (omissions) Yes (5.3 ± 10.5) Yes (5.4 ± 5.2) No (2.3 ± 5.0) Reading performance (errors) No (0.9 ±
2.4) Yes (0.9 ± 1.1) No (0.7 ± 1.0) Attention (TAP, Alertness) Yes (28.5 ± 56.9) Yes (77.8 ± 112.9) No (−13.3 ± 112.7) Visual
conjunction search (TAP, visual scanning) No (2.7 ± 5.1) Yes (7.0 ± 5.0) No (3.5 ± 6.8) Extended Barthel Index Yes (1.5 ± 2.8) Yes
(3.3 ± 3.6) Yes (1.8 ± 2.0)
Abbreviations: BIT, Behavioural Inattention Test; TAP, Testing Battery for Attention Performance.
a
Number of additional detected stimuli during testing.
b
The figures in parentheses show the difference score between t2 (after treatment) and t1 (before treatment) and the standard deviation for this differ
ence score. Positive values indicate improvements, negative values deteriorations.
not after OT (P = .316).
Visual search performance. At t1, omissions in search
(BIT, cancellation tasks) did not differ between the groups
in compliance, but some patients complained that RT was (RT/OT, P = .444; CT/OT, P = .554; RT/CT, P = .752).
exhausting and led to sensitive eyes. There was a significant improvement of performance in
visual search for CT and OT (P = .048), but no significant
benefit for RT could be found in comparison with OT.
Primary End Points There were also no significant differences in improvement
Visual field expansion. The mean visual field size (TAP com
visual field—omissions) at t1 did not differ between the paring RT with CT. Significant intragroup changes compar
groups (RT/OT, P = .852; CT/OT, P = .506; RT/CT, P = ing t1 and t2 were only observed for RT (P = .005) and CT
.589), and there was no significant group difference for the (P = .003) but not for OT (Table 2).
increase of the visual field after treatment. However, the Reading performance. At t1, errors in reading did not dif
intragroup comparisons (Table 2) showed a significant field fer between the groups: RT/OT, P = .437; CT/OT, P = .781;
expansion after both RT (P = .003) and CT (P = .013) but RT/CT, P = .257. Compared with OT, neither RT nor CT
Mödden et al 467
e
Visual expansion c

improvements after RT (P = .027), CT (P = .005), and OT


16
(P = .003).
n

a whether these groups differed in our


8
m

fr

p 4
n

i
t

0
primary and secondary outcome
no improvement improved parameters and found that improved
e

v
EBI visual search was the only parameter
o

r To analyze the association between that differed significantly between the


treatment effects and ADL on a more groups (Z = −3.001; P = .003). Patients
p

global level, we divided the total group with an improvement in ADL also
m

Search performance
Reading performane of patients into those who improved in showed significantly improved search
12
ADL performance (n = 27) and those performance and
who did not (n = 18). We then analyzed
when comparing RT/OT, CT/OT, and RT/CT, the
Figure 2. Patients with improvement in the EBI also improved in
intragroup comparisons (Table 2) demonstrated significant
visual search performance significantly but not in field expansion.
Abbreviation: EBI, extended Barthel Index. vice versa (Figure 2).
Intercorrelation of improvements in primary outcome
param eter with secondary outcome parameter. The
significantly reduced reading errors. There were also no dif nonparametric correlational analysis of improvement in
ferences between RT and CT. In contrast, the intragroup the different tasks for all patients showed that visual field
comparisons comparing t1 and t2 showed a significant expansion was not significantly correlated with any other
reduction in reading errors after CT (P = .016) but not after improvement, but visual search performance correlated
RT or OT (Table 2). with improvement in the Barthel Index (P = .004), in
There were no significant differences of the treatment attention performance (P = .016), and in conjunction
effects in reading speed (measurement in words per sec search (P = .046).
ond). The intragroup comparisons comparing t1 and t2
showed a significant improvement of reading perfor mance
Discussion
after CT: from 64.2 to 51.8 s (P = .006) but not after RT or
OT (RT from 86.6 to 101.5 s and OT from 58.8 to 49.4 s). We compared the efficacy of RT and CT for visual field
loss in stroke patients in a placebo-controlled randomized
trial. Whereas previous studies have shown that both
Secondary End Points treatment strategies can principally improve lost visual
Attention. At t1, the alertness test (TAP Phasic functions at least to a small extent, our results demonstrate
Alertness) did not differ between the groups: RT/OT, P = that CT appears to be superior to RT and thus should be
.443; CT/OT, P = .547; RT/CT, P = .330. After Bonferroni the treat
correction, nei ther CT nor RT led to an improvement in ment modality of choice in this patient group. To date,
attention perfor mance compared with OT, although the several studies have reported an enlargement of the visual
comparison between CT and OT was almost significant (P field in hemianopic patients after RT.6-11,30 Yet none of
= .020). Intragroup comparisons comparing t1 and t2 these studies used an active control group. We also found a
showed that only RT (P = .033) and CT (P = .001) small increase of the visual field of about 1° to 1.5° after
significantly improved alertness (Table 2). CT and RT in this study, but such a small enlargement of
Conjunction search. Omissions in the exploration task the visual field is unlikely to improve functional outcome
(TAP visual scanning) at t1 did not differ between the of the patient groups. In addition, the improvement was
groups: RT/OT, P = .454; CT/OT, P = .868; RT/CT, P = only found in the intragroup comparisons of RT and CT.
.289. Compared with OT, both the neuropsychological Because of the nature of our study design, the reason for
treatments did not result in superior improvements, and the the visual field enlargement cannot be explained
improvement of the CT compared with the RT group did adequately. It might be attributable to the responsiveness
not meet the defined significance level after Bonferroni of neurons adjacent to the lesioned visual cortex, a
correc phenomenon that has been shown in human and animal
tion (P = .023). For intragroup comparisons only CT (P = studies.8,31 On the other hand, patients might also have
.001) led to a significant improvement (Table 2). Activities learned to focus their atten tion toward the hemifield and
of daily living. The EBI score at t1 did not differ between thereby improve the size of their visual field
groups: RT/OT, P = .495; CT/OT, P = .916; RT/CT, P = conceptualized as “an unspecific increase of alertness in
.649. Although no significant treatment effects were found unattended areas.”11,17 In this respect it seems noteworthy
that a small but significant enlargement of the visual field and also to OT in visual conjunction search. Roth et al16
was found in this and other studies using com pensatory also found that CT led to a higher improvement in visual
training strategies.14 Moreover, our RT and CT groups conjunction
improved significantly in alertness.
With respect to visual search, CT was superior to RT
468 Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair 26(5)
theoretically, the chances for restitution might be greater in
patients with cortical lesions not extending deeply into the
search than RT. The study of Roth et al involved patients in optic radiation.
a chronic stage of hemianopia caused by various diseases.
Our study results, therefore, extend their findings to a Summary
homoge neous group of first-ever stroke patients treated
In conclusion, visual field deficits improve mildly after
relatively early in their recovery process.
both RT and CT compared with usual OT training.
The specific PC program we used for CT (“Exploration” However,
task from RehaCom, HASOMED) forces the patients to use
for most of the outcome parameters analyzed in this study,
a particular search strategy associated with improved eye
CT was superior to RT and thus should be further evaluated
movement patterns.16 Hence the reduction of omissions in as the modality of choice in the early phase of treatment for
visual search in our CT group may be interpreted as a result stroke.
of an improved top-down compensation strategy.19
Whereas some previous studies reported increased Acknowledgments
reading abilities after RT,7,32 we and others did not observe We thank all patients for participating. The authors thank
such an effect.30 In our study, only CT had a small but TELTRA (Neurological Department, University of Bochum,
significant effect on reading. Generalization of CT to Bochum, Germany) and HASOMED (GmbH, Magdeburg,
reading performance has not been documented.16,17 Our Germany) for support in providing their programs.
result might be attributable to the structure of CT used in
this study. As already mentioned, the Exploration task of Declaration of Conflicting Interests
the RehaCom forces the patients to fix on an outermost The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with
starting point on the left, where a circular cur sor starts respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this
moving rightward, item by item, until it reaches the article.
outermost item on the right and then jumps back to the next
outermost item on the left in the next line. This is highly Funding
simi The author(s) received no financial support for the research,
lar to the eye movements during reading and this similarity authorship, and/or publication of this article.
in top-down control might be the reason our CT group also
improved in reading. References
We used the EBI to analyze the impact of our treatments 1. Kerkhoff G. Neurovisual rehabilitation: recent develop ments
on the activities of daily living because the Barthel Index and future directions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry.
focuses exclusively on basic skills like transfer, dressing, 2000;68:691-706.
and eating among others. The EBI includes some cognitive 2. Gilhotra JS, Mitchell P, Healey PR, Cumming RG, Currie J.
and percep Homonymous visual field defects and stroke in an older popu
tual items, so we anticipated that it might be more sensitive lation. Stroke. 2002;33:2417-2420.
for the functional problems of our patient group. The 3. Zhang X, Kedar S, Lynn MJ, Newman NJ, Biousse V. Natural
pretreat ment EBI scores for all groups were high, however, history of homonymous hemianopia. Neurology. 2006;66:
which may have created a ceiling effect. In a post hoc 901-905.
evaluation only improvements in visual search were 4. Lane AR, Smith DT, Schenk T. Clinical treatment options for
associated with increases in the EBI scores, whereas the
patients with homonymous visual field defects. Clin Ophthal
visual field enlarge ments were not associated with ADL
mol. 2008;2:93-102.
improvements. The association between the increase in
5. Warren M. Pilot study on activities of daily living limita tions
ADL score and improve ment in visual search underscores
in adults with hemianopia. Am J Occup Ther. 2009;63:
the impact of compensation of visual field defects for
626-633.
functional outcome.
6. Kasten E, Bunzenthal U, Sabel, BA. Visual field recovery
Limitations of our study design include the relatively
after vision restoration therapy (VRT) is independent of eye
small sample size of each group and no follow-up data
move ments: an eye tracker study. Behav Brain Res.
beyond the inpatient stay to test for stability of the
2006;175:18-26.
treatment effects. Although the number of treatment
7. Bergsma DP, van der Wildt G. Visual training of cerebral
sessions was realistic for postacute inpatient treatment,
blindness patients gradually enlarges the visual field. Br J
more sessions might be needed to achieve a restitutive
Ophthalmol. 2010;94:88-96.
effect. Finally, the location of the lesions (cortical vs white
matter) was not obtained in this study. At least 8. Marshall RS, Ferrera JJ, Barnes A, et al. Brain activity asso
ciated with stimulation therapy of the visual border zone in apy. Clin Rehabil. 2010;24:1027-1035.
hemianopic stroke patients. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 10. Gall C, Müller I, Gudlin J, et al. Vision- and health-related
2008;22:136-144. quality of life before and after vision restoration training in
9. Marshall RS, Chmayssani M, O’Brien KA, Handy C, Green cerebrally damaged patients. Restor Neurol Neurosci.
stein VC. Visual field expansion after visual restoration ther 2008;26:341-353.
Mödden et al 469
27. Jansa J, Pogacnik T, Gompertz P. An evaluation of the
Extended Barthel Index with acute ischemic stroke patients.
11. Poggel DA, Müller I, Kasten E, Bunzenthal U, Sabel B. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2004;18:37-41.
Subjec tive and objective outcome measures of computer-based 28. Hildebrandt H, Benetz J, Schröder A, Sachsenheimer W.
vision restoration training. NeuroRehabilitation. Behandlungserfolge bei Gesichtsfeldausfall und Neglect
2010;27:173-187. durch kompensatorisches Training und sensible Anbahnung.
12. Sabel BA, Kenkel S, Kasten E. Vision restoration therapy Neurol Rehabil. 1998;4:132-136.
(VRT) efficacy as assessed by comparative perimetric analy 29. Hildebrandt H, Brasse M, Pfefferkorn D, von der Fecht A,
sis and subjective questionnaires. Restor Neurol Neurosci. Sachsenheimer W. Intersensorische Kombinationsbehandlung
2004;22:399-420. bei unilateralen Sehstörungen: Eine Replikation ihrer Effek
13. Pambakian AL, Wooding DS, Patel N, Morland AB, Kennard tivität und eine Spezifikation ihres Aufbaus. Neurol Rehabil.
C, Mannan SK. Scanning the visual world: a study of patients 1999;5:328-334.
with homonymous hemianopia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychia 30. Jobke S, Kasten E, Sabel BA. Vision restoration through
try. 2000;69:751-759. extrastriate stimulation in patients with visual field defects: a
14. Pambakian AL, Mannan SK, Hodgson TL, Kennard C. double-blind and randomized experimental study. Neuroreha
Saccadic visual search training: a treatment for patients with bil Neural Repair. 2009;23:246-255.
homony mous hemianopia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 31. Schweigart G, Eysel UT. Activity-dependent receptive field
2004;75: 1443-1448. changes in the surround of adult cat visual cortex lesions. Eur
15. Bolognini N, Rasi F, Coccia M, Ladavas E. Visual search J Neurosci. 2002;15:1585-1596.
improvement in hemianopic patients after audio-visual stimu 32. Schlueter D, Schulz P, Kenkel S, Raton B, Romano JG. Func
lation. Brain. 2005;128:2830-2842. tional improvement after visual rehabilitation for patients
16. Roth T, Sokolov AN, Messias A, et al. Comparing explorative with homonymous visual field defects. NovaVision.
saccade and flicker training in hemianopia: a randomized con http://www.
trolled study. Neurology. 2009;72:324-331. vision-impairment.com/forschungsstudien/29.htm. Accessed
17. Lane AR, Smith DT, Ellison A, Schenk T. Visual exploration October 19, 2011.
training is no better than attention training for treating hemi
anopia. Brain. 2010;133:1-12.
18. Keller I, Lefin-Rank G. Improvement of visual search after
audiovisual exploration training in hemianopic patients. Neu
rorehabil Neural Repair. 2010;24:666-673.
19. Mannan SK, Pambakian AL, Kennard C. Compensatory
strategies following visual search training in patients with
homonymous hemianopia: an eye movement study. J Neurol.
2010;257:1812-1821.
20. Kerkhoff G. Evidenzbasierte Verfahren in der neurovisuellen
Rehabilitation. Neurol Rehabil. 2010;16:82-90.
21. Trauzettel-Klosinski S. Rehabilitation for visual disorders. J
Neuroophthalmol. 2010;30:73-84.
22. Pollock A, Hazelton C, Henderson CA, Angilley J, Dhillon
B, Langhorne P, Livingstone K, Munro FA, Orr H, Rowe FJ,
Shahani U. Interventions for visual field defects in patients
with stroke. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2011 Oct 5;(10):
CD008388.
23. Zimmermann P, Fimm B. Testbatterie zur Aufmerksam
keitsprüfung (TAP). Herzogenrath, Germany: Psytest; 2002. 24.
Hildebrandt H. Sensitivität des TAP-Neglekttests bei der
Erfassung postchiasmatischer Gesichtsfeldausfälle: eine
Vergleichsstudie zur Goldmannperimetrie. Neurol Rehabil.
2006;12:270-277.
25. Wilson, BA Cockburn J, Halligan PW. Behavioral Inattention
Test (BIT). Hants, UK: Thames Valley Test Company; 1987. 26.
Wechsler, D. Wechsler Memory Test (WMS-R, revised version,
1987). Göttingen, Germany: Huber; 2004.

You might also like