Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 25

Journal Pre-proof

The anticipatory dopamine response in addiction: A common


neurobiological underpinning of gambling disorder and substance
use disorder?

Jakob Linnet

PII: S0278-5846(19)30527-5
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2019.109802
Reference: PNP 109802

To appear in: Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry

Received date: 29 June 2019


Revised date: 26 October 2019
Accepted date: 30 October 2019

Please cite this article as: J. Linnet, The anticipatory dopamine response in addiction: A
common neurobiological underpinning of gambling disorder and substance use disorder?,
Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry(2018), https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.pnpbp.2019.109802

This is a PDF file of an article that has undergone enhancements after acceptance, such
as the addition of a cover page and metadata, and formatting for readability, but it is
not yet the definitive version of record. This version will undergo additional copyediting,
typesetting and review before it is published in its final form, but we are providing this
version to give early visibility of the article. Please note that, during the production
process, errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers
that apply to the journal pertain.

© 2018 Published by Elsevier.


Journal Pre-proof

of
The anticipatory dopamine response in addiction: A common

ro
neurobiological underpinning of gambling disorder and substance

-p
use disorder?
re
Jakob Linnet1*
1
lP

Clinic for Gambling- and Binge Eating Disorder, Department of Occupational and

Environmental Medicine, Odense University Hospital, Denmark.


na
ur
Jo

Keywords: Dopamine; Gambling Disorder; Substance Use Disorder; Reward; Anticipation

*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Jakob Linnet (jlinnet@mac.com)
Journal Pre-proof

Abstract

The dopamine system is associated with reward processes in both gambling disorder and

substance use disorder, and may constitute a common neurobiological underpinning in addiction.

The present review examines differences and similarities of dopaminergic reward processes in

gambling disorder and substance use disorder. First, it is suggested that baseline binding poten-

tials of the dopamine system may not be a common pathway, since substance use disorder is as-

sociated with reduced binding potentials, whereas gambling disorder is not. Second, it is sug-

of
gested that dopaminergic reward response may be not a common pathway, since substance use

ro
disorder is associated with a blunted dopamine response toward drugs, while conflicting findings

-p
of reward response has been reported in gambling disorder. Instead, it is suggested that the an-
re
ticpatory dopamine response may constitute a common underpinning of gambling disorder and

substance use disorder, which may be associated with increased dopamine activity in both types
lP

of disorder, and does not involve the intake of substances. The notion of the anticipatory dopa-
na

mine response as a common underpinning of gambling disorder and substance use disorder is

consistent with dopaminergic models of addictions such as the incentive-sensitization model, the
ur

ingrative neurodevelopmental model of vulnerability towards addiction and the reward predic-
Jo

tion error model.

2
Journal Pre-proof

Introduction.

The dopamine system is associated with addictive behavior both with regard to drug addic-

tion and behavioral addictions such as gambling disorder [1]. While the role of the dopamine

system in gambling disorder is currently not fully understood [2] there are several parallels of

dopaminergic dysfunctions in gambling disorder and substance use disorder including reward

anticipation, reward response, reward uncertainty and motivational aspects of gambling [1, 3, 4] .

For instance, Linnet [4] suggested that the distinction between reward anticipation, defined as

of
dopaminergic processes occurring prior to reward or omission/loss, and reward response, defined

ro
as dopaminergic processes occurring after reward or omission/loss, may have important implica-

-p
tions for the understanding of dopaminergic dysfunctions in gambling disorder and addiction.
re
The distinction between reward anticipation and reward response is consistent with dopaminer-

gic models of reward prediction (reward prediction vs. reward prediction error) [5] and incentive
lP

sensitization (“wanting” vs. “liking”) [6-10].


na

The present review aims at investigating similarities and differences in dopaminergic dys-

functions in gambling disorder and substance use disorder. Specifically, it is proposed that do-
ur

paminergic dysfunctions in reward anticipation may constitute a common neurobiological under-


Jo

pinning of gambling disorder and substance use disorder. Other common underpinnings may

exist as well. The review first examines evidence from positron emission tomography (PET)

studies of gambling disorder, then evidence from functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging

(fMRI) studies. Reward anticipation is discussed in relation to theories of dopaminergic dysfunc-

tions in addiction, and possible implications are outlined.

3
Journal Pre-proof

Evidence from PET studies

The dopamine system is sensitive to behavioral stimulation related to monetary reward [11-13].

While the use of substances such as cocaine is associated with a dopamine increase throughout

the striatum, the ventral striatum is specifically involved in reward expectation and monitoring of

reward [14, 15], and this region appears to be central to gambling disorder [16-19]. Evidence

from the animal literature also supports the involvement of the ventral striatum in reward seeking

behavior [20-22].

of
Dopaminergic dysfunctions in gambling disorder differ from the substance use disorder liter-

ro
ature in at least three important ways. Firstly, substance use disorder sufferers have significantly

-p
lower baseline binding potentials than healthy controls [23, 24]. These findings are inconsistent
re
with PET studies of gambling disorder, meaning that there are no significant differences in base-
lP

line binding potentials between gambling disorder sufferers and healthy controls [18, 19, 25].

Secondly, evidence from PET studies show that substance use disorder sufferers have a
na

“blunted” dopamine response to drugs [24, 26-28]. These findings are inconsistent with PET
ur

studies of gambling disorder, which show no significant differences in dopaminergic changes

during gambling between gambling disorder sufferers and healthy controls [16, 29].
Jo

Thirdly, PET-studies on dopaminergic changes when winning report mixed results with re-

gard to gambling disorder. Steeves et al. [17] found a significantly higher dopamine increase

from winning in Parkinson’s disease patients with gambling disorder compared to Parkinson’s

Disease patients without gambling disorder. Joutsa et al. [16] found no differences in dopamine

increase from winning between gambling disorder sufferers and healthy controls. And Linnet et

al. [18] found that gambling disorder sufferers had a significantly higher dopamine increase

when losing compared with healthy controls. While lower baseline binding potentials, as well as
4
Journal Pre-proof

other factors, may have confounded the dopamine increase of Parkinson’s disease patients with

gambling disorder in the Steeves et al. study [17], the implications of dopamine increase when

winning in gambling disorder is still not fully understood.

It has been suggested that reward uncertainty is a key factor in gambling disorder [1, 3].

Therefore, it is possible that dopaminergic coding of uncertainty could account for neurobiologi-

cal dysfunctions in gambling disorder. For instance, Linnet et al. [30] found an increased dopa-

mine response associated with decision-making uncertainty in Gambling Disorder on the Iowa

of
Gambling Task (IGT). Gambling disorder sufferers who made an equal number of “good” and

ro
“bad” decisions had higher dopamine increase than gambling disorder sufferers who consistently

-p
made good or bad decisions. In other words, uncertainty of gains or losses was associated with
re
dopamine increase in gambling disorder, while certainty of gains and losses were not.
lP

Dopaminergic coding of reward uncertainty, rather than actual reward (i.e., overall winning

or losing), could possibly account for the differences in results reported by Steeves et al. [17] and
na

Linnet et al. [18]. Steeves et al. [17] used a rigged task, which randomly resulted in either a win
ur

or a loss, but always produced an overall gain. Linnet et al. [18] used the IGT, where uncertainty

in outcome varied depending on card selections. In both studies, it is possible that the variation
Jo

(i.e., uncertainty of gains and losses) rather than the overall outcome (i.e., winning or losing),

reinforced the dopamine response in gambling disorder sufferers. Therefore, reward anticipation

associated with dopaminergic coding of uncertainty could explain the reinforcement of continued

gambling despite losses (‘chasing one’s losses’) often seen in gambling disorder. Conversely, it

may be less likely that the dopamine response toward of reward receipt can explain the rein-

forcement of continued gambling despite losses, since reward receipt (as well as omission or

losses) is associated with a blunted dopamine response. Furthermore, since losses are much for

5
Journal Pre-proof

frequent than gains in gambling, it seems unlikely that the dopamine response to reward receipt

could reinforce the behavior. However, in order to compare reward anticipation in gambling dis-

order and substance use disorder, it is necessary to consider important methodological differ-

ences in PET studies of gambling and drug use.

Reward anticipation and reward response in PET studies

of
PET studies have used drug challenge paradigms to investigate dopaminergic reward re-

ro
sponses in substance use disorder. Here, the reaction to drug intake is measured. Drug challenge

paradigms show a blunted dopamine increase in substance use disorder sufferers compared to
-p
healthy controls [24]. The reduced dopamine response toward reward receipt has been demon-
re
strated across different types of drug addictions (e.g., alcohol use disorder, cocaine use disorder
lP

and amphetamine use disorder) [31]. However, it might not relate to all types of drug addiction.
na

In contrast, PET studies of reward anticipation in substance use disorder have used cue expo-

sure or cue-elicited paradigms that compare binding potentials toward drug related cues with
ur

binding potentials toward neutral cues without association to drugs. Evidence from cue exposure
Jo

paradigms suggest that cocaine use disorder sufferers have a significant dopamine increase to-

ward exposure to drug cues compared with exposure to neutral stimuli [32, 33]. These findings

are consistent with the hypothesis of an increased anticipatory dopamine response toward reward

in substance use disorder.

A limitation in the PET studies of gambling disorder is that they might fall somewhere be-

tween a drug challenge paradigm and a cue exposure paradigm. Methodologically, gambling

studies may resemble drug challenge paradigms (because they investigate the response to gam-

6
Journal Pre-proof

bling), but neurobiologically they may involve both reward anticipation and reward response,

due to the immediacy of uncertainty and outcome in gambling. Therefore, a possibly “blunted”

dopamine increase related to reward response may be confounded by a possibly higher dopamine

increase related to reward anticipation. Such an immediate interaction between reward anticipa-

tion and reward response is not detectable with the temporal resolution of PET, and it is therefore

important to include evidence from functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) studies to

elucidate the temporal relation between reward anticipation and reward response in gambling

of
disorder.

ro
Evidence from fMRI studies
-p
re
The evidence from fMRI studies support the involvement of reward anticipation in gambling
lP

and gambling disorder. [34-37]. Abler et al. [34] found increased Blood Oxygen Level Depend-

ent (BOLD) activation in the nucleus accumbens of healthy controls in anticipation of reward in
na

a gambling task. Preuschoff et al. [38] found that reward probability was linearly associated with
ur

immediate anticipatory BOLD activation in in the ventral striatum of healthy controls, while un-
Jo

certainty was quardratically associated with late anticipatory BOLD activation. In other words:

the late anticipatory BOLD activation was largest toward stimuli with maximum uncertainty (P =

0.5) and smallest toward stimuli with maximum certainty (P = 0.0 and P = 1.0).

Looking at gambling disorder Reuter et al. [36] found a significantly lower BOLD response

in the ventral striatum toward winning in gambling disorder sufferers compared to healthy con-

trols. Furthermore, gambling disorder sufferers showed a significant negative correlation be-

tween the BOLD activation and severity in gambling symptoms. Van Holst et al. [37] found that

gambling disorder sufferers had a significantly increased anticipatory BOLD activation in the
7
Journal Pre-proof

bilateral ventral striatum and left orbitofrontal cortex toward gain-related expected value com-

pared to healthy controls. There were no differences in BOLD activation toward reward re-

sponse.

A meta review by Luijten et al. [39], however, found significantly decreased anticipatory

BOLD activation in the ventral striatum of substance dependence sufferers during performance

on the Monetary Incentive Delay Task (MIDT). Similar findings are reported by Balodis et al.

[40] in gambling disorder sufferers. The differences in findings between van Holst et al. [37] and

of
Balodis et al. [40] are complex and may relate to anticipatory differences of guessing in contrast

ro
to formulation of prospects. The study by van Holst et al. [37] used a modified version of a

-p
guessing task, and the anticipatory processes could therefore be associated with outcome uncer-
re
tainty. The object of the MIDT [40] is to identify winning and losing cues, and to win (or avoid
lP

losing money) by pressing a button before the disappearance of a target. The anticipatory process

in the MIDT task could therefore be more related to the formulation of prospects and whether or
na

not the target was hit than to the anticipation of gains. These differences in task objectives, as

well as other factors, could contribute to the differences in findings [41].


ur

The anticipatory dopamine response toward guessing wins and losses could be associated
Jo

with reinforcement of addiction behavior, and could in important ways resemble real life gam-

bling situations. Indeed Clark et al. [3] in a recent review concluded that “…studies using more

realistic gambling games have indicated hyper-reactivity of striatal, dopaminergic midbrain, and

medial/PFC in GD” (p. 682). While the anticipatory dopamine in realistic games may be associ-

ated with reinforcement of gambling behavior in gambling disorder, the findings from the MIDT

literature may identify important anticipatory cognitive deficits in prospect formulation of gam-

8
Journal Pre-proof

bling disorder and substance use disorder. By this account there may be several anticipatory pro-

cesses associated with gambling disorder and substance use disorder.

Summary of PET and fMRI studies

The evidence from neuroimaging studies of anticipatory dopamine processes in gambling

disorder and substance use disorder has, at least, two important implications. First, gambling

disorder and substance use disorder may be influenced by similar dopamine processes associated

of
with reward anticipation, since reward anticipation is endogenous and does not involve the intake

of substances. Such similarities in anticipatory dopamine responses may be associated with simi-

ro
larities in clinical behavior, like craving and preoccupation with stimuli associated with addictive

behavior. -p
re
Second, gambling disorder and substance use disorder may be influenced by different dopa-
lP

mine processes associated with reward response. For instance, amphetamine and cocaine are

exogenous substances associated with distinct dopaminergic processes (amphetamine stimulates


na

dopamine release, while cocaine inhibits dopamine reuptake) [42]. Gambling disorder, on the
ur

other hand, is associated with endogenous dopamine processes. Although these are distinct pro-
Jo

cesses that alter the dopamine system in different ways, they may result in similar patterns of

dopamine response and clinical behavior, such as a “blunted” dopamine response toward reward

receipt or tolerance.

Here, it is suggested that the endogenous dopaminergic reward anticipation in gambling dis-

order, amphetamine use disorder and cocaine use disorder is similar across disorders, since there

is no intake of substances in reward anticipation. Endogenous dopamine dysfunctions in reward

anticipation may thus represent a common basis of addiction. In contrast, gambling disorder,

amphetamine use disorder and cocaine use disorder may differ in dopaminergic reward response,
9
Journal Pre-proof

where gambling is characterized by endogenous dopamine release, amphetamine use by exoge-

nous stimulation of dopamine release, and cocaine use by exogenous stimulation of dopamine

reuptake inhibition. These three distinct types of dopamine responses may be associated with

different forms of neural adaptation. Several models of dopamine dysfunctions in addiction have

addressed the dopaminergic reinforcement of reward anticipation and neural adaptation.

The incentive-sensitization theory

of
Terry E. Robinson and Kent C. Berridge [6-10] have proposed an incentive-sensitization the-

ory, which distinguishes between hedonic aspects (“liking”) and motivational aspects (“want-

ro
ing”) of stimuli in addiction. The model focuses on the dopamine system as a core neurobiologi-

-p
cal basis of addiction. The ventral striatum and its main component the nucleus accumbens
re
(NAcc) are associated with addiction in the theory. Robinson and Berridge argue that behavior
lP

reinforcement or associative learning is insufficient for explaining addictive behavior, because

most reinforced behavior is not associated with addiction (e.g., learning how to drive a car), and
na

the majority of people using drugs never develop an addiction. Instead, they suggest that changes
ur

in the dopamine system associated with drug exposure render the brain circuits hypersensitive or

“sensitized” to drugs or drug cues, and that this sensitization may explain addictive behavior.
Jo

Sensitization from repeated drug exposure may also occur at the level of psychomotor or loco-

motor activity.

The sensitization is linked with increased incentive salience, which is the cognitive process

associated with drug seeking and drug taking behavior. Incentive salience (“wanting”) refers to a

motivational state, which is not necessarily associated with declarative goals or conscious pro-

cesses. Incentive salience must be coupled with sensitization to account for addictive behavior:

an increase in dopamine binding does not define incentive-sensitization, but an increase in do-
10
Journal Pre-proof

pamine binding in relation to particular drug cues does; locomotor activity does not indicate in-

centive-sensitization, but running around to get drugs does; psychomotor preoccupation does not

indicate incentive-sensitization, but an obsession with taking drugs does. Therefore, simple rein-

forcement of behavior is insufficient to account for addictive behavior.

The incentive-sensitization theory suggests that “Many potentially addictive drugs initially

produce feelings of pleasure (euphoria), encouraging users to take drugs again. However, with

the transition to addiction, there appears to be a decrease in the role of drug pleasure. How can it

of
be that drugs come to be wanted more even if they become ‘liked’ less? According to incentive

ro
sensitization theory, the reason for this paradox is because repeated drug use sensitizes only the

-p
neural systems that mediate the motivational process of incentive salience (wanting), but not
re
neural systems that mediate the pleasurable effects of drugs (liking)” [9]. The notion that drugs

come to be ‘wanted’ more but ‘liked’ less in addiction is consistent with the blunted dopamine
lP

reward response in substance use disorder and gambling disorder [see e.g., 24, 36],and with the
na

increased anticipatory dopamine response in substance use disorder and gambling disorder [see

e.g., 32, 33, 37].


ur

Incentive sensitization theory was originally developed with a focus on drug use and sub-
Jo

stance use disorder, but Berridge and Robinson [10] have recently expanded the theory to in-

clude behavioral addictions such as gambling disorder. However, it is currently not clear which

mechanisms of neuroadaptation might account for the sensitization seen in gambling disorder:

“Conceivably, sensitization-related brain changes arise in some highly susceptible individuals to

produce these addictions without the need of drugs, through mechanisms not yet understood”,

[10, p. 678].

11
Journal Pre-proof

The integrative neurodevelopmental model of vulnerability towards addiction.

Leyton and Vezina have suggested an integrative neurodevelopmental model of vulnerability

towards addiction [43, 44], which proposes that in addiction drug cues are sensitized, i.e. associ-

ated with increased dopamine response, while non-drug cues may be associated with inhibitory

processes. Critically, the dopamine response is cue-driven, which has several important implica-

tions: First, drug-cues alone can increase the dopamine activity in addiction, consistent with the

notion that behavior (or behavioral addiction) in itself may be associated with sensitization. Sec-

of
ond, increased dopamine response to drug cues relative to non-drug cues in addiction could help

ro
explain the process of increased attention and preoccupation toward drugs relative to other cues.

-p
The relative increase of dopamine activation to drug cues versus non-drug cues has been shown
re
in exposure studies in substance use disorder [33, 45]. Third, ingestion of drugs without drug-

cues in substance use disorder may be associated with blunted dopamine activation. This is con-
lP

sistent with the proposed blunted dopamine response toward reward receipt. For instance, an
na

association between blunted dopamine response and decreased feelings of “rush” and “high” was

found in cocaine use disorder sufferers compared to healthy controls [46].


ur
Jo

Leyton and Vezina [43, 44] further suggests that individual differences may account for vul-

nerability toward engaging in drug use and developing addictive behavior. These preexisting

conditions may be elicited or exacerbated by environmental stimuli, such as the presence of

drugs or exposure to drug cues. Hence, there may be neurodevelopmental vulnerability factors of

addiction, which can protect against or exacerbate the risk of developing addiction.

12
Journal Pre-proof

Reward Prediction Error

The dopamine system plays a key role in reward learning: stimuli with higher reward proba-

bilities (i.e., stimuli that are more likely than not to be followed by a reward) are associated with

an increased anticipatory phasic dopamine activation prior to the outcome (reward or reward

omission). In contrast, stimuli with lower reward probabilities are associated with a reduced an-

ticipatory phasic dopamine activation. This mechanism is referred to as reward prediction [5],

because the dopaminergic signal is associated with the predictive value of the stimuli prior to

of
reward or omission. Reward prediction error [5] refers to a phasic dopamine activation, which

ro
occurs after the reward or omission, where the stimulus prediction differs from the outcome. For

-p
instance, there is an increased phasic dopamine activation when stimuli with low reward proba-
re
bilities are followed by reward, and there is a reduced phasic dopamine activation when stimuli

with high reward probabilities are no longer followed by reward. This way, the interaction be-
lP

tween reward prediction and reward prediction error updates the reward information of stimuli.
na

In random binary outcome conditions, e.g., reward vs. omission, the expected value (EV) is

the average value that can be expected from a given stimulus, which is a linear function of re-
ur

ward probability (Figure 1A). In contrast, uncertainty, which can be defined as the variance (2)
Jo

of a probability distribution [47], is the mean squared deviation from the expected value. Vari-

ance is an inverted quadratic function of reward probability; it can be expressed as (1-P)*P,

where P is the probability of reward represented by the value 1 and no reward represented by the

value 0. Maximum variance is found in P = 0.5 (2 = 0.5(1-0.5) = 0.25), and the variance de-

clines toward higher and lower probabilities (e.g., P = 0.25, 2 = 0.25(1-0.25) = 0.1875; P =

0.75, 2 = 0.75(1-0.75) = 0.1875) (Figure 1B).

13
Journal Pre-proof

(INSERT FIGURE 1 AROUND HERE).

Midbrain and striatal dopamine coding of expected value and uncertainty follow linear and

quadratic functions of reward similar to their mathematical expressions [30, 38, 48]. For in-

stance, Fiorillo, Tobler and Schultz [48] investigated dopamine activation in relation to expected

of
value and uncertainty in monkeys exposed to stimuli with varying reward probabilities (P = 0, P

ro
= 0.25, P = 0.5, P = 0.75 and P = 1.0). The authors found phasic dopamine reward prediction

-p
and reward prediction error signals associated with reward probability, consistent with the notion

of a linear relation between expected value and dopaminergic coding of reward (Figure 1A). Fur-
re
thermore, they found a sustained phasic dopamine signal associated with reward uncertainty,
lP

consistent with the notion of a quadratic relation between reward anticipation and dopaminergic

coding of uncertainty (Figure 1B).The sustained dopamine response toward uncertainty was a
na

distinct response from dopaminergic reward prediction and reward prediction error, which acti-
ur

vated separate dopamine neurons in the midbrain (although some overlap was seen between the
Jo

two signals). Fiorillo et al. [48] suggest that this dopaminergic coding of uncertainty and risk

taking could be associated with advantageous behaviors in natural environments such as learning

of stimuli or actions. However, in gambling environments dopaminergic coding of uncertainty

and risk taking may represent a risk factor for maladaptive behavior. Therefore, the sustained

dopaminergic reward anticipation associated with the coding of uncertainty could offer a rein-

forcement model for explaining why gambling disorder sufferers often: (1) are drawn toward the

risk and uncertainty of gambling; (2) continue gambling despite losses; and (3) do not adapt ad-

vantageous strategies despite negative feedback. This is consistent with a recent review by Clark
14
Journal Pre-proof

et al. [3] which suggests that uncertainty in itself can have reinforcing properties in gambling

disorder.

In a PET study Linnet et al. [30] tested competing hypotheses of dopamine increase of gam-

bling disorder sufferers playing the IGT. The hypothesis of expected value assumed a linear rela-

tion between IGT performance and dopamine increase (Figure 1C), suggesting that gambling

disorder sufferers who performed better on the IGT had higher dopamine release. In contrast, the

uncertainty hypothesis assumed a quadratic relation between IGT performance and dopamine

of
increase (Figure 1D), suggesting that gambling disorder sufferers with maximum uncertainty of

ro
outcome had higher dopamine release . The results showed a significant quadratic relation of

-p
dopamine release in gambling disorder, while no support was found for the linear relation, sug-
re
gesting that the dopamine release in gambling disorder was reinforced by uncertainty of out-
lP

come. Uncertainty is a key component in all forms of gambling, and the most addictive forms of

gambling such as slot machines, casino games and sports betting are characterized by maximized
na

uncertainty (variance) and high play frequencies (plays per minute). These games typically have

high payback percentages around 80–99% (e.g., slot machines often have payback percentages
ur

around 82–90%, and black jack has payback percentages as high as 99%).
Jo

Therefore, game properties that maximize uncertainty and variance may constitute an inher-

ent risk for gambling disorder sufferers and individuals at risk for developing gambling disorder.

For these individuals, dopaminergic dysfunctions of reward anticipation in coding of uncertainty

may reinforce maladaptive and erratic gambling behavior, and preclude the individual from in-

hibiting the gambling behavior in order to avoid or reduce losses.

15
Journal Pre-proof

Conclusions and future directions

Several parallels of dopaminergic dysfunctions in gambling disorder and substance use dis-

order have been proposed. Evidence from PET and fMRI studies suggest that dysfunctions in

anticipatory dopamine processes may constitute a common neurobiological pathway of gambling

disorder and substance use disorder. In particular, the anticipatory dopamine toward reward cues

may reinforce addictive behaviors, and might constitute a common neurobiological pathway of

gambling disorder and substance use disorder. Such dysfunctions in anticipatory dopamine re-

of
sponse is consistent with theories of incentive sensitization, vulnerability of neurodevelopment

ro
of addiction, and reward prediction.

-p
Dysfunctions in anticipatory dopamine response may involve similar neurobiological mecha-
re
nisms across gambling disorder and substance use disorder, since reward anticipation is not asso-
lP

ciated with exogenous intake of substances. Yet, it is not clear if dysfunctions in anticipatory

dopamine responses are associated with different types of neuroadaptation related to, e.g., type
na

of addiction, predisposed conditions, or environmental influences.


ur

However, the findings of dysfunctions in anticipatory dopamine response in addictive behav-

ior may guide future directions of treatment of addiction both with regard to understanding neu-
Jo

robiological deficits in addiction, predictive measures of treatment outcome, and possible neuro-

biological mechanisms associated with cognitive restructuring of addictive behavior. For in-

stance, reward anticipation may be associated with motivational processes, cognitive biases and

impulsiveness in gambling disorder and substance use disorder and may affect the treatment out-

come across patient populations. Conversely, dopaminergic dysfunctions of reward anticipation

could be linked with treatment outcome in specific patient sub-populations.

16
Journal Pre-proof

While much of the early research on substance use disorder have focused on how the brain re-

sponds to drugs, a focus on reward anticipation could broaden this focus to also include the neu-

robiology of motivational and learning processes associated with addiction. As suggested by

Clark et al. [3] this could frame addiction as a “learning disorder” rather than a “substance disor-

der”.

of
ro
-p
re
lP
na
ur
Jo

17
Journal Pre-proof

References

1. Zack, M. and C.X. Poulos, Parallel roles for dopamine in pathological gambling and
psychostimulant addiction. Curr Drug Abuse Rev, 2009. 2(1): p. 11-25.
2. Potenza, M.N., How central is dopamine to pathological gambling or gambling
disorder? Front Behav Neurosci, 2013. 7: p. 206.
3. Clark, L., I. Boileau, and M. Zack, Neuroimaging of reward mechanisms in Gambling
disorder: an integrative review. Mol Psychiatry, 2019. 24(5): p. 674-693.
4. Linnet, J., Neurobiological underpinnings of reward anticipation and outcome evaluation
in gambling disorder. Front Behav Neurosci, 2014. 8: p. 100.
5. Schultz, W., Behavioral theories and the neurophysiology of reward. Annual Review of
Psychology, 2006. 57: p. 87-115.
6. Robinson, T.E. and K.C. Berridge, The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-

of
sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Brain Res Rev, 1993. 18(3): p. 247-91.
7. Robinson, T.E. and K.C. Berridge, The psychology and neurobiology of addiction: an

ro
incentive-sensitization view. Addiction, 2000. 95 Suppl 2: p. S91-117.
8. Robinson, T.E. and K.C. Berridge, Addiction. Annu Rev Psychol, 2003. 54: p. 25-53.
9. Robinson, T.E. and K.C. Berridge, Review. The incentive sensitization theory of

-p
addiction: some current issues. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci, 2008. 363(1507): p.
3137-46.
re
10. Berridge, K.C. and T.E. Robinson, Liking, wanting, and the incentive-sensitization theory
of addiction. Am Psychol, 2016. 71(8): p. 670-679.
11. Breiter, H.C., et al., Functional imaging of neural responses to expectancy and
lP

experience of monetary gains and losses. Neuron, 2001. 30(2): p. 619-39.


12. Koepp, M.J., et al., Evidence for striatal dopamine release during a video game. Nature,
1998. 393(6682): p. 266-8.
na

13. Zald, D.H., et al., Dopamine transmission in the human striatum during monetary reward
tasks. J Neurosci, 2004. 24(17): p. 4105-12.
14. de la Fuente-Fernandez, R., et al., Dopamine release in human ventral striatum and
ur

expectation of reward. Behavioural Brain Research., 2002. 136(2): p. 359-63.


15. Delgado, M.R., et al., Tracking the hemodynamic responses to reward and punishment in
the striatum. Journal of Neurophysiology., 2000. 84(6): p. 3072-7.
Jo

16. Joutsa, J., et al., Mesolimbic dopamine release is linked to symptom severity in
pathological gambling. Neuroimage, 2012. 60(4): p. 1992-9.
17. Steeves, T.D., et al., Increased striatal dopamine release in Parkinsonian patients with
pathological gambling: a [11C] raclopride PET study. Brain, 2009. 132(Pt 5): p. 1376-
85.
18. Linnet, J., et al., Dopamine Release in Ventral Striatum of Pathological Gamblers Losing
Money. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 2010. 122: p. 326-333.
19. Boileau, I., et al., The D2/3 dopamine receptor in pathological gambling: a positron
emission tomography study with [11C]-(+)-propyl-hexahydro-naphtho-oxazin and
[11C]raclopride. Addiction, 2013. 108(5): p. 953-63.
20. Dalley, J.W., et al., Nucleus accumbens D2/3 receptors predict trait impulsivity and
cocaine reinforcement. Science, 2007. 315(5816): p. 1267-70.
21. Doya, K., Modulators of decision making. Nat Neurosci, 2008. 11(4): p. 410-6.

18
Journal Pre-proof

22. Uhl, G., Premature poking: impulsivity, cocaine and dopamine. Nat Med, 2007. 13(4): p.
413-4.
23. Volkow, N.D., et al., Low level of brain dopamine D2 receptors in methamphetamine
abusers: association with metabolism in the orbitofrontal cortex. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 2001. 158(12): p. 2015-21.
24. Volkow, N.D., et al., Decreased striatal dopaminergic responsiveness in detoxified
cocaine-dependent subjects. Nature, 1997. 386(6627): p. 830-3.
25. Clark, L., et al., Striatal dopamine D(2)/D(3) receptor binding in pathological gambling
is correlated with mood-related impulsivity. Neuroimage, 2012. 63(1): p. 40-6.
26. Kalivas, P.W. and N.D. Volkow, The neural basis of addiction: a pathology of
motivation and choice. Am J Psychiatry, 2005. 162(8): p. 1403-13.
27. Volkow, N.D., et al., Brain DA D2 receptors predict reinforcing effects of stimulants in
humans: replication study. Synapse, 2002. 46(2): p. 79-82.

of
28. Volkow, N.D., et al., Dopamine increases in striatum do not elicit craving in cocaine
abusers unless they are coupled with cocaine cues. Neuroimage, 2008. 39(3): p. 1266-73.

ro
29. Linnet, J., et al., Inverse association between dopaminergic neurotransmission and Iowa
Gambling Task performance in pathological gamblers and healthy controls.
Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 2011. 52(1): p. 28-34.
30.
-p
Linnet, J., et al., Striatal dopamine release codes uncertainty in pathological gambling.
Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 2012. 204: p. 55-60.
re
31. Volkow, N.D., et al., Overlapping neuronal circuits in addiction and obesity: evidence of
systems pathology. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci, 2008. 363(1507): p. 3191-200.
32. Volkow, N.D., et al., Cocaine cues and dopamine in dorsal striatum: mechanism of
lP

craving in cocaine addiction. J Neurosci, 2006. 26(24): p. 6583-8.


33. Wong, D.F., et al., Increased occupancy of dopamine receptors in human striatum during
cue-elicited cocaine craving. Neuropsychopharmacology, 2006. 31(12): p. 2716-27.
na

34. Abler, B., et al., Prediction error as a linear function of reward probability is coded in
human nucleus accumbens. Neuroimage, 2006. 31(2): p. 790-5.
35. Chase, H.W. and L. Clark, Gambling severity predicts midbrain response to near-miss
ur

outcomes. J Neurosci, 2010. 30(18): p. 6180-7.


36. Reuter, J., et al., Pathological gambling is linked to reduced activation of the mesolimbic
reward system. Nature Neuroscience, 2005. 8(2): p. 147-8.
Jo

37. van Holst, R.J., et al., Distorted expectancy coding in problem gambling: is the addictive
in the anticipation? Biol Psychiatry, 2012. 71(8): p. 741-8.
38. Preuschoff, K., P. Bossaerts, and S.R. Quartz, Neural differentiation of expected reward
and risk in human subcortical structures. Neuron, 2006. 51(3): p. 381-90.
39. Luijten, M., et al., Disruption of Reward Processing in Addiction : An Image-Based
Meta-analysis of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Studies. JAMA Psychiatry,
2017. 74(4): p. 387-398.
40. Balodis, I.M., et al., Diminished frontostriatal activity during processing of monetary
rewards and losses in pathological gambling. Biol Psychiatry, 2012. 71(8): p. 749-57.
41. Balodis, I.M., et al., Attending to striatal ups and downs in addictions. Biol Psychiatry,
2012. 72(10): p. e25-6.
42. Stahl, Essential Psychopharmachology. 2006, New Yor: Cambridge University Press.
43. Leyton, M. and P. Vezina, Dopamine ups and downs in vulnerability to addictions: a
neurodevelopmental model. Trends Pharmacol Sci, 2014. 35(6): p. 268-76.

19
Journal Pre-proof

44. Leyton, M. and P. Vezina, Striatal ups and downs: their roles in vulnerability to
addictions in humans. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 2013. 37(9 Pt A): p. 1999-2014.
45. Volkow, N.D., et al., Cognitive control of drug craving inhibits brain reward regions in
cocaine abusers. Neuroimage, 2010. 49(3): p. 2536-43.
46. Wang, G.J., et al., Expectation effects on brain dopamine responses to methylphenidate in
cocaine use disorder. Transl Psychiatry, 2019. 9(1): p. 93.
47. Schultz, W., et al., Explicit neural signals reflecting reward uncertainty. Philosophical
transactions of the Royal Society of London: Biological sciences, 2008. 363(1511): p.
3801-11.
48. Fiorillo, C.D., P.N. Tobler, and W. Schultz, Discrete coding of reward probability and
uncertainty by dopamine neurons. Science., 2003. 299(5614): p. 1898-902.

of
ro
-p
re
lP
na
ur
Jo

20
Journal Pre-proof

Figure 1. Expected value and uncertainty as functions of reward probability and dopamine

changes (BPND).

of
ro
-p
re
lP
na
ur

A. Expected value is a linear function of reward probability, where the expected value is minimal
at P = 0.0 and maximal at P = 1.0. B. Uncertainty, defined as variance, is a quadratic function of
reward probability, where uncertainty is maximal at P = 0.5 and minimal at P = 0.0 and P = 1.0.
Jo

C. Hypothetical relation between expected value and dopamine changes (BPND) in relation to
IGT performance, where higher dopamine increase is associated with higher probability of re-
ward. D. Hypothetical relation between uncertainty and dopamine changes (BPND), where do-
pamine increase is highest when probability of reward is maximally uncertain (P = 0.5) and de-
clines as probability increases or decreases. Reprint with permission from Psychiatry Research:
Neuroimaging 204 (2012) 55–60 © 2012.

21
Journal Pre-proof

• Dopamine is associated with reward in gambling disorder and substance use disorder.
• Dopaminergic reward anticipation may be dysfunctional in gambling- and drug addiction.

• Reward anticipation does not require the use of drugs.

• Dopaminergic reward ancitiapation may be a common basis of addiction.

of
ro
-p
re
lP
na
ur
Jo

22
Journal Pre-proof

The author has no conflict of interest in the present article.

of
ro
-p
re
lP
na
ur
Jo

23
Figure 1

You might also like