Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Neuron 52, 941–952, December 21, 2006 ª2006 Elsevier Inc. DOI 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.12.

002

The Neural Basis of Language Review


Development and Its Impairment

Angela D. Friederici1,* comes to explaining early language development and


1
Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive its impairment, the present review will focus on these.
and Brain Sciences The neural parameters of the neural basis of normal
Leipzig language development, however, are still not under-
Germany stood entirely, and the description of its impairment truly
remains incomplete. This is due to the fact that there are
dramatic constraints on the investigation of the neural
The neural correlates of early language development correlates of language development. Direct animal
and language impairment are described, with the adult model, cellular, and molecular approaches are impossi-
language-related brain systems as a target model. ble, and genetic approaches can only describe correla-
Electrophysiological and hemodynamic studies indi- tions between phenotype and genotype. At the macro-
cate that language functions to be installed in the scopic level, functional magnetic resonance imaging
child’s brain are similar to those of adults, with lateral- (fMRI) and structural imaging (diffusion tenor imaging
ization being present at birth, phonological processes [DTI]) and magnetencephalography (MEG) are not
during the first months, semantic processes at 12 always applicable in young children and infants. Electro-
months, and syntactic processes around 30 months. encephalography (EEG) and near-infrared spectroscopy
These findings support the view that the brain basis (NIRS) are the only methods that can readily be applied
of language develops continuously over time. Discon- to this age group without much constraint.
tinuities are observed in children with language im- The review consists of two parts. The first part
pairment. Here, the observed functional abnormalities discusses the neural correlates of normal language
are accompanied by structural abnormalities in infe- development using adult data as the reference model,
rior frontal and temporal brain regions. and the second part covers the available neurophysio-
logical and neuroanatomical studies on specific lan-
Introduction guage impairment.
Language is acquired without much effort and seems to
develop as the brain matures. The milestones of normal Neural Correlates of Normal Language Development
language development as evident from behavior can be Studies on the neural basis of normal language develop-
defined as follows. In the first days after birth, infants are ment have focused on different aspects of the language
able to discriminate between different phonemes and system, namely (1) phonological processes concerning
distinguish the sentence melody (prosody) of their suprasegmental information (i.e., prosody, sentence
mother tongue from that of other languages. By the melody) and segmental information (phonemes, i.e.,
age of 9 months, they have acquired the inventory of speech sounds relevant for word meaning), (2) lexical-
phonemes as well as the specific stress patterns of their semantic processes (i.e., processes that deal with
mother tongue (for a review, see Jusczyk, 1997). The word forms and word meanings), and (3) syntactic pro-
child now understands the first words and starts to cesses concerning the grammatical relation of different
produce first words between 11 and 13 months, with words in a sentence.
a lexicon of about 50 to 75 items by the age of 16 months
and a clear vocabulary spurt between the age of 18 and Phonological Processes
24 months (see Bates and Goodman, 1999). Syntactic An infant’s first exposure to language is based on
structures are acquired continuously in the second acoustic-phonetic and phonological information (for
and third year of life, with first productions of two- a review, see Kuhl, 2004). As an initial step into language,
word utterances at the age of 18 to 24 months, and later the infant must be able to differentiate speech from non-
more-word utterances. The basic knowledge of the syn- speech sounds. When comparing forward to backward
tactic word order constraints is present around the age played speech in a NIRS experiment, sleeping newborns
of 2.5 years (Höhle et al., 2001). showed a larger increase in cerebral blood volume over
The complexity of the language system is often only the left temporal brain regions for forward speech (Pena
detected when examined developmentally and realized et al., 2003). In an fMRI experiment with 3-month-olds,
when language development derails. The way in which Dehaene-Lambertz et al. (2002) also found that strong
language development can be impaired is multifaceted. left hemispheric activation comprised the superior tem-
It can manifest itself in the inability to acquire phonolog- poral gyrus for speech sounds as measured by forward
ical, semantic, and syntactic information (specific lan- and backward speech compared to silence. This activa-
guage impairment [SLI]) (Levy and Schaeffer, 2003), or tion included Heschl’s gyrus and extended to the supe-
grammatical aspects selectively (grammatical-SLI) rior temporal sulcus and the temporal pole (see Figure 1).
(Van der Lely, 2005), the inability to read (Dyslexia) Differences between forward and backward speech
(Shaywitz et al., 1990), or to understand others (autism) were found in the left angular gyrus and precuneus. An
(Baron-Cohen et al., 1985, 1997). As the former two additional right frontal brain activation observed only
types of impairments are the most relevant when it in awake infants was interpreted to reflect attentional
factors. Both studies suggest a left hemispheric domi-
nance for speech in early infancy which is similar to
*Correspondence: angelafr@cbs.mpg.de that of adults.
Neuron
942

Figure 1. Brain Activation in Response to


Speech Sounds in 3-Month-Olds
(A) Averaged brain activation in response to
speech sounds (forward speech and back-
ward speech against rest) in 3-month-old in-
fants. L, left hemisphere; R, right hemisphere.
(B) (Left panel) Averaged brain activation in 3-
month-old awake infants for forward speech
against backward speech. (Right panel) Aver-
aged hemodynamic responses of forward
speech and backward speech in awake and
asleep infants.

One language-relevant aspect of the speech input are relevant during spoken language perception, as it differ-
features of its intonational contour, which allow the seg- entiates not only between different phonemes (i.e., short
mentation of the input into structural units. Crucially, vowel [full] versus a long vowel [fool]) but also between
prosodic breaks (intonational phrase boundaries) signal words with different stress patterns (i.e., long first sylla-
syntactic phrase boundaries, thus allowing an easy en- ble [the ábstract] versus short first syllable [to abstráct]).
trance into the target language. Using NIRS, Homae There are a number of ERP studies that provide
et al. (2006) investigated the brain basis for prosodic evidence for the early discrimination of different
processes in 3-month-old infants. Examining the hemo- language-relevant sounds and sound patterns. These
dynamic response to normal speech and speech with studies used the auditory oddball paradigm in which
flattened intonational contours, they found bilateral participants are presented with a sequence of stimuli,
activation in the temporo-parietal and the frontal cor- most of which are identical (standard) but interrupted
tices for both conditions. A direct comparison of normal by a deviant stimulus differing on one or more acoustic
and flattened speech revealed the right temporo- parameters.
parietal cortex, suggesting a right hemispheric domi- ERP studies in adults have reported a mismatch
nance for the processing of sentential prosody (pitch negativity (MMN) as a neural correlate of auditory
information) similar to adults (Meyer et al., 2004) to be discrimination in such a paradigm (Näätänen, 1990; for
present already at 3 months of age. a review, see Näätänen et al., 2001). By means of
Using a temporally more sensitive method, namely the MEG, the MMN in adults was localized in the auditory
measurement of event-related brain potentials (ERP), it cortices bilaterally (e.g., Alho et al., 1998). fMRI studies
was shown that the specific ERP component, identified that applied a mismatch paradigm also reported activa-
as an adult brain response to intonational phrase tion in the auditory cortices bilaterally with stronger
boundaries (Steinhauer et al., 1999; Pannekamp et al., activation in the right superior temporal gyrus to reflect
2005), was present in 8-month-olds (Pannekamp et al., the discrimination of pitch and duration in nonspeech
2006). Its topographic distribution over the scalp was sounds and additional frontal activations to be attribut-
similar to adults, but its peak was somewhat delayed. able to attentional processes (Opitz et al., 2002; Doeller
This suggests that the brain systems involved in pro- et al., 2003; Schall et al., 2003; Molholm et al., 2005).
cesses of identifying intonational phrase boundaries When directly comparing speech and music with
are similar in infants and in adults, but take more time respect to duration discrimination, significantly more
in earlier development. activation was found in the left superior temporal gyrus
The other language-relevant aspect in the speech (BA 22/42) for speech and in the right Heschl’s gyrus
input is acoustic information concerning the different (BA 41) for music (Tervaniemi et al., 2006).
phonemes and stress patterns of words in a given While the mismatch response in adults is always
language. The acoustic parameter of duration is most expressed in a negativity, the response in infants is
Review
943

reported as either a negativity (Cheour et al., 1998; Kush- Friederici et al., 2003) and sometimes the basal ganglia
nerenko et al., 2001) or a positivity (Dehaene-Lambertz bilaterally (Friederici et al., 2003).
and Dehaene, 1994; Friederici et al., 2002; Leppänen The adequate description of the brain basis of the
et al., 1999; Weber et al., 2004) and sometimes with dif- N400 observed for pseudowords requires that the
ferent scalp distributions and latencies (for a review, see paradigms used in the fMRI studies and the ERP ex-
Cheour et al., 2000). periments are comparable. Such fMRI studies found
Presenting a sequence of five syllables with the last a variety of activations including anterior and inferior
one either being the same or a different one, Dehaene- portions of the left temporal lobe (Mummery et al.,
Lambertz and Dehaene (1994) reported a mismatch 1999), anterior and middle portions of the left superior
response in the form of a posterior positivity for the temporal gyrus (Kotz et al., 2002), and the inferior frontal
deviant syllable (/ga/) compared to the standard syllable gyrus/sulcus bilaterally (Mummery et al., 1999; Rossell
(/ba/) in 2- to 3-month-old infants. An MMN-like negativ- et al., 2001; Kotz et al., 2002). The frontal activations
ity was found for vowel discrimination in the first months are discussed as being related to task demands,
of life (Cheour et al., 1997) and even in newborns whereas the temporal activations are taken to reflect
(Cheour-Luhtanen et al., 1995). Mismatch responses lexical processes.
either expressed as a negativity or as a positivity have
been observed in different languages, such as Finnish, Lexical and Semantic Processes at the Word Level
German, and English, for vowel contrasts (Cheour An ERP study with 11-month-old infants suggests a dif-
et al., 1997; Leppänen et al., 1999; Pihko et al., 1999; ferential brain reaction to known and unknown words ex-
Friederici et al., 2002, 2004) and for consonant contrasts pressed as a negativity around 200 ms after word onset
(Dehaene-Lambertz and Baillet, 1998; Rivera-Gaxiola with longer amplitude to familiar versus unfamiliar words
et al., 2005) early during development. (Thierry et al., 2003). Testing 14- to 20-month-olds, Mills
Interestingly, evidence for a language-specific phone- et al. (2004) also found a negativity between 200 and 400
mic discrimination seems to establish between the age ms that was larger for known than for unknown words.
of 6 and 12 months (Cheour et al., 1998; Rivera-Gaxiola The distribution of which, however, seems to change
et al., 2005). While younger infants aged 6 and 7 months from bilateral at 13 months to left hemisphere dominant
show discrimination for the phonemic contrast relevant at 20 months of age (Mills et al., 1997). These data dem-
and not-relevant for their target language, older infants onstrate that by the end of their first year infants are able
aged 11 and 12 months only display a discrimination to differentiate between familiar and unfamiliar words,
response for the phonemic contrast in their target but it is not clear whether infants at this age process
language. the semantics of words similar to adults.
The importance of word stress for word recognition In another word-processing study, the effects of word
during speech perception was shown in a recent ERP experience (training) and vocabulary size (word produc-
study with infants learning Dutch (Kooijman et al., tion) were tested (Mills et al., 1997). In this word-learning
2005). In this study, 10-month-olds recognized two- paradigm, 20-month-olds acquired novel words either
syllable words with stress on the first syllable in contin- paired with a novel object or without an object. After
uous speech after they had heard the words in isolation. training, the infants’ ERPs showed a repetition effect
Recognition was reflected in a greater negativity indicated by a reduced N200-500 amplitude to familiar
between 350 and 500 ms over the left hemisphere for and novel unpaired words, whereas ERPs indicated an
familiar words than for unfamiliar words. increased bilaterally distributed N200-500 for novel
paired words. This finding is taken to indicate that the
Lexical-Semantic Processes N200-500 is linked to word meaning. The interpretation
In adults, a particular ERP component, the N400 (i.e., a of this early effect as a semantic one is challenged, given
negative going wave form peaking at around 400 ms), that semantic effects in adults are observed later, i.e., in
has been identified to correlate with lexical-semantic the N400.
processes. The semantic N400 effect is reflected in Friedrich and Friederici (2004, 2005a, 2005b) used an
a larger amplitude for words that are semantically incon- ERP paradigm appropriate to test for semantic knowl-
gruous to a given context than words that are congruous edge and knowledge about lexical forms in young
and it is taken to indicate semantic integration difficul- children between 12 and 19 months. In this paradigm,
ties. Moreover, the N400 amplitude is found to be larger a picture of an object is presented together with an
for pseudowords than for words. This finding is inter- auditory stimulus that is either a word matching the
preted to reflect the difficulty in identifying a lexical object’s name or not (semantic knowledge) or with
representation for the pseudoword in the mental lexicon a pseudoword that is phonotactically legal or illegal
(for reviews, see Kutas and Federmeier, 2000; Kutas and (phonological knowledge). In 12-month-olds, an early
van Petten, 1994). fronto-central negativity between 100-400 ms was
Concerning the neural basis of semantic processes, found for words that were congruous with a picture
MEG studies with adults localized the sentential N400 compared to words that were incongruous. This early
in the auditory cortex bilaterally (Halgren et al., 2002; effect was interpreted as a familiarity effect reflecting
Mäkelä et al., 2001) or with an additional left inferior the fulfillment of a lexical expectation after seeing the
frontal source (Maess et al., 2006). fMRI experiments picture of an object. At the age of 14 months, an N400
with adults applying comparable sentential paradigms effect was observed for semantically incongruous
indicate an involvement of the middle and superior words in addition to the early negativity effect for the
temporal gyri bilaterally with a larger activation in the congruous words. By 19 months, an N400 was found
left hemisphere (Kuperberg et al., 2000; Ni et al., 2000; for semantically incongruous and for phonotactically
Neuron
944

legal pseudowords, but not for phonotactically illegal aged 19 and 24 months (Friedrich and Friederici, 2005c).
pseudowords. This indicates that at this age both real This study used sentences in which there was the
words and phonotactically legal pseudowords are con- presence or absence of a semantic mismatch between
sidered as word candidates, whereas phonotactically the verb and its object argument (e.g., The cat drinks
illegal pseudowords are not. The observed semantic the ball/the milk). For 19-month-olds, a first negativity
N400 effect in 14- and 19-month-olds reached signifi- was observed between 400 and 500 ms, followed by
cance later, lasted longer, and had more frontal distribu- a sustained negativity between 600 and 1200 ms. In
tion when compared to adults (compare Figure 3). The 24-month-olds, the negativity was found to start at
latency differences suggest slower lexical-semantic 300 ms and to last until 1200 ms, whereas in adult
processing in children than in adults. The more frontal listeners, the N400 effect was present between 300
distribution could mean either that children’s semantic and 800 ms. These data indicate that the processes
processes are still more image-based, as adults show underlying the N400 effect in children are similar to
a frontal distribution when pictures instead of words adults, but that the integration of the object noun into
are processed (West and Holcomb, 2002), or that chil- the sentence context requires more processing time in
dren recruit frontal brain regions associated with in- young children.
creased attentional processes (e.g., Opitz et al., 2002) Semantic processes of word integration into senten-
in addition to those subserving semantic processing. tial context have been tested in English and German
There are a few fMRI studies with children investigat- children between the ages of 1 year and 26 years using
ing semantic processes at the word level. Using different sentence constructions. The studies available
a semantic judgment task requiring the evaluation of suggest an early sensitivity to semantic anomalies as
the semantic relatedness of two auditory words, a study reflected by an N400-like negativity (1) whose duration
with 9- to 15-year-old children revealed activation in the decreases with age, as shown in children between
temporal gyri bilaterally (BA 22), in the left middle tempo- 19 months and 2 years (Friedrich and Friederici, 2005c)
ral gyrus (BA 21), and in the inferior frontal gyri bilaterally and between 5 and 15 years (Holcomb et al., 1992), indi-
(BA 47/45) (Chou et al., 2006). Correlations with age were cating faster integration processes, (2) whose amplitude
observed in the left middle temporal (BA 21) and the decreases linearly between 5 and 15 years, indicating
right inferior frontal gyrus (BA 47). The increased frontal less reliance on sentential context with older age
activation was interpreted to reflect a broader semantic (Holcomb et al., 1992), and (3) whose distribution is
search and the temporal activation to relate to more wider in younger children than in older children or adults
efficient access to lexical-semantic representations. (Friedrich and Friederici, 2005c; Atchley et al., 2006;
Using a semantic categorization task, a recent fMRI Holcomb et al., 1992). Its general morphology, however,
study with 5- to 10-year-old children found activation appears to remain similar across the age from childhood
in similar frontal and temporal regions of the left hemi- to adulthood.
sphere and in the left fusiform gyrus (BA 37, BA 20), sug- Given the ERP evidence, it is likely that the same brain
gesting language to be left-lateralized as early as 5 years regions observed in adults are recruited during child-
(Balsamo et al., 2006). hood. The available fMRI studies conducted with chil-
dren at the sentential level, however, have not looked
Semantic Processes at the Sentential Level at semantic processes in isolation but rather at text com-
The processing of semantic information at the sentential prehension. fMRI data from 6- to 10-year-olds listening
level has only recently been investigated in children to short texts (compared to resting baseline) revealed
younger than 4 years of age (Silva-Pereyra et al., bilateral activation in the auditory cortices, specifically
2005a; Friedrich and Friederici, 2005b). Previous studies in the superior temporal gyrus and Heschl’s gyrus, the
with 5- to 26-year-olds (Holcomb et al., 1992), 8- to planum temporale, as well as in the inferior frontal gyrus,
13-year-olds (Atchley et al., 2006), and 6- to 13-year- the anterior cingulate regions, and parietal regions
olds (Hahne et al., 2004) reported N400-like negativities (Ulualp et al., 1998). When listening to stories (compared
for semantically anomalous sentences in children of all to reversed speech), 6-year-old children showed activa-
age groups. In a study with 2.5-, 3-, and 4-year-olds, tion in the left superior temporal gyrus and sulcus
English learning children listened passively to sentence extending back to the angular gyrus and in the left middle
stimuli that were either semantically correct or anoma- temporal gyrus (Ahmad et al., 2003). When listening to
lous (e.g., My uncle will watch/blow the movie) (Silva- stories in which content words were missing (compared
Pereyra et al., 2005a, 2005b). Children at 2.5 years to resting baseline), 6- to 14-year-olds displayed activa-
showed an early starting frontally distributed negativity tion in anterior superior temporal and posterior temporal
peaking around 500 ms for semantically anomalous areas bilaterally and in the ‘‘classical inferior-frontal
compared to normal sentences (Silva-Pereyra et al., language areas’’ (Wilke et al., 2005). In this study, the
2005a). In contrast, 3- and 4-year-olds demonstrated a inferior frontal activation was interpreted to result from
negative slow wave negativity peaking at around 400 semantic memory retrieval necessary to fill in the missing
ms, 600 ms, and 800 ms. According to the authors, these word. Future fMRI studies separating semantic and syn-
anteriorly distributed negativities may reflect either one tactic processes at the sentential level will have to reveal
general semantic integration mechanisms or different which of the activations found for text comprehension
semantic mechanisms, which, however, are not speci- are semantic in nature.
fied further, except the late negativity, taken to reflect
sentence closure. Syntactic Processes
An N400-like semantic effect at the sentence level has ERP studies of syntactic processing in adults have
been reported even in younger German learning children shown that violations of syntactic rules are associated
Review
945

Figure 2. Averaged Event-Related Brain Po-


tentials to Auditorily Presented Correct and
Syntactically Incorrect Sentences in 24-
Month-Olds, 32-Month-Olds, and Adults
Vertical line indicates the onset of the violat-
ing word. (A) ERPs at left anterior electrode
F7 for the early syntactic effect (ELAN). (B)
Distribution maps for the difference between
correct and incorrect sentences in the time
windows in which the ELAN effect is ex-
pected. Dark blue indicates negativity, which
is clearly present at left anterior site for 32-
month-olds and for adults. (C) ERPs at cen-
tro-parietal electrode PZ for the late syntactic
effect (P600).

with two ERP components: a late, centro-parietal Studies investigating morphosyntactic violations in
positivity (P600) preceded by a left anterior negativity English (e.g., My uncle will watch/watching the movie),
(LAN), or an early LAN (ELAN). The P600 is not only which usually elicit an LAN-P600 pattern in adults,
seen for syntactically anomalous sentences requiring reported no LAN effects but only a P600-like positivity
syntactic repair, but also for temporally syntactic ambig- for 3- and 4-year-olds (Silva-Pereyra et al., 2005a). For
uous sentences requiring syntactic reanalysis (Osterh- slightly younger 30-month-old children, the positivity
out and Holcomb, 1992; Osterhout et al., 1994) and observed between 600 and 1000 ms did not reach signif-
moreover appears to modulate as a function of syntactic icance (Silva-Pereyra et al., 2005b).
complexity (Kaan et al., 2000). According to a recent A recent ERP study in German investigating the
neurocognitive model of auditory language comprehen- processing of local phrase structure violations (e.g.,
sion (Friederici, 2002), the ELAN component is viewed The lion in the roars instead of The lion in the zoo roars),
to reflect difficulties in the process of initial local which elicits an ELAN-P600 in adults, found a biphasic
structure building. The LAN is taken to reflect difficulties ERP pattern consisting of a delayed ELAN and a late
in morpho-syntactic processing during the assignment P600 in children at 2.5 years (Oberecker et al., 2005).
of grammatical relations in a second stage. The P600 When applying the same phrase structure violation
is seen to reflect difficulties in the stage of integration paradigm to 2-year-olds, the ERPs revealed a late
where syntactic and thematic structure have to be P600, but no ELAN component nor any other left-
mapped onto each other. lateralized negativity preceding the P600 (Oberecker
The brain basis of these processes has been investi- and Friederici, 2006) (see Figure 2). The combined
gated in adults using space- and time-sensitive imaging results indicate that the neural basis for the ELAN and
methods. Studies investigating syntactic processes the P600 follow a different developmental pace. What
using fMRI usually report superior temporal activation could be the explanation for the finding that the ELAN
(including the anterior and posterior portion) as well as establishes later than the P600? Adult data indicate
activation in the inferior frontal gyrus (including Broca’s that the ELAN reflects highly automatic processes of
area and/or the frontal operculum) (for reviews, see initial structure building that are unaffected by
Bookheimer, 2002; Friederici, 2002; Grodzinsky and strategy-inducing factors such a instruction (Hahne
Friederici, 2006). The anterior portion of the superior and Friederici, 2002) or a variation of the amount of
temporal gyrus and the frontal operculum have been correct versus incorrect sentences in an experimental
identified as those brain regions that are involved in set (Hahne and Friederici, 1999). In contrast, the P600
syntactic phrase structure building processes reflected modulates as a function of these factors and thus seems
by the ELAN (Friederici et al., 2000, 2006). to reflect late controlled processes. On the basis of
Currently, no fMRI study and only very few ERP stud- these adult findings, the ERP data of the children would
ies on syntactic processing in children are available. suggest that those highly automatic processes reflected
Neuron
946

by the ELAN are not yet established in 2-year-olds, tion is not yet clear, our conclusion will only consider
whereas processes of syntactic and thematic integra- deviances from the normal pattern and not their direc-
tion are reflected by the P600 are. By pure logic, the find- tion. Functional abnormalities are seen in speech/
ing of a P600 in the absence of the ELAN would mean language-relevant areas in the temporal and inferior
that online syntactic structure building either was pres- frontal cortex but also for those structures in the basal
ent by not reflected in the ERP or that the build up of the ganglia, namely caudate and putamen known to be
sentence’s structure was not entirely based on syntactic crucial for controlling and selecting motor sequences
features (as the preposition in not only constrains the necessary for articulation (Volkmann et al., 1992).
next word syntactically [i.e., noun instead of verb] but Structural studies indicate reduced gray matter
also semantically [i.e., location instead of action]). The volume and density of Broca’s area (BA 44/45) and
presence of a delayed but adult-like ELAN component increased gray matter volume and density for the poste-
at 2.5 years appears to indicate that the neural mecha- rior temporal gyrus/sulcus bilaterally (Vargha-Khadem
nisms of online syntactic structure building are present et al., 1998; Belton et al., 2002). All studies available
at this age, although the processes are clearly slower found reduced volume or gray matter density in the
in children than in adults. head of the caudate nucleus (Vargha-Khadem et al.,
In summary, the studies on normal language develop- 1998; Belton et al., 2002; Watkins et al., 2002). This univ-
ment indicate that the language-related ERP compo- ocal finding for the subcortical structures has led to the
nents reflecting (1) lexical-semantic processes (N400 suggestion that the genetic abnormality in the KE family
by ERP) that involve the middle and superior temporal may selectively affect the development of the caudate
gyri and possibly the left inferior frontal gyrus, (2) syntac- nucleus or the basal ganglia in general (Watkins et al.,
tic processes (ELAN/LAN-P600 by ERP) which in adults 1999).
rely on the left superior temporal gyrus mainly its ante- The genetic analyses conducted initially proposed
rior and posterior portion and the left inferior frontal region 7q31 at chromosome 7 (Fisher et al., 1998) and
gyrus, and (3) prosodic processes (CPS by ERP) which later more specifically FOXP2 located on chromosome
rely on the superior temporal and frontal opercular 7q31 (Lai et al., 2001) as being functionally related to
cortex of the right hemisphere appear to change in their the observed pathology of speech and language in the
latency and duration, but not in their basic morphology KE family. In further studies, FOXP2 expression was
from childhood to adulthood. correlated with the development of motor-related brain
circuits in the mouse and the human (Lai et al., 2003).
Neural Correlates of Language Impairment This latter finding is in line with the phenotype of the
Before turning to the studies on individuals diagnosed KE family, which is characterized by severe facial dys-
with SLI, I will briefly discuss a unique family, spanning praxia, affecting an articulatory movements accompa-
four generations, in which about half of the members nied by expressive and receptive language impairments
of the first three generations are affected by a severe and which can clearly be differentiated from the SLI
disorder of language production. phenotype. A differentiation of these two phenotypes
The KE family was initially described as a severe form is supported by genetic evidence that FOXP2 is not
of verbal apraxia (Hurst et al., 1990). Subsequently, the a major susceptibility gene for SLI (Newbury et al.,
family’s deficit has been described as a linguistic deficit 2002; O’Brien et al., 2003) and therefore cannot be
concerning inflectional morphology (Gopnik, 1990; considered to be ‘‘the language gene.’’
Gopnik and Crago, 1991). Further studies indicated
that the disorder is not specific to language, but affects Specific Language Impairment
the processing of phonology and syntax as well as non- Specific language impairment (SLI) is defined as a devel-
linguistic oral praxia (Vargha-Khadem et al., 1995; opmental disorder that selectively affects the domain of
Watkins et al., 1999). Thus, it appears that the primary language processing. Children with SLI perform below
deficit is a nonlinguistic one that affects language in their age on language tasks requiring the processing
a secondary manner, and therefore, studies of this fam- of phonological, semantic, or syntactic information
ily may not directly reflect true language abnormalities. (Levy and Schaeffer, 2003), despite normal intelligence,
Members of the KE family have been investigated with an adequate learning environment, and the absence of
respect to abnormalities in brain function and brain peripheral hearing problems or emotional problems
structure. The functional data available have used differ- (Bishop, 1992). Its genetic basis has been described
ent methods (PET, fMRI) and different tasks (word by the SLI Consortium (2002, 2004) as involving chromo-
repetition, verb generation). These report overactiva- somes 16q and 19q.
tions and underactivations for particular brain regions. Different views on the underlying deficit of SLI have
Liegeois et al. (2003) using verb generation tasks in the been put forward. One view posits that children with
fMRI reported underactivation for Broca’s area (BA SLI, or at least certain subgroups, suffer from a deficit
44/45), and Vargha-Khadem et al. (1998) found overacti- specific to the domain of language or a subdomain,
vation in Broca’s area for a word-repetition task in a PET i.e., grammar (Clahsen, 1989; Rice and Oetting, 1993;
study. The latter study registered underactivation in Van der Lely, 1994). Another perspective holds that
the left posterior middle temporal gyrus, whereas the SLI is not due to a processing deficit specific to lan-
former found overactivation in the superior and middle guage but to a deficit in temporal auditory processing
temporal gyrus bilaterally. Abnormalities in the basal (Merzenich et al., 1993; Tallal et al., 1993) or even to def-
ganglia (again over and underactivation) were reported icits that are general in nature, such as a reduced capac-
for the caudate nucleus and the putamen. As the func- ity in processing (Bishop, 1994; Kail, 1994), or a deficit in
tional interpretation of overactivation and underactiva- the procedural memory system (Ullman and Pierpont,
Review
947

Figure 3. Averaged ERP for Spoken Words Congruous and Incongruous with a Picture Presented
Vertical line indicates the word onset. (A) ERPs of typically developing children and adults showing a semantic N400 effect except for the
youngest group. (B) ERPs of 19-month-old infants who were diagnosed with risk for SLI at the age of 2.5 years.

2005). All of these views are based on empirical data— semantic mismatch between the word and the picture
mostly, however, from behavioral studies. Here, I will context.
not advocate one or the other view, but rather review An ERP study with 10- to 12-year-old children diag-
the neurophysiological studies conducted with children nosed with SLI on the processing of semantic informa-
with SLI. tion at the sentence level still reveals a pattern of brain
responses that are different from age-matched typically
Phonological and Lexical-Semantic Processes developing children. The latter show an N400-P600
Neurophysiologically, 2-month-old infants from families pattern in response to a semantic violation, whereas
at risk for SLI demonstrate a massively delayed brain the SLI children show no N400 component, but only
responses for the discrimination of two syllables with a P600 (see Figure 4). The absence of the N400 effect
vowels of different durations, i.e., long versus short suggests weaker lexical-semantic representation of
(Friedrich et al., 2004). Note that a mismatch discrimina- the critical words (Sabisch et al., 2006).
tion response for long versus short vowels in CV sylla- Thus, ERP studies with SLI children indicate that
bles even differentiated newborns with risk for dyslexia those who are diagnosed with impaired verbal language
from those without risk (Leppänen et al., 1999; Pihko skills at the age of 3 or later fail to show normal age-
et al., 1999). adequate ERP patterns already during their first months
By the age of 4–5 months, infants from families at risk of life. During the first months of life, infants at risk for
displayed a reduced brain response for the discrimina- SLI are characterized by a massively delayed mismatch
tion of the stress pattern of a two-syllable word (Weber response for the discrimination of syllable duration.
et al., 2004). A longitudinal study showed in a retrospec- Such a delay may affect processes at the level of
tive analysis that the reduced brain response observed a word’s phonological stress pattern, at the level of
by the age of 4–5 months correlated with the children’s word form and word semantics, and even semantic pro-
impaired language abilities diagnosed at the age of cesses at the sentential level. The studies reviewed indi-
4 years (Weber et al., 2005). These data suggest cate that impairments can indeed be observed at all of
that early insufficiencies in the processing of duration these levels. Whether the N400 deficits observed in the
information can lead to language impairments in pro- latter two studies, however, are based on a primary
duction and comprehension later in life. deficit in temporal auditory processing cannot be deter-
With respect to semantic processes, children who mined on the basis of the data presented.
were diagnosed as having an advanced risk for SLI The deficit in phonological and lexical-semantic
based on behavioral tasks at the age of 2.5 years already processes as revealed by the SLI children’s ERP pattern
by 19 months of age demonstrate ERP patterns for can be related to a recent fMRI study with a Finnish
lexical-semantic processes that differed from their age- family diagnosed to suffer from SLI. Hugdahl et al.
matched controls. These children did not show an N400 (2004) conducted an auditory fMRI experiment with
as found with normally developing children at this age five members of a family with SLI (10–70 years) and six
(Friedrich and Friederici, 2006) and even at the age of age-matched controls. The controls’ brain activation to
14 months (Friedrich and Friederici, 2005b) (see Fig- vowels, pseudowords, and words was bilateral in the
ure 3). The absence of the N400 effect in 19-month-old superior and middle temporal gyri with a slight laterali-
SLI children may reflect insufficient lexical-semantic zation to the left, areas usually involved in auditory, pho-
representations that prohibit the normal detection of the nological, and word processing. There was also a small
Neuron
948

Figure 4. Averaged ERPs to Auditorily Presented Correct and Semantically Incorrect Sentences
Vertical line indicates the onset of the violating word. (A) ERPs of typically developing children and adults showing a semantic N400 effect. (B)
ERPs of 10- to 12-year-old children diagnosed with SLI.

activation in the right inferior frontal lobe, a region known Neuroanatomy of the SLI Brain
to support memory and attention (Demonet et al., 1994). The brain basis of SLI is not well investigated. There are,
In the SLI family, the activation was also bilateral in the however, a number of MRI studies assessing structural
temporal lobes, but it was smaller and weaker than in brain abnormalities in children with SLI (for an overview,
controls. The reduction of the activation in the STS/ see Ullman and Pierpont, 2005). Regional abnormalities
MTG, in areas usually involved in speech perception have been reported for language-related and other
and the mapping of acoustic-phonetic cues onto lexical areas. Investigating 9-year-olds, Gauger et al. (1997)
representations (Scott and Johnsrude, 2003), could found a volume decrease in the left pars triangularis as
likely be related to the impairments in decoding the part of the Broca’s area, and Jernigan et al. (1991)
phonological structure of pseudowords and words. This reported a volume decrease in the left posterior tempo-
interpretation would also be in line with the observed ral region. Moreover, it appears that children with SLI
ERP abnormalities in SLI children. have a less leftward structural asymmetry for frontal
and temporal language-related regions (Gauger et al.,
Syntactic Processes 1997; Plante et al., 1991).
With respect to syntactic processes, Van der Lely (2005) In addition to abnormalities in the temporal, orbito-
hypothesizes a specific subgroup of SLI characterized frontal, dorsolateral, and medial frontal cortex, abnor-
by selective impairment of a syntactic processing com- malities in the inferior frontal regions in SLI children
ponent in the core language system. Others view the have been reported in some studies (Gauger et al.,
grammatical impairment to result from a deficit in proce- 1997; Jernigan et al., 1991). In adults, the inferior frontal
dural memory (Ullman and Pierpont, 2005). gyrus (Broca’s area) has been functionally defined to
In one of the first neurophysiological studies, Neville support phonological processes (dorsal portion) and
et al. (1993) compared children with language impair- semantic processes (anterior ventral portion) as well
ment with typically developing children. SLI children as syntactic processes (posterior ventral portion) (Book-
presented a normal ERP pattern for the processing of heimer, 2002; Friederici, 2002). In the absence of de-
content words, but not for function words that carry tailed fMRI studies in normal children, it is not entirely
grammatical information. Function words that normally clear to which functional aspects the observed struc-
elicit a LAN evoked a more bilateral or even right- tural abnormalities should be related. There is, however,
lateralized negativity in language-impaired children. a recent proposal which takes frontal abnormality to
In an ERP study with 12- to 14-year-old children diag- support the notion of a deficit in procedural memory
nosed with a selective grammatical impairment (G-SLI) (Ullman and Pierpont, 2005).
on the basis of behavior tests (e.g., Van der Lely et al., Leonard et al. (2002) compared the neuroanatomy of
1998; Van der Lely, 2005), it was found that these chil- children with SLI and those suffering from dyslexia. They
dren do not demonstrate the syntax-related ELAN com- reported a reduced leftward asymmetry in the planum
ponent for violations of nonlocal syntactic violations temporale for the SLI group. Like most other anatomical
observed in normal age-matched controls. As these studies, this one suggests a reduced volume in the left
children showed an N400 in relation to semantic aspects planum temporale. In adults, this area is known to be in-
of processing, the results were taken as evidence for volved in the segregation and identification of sound pat-
a selective impairment of grammatical aspects of lan- terns in speech and other acoustic patterns containing
guage processing (Van der Lely and Fonteneau, 2006). spectotemporal information (Griffiths and Warren, 2002).
Review
949

A structural abnormality in the planum temporale could Bookheimer, S. (2002). Functional MRI of language: New ap-
explain the deficit in temporal auditory processing ob- proaches to understanding the cortical organization of semantic
processing. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 25, 151–188.
served behaviorally (Tallal et al., 1993) and in ERP studies
(Friedrich et al., 2004), which in turn could have major Cheour, M., Alho, K., Sainio, K., Reinikainen, K., Renlund, M.,
Aaltonen, O., Eerola, O., and Näätänen, R. (1997). The mismatch
impact on phonological and therefore on semantic and
negativity to changes in speech sounds at the age of three months.
syntactic processes. Dev. Neuropsychol. 13, 167–174.
Given the limited data available to explain SLI, we are
Cheour, M., Ceponiene, R., Lehtokoski, A., Luuk, A., Allik, J., Alho,
not in the position to decide which of the views put
K., and Näätänen, R. (1998). Development of language-specific pho-
forward is the most valid. It may well be that there are neme representations in the infant brain. Nat. Neurosci. 1, 351–353.
at least two different subgroups, one that affects gram-
Cheour, M., Leppanen, P., and Kraus, N. (2000). Mismatch negativity
matical processes specifically, be it due to a deficit in (MMN) as a tool for investigating auditory discrimination and sen-
the core language system (Van der Lely, 2005) or to sory memory in infants and children. Clin. Neurophysiol. 111, 4–16.
a deficit in the procedural memory system (Ullman and Cheour-Luhtanen, M., Alho, K., Kujala, T., Sainio, K., Reinikainen, K.,
Pierpont, 2005), and one that affects all aspects of Renlund, M., Aaltonen, O., Eerola, O., and Näätänen, R. (1995).
language processing due to a deficit in auditory tempo- Mismatch negativity indicates vowel discrimination in newborns.
ral processing (Tallal et al., 1993). If this is the case, the Hear. Res. 82, 53–58.
former group should show abnormalities in the left Chou, T.L., Booth, J.R., Burman, D.D., Bitan, T., Bigio, J.D., Lu, D.,
inferior frontal cortex, whereas the latter group should and Cone, N.E. (2006). Developmental changes in the neural corre-
demonstrate abnormalities in the superior temporal lates of semantic processing. Neuroimage 29, 1141–1149.
gyrus, in particular the planum temporale. Clahsen, H. (1989). The grammatical characterization of develop-
mental dysphasia. Linguistics 27, 897–920.
Conclusions Dehaene-Lambertz, G., and Baillet, S. (1998). A phonological repre-
The available studies on the neural basis of normal sentation in the infant brain. Neuroreport 9, 1885–1888.
language development suggest that the brain systems Dehaene-Lambertz, G., and Dehaene, S. (1994). Speed and cerebral
underlying language processing are in place already in correlates of syllable discrimination in infants. Nature 370, 292–295.
early development. The processes supported by these Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Dehaene, S., and Hertz-Pannier, L. (2002).
developing brain systems change quantitatively though Functional neuroimaging of speech perception in infants. Science
not qualitatively over time. In contrast, impaired lan- 298, 2013–2015.
guage development is correlated with abnormalities in Demonet, J.F., Price, C., Wise, R., and Frackowiak, R.S.J. (1994).
the neurophysiological patterns of different aspects Differential activation of right and left posterior sylvian regions by
of language processing and with abnormalities in the semantic and phonological tasks—a positron-emission tomography
study in normal human subjects. Neurosci. Lett. 182, 25–28.
structures of areas known to support language pro-
cesses in the healthy adult brain. Doeller, C.F., Opitz, B., Mecklinger, A., Krick, C., Reith, W., and
Schröger, E. (2003). Prefrontal cortex involvement in preattentive
References auditory deviance detection: neuroimaging and electrophysiological
evidence. Neuroimage 20, 1270–1282.
Ahmad, Z., Balsamo, L.M., Sachs, B.C., Xu, B., and Gaillard, W.E. Fisher, S.E., Vargha-Khadem, F., Watkins, K.E., Monaco, A.P., and
(2003). Auditory comprehension of language in young children - Pembrey, M.E. (1998). Localisation of a gene implicated in a severe
Neural networks identified with fMRI. Neurology 60, 1598–1605. speech and language disorder. Nat. Genet. 18, 168–170.
Alho, K., Winkler, I., Escera, C., Huotilainen, M., Virtanen, J., Jaaske- Friederici, A.D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence
lainen, I.P., Pekkonen, E., and Ilmoniemi, R.J. (1998). Processing of processing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 78–84.
novel sounds and frequency changes in the human auditory cortex:
Friederici, A.D., Wang, Y., Herrmann, C.S., Maess, B., and Oertel, U.
Magnetoencephalographic recordings. Psychophysiology 35,
(2000). Localization of early syntactic processes in frontal and tem-
211–224.
poral cortical areas: A magnetoencephalographic study. Hum. Brain
Atchley, R.A., Rice, M.L., Betz, S.K., Kwasny, K.M., Sereno, J.A., and Mapp. 11, 1–11.
Jongman, A. (2006). A comparison of semantic and syntactic event
related potentials generated by children and adults. Brain Lang. Friederici, A.D., Friedrich, M., and Weber, C. (2002). Neural manifes-
99, 236–246. tation of cognitive and precognitive mismatch detection in early in-
fancy. Neuroreport 13, 1251–1254.
Balsamo, L.M., Xu, B., and Gaillard, W.D. (2006). Language laterali-
zation and the role of the fusiform gyrus in semantic processing in Friederici, A.D., Rüschemeyer, S.-A., Hahne, A., and Fiebach, C.J.
young children. Neuroimage 31, 1306–1314. (2003). The role of left inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex
Baron-Cohen, S., Baldwin, D., and Crowson, M. (1985). Do children in sentence comprehension: Localizing syntactic and semantic pro-
with autism use the Speaker’s Direction of Gaze (SDG) strategy to cesses. Cereb. Cortex 13, 170–177.
crack the code of language? Child Dev. 68, 48–57. Friederici, A.D., Gunter, T.C., Hahne, A., and Mauth, K. (2004). The
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A.M., and Frith, U. (1997). Does the autistic relative timing of syntactic and semantic processes in sentence
child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cognition 21, 37–46. comprehension. Neuroreport 15, 165–169.
Bates, E., and Goodman, J.C. (1999). On the emergence of grammar Friederici, A.D., Bahlmann, J., Heim, S., Schubotz, R.I., and Anwan-
from the lexicon. In Emergence of Language, B. McWhinney, ed. der, A. (2006). The brain differentiates human and non-human gram-
(Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates), pp. 29–80. mars: Functional localization and structural connectivity. Proc. Natl.
Belton, E., Salmond, C., Watkins, K., Vargha-Khadem, F., and Acad. Sci. USA 103, 2458–2463.
Gadian, D. (2002). Bilateral grey matter abnormalities in a family Friedrich, M., and Friederici, A.D. (2004). N400-like semantic incon-
with a mutation in FOPX2. Neuroimage 16, 10144. gruity effect in 19-month-olds: Processing known words in picture
Bishop, D.V.M. (1992). The underlying nature of specific language contexts. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1465–1477.
impairment. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 33, 3–66. Friedrich, M., and Friederici, A.D. (2005a). Phonotactic knowledge
Bishop, D.V.M. (1994). Is specific language impairment a valid diag- and lexical-semantic processing in one-year-olds: Brain responses
nostic category? Genetic and psycholinguistic evidence. Philos. to words and nonsense words in picture contexts. J. Cogn.
Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 346, 105–111. Neurosci. 17, 1785–1802.
Neuron
950

Friedrich, M., and Friederici, A.D. (2005b). Lexical priming and Kuhl, P.K. (2004). Early language acquisition: Cracking the speech
semantic integration reflected in the ERP of 14-month-olds. code. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 831–843.
Neuroreport 16, 653–656. Kushnerenko, E., Cheour, M., Ceponiene, R., Fellman, V., Renlund,
Friedrich, M., and Friederici, A.D. (2005c). Semantic sentence pro- M., Soininen, K., Alku, P., Koskinen, M., Sainio, K., and Naatanen,
cessing reflected in the event-related potentials of one- and two- R. (2001). Central auditory processing of durational changes in com-
year-old children. Neuroreport 16, 1801–1804. plex speech patterns by newborns: An event-related brain potential
Friedrich, M., and Friederici, A.D. (2006). Early N400 development study. Dev. Neuropsychol. 19, 83–97.
and later language acquisition. Psychophysiology 43, 1–12. Kuperberg, G.R., McGuire, P.K., Bullmore, E.T., Brammer, M.J.,
Friedrich, M., Weber, C., and Friederici, A.D. (2004). Electrophysio- Rabe-Hesketh, S., Wright, I.C., Lythogoe, D.J., Williams, S.C.R.,
logical evidence for delayed mismatch response in infants at-risk and David, A.S. (2000). Common and distinct neural substrates for
for specific language impairment. Psychophysiology 41, 772–782. pragmatic, semantic, and syntactic processing of spoken senten-
ces: an fMRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 321–341.
Gauger, L.M., Lombardino, L.J., and Leonard, C.M. (1997). Brain
morphology in children with specific language impairment. Kutas, M., and Federmeier, K.D. (2000). Electrophysiology reveals
J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 40, 1272–1284. semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends Cogn.
Sci. 4, 463–470.
Gopnik, M. (1990). Genetic basis of grammar defect. Nature 347, 26.
Kutas, M., and van Petten, C.K. (1994). Psycholinguistics electrified:
Gopnik, M., and Crago, M. (1991). Familial aggregation of a develop-
Event-related brain potential investigations. In Handbook of Psy-
mental language disorder. Cognition 39, 1–50.
cholinguistic, M.A. Gernsbacher, ed. (New York: Academic Press),
Griffiths, T.D., and Warren, J.D. (2002). The planum temporale as pp. 83–143.
a computational hub. Trends Neurosci. 25, 348–353.
Kotz, S.A., Cappa, S.F., von Cramon, D.Y., and Friederici, A.D.
Grodzinsky, Y., and Friederici, A.D. (2006). Neuroimaging of syntax (2002). Modulation of the lexical-semantic network by auditory se-
and syntactic processing. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 16, 240–246. mantic priming: an event-related functional MRI study. Neuroimage
Hahne, A., and Friederici, A.D. (1999). Electrophysiological evidence 17, 1761–1772.
for two steps in syntactic analysis: Early automatic and late con- Lai, C.S.L., Fisher, S.E., Hurst, J.A., Vargha-Khadem, F., and Mon-
trolled processes. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 11, 194–205. aco, A.P. (2001). A novel forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a se-
Hahne, A., and Friederici, A.D. (2002). Differential task effect on se- vere speech and language disorder. Nature 413, 519–523.
mantic and syntactic processes as revealed by ERPs. Brain Res. Lai, C.S.L., Gerrelli, D., Monaco, A., Fisher, S., and Copp, A. (2003).
Cogn. Brain Res. 13, 339–356. FOXP2 expression during brain development coincides with adult
Hahne, A., Eckstein, K., and Friederici, A.D. (2004). Brain signatures sites of pathology in a severe speech and language disorder. Brain
of syntactic and semantic processes during children’s language de- 126, 2455–2462.
velopment. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1302–1318. Leonard, C.M., Lombardino, L.J., Walsh, K., Eckert, M.A., Mockler,
Halgren, E., Dhond, R.P., Christensen, N., Van Petten, C., Marin- J.L., Rowe, L.A., Williams, S., and DeBose, C.B. (2002). Anatomical
kovic, K., Lewine, J.D., and Dale, A.M. (2002). N400-like magnetoen- risk factors that distinguish dyslexia from SLI predict reading skill
cephalography responses modulated by semantic context, word in normal children. J. Commun. Disord. 35, 501–531.
frequency, and lexical class in sentences. Neuroimage 17, Leppänen, P.H.T., Pihko, E., Eklund, K.M., and Lyytinen, H. (1999).
1101–1116. Cortical responses of infants with and without a genetic risk for dys-
Höhle, B., Weissenborn, J., Schmitz, M., and Ischebeck, A. (2001). lexia: II. Group effects. Neuroreport 10, 969–973.
Discovering word order regularities: The role of prosodic information Levy, Y., and Schaeffer, J. (2003). Language Competence across
for early parameter setting. In Approaches to Bootstrapping: Phono- Populations: Toward a Definition of Specific Language Impairment
logical, Lexical, Syntactic and Neurophysiological Aspects of Early (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers).
Language Acquisition, J. Weissenborn and B. Höhle, eds. (Amster-
Liegeois, F., Connelly, A., Baldeweg, T., Connelly, A., Gadian, D.G.,
dam: John Benjamins), pp. 249–265.
Mishkin, M., and Vargha-Khadem, F. (2003). Language fMRI abnor-
Holcomb, P.J., Coffey, S.A., and Neville, H.J. (1992). Visual and audi- malities associated with FOXP2 gene mutation. Nat. Neurosci. 6,
tory sentence processing: A developmental analysis using event- 1230–1237.
related brain potentials. Dev. Neuropsychol. 8, 203–241.
Maess, B., Herrmann, C.S., Hahne, A., Nakamura, A., and Friederici,
Homae, F., Watanabe, H., Nakano, T., Asakawa, K., and Taga, G. A.D. (2006). Localizing the distributed language network responsible
(2006). The right hemisphere of sleeping infant perceives sentential for the N400 measured by MEG during auditory sentence process-
prosody. Neurosci. Res. 54, 276–280. ing. Brain Res. 1096, 163–172.
Hugdahl, K., Gundersen, H., Brekke, C., Thomsen, T., Rimol, L.M., Mäkelä, A.M., Makinen, V., Nikkila, M., Ilmoniemi, R.J., and Tiitinen,
and Ersland, L. (2004). fMRI brain activation in a Finnish family H. (2001). Magnetoencephalographic (MEG) localization of the audi-
with specific language impairment compared with a normal control tory N400m: effects of stimulus duration. Neuroreport 12, 249–253.
group. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 47, 162–172.
Merzenich, M.M., Schreiner, C., Jenkins, W., and Wang, X.Q. (1993).
Hurst, J.A., Baraitser, M., Auger, E., Graham, F., and Norrell, S. Neural mechanisms underlying temporal integration, segmentation,
(1990). An extended family with a dominantly inherited speech disor- and input sequence representation—some implications for the ori-
der. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 32, 347–355. gin of learning-disabilities. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 682, 1–22.
Jernigan, T.L., Hesselink, J.R., Sowell, E., and Tallal, P.A. (1991). Meyer, M., Steinhauer, K., Alter, K., Friederici, A.D., and von Cramon,
Cerebral structure on magnetic-resonance-imaging in language- D.Y. (2004). Brain activity varies with modulation of dynamic pitch
impaired and learning-impaired children. Arch. Neurol. 48, 539–545. variance in sentence melody. Brain Lang. 89, 277–289.
Jusczyk, P.W. (1997). The Discovery of Spoken Language (Cam- Mills, D.L., Coffey-Corina, S., and Neville, H.J. (1997). Language
bridge: MIT Press). comprehension and cerebral specialization from 13 to 20 months.
Kaan, E., Harris, A., Gibson, E., and Holcomb, P. (2000). The P600 as Dev. Neuropsychol. 13, 397–445.
an index of syntactic integration difficulty. Lang. Cogn. Process. 15, Mills, D.L., Prat, C., Zangl, R., Stager, C.L., Neville, H.J., and Werker,
159–201. J.F. (2004). Language experience and the organization of brain activ-
Kail, R. (1994). A method for studying the generalized slowing hy- ity to phonetically similar words: ERP evidence from 14-and 20-
pothesis in children with specific language impairment. J. Speech month-olds. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1452–1464.
Hear. Res. 37, 418–421. Molholm, M., Martinez, A., Ritter, W., Javitt, D.C., and Foxe, J.J. (2005).
Kooijman, V., Hagoort, P., and Cutler, A. (2005). Electrophysiological The neural circuitry of pre-attentive auditory change-detection: an
evidence for prelinguistic infants’ word recognition in continuous fMRI study of pitch and duration mismatch negativity generators.
speech. Brain Res. 24, 109–116. Cereb. Cortex 15, 545–551.
Review
951

Mummery, C.J., Patterson, K., Wise, R.J.S., Vandenbergh, R., Price, Schall, U., Johnston, P., Todd, J., Ward, P.B., and Michie, P.T.
C.J., and Hodges, J.R. (1999). Disrupted temporal lobe connections (2003). Functional Neuroanatomy of auditory mismatch processing:
in semantic dementia. Brain 122, 61–73. an event-related fMRI study of duration-deviant oddballs. Neuro-
Näätänen, R. (1990). The role of attention in auditory information pro- image 20, 729–736.
cessing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain Scott, S.K., and Johnsrude, I.S. (2003). The neuroanatomical and
measures of cognitive function. Behav. Brain Sci. 13, 201–232. functional organization of speech perception. Trends Neurosci. 26,
Näätänen, R., Tervaniemi, M., Sussman, E., Paavilainen, P., and Win- 100–107.
kler, I. (2001). Primitive intelligence’ in the auditory cortex. Trends Shaywitz, S.E., Shaywitz, B.A., Fletcher, J.M., and Escobar, M.D.
Neurosci. 24, 283–288. (1990). Prevalance of reading-disability in boys and girls—results
Neville, H.J., Coffey, S.A., Holcomb, P.J., and Tallal, P. (1993). The of the Connecticut Longitudinal Study. JAMA 264, 998–1002.
neurobiology of sensory and language processing in language- Silva-Pereyra, J., Klarman, L., Lin, L.J., and Kuhl, P.K. (2005a).
impaired children. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 5, 235–253. Sentence processing in 30-month-old children: An event-related
Newbury, D.F., Bonora, E., Lamb, J.A., Fisher, S., Lai, C.S.L., Baird, potential study. Neuroreport 16, 645–648.
G., Jannoun, L., Slonims, V., Scott, C.M., Merricks, M.J., et al., and Silva-Pereyra, J., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., and Kuhl, P.K. (2005b). An
International Molecular Genetic Study of Autism Consortium. event-related brain potential study of sentence comprehension in
(2002). FOXP2 is not a major susceptibility gene for autism or spe- preschoolers: semantic and morphosyntactic processing. Brain
cific language impairment. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 70, 1318–1327. Res. 23, 247–258.
Ni, W., Constable, R.T., Mencl, W.E., Pugh, K.R., Fulbright, R.K., SLI Consortium. (2002). A genomewide scan identifies two novel loci
Shaywitz, S.E., Shaywitz, B.A., Gore, J.C., and Shankweiler, D. involved in specific language impairment. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 70,
(2000). An event-related neuroimaging study distinguishing form 384–398.
and content in sentence processing. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 120–133.
SLI Consortium. (2004). Highly significant linkage to the SLI1 locus in
O’Brien, E.K., Zhang, X., Nishimura, C., Tomblin, J.B., and Murray, an expanded sample of individuals affected by specific language im-
J.C. (2003). Association of Specific Language Impairment (SLI) to pairment. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 74, 1225–1238.
the region of 7q31. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 72, 1536–1543.
Steinhauer, K., Alter, K., and Friederici, A.D. (1999). Brain potentials
Oberecker, R., and Friederici, A.D. (2006). Syntactic ERP compo- indicate immediate use of prosodic cues in natural speech process-
nents in 24-month-olds’ sentence comprehension. Neuroreport 17, ing. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 191–196.
1017–1021.
Tallal, P., Miller, S., and Fitch, R.H. (1993). Neurobiological basis of
Oberecker, R., Friedrich, M., and Friederici, A.D. (2005). Neural cor-
speech – a case for the preeminence of temporal processing. Ann.
relates of syntactic processing in two-year-olds. J. Cogn. Neurosci.
N Y Acad. Sci. 682, 27–47.
17, 1667–1678.
Tervaniemi, M., Szameitat, A.J., Kruck, S., Schröger, E., Alter, K., De
Opitz, B., Rinne, R., Mecklinger, A., von Cramon, D.Y., and Schroger,
Baene, W., and Friederici, A.D. (2006). From air oscillations to music
E. (2002). Differential contribution of frontal and temporal cortices to
and speech: Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for
auditory change detection: fMRI and ERP results. Neuroimage 15,
fine-tuned neural networks in audition. J. Neurosci. 26, 8647–8652.
167–174.
Thierry, G., Vihman, M., and Roberts, M. (2003). Familiar words cap-
Osterhout, L., and Holcomb, P.J. (1992). Event-related brain poten-
ture the attention of 11-month-olds in less than 250 ms. Neuroreport
tials elicited by syntactic anomaly. J. Mem. Lang. 31, 785–806.
14, 2307–2310.
Osterhout, L., Holcomb, P.J., and Swinney, D.A. (1994). Brain poten-
Ullman, M.T., and Pierpont, E.I. (2005). Specific language impair-
tials elicited by garden-path sentences—evidence of the application
ment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis.
of verb information during parsing. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem.
Cortex 41, 399–433.
Cogn. 20, 786–803.
Ulualp, S.O., Biswal, B.B., Yetkin, F.Z., and Kidder, T.M. (1998).
Pannekamp, A., Toepel, U., Alter, K., Hahne, A., and Friederici, A.D.
Functional magnetic resonance imaging of auditory cortex in chil-
(2005). Prosody-driven sentence processing: An event-related brain
dren. Laryngoscope 108, 1782–1786.
potential study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 407–421.
Pannekamp, A., Weber, C., and Friederici, A.D. (2006). Prosodic pro- Van der Lely, H.K.J. (1994). Canonical linking rules: Forward vs Re-
cessing at sentence level in infants. Neuroreport 17, 675–678. verse linking in normally developing and specifically language im-
paired children. Cognition 51, 29–72.
Pena, M., Maki, A., Kovacic, D., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Koizumi, H.,
Bouquet, F., and Mehler, J. (2003). Sounds and silence: An optical Van der Lely, H.K. (2005). Domain-specific cognitive systems: insight
topography study of language recognition at birth. Proc. Natl. from Grammatical-SLI. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 53–59.
Acad. Sci. USA 1000, 11702–11705. Van der Lely, H.K., and Fonteneau, E. (2006). ERP signatures in lan-
Pihko, E., Leppänen, P.H.T., Eklund, K.M., Cheour, M., Guttorm, guage-impaired children reveal a domain-specific neural correlate
T.K., and Lyytinen, H. (1999). Cortical responses of infants with of syntactic dependencies. Poster presented at The 19th Annual
and without a genetic risk for dyslexia: I. Age effects. Neuroreport CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing.
10, 901–905. Van der Lely, H.K., Rosen, S., and McClelland, A. (1998). Evidence for
Plante, E., Swisher, L., Vance, R., and Rapcsak, S. (1991). MRI find- a grammar-specific deficit in children. Curr. Biol. 8, 1253–1258.
ings in boys with specific language impairment. Brain Lang. 41, Vargha-Khadem, F., Watkins, K., Alcock, K., Fletcher, P., and Pas-
52–66. singham, R. (1995). Praxic and nonverbal cognitive deficits in a large
Rice, M.L., and Oetting, J.B. (1993). Morphological deficits of chil- family with genetically transmitted speech and language disorder.
dren with SLI – Evaluation of number marking and agreement. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 92, 930–933.
J. Speech Hear. Res. 36, 1249–1257. Vargha-Khadem, F., Watkins, K.E., Price, C.J., Ashburner, J., Al-
Rivera-Gaxiola, M., Klarman, L., Garcia-Sierra, A., and Kuhl, P.K. cock, K.J., Connelly, A., Frackowiak, R.S., Friston, K.J., Pembrey,
(2005). Neural patterns to speech and vocabulary growth in Ameri- M.E., Mishkin, M., et al. (1998). Neural basis of an inherited speech
can infants. Neuroreport 16, 495–498. and language disorder. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 95, 12695–12700.
Rossell, S.L., Bullmore, E.T., Williams, S.C.R., and David, A.S. (2001). Volkmann, J., Hefter, H., Lange, H.W., and Freund, H.J. (1992). Im-
Brain activation during automatic and controlled processing of pairment of temporal organization of speech in basal ganglia dis-
semantic relations: a priming experiment using lexical-decision. eases. Brain Lang. 43, 386–399.
Neuropsychologia 39, 1167–1176. Watkins, K.E., Gadian, D.G., and Vargha-Khadem, F. (1999). Func-
Sabisch, B., Hahne, A., Glass, E., von Suchodoletz, W., and Frieder- tional and structural brain abnormalities associated with a genetic
ici, A.D. (2006). Lexical-semantic processes in children with Specific disorder of speech and language. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 65,
Language Impairment. Neuroreport 17, 1511–1514. 1215–1221.
Neuron
952

Watkins, K.E., Vargha-Khadem, F., Ashburner, J., Passinham, R.,


Connelly, A., Friston, K., Frackowiak, R., Mishkin, M., and Gadian,
D. (2002). MRI analysis of an inherited speech and language disor-
der: Structural brain abnormalities. Brain 125, 465–478.
Weber, C., Hahne, A., Friedrich, M., and Friederici, A.D. (2004).
Discrimination of word stress in early infant perception: Electro-
physiological evidence. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 18, 149–161.
Weber, C., Hahne, A., Friedrich, M., and Friederici, A.D. (2005).
Reduced stress pattern discrimination in 5-month-olds as a marker
of risk for later language impairment: Neurophysiological evidence.
Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 25, 180–187.
West, W.C., and Holcomb, P.J. (2002). Event-related potentials dur-
ing discourse-level semantic integration of complex pictures. Cogn.
Brain Res. 13, 363–375.
Wilke, M., Lidzba, K., Staudt, M., Buchenau, K., Grodd, W., and
Krageloh-Mann, I. (2005). Comprehensive language mapping in chil-
dren, using functional magnetic resonance imaging: what’s missing
counts. Neuroreport 16, 915–919.

You might also like