Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

John Cohen and the psychology of time

John Wearden

The article discusses the writings on time psychology by John Cohen, Professor of Psychology at Manchester
University between 1951 and 1979. His principal empirical contribution was the popularisation of the
kappa effect, which showed that time judgements were influenced by spatial distance, which he found
both in laboratory experiments and in studies in real-life settings. Cohen also wrote extensively about
many aspects of the psychology of time, with emphases on time in literature, and time in myth. John
Cohen exemplifies an academic who crossed with great facility between C.P. Snow’s ‘Two Cultures’, with
rigorous laboratory studies in experimental psychology being contrasted with writings based on his extensive
knowledge of the Classics and Arts.
Keywords: Time perception; kappa effect; tau effect; time in literature; time in myth.

J
OHN COHEN (1911–1985) was only and psychiatrists in the armed services, and
the second Professor of Psychology at in 1948 was the technical secretary of the
Manchester University, holding the posi- International Congress on Mental Health.
tion from 1951 until 1979 in succession to In Psychology, John Cohen’s main
T.H. Pear, Britain’s first full-time Professor of research interests were psychological prob-
Psychology. Prior to that, Cohen had been ability (see Cohen, 1972, for a summary of
a Lecturer in Psychology at Leeds Univer- his work) and time perception, and it is the
sity and at Birkbeck College, London, and latter that is the subject of this article. In
an Associate Professor of Psychology at the spite of the fact that most of his research
Hebrew University of Jerusalem. He received was focussed on these rather unusual topics,
his M.A. and Ph.D. degrees from University Cohen should not be viewed as an isolated
College, London, but also conducted post- eccentric, being both a Fellow of the British
graduate research at Oxford. From 1941 to Psychological Society and sometime member
1948 he worked in the office of the War of its Council, as well as the author of many
Cabinet and the Central Statistical Office. publications in mainstream psychology and
The expertise that he acquired in these scientific journals.
roles during and after the Second World Up until Cohen’s research which began
War led to his participation in the Wood in the 1950s, interest in time perception
Committee on the recruitment and training in British psychology had been sparse and
of nurses in 1945–1946. Cohen disagreed sporadic, although one well-known figure,
with the approach taken by the committee, Beatrice Edgell, published an early article
regarding it as being insufficiently informed on the topic (Edgell, 1903, see Wearden,
by data and statistics, and lacking any links in press, for a discussion of Edgell’s work).
to other aspects of health care. He produced Cohen produced a short book on psycho-
a minority report (Cohen, 1948), described logical time (Cohen, 1967), and a number
by Hatchett (2005) as ‘one of the most radical of articles and book chapters on the subject,
critiques of the failings of workforce policy by far the largest body of work on time
and healthcare planning during the period’ perception by any British psychologist until
and ‘…a salutary reminder of the inade- the 1990s. He had a wide range of interests
quacies [of] planning within the nursing within the topic of time psychology, as will be
occupation’ (pp.19 and 42). During the war seen later in this article. Pullan and Abend-
years Cohen served on an expert committee stern (2004) quote from a farewell eulogy
concerned with the work of psychologists for Cohen where it was said that he was ‘an

4 History & Philosophy of Psychology Vol. 22 No. 1


John Cohen and the psychology of time

experimental psychologist who writes like time perception, then later his writings on
a classical scholar’ (p.60). These two sides time in literature and myth.
of Cohen’s work, experimental research and
scholarship, although not of an exclusively Cohen the experimental psychologist
classical sort, are explored below. Of his experimental studies, perhaps the
C.P. Snow (1959) lamented the best known is the rediscovery of what he
well-known division of British academic life called the kappa effect (Cohen, Hansel &
into ‘Two Cultures’: one based on science, Sylvester, 1953). As Cohen later acknowl-
and the other on arts, with practitioners edged, this had actually first been discov-
of one culture exhibiting almost total igno- ered by a Japanese psychologist, Abe, in the
rance of the other. This division was certainly 1930s, but there is no doubt that Cohen
not true of Cohen’s work, particularly his et al.’s work brought the phenomenon to
research on time, where he readily crossed the attention of Western psychologists. The
over from one to the other, but this also kappa effect was in some ways the reverse of
applied to his approach to psychology the tau effect described by Helson and King
more generally. For example, even in a very (1931). The tau effect arises when three
early publication, Cohen (1952), originally points on the skin are successively stimu-
submitted in 1950 when he worked at Birk- lated. If the time between two of these stimu-
beck College, he included references to lations is longer than the time between the
Proust and Balzac in a discussion of percep- other two, then the distance between them is
tion using different senses. In addition, he judged as longer, even though it may be the
clearly signalled his position by giving his same or shorter.
book Homo Psychologicus (Cohen, 1970) the Cohen et al. (1953) presented observers
subtitle ‘Bridging the Arts and Sciences by with three spatially separated light flashes in
a study of Man as worker player and idler’. a horizontal array, and varied the distance
Where did Cohen’s literary leanings between the pairs, over ratios of from 1:10 to
come from? He was born in the Welsh town 10:1. The observer was instructed to adjust
of Tredegar, and was one of nine siblings the time between the flashes to make the
in a prominent Jewish family. His brother time between the first and second the same
Moses (or Moshe) was a well-known rabbi, as between the second and the third. In
and Moshe’s obituary, in The Light magazine Cohen et al.’s own words ‘the observer…
in 1975, describes his ‘broad erudition in makes the time interval between the two
general culture and literature’, so it seems flashes spaced farther apart shorter than
likely that Cohen also shared this strong the time interval between two flashes closer
cultural background from a young age. together’ (p.901). In the case where the
As well as being known for his academic times between the flashes are actually equal,
work, Cohen was an early example of a this results in the time between the flashes
‘television psychologist’ and was familiar to occurring over the longer spatial distance
viewers in the North-West of England in the being judged as longer. Some results are
1960s and 1970s. His pronouncements on shown in Figure 1.
a range of very diverse topics were often If the distances between flashes 1 and
delivered from behind a cloud of smoke 2 and 2 and 3 are d1 and d2, and the corre-
produced by his pipe, something which was sponding times t1 and t2, the percentage
possible on television in those days. difference needed to make the times subjec-
In the remainder of this article, I discuss tively equal can be given by (t1 – t2)/t1 X 100.
Cohen’s work on time from the perspec- Figure 1 shows that when d2>d1 then some
tive of the two different ‘cultures’ identi- quantity needs to be added to t2 to make the
fied by Snow (1959). First, I will discuss his times subjectively equal, whereas when d1 >
research in the experimental psychology of d2 some quantity needs to be subtracted.

History & Philosophy of Psychology Vol. 22 No. 1 5


John Wearden

Figure 1: Data from Cohen et al. (1953). Percentage difference in time judgements as a function of
the ratio of the distance between the first and second, and second and third, light flashes. Data are
shown separately for the cases where the second distance was greater than the first (d2 > d1) and the
reverse case.

Cohen, Hansel, and Sylvester (1955) Cohen et al. then attempted a kappa
replicated the kappa effect with horizontal equivalent, where the times between the
distance, and showed that it occurred for stimuli were varied with the pitches them-
vertical distance as well. Later research gener- selves constant, but little effect was obtained.
ally replicated the kappa effect when the phys- In fact, some later studies did report an audi-
ical distance between stimuli varied, although tory kappa effect but, consistent with Cohen
it was not always found when the time inter- et al.’s work, this effect appears more fragile
vals between the stimuli were very short (see than the tau effect and is not always found.
Huang & Jones, 1982, for discussion). However, some later workers obtained clear
But does the kappa effect occur when effects, e.g. Shigeno (1986) who found kappa
the ‘distance’ between successive stimuli effects with both pure tones and synthetic
is not spatial, for example, does it apply vowel sounds.
when differences in pitch of successive audi- Cohen and Cooper (1962) found tau
tory stimuli are arranged? Cohen, Hansel and kappa-like effects in situations involving
and Sylvester (1954a) examined potential real movement. Small groups of people were
tau and kappa effects with pitch. In the tau transported in a blacked-out bus, so they
study, three successive brief auditory stimuli could not observe the environment outside.
of different pitch were presented, and the Half-way through the trip a bell was sounded.
participant’s task was to adjust the pitch of After the bell sounded, the bus either went
the second stimulus so that it was equidistant faster, went slower, or went at the same speed
in pitch from the first and the third. The time as before the bell. If the bell rang at the
between the first and second, and second and exact temporal mid-point of the trip, ‘that
third, stimuli was varied. A marked tau-type part will seem to last longer in which the
effect was found: if the first two pitches were distance and speed are greater’ (p.1234),
closer together in time than the second two, what they called the kappa-movement effect.
then the pitch midpoint was changed so that There was also a tau-movement effect, where,
the shorter first interval resulted in a higher if the distance traversed before and after the
pitch midpoint. bell was actually the same, the distance that

6 History & Philosophy of Psychology Vol. 22 No. 1


John Cohen and the psychology of time

took the longer time was judged as longer. Cohen’s interest in the experimental
In addition ‘…if two parts are travelled at Psychology of time also extended to reviews
the same speed that part will seem faster in of the early history of the subject (see Cohen,
which the distance and time are shorter’, 1967, chapters 1 and 2, for example). Here,
referred to as the kappa-tau effect. he briefly discusses the 19th century work
Cohen, Cooper and Ono (1963) of the physicist Mach, a favourite of his
conducted another ‘real life’ experiment mentioned in several articles, as well as that of
investigating interactions between time, the psychologist Meumann (for some exam-
distance, and speed. Schoolchildren between ples of Meumann’s work see Wearden and
16 and 18 years of age had to walk or run Ogden, in press). He also discussed early ideas
along with an experimenter. The different that people might possess an ‘internal clock’.
conditions involved walking then running, or Cohen mentions in particular the article by
the reverse, and two control conditions where François (1927) on body temperature and
the children always walked or always ran. The judgements of time, conducted under the
basic overall result was a small tendency for auspices of the French psychologist Piéron.
the distance that took the longer time to be In chapter 7 of his 1967 book, Cohen
judged as longer, but this concealed indi- proposed some modifications of the
vidual differences between what Cohen et al. well-known Treisman pacemaker-counter
called ‘tortoises’ and ‘hares’. Tortoises were model of time perception (Treisman,
said to be especially sensitive to the passage 1963). A full discussion of this model and
of time, thus judged temporally longer trips Cohen’s own ideas about it, is beyond the
as involving greater distances. Hares, on the scope of the present article, but one of
other hand, focused on speed, and gave Cohen’s suggestion is of particular interest.
longer distance estimates for shorter times. An important aspect of Treisman’s model is
Cohen, Hansel and Sylvester (1954b) a pacemaker, pulses from which are accumu-
reported a quite different sort of experi- lated in a counter, thus providing the ‘raw
ment where groups of children, students, material’ for time judgements. From one
and older adults were required to make rela- trial to another the rate of the pacemaker can
tive judgements of long times. The scale vary, and Cohen suggests that ‘the variability
used involved a 10-inch (25.4cm) line length [is] due to the effect of other experiences
representing the time from birth to ‘now’ undergone at the same time’ as the timing
(so this scale changed depending how task to hand. This suggests an ‘attentional’
old the participants were), then distances modification of Treisman’s model, rather
corresponding to different lengths of time, similar to the ‘attentional gate’ proposal
ranging from one day to 35 years, had to be advanced nearly 30 years later by Zakay and
judged, using the same scale. The lengths of Block (1995), although Cohen situates his
time estimated depended to some degree attention-based ‘calibration monitor’ before
on the age of the participants, so younger the pacemaker rather than after it as in
participants were never asked to judge times the attentional gate model. Unfortunately,
longer than their lifespan, for example. The Cohen never developed his suggestions into
results suggested that the estimated length any formal theoretical model and, in fact,
of time increased logarithmically with real published no further work on the experi-
duration up until about six months, then mental psychology of time perception after
linearly afterwards. Cohen et al. suggested the appearance of his book in 1967.
that there were two different sorts of judge-
ments, one based on some sort of lived expe- Cohen the [sometimes classical] scholar
rience, up until six months, then afterwards In addition to his experimental work on time
dependent on calculations based on knowl- and his 1967 book, Cohen produced two
edge of the calendar. book chapters (Cohen, 1958, and Cohen,

History & Philosophy of Psychology Vol. 22 No. 1 7


John Wearden

1970) an early article (Cohen, 1954), and two (2015) calls ‘passage of time judgements’,
popular accounts (Cohen, 1957 and 1964), that is, the feeling of the ‘speed’ at which
all reviewing aspects of the psychology of time seems to pass in some situation. While
time. These writings share a lot of common experimental psychologists have only fairly
content, with the kappa effect mentioned in recently become interested in this, there is
almost all of them. an older literature (e.g. Flaherty, 1993) on
Cohen had particular interests in the what sociologists usually called ‘temporality’.
relation between literature and time (and his Another somewhat similar focus of recent
literary interests were broader than just in interest has been research on the common
time, there is a whole chapter on ‘Psychology assertion that time appears to pass more
and literature’ in Cohen, 1970), and time quickly as people age, something that Cohen
in myth. As these are unusual topics in time mentions in several of his publications. This
perception research these days, I will discuss is a result that has been more difficult to find
some of his writings on these issues, starting in modern empirical studies than might be
with his views on literature and time. supposed, given its wide popular currency
As mentioned earlier, Cohen never had (see Wittman & Lehnhoff, 2005, for an
problems bridging Snow’s ‘Two Cultures’. In early study, and Droit-Volet et al., 2021, for
Cohen (1970, p.104), he writes, ‘…academic a recent one).
psychologists, fearful lest their self-image as Whatever Cohen’s literary examples were
scientists will be tarnished, feel uneasy if, intended to prove, the relation of literature
in the midst of a technical discussion, the and the psychology of time was certainly one
reader’s mind is directed to literature or the of his preoccupations, so in Cohen (1967,
arts’. Cohen had no such qualms, and he p.34) we have an example from Turgenev
often used literary examples such as illus- of the apparent slowness of the passage of
trating effects of drugs on time perception time, and in Cohen (1970, pp.104–105) he
by a reference to Quincey’s Confessions of an provides quotes from Leonardo da Vinci,
Opium Eater or Walter de la Mare’s account Nietzsche, Proust and Valery, all illustrating
of a friend’s experience with mescalin (e.g. the complexity of time experience. In Cohen
Cohen, 1958, pp.108–109, but repeated in (1967, pp.74–75) he classifies writers in
Cohen, 1967, pp.64–65) rather than a piece terms of the way that time is depicted in
of psychopharmacological research. their work. One example of ‘slow motion’
In the 1970 chapter on time, Cohen time is from Sterne’s Tristram Shandy, where
quotes extensively from various writers, but the first two minutes of the protagonist’s life
then, rather disarmingly, comments, ‘These take much longer to read than two minutes.
passages are not cited to prove anything but Proust’s famously elongated time, his
to illustrate the limited and impoverished ‘motionless description’, where hundreds of
conception of psychological time which pages of text may be devoted to an event
would confine it to the study of apparent lasting an hour or so, is also noted.
duration’ (Cohen, 1970, p.105). Cohen was also interested in the repre-
It is true that in Cohen’s time what sentation of time in myth and religion, and
research there was on time perception in classical Greek and Roman writing and
was focussed mostly on the perception of art. As he shows in Cohen (1967) many
duration, often involving times too short religions have developed conceptions of
to make counting possible, and this is true time quite different from that of the exper-
to a large extent even today. However, the imental psychologist. Different times may
focus has widened somewhat in recent be regarded as sacred or profane, as in
years and now even includes work on more Hesiod’s Works and Days, cited in several of
‘subjective’ aspects of time perception, such Cohen’s writing on time, where different days
as the neglected topic of what Wearden are regarded as ‘propitious or unpropitious’

8 History & Philosophy of Psychology Vol. 22 No. 1


John Cohen and the psychology of time

for different activities and ‘each period is literature and myth, the ‘classical scholar’
animated by a personal spirit’ (p.72). Cohen seemed definitely to have the upper hand
also discusses the idea that time is viewed as over the ‘experimental psychologist’ and,
cyclical rather than linear in some eastern furthermore, there is no suggestion overall
religious systems. in his work that his experimental studies
Another section in Cohen (1967) were influenced by his interest in litera-
discusses how thought in antiquity appeared ture and the classics; the two aspects of his
to have different ‘basic images of time’. research seem to have been kept completely
For example, time has been ‘represented separate.
by the figure of Opportunity’, ‘when the
time is ripe and opportune’ (p.77). Another Conclusion
is exemplified by the Iranian god ‘Aion, Among British psychologists of his genera-
a symbol of divine and inexhaustible crea- tion John Cohen’s interest in time percep-
tiveness’ (p.78), associated with the cult of tion was virtually unique, and the breadth
Mithras. Cohen also points out other asso- of his interests marks him out even now.
ciations with time in classical thinking, in To use a classical metaphor which seems
particular the contradiction between time particularly appropriate for Cohen’s work,
as a ‘destroyer’ of all things and, conversely, in his research on time he was a Janus-like
time as representing renewal and fecundity. figure, facing in two directions at once. One
He attributes this to a confusion between direction was conventional experimental
Kronos (Κρόνος) and Cronos (Χρόνος). psychology. The body of empirical work he
The former was the chief of the Titans, left is fairly small, but contains interesting
and the father of Zeus, among other gods, results which others have built on, and he
and is generally associated with the destruc- showed an unusual desire to extend findings
tive power of time. Perhaps the fact that he made in the laboratory to real-life settings.
castrated his father, Saturn, and swallowed His other face was directed towards literary
his own children (albeit not permanently) and classical interests. This double aspect
contributed to his sinister aspect. Cronos, permeated most of Cohen’s later writings
in contrast, is the Greek god of time, and of on time, giving his publications a rather
harvests, so represents the renewing aspect different flavour from the mostly technical
of time, which in the case of harvests is of discussions of time perception found today.
course cyclical. The scythe associated with
‘Father Time’ in modern representations Acknowledgements
could come from either, as a weapon in the I am grateful to an anonymous reviewer for
former case, and as an agricultural imple- suggestions which have improved the organ-
ment in the latter. ization and content of this article. Many
A critic might complain that it is hard thanks are also due to Dr Claire Jackson
to know exactly what Cohen’s explorations for providing information about John
of time in literature and myth, interesting Cohen’s background and education. The
and engagingly written as they are, actu- preparation of the article benefitted from
ally tell us about time perception, except resources provided by an Emeritus Fellow-
that it has many complex aspects, which ship from the Leverhulme Trust.
hardly anyone would deny. As suggested
by the quote cited earlier, Cohen himself John Wearden
also sometimes seemed rather unsure just School of Psychology, Keele University,
exactly what the message from literature was Keele, ST5 5BG, U.K.
supposed to be. In Cohen’s writings about j.h.wearden@keele.ac.uk

History & Philosophy of Psychology Vol. 22 No. 1 9


John Wearden

References
Cohen, J. (1948). The recruiting and training of nurses. François, M. (1927). Contributions à l’étude du
London: HMSO. sens de temps: La température interne comme
Cohen, J. (1952). A note on social and personal facteur de variation de l’appréciation subjective
factors in sense perception. Journal of General des durées. L’Année Psychologique, 27, 186–204.
Psychology, 43, 103–106. Flaherty, M.G. (1993). Conceptualizing variation in
Cohen, J. (1954). The experience of time. Acta the experience of time. Sociological Enquiry, 63,
Psychologica, 10, 207–219. 394–405.
Cohen, J. (1957). Subjective time. Discovery, 18, 151– Hackett, R.P. (2005). The history of workforce policy and
154. planning in British nursing. Unpublished Ph.D.
Cohen, J. (1958). Humanistic psychology. London: thesis, University of London.
George Allen & Unwin. Huang, A.L. & Jones, B. (1986). On the
Cohen, J. (1964). Psychological time. Scientific interdependence of spatial and temporal
American, 116–124. judgments. Perception and Psychophysics, 32, 7–14.
Cohen, J. (1967). Psychological time in health and Helson, H. & King, S.M. (1931). The tau effect: An
disease. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. example of psychological relativity. Journal of
Cohen, J. (1970). Homo psychologicus. London: George Experimental Psychology, 14, 202–217.
Allen & Unwin. Pullan, B. & Abendstern, M. (2004). A History of the
Cohen, J. (1972). Psychological probability. London: University of Manchester: 1973–1990. Manchester:
George Allen & Unwin. Manchester University Press.
Cohen, J. & Cooper, P. (1962). New phenomena in Shigeno, S. (1986). The auditory tau and kappa
apparent duration, distance, and speed. Nature, effects for speech and nonspeech stimuli.
196, 1233–1234. Perception and Psychophysics, 40, 9–19.
Cohen, J., Cooper, P. & Ono, A. (1963). The hare and Snow, C.P. (1959). The two cultures and the scientific
the tortoise: A study of the tau-effect in walking revolution. London: Cambridge University Press.
and running. Acta Psychologica, 21, 387–393. Treisman, M. (1963). Temporal discrimination
Cohen, J., Hansel, C.E.M. & Sylvester, J.D. (1953). and the indifference interval: Implications for
A new phenomenon in the judgement of time. a model of the ‘internal clock’. Psychological
Nature, 172, 901. Monographs, whole number 576.
Cohen, J., Hansel, C.E.M. & Sylvester, J.D. (1954a). Wearden, J.H. (2015). Passage of time judgements.
Interdependence of temporal and auditory Consciousness and Cognition, 38, 165–171.
judgements. Nature, 174, 642–644. Wearden, J.H. (in press). Three women in time:
Cohen, J., Hansel, C.E.M. & Sylvester, J.D. (1954b). An Beatrice Edgell, Josephine Nash Curtis, and Mary
experimental study of comparative judgements Sturt. Timing and Time Perception.
of time. British Journal of Psychology, 45, 108–114. Wearden, J.H. & Ogden, R.S. (in press). Filled-
Cohen, J., Hansel, C.E.M. & Sylvester, J.D. (1955). interval illusions. Timing and Time Perception.
Interdependence in judgements of time, space, Wittman, M. & Lehnhoff, S. (2005). Age effects on
and movement. Acta Psychologica, 11, 360–372. the perception of time. Psychological Reports, 97,
Droit-Volet, S., Martinelli, N., Dannebrun, M., Vallet, 921–935.
G.T. & Lorandi, F. (2021). The retrospective and Zakay, D. & Block, R.A. (1995). An attentional-
present judgment of the passage of time in the gate model of prospective time estimation in
elderly. Timing and Time Perception, 9, 335–352. M. Richelle, V. de Keyser, G. d’Ydewalle, & A.
Edgell, B. (1903). On time judgment. American. Vandierendonck (Eds.) Time and the dynamic
Journal of Psychology, 14, 154–174. control of behavior (pp.167–178). Liège: University
of Liège Press.

10 History & Philosophy of Psychology Vol. 22 No. 1


Copyright of History & Philosophy of Psychology is the property of British Psychological
Society and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv
without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print,
download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like