Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology

ISSN: 1754-9507 (Print) 1754-9515 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/iasl20

“I remember when … ”: The impact of


reminiscence therapy on discourse production in
older adults with cognitive impairment

Naomi Rose, Anne Whitworth, Sharon Smart, Elizabeth Oliver & Jade
Cartwright

To cite this article: Naomi Rose, Anne Whitworth, Sharon Smart, Elizabeth Oliver & Jade
Cartwright (2020): “I remember when … ”: The impact of reminiscence therapy on discourse
production in older adults with cognitive impairment, International Journal of Speech-Language
Pathology, DOI: 10.1080/17549507.2020.1747542

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/17549507.2020.1747542

Published online: 22 Apr 2020.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 61

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=iasl20
International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 2020; Early Online: 1–13

“I remember when … ”: The impact of reminiscence therapy on


discourse production in older adults with cognitive impairment

NAOMI ROSE, ANNE WHITWORTH , SHARON SMART , ELIZABETH OLIVER &


JADE CARTWRIGHT

School of Occupational Therapy, Social Work and Speech Pathology, Faculty of Health Sciences, Curtin
University, Perth, Australia

Abstract
Purpose: Positive outcomes following reminiscence therapy have been reported for older adults with mild cognitive
impairment and dementia in cognition and quality of life and, in a small number of studies, communication. Despite the
close relationship between cognition and language, the impact on communication has received limited attention. This
study aimed to investigate whether the spoken discourse of older adults with mild cognitive impairment or dementia
could be improved within the genre of nostalgic recounts following group reminiscence therapy, and whether change gener-
alised to everyday discourse.
Method: Four females (mean: 87 years, SD: 7.3) who lived in a residential aged care facility and were diagnosed with
mild (n ¼ 2) or major (n ¼ 2) neurocognitive impairment were recruited to attend a group reminiscence programme deliv-
ered in eight one-hour treatment sessions over four weeks. Multiple baseline samples of discourse were obtained in the
week prior to intervention to monitor stability. Macrostructure, rate, informativeness and efficiency of discourse produc-
tion were measured to identify change within nostalgic recounts and monitor evidence of generalised change in everyday
discourse genres. Cognitive performance and quality of life were also monitored.
Result: While variability was evident, significant increases in macrostructure and richness of nostalgic recounts were
found for two participants, with significant generalisation to everyday discourse. Both participants had diagnoses of mild
cognitive impairment. No significant improvements were seen in cognition or quality of life with all participants.
Conclusion: This study provides promising evidence for the spoken recall of memories having the potential to improve
the communication of people with neurocognitive disorders, with some indication that people with milder impairment
may be more amenable to this form of intervention. Nostalgic recounts may provide an explicit context in which speech-
language pathologists can facilitate the planning of spoken production in people with cognitive impairment, and influence
speaking in everyday contexts.

Keywords: discourse production; nostalgic recount; reminiscence therapy; cognitive impairment; dementia; aged care

Introduction presentation, cognitive decline in one or more


The number of people living with mild cognitive domains is always present (DSM-5, 2013). Memory
impairment (MCI) and dementia is expected to loss is frequently the major component of decline and
accelerate globally in coming years, with growing the most common early symptom of dementia
interest in evidence-based interventions that have (Budson, 2014). In the early stages of Alzheimer’s
potential to enhance cognitive ability, participation in disease, which accounts for half of people who have
everyday activities, psychosocial wellbeing, and qual- dementia (Burns & Iliffe, 2009), memory impairment
ity of life (Folkerts, Roheger, Franklin, Middelst€adt, is within both working memory, where sensory infor-
& Kalbe, 2017). While the clinical syndrome of mation is processed, and the episodic memory sub-
dementia, more recently termed major neurocogni- system of declarative long-term memory, with deficits
tive disorder (DSM-5; American Psychiatric resulting in increasing difficulties receiving and
Association, 2013) is seen within a range of neuro- encoding new information to create memories
logical conditions, each having unique pathology and (Budson, 2014).

Correspondence: Naomi Rose, School of Occupational Therapy, Social Work and Speech Pathology, Faculty of Health Sciences, Curtin University, Perth,
Australia. Email: naomi.rose.sp@gmail.com

ISSN 1754-9507 print/ISSN 1754-9515 online ß 2020 The Speech Pathology Association of Australia Limited
Published by Taylor & Francis
DOI: 10.1080/17549507.2020.1747542
2 N. Rose et al.

Episodic autobiographical memory (i.e. memory Communication interventions in dementia


for the events and facts of one’s own life) may also be
The progressive nature of dementia and its wide-
impaired, however, studies have shown that more
ranging effects on cognition and language make com-
remote memories are better preserved than more
munication deficits an inevitable symptom (Lanzi,
recent ones, particularly for those memories that have
Burshnic, & Bourgeois, 2017). During the early
been recalled and shared frequently throughout a per-
stages of Alzheimer’s disease, for example, communi-
son’s life (De Simone et al., 2016). Autobiographical
cation is characterised by memory related word find-
memory loss for recent life memories, along with sub-
ing difficulties (Bourgeois & Hickey, 2009), with
tle changes in other cognitive domains, is also seen in
memory loss also interfering with conversational
MCI (Leyhe, M€ uller, Milian, Eschweiler, & Saur,
exchange through such behaviours as repeated ques-
2009) which frequently precedes dementia. It is,
tioning (Hamdy et al., 2018), and gradually worsen-
however, only when the cognitive deficits affect an
ing during the moderate and severe stages of the
individual’s functional ability to engage in their activ-
disease (Lanzi et al., 2017). Hopper and Bayles
ities of daily living that a diagnosis of dementia is
(2008) suggest that such early communication prob-
given (DSM-5, 2013). Autobiographical memory
lems in people with dementia are often not due to
serves important functions for sense of self and social
specific degeneration in lexical access processes as
bonding, allowing the past to be re-experienced
may occur in post-stroke aphasia, but arise directly
(Bluck, 2003). For people with MCI and dementia,
from the dysfunctional processing of sensory informa-
harnessing relative strengths in remote personal to
tion in the working memory, and the episodic mem-
memories, is important, both as a potential way of
ory subsystem deficits. Processing of sensory
stimulating meaningful conversations, while also
information is critical when conceiving an idea to
working therapeutically to help recall and consolidate
express, and in the subsequent planning and organ-
memories that are important to the person, promot-
isation of the spoken message, including monitoring
ing personhood.
word choice and order for delivery when communi-
cating to others (Levelt, 1995). Disruption to these
Reminiscence therapy
processes may be reflected in the spontaneous speech,
Reminiscence therapy (RT) is one of the most com- or discourse, of people with dementia where output
monly used therapeutic interventions in aged care set- can be fluent, but also repetitive, incoherent, lacking
tings that encourages recall of past memories to in essential information, and frequently unrelated to
stimulate mental activity and improve wellbeing the conversational topic (Hopper & Bayles, 2008).
(Woods, Spector, Jones, Orrell, & Davies, 2005). A This same memory impairment will likely prevent the
meta-analysis of 128 studies involving 4067 subjects person with dementia from recognising and repairing
(mean: 73.1 years; SD: 12.7 years) who had under- errors in the message delivered (Levelt, 1995).
gone RT intervention, found overall significant effects While speech-language pathology interventions
on variables such as ego-integrity, depression, pur- focussing on communication in dementia have trad-
pose in life, death preparation, mastery, mental itionally revolved around working with families to
health, positive wellbeing, social integration, and cog- adapt the environment to maximise participation
nitive performance (Pinquart & Forstmeier, 2012). (Royal College of Speech & Language Therapists,
The contribution of RT to mental health and well- 2014), in recent years, services have broadened to
being has been directly attributed to nostalgic behav- provide a host of evidence-based interventions that
iour, where strong, positive, episodic memories are person-centred and focus on life participation and
reflective of an individual’s sense of self are recalled social connection. These have included facilitation of
and examined (Westerhof & Bohlmeijer, 2014). As personalised memory aids and communication strat-
memories often contain a sensory component, RT egies, script training, language cueing, and group
adopts use of multi-sensory prompts, including items, intervention to practice retained communication
photographs, music, and questions, to stimulate the skills (Bourgeois, Brush, Douglas, Khayum, &
recall of memories within a supportive therapeutic Rogalski, 2016), each aiming to assist people with
group environment, with the person with dementia dementia to retain and maximise communication
supported to conceptualise their thoughts skills, actively maintain relationships and enhance
(Schweitzer, 2011). The impairment to working quality in daily life (Royal College of Speech &
memory further reinforces the need for this sensory Language Therapists, 2014). Interventions have also
stimulation, maximising access to longer term mem- drawn from the post-stroke literature which explicitly
ory. Regarded as both a cognitive and psychosocial focuses on improving spoken discourse in daily com-
intervention, speech-language pathologists working munication. A recent study aimed to assist people
with older people have been drawn to this interven- with dementia overcome word retrieval difficulties
tion, with increasing evidence of communication and planning of everyday connected speech through a
improvements post-therapy (e.g. Duru Aşiret & discourse intervention (Whitworth, Cartwright, et al.,
Kapucu, 2016; O’Rourke, Tobin, O’Callaghan, 2018) which has been successful for people with
Sowman, & Collins, 2011). post-stroke aphasia (Whitworth, Claessen, Leit~ao, &
Nostalgic Recounts and Reminiscence Therapy 3

Webster, 2015). Two people with primary progressive changes in verbal fluency and non-verbal communi-
aphasia (PPA) demonstrated significant gains in cation exchanges in people with dementia attending
amount of language produced, access to words, and an RT group with a matched group attending an
planning and organisation of a range of everyday everyday conversation group; in the latter, topics
speech tasks. This was particularly evident in related to everyday topics and were not explicitly
improved macrostructure elements in spoken produc- related to recall of past events. A significant inter-
tion, for example, increased orientation (beginning) action was found between the number of words
and body (middle) elements in novel monologues. recalled at the beginning and end of the therapy
While such interventions are few in number, they period, and the type of intervention, with people
offer a potential segue from existing psychosocial and attending the RT group achieving higher verbal flu-
cognitive interventions, such as RT, which encourage ency compared to no change for participants in the
the spoken recall of memories, to the planning and other group. Significant improvements were also
organisation of thoughts for speaking more generally. observed in non-verbal communication for the RT
participants compared to the group that received
Reminiscence therapy and communication everyday conversation exposure. Limited discussion
With RT providing opportunities to recount nostalgic as to how communication performance could be
memories, participants are required to access and explicitly facilitated, or why improvement was seen,
organise their thoughts for communication in a genre however, was offered by these studies.
hereafter referred to as nostalgic recounts. Interestingly, Greater access to language following such inter-
Merriam (1989) analysed the macrostructure of dis- ventions as RT may be partially accounted for in the
course used during reminiscence monologues of 25 relationship between emotional states and the lan-
healthy older people between 61 and 80 years, and guage of appraisal, or evaluation, a concept arising
found four components were present: selection, from the linguistics literature. This has been shown to
immersion, withdrawal and closure. These concepts be present in reminiscence discourse where there is a
align closely with discourse structure seen more gen- focus on conveying interpersonal meaning using
erally within the healthy adult literature (Whitworth evaluative language (e.g. expressions of attitudes, feel-
et al., 2015). Recounting memories is likely to involve ings, and opinions) (Martin & White, 2005).
a similar set of cognitive and language skills to those Armstrong, Ciccone, Godecke, and Kok (2011) pro-
involved in other discourse contexts, such as, general posed that evaluative language is a necessary compo-
event recall or procedural discourse. The preparation nent to relate information related to sense of self,
and planning for sharing a memory (e.g. establishing however, research, to date, has primarily focussed on
perspective and focus on key episodes, temporal evaluative language used by people with aphasia after
sequencing of events), combined with the necessary stroke. Bloom, Borod, Obler, and Gerstman (1992),
linguistic skills to construct the macro- and micro- among others, have reported that people with aphasia
structure for spoken production (Levelt, 1999), is not following left hemisphere damage frequently produce
unlike that required of other genres. language that is more elaborate when discussing emo-
Despite an extensive literature discussing the rela- tive topics compared to discussing non-emotive
tionships between memory, executive function and topics. Fromm et al. (2011) also reported that the
language production (Cahana-Amitay & Albert, inclusion of personal and evaluative tasks in therapy
2014), there is limited evidence available on the effect contexts with people with aphasia resulted in both
of RT on communication and language skills. One improved self-image and language gains. The inter-
study sought to measure the impact of twice-weekly play between RT and evaluative language, while
RT, delivered over 6 weeks, on communication, inter- potentially integral to discourse produced during
action, and engagement of participants with moder- reminiscing, has not been explored.
ate to mild dementia from a variety of aetiologies In summary, given the likely relationship between
(O’Rourke et al., 2011). On the Functional autobiographical memory and the preparation of nos-
Linguistic Communication Inventory, communica- talgic recount production, supported by gains in
tion either improved or remained stable in five of the organisation of spoken discourse in both people with
six participants with dementia, with increased inter- aphasia and PPA with targeted discourse therapy, it
actions reported with family, despite reductions in might be anticipated that communicative gains are
cognitive abilities in three of the six participants. seen in people undertaking RT. While communica-
Duru Aşiret and Kapucu (2016) also reported an tion measures have been shown to improve in previ-
increase in the communication components of the ous studies involving RT, there has been no
Daily Living Activities Observation instrument in systematic examination as to what is improving and
people with moderate to mild dementia, when com- whether spoken production can reasonably be
pared to a control group, although this was not expected following RT. The presence of evaluative
explored in terms of language or spoken production. language in reminiscence discourse following RT has
Positive outcomes were also found by Okumura, also not been explored, such that it is not known
Tanimukai, and Asada (2008) who compared whether the language of appraisal is retained within
4 N. Rose et al.

nostalgic recounts in the event of cognitive decline Method


and is responsive to increased practice of this dis-
Research design
course genre. It is also unknown whether any change
to nostalgic recounts might generalise to other genres A case series design was utilised for this exploratory
used within everyday contexts, thereby extending intervention study, using convenience sampling pro-
potential benefits of RT to other social encounters. cedures. Ethical approval was obtained from the
Curtin University Human Research Ethics
The current study Committee (HRE2018-0382).

This study examines the relationship between Participants


communicative gains and targeted recall of autobio-
graphical memories following RT to inform how Four female participants (mean age: 87 years; SD:
speech-language pathologists may be able to better 7.3, range: 77–93) with mild cognitive impairment
capitalise on an established therapy with proven bene- (n ¼ 2) or dementia (n ¼ 2) were recruited to partici-
fits for people living with MCI and dementia. The pate from a residential aged care facility (see Table I).
study explored whether four people with impaired Both participants with dementia were recorded as
memory as a result of MCI or dementia demon- having probable Alzheimer’s disease by their medical
practitioner although no scan data was available. Two
strated, following an RT programme delivered twice
further participants were recruited but were lost to
weekly over 4 weeks, improved communicative
attrition, due to ill health and personal circumstances.
behaviour in nostalgic recounts. As this was an
Inclusion criteria included: (a) a score  11 and  25
exploratory pilot study, no previous evidence was pre-
indicating moderate or mild cognitive impairment, as
sent on which to base firm hypotheses relating to dis-
measured on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment
course measures, however, the proposed relationship (MoCA) (Nasreddine et al., 2005); (b) adequate
between cognitive processing, in this instance, auto- hearing acuity to allow participation in therapy; (c)
biographical memory, and the transforming of inten- ability to speak English at a level sufficient to allow
tion into spoken production was expected to result in participation in therapy; and (d) capacity to provide
positive changes to language output through the informed consent, as advised by the on-site profes-
stimulation and repeated practice of this genre. sional staff and confirmed by the participant’s
We hypothesised that RT would result in (a) within General Practitioner. Exclusion criteria included any
genre change, indicating therapy effectiveness, as seen significant history of trauma or disease, such as head
by significant increases in discourse coherence injury, stroke, intellectual disability, or Parkinson’s
(macrostructure) and significant increases in rate, disease, that may have impacted on cognitive per-
efficiency, and informativeness of nostalgic recounts; formance. Three of the four participants (P2, P3, and
and (b) across genre change, indicating therapy general- P4) had co-morbid mental health diagnoses, and P4
isation, as seen by significant increases in discourse was blind. A rigorous informed consent procedure
coherence (macrostructure) and significant increases was undertaken to ensure a high level of support in
in rate, efficiency, and informativeness of everyday making informed decisions and providing writ-
discourse (everyday recounts, procedures, exposi- ten consent.
tions). Performance on everyday discourse was com-
pared to normative data for these genres to permit Discourse sampling for baseline and
comparison to healthy adult participants. As previous outcome measures
evidence has reported that RT has resulted in signifi- Discourse samples were audio-recorded using an
cant improvements to cognition and quality of life Olympus VN-741PC Digital Voice Recorder.
(QoL) measures (Pinquart & Forstmeier, 2012), sig- Nostalgic recounts: Nine topics were identified
nificant increases in cognition and QoL were hypoth- by the research team as likely to stimulate previous
esised for all participants. Performance on memories of childhood or specific events and sample
communication measures would need to be inter- spoken production of nostalgic recounts across a
preted in the event of changes to either or both of broader range of people (see Table II). The topics
these measures, where no change to cognition or were not specific to the participants in this study and
QoL would suggest that the participants were not were sampled in groups of three over three separate
responsive to RT. sessions pre-intervention to ensure repeated baseline

Table I. Demographic details of participants.


Participant Age (years) Gender Baseline MoCA Diagnosis Additional health information
1 92 Female 18 Dementia N/A
2 77 Female 23 Dementia Bipolar disorder
3 93 Female 15 MCI Anxiety
4 86 Female 16 MCI Blind, depression & anxiety
MoCA: Montreal Cognitive Assessment; MCI: Mild Cognitive Impairment.
Nostalgic Recounts and Reminiscence Therapy 5

Table II. Nostalgic recount discourse topics. complete the DEMQOL (Smith et al., 2007). Three
1: Tell me about your first car? samples of nostalgic recounts and one sample of each
2: Tell me about your wedding day?
3: Tell me about your best friend growing up? everyday discourse genre (i.e. giving opinions,
4: Tell me what your children were like when they were little? explaining procedures, and recounting events) were
5: Tell me about your favourite pet growing up?
6: Tell me about your favourite holiday? collected each session. The MoCA was completed by
7: Tell me about how your life was impacted by war? the on-site Occupational Therapist.
8: Tell me about how you kept warm in winter when you were
growing up? Post-intervention assessment procedure:
9: Tell me about your favourite food as you were growing up? During the week post-intervention, repeated dis-
course samples were again collected for each partici-
pant over three sessions, and the DEMQOL were re-
administered. The MoCA was also re-administered
sampling and monitor stability of discourse. Topics
during this phase by the on-site Occupational
were then collapsed for comparison with post-
Therapist. Intervention effectiveness was assessed
intervention data. No follow-up maintenance meas-
through comparison of the aggregated measures for
ures were collected over time, due to the progressive
nostalgic recounts pre- and post-therapy, with gener-
nature of the conditions and attention to repeated
sampling post-therapy. alisation measured through comparison of pre- and
Everyday discourse genres: Generalisation of post-therapy everyday discourse samples.
any gains to other genre, following intervention, was
measured through change on nine monologue topics Discourse analysis
elicited within the genres of everyday recount, proced- Segmentation of sample: All discourse samples
ural, and expository discourse (collectively known as were segmented into communication units (C units)
everyday discourse genres) from the Curtin University and coded using Systematic Analysis of Language
Discourse Protocol (CUDP) (Whitworth et al., 2015), Transcripts (SALT) transcription conventions
also sampled over three different sessions to monitor (Miller & Iglesias, 2016).
stability. The CUDP provides normative data for dis- Macrostructure analysis: Analysis of macro-
course measure for healthy adults (Whitworth et al., structure was undertaken for each discourse sample
2015), with good intra-subject reliability established using the codes for coherence from the CUDP
(Whitworth, Budiman, et al., 2018). (Whitworth et al., 2015), with adaptations required
for nostalgic recounts (see Supplementary Table I).
Assessment of cognition and quality of life Each genre was coded and frequencies tallied for (a)
number of orientation elements (beginning), (b)
Measures of cognitive performance and quality of life number of body elements (middle), and (c) number
were collected before and after the intervention to of conclusion elements (end), and with the exception
monitor presence of change. of procedures, (d) number of richness elements, a
Cognition: The Montreal Cognitive Assessment category unique to this study, and which aimed to
(MoCA) (Nasreddine et al., 2005) was used to screen capture an expected level of evaluative language and
eight domains of cognitive function (short-term elaboration. Two richness codes were developed to
memory, visuospatial abilities, executive functioning, capture details within the monologues considered to
attention, concentration, to working memory, and be relevant to providing a coherent macrostructure,
orientation to time and place). The MoCA is but which were difficult to allocate under the extant
designed to detect impairment in these domains, and
CUDP categories. These codes were (1) evaluative
is not intended to be a substitute for a full neuro-
comments [EC], and (2) elaborations [Elab] (see
psychological assessment. The screening assessment
Supplementary Table II) and were collapsed
has a high level of reliability and validity (Nasreddine
for analysis.
et al. 2005).
Correct Information Unit analysis: An ana-
Quality of Life: The Dementia Quality of Life
lysis of rate, efficiency, and informativeness was com-
Scale (DEMQOL) was used to measure five QoL
pleted using a Correct Information Unit (CIU)
domains, including daily activities, health/wellbeing,
analysis of the discourse samples (Nicholas &
cognition, social relationships and self-concept
Brookshire, 1993). The following scores were calcu-
(Smith et al., 2007). The DEMQOL has high reli-
lated for (a) rate (word count per minute [WPM]),
ability and moderate validity in people with mild or
(b) efficiency (CIU’s per minute [CIU/min]), and (c)
moderate dementia (Smith et al., 2007).
informativeness (percentage of CIUs to total number
of words [%CIUs]) (Nicholas & Brookshire, 1993).
Procedure
Inter-rater reliability: Inter-rater reliability of
Assessment coding was established on 20% of the discourse sam-
Baseline assessment procedure: Three 30-min ples with an independent rater. Discourse samples
assessment sessions were conducted for each partici- were independently coded then discussed, with agree-
pant during the week prior to intervention to establish ment reached on any discrepancy. No adjustments
baseline performance on discourse measures and were made to the coding protocol.
6 N. Rose et al.

Intervention each of the four genres taken over three time points in
Sessions of RT were conducted twice-weekly for each phase were analysed using a two-way mixed-
60 min over 4 weeks (total of eight sessions) and were effects model with averaged measures and absolute
scheduled in the morning to maximise attention and agreement (Koo & Li, 2016). ICC measures were
minimise fatigue. Therapy sessions were held in an interpreted using Koo and Li (2016) guidelines
activity room within the residential aged care facility (<0.40 ¼ poor, 0.40–0.59 ¼ fair, 0.60–0.74 ¼ good,
reserved exclusively for group use. Sessions were >0.75 ¼ excellent). This takes into consideration
facilitated by the first author, with a final year speech- individual performance.
language pathology student recruited to assist. Using Frequencies of discourse measures (macrostruc-
a structured RT programme, participants were intro- ture elements, words/min, CIUs/min, %CIUs) were
duced to a unique nostalgic theme, chronologically then tallied across all nine nostalgic recount topics for
ordered, during each session, exploring associated each participant at baseline and post-intervention,
multi-sensory items, including an old journal, nostal- with the three time points at both baseline and post-
gic items, and old photos, and listening to a scripted intervention phases collapsed. All discourse measures
story related to the theme to stimulate memories. were compared using Fisher’s exact test (two tailed)
Verbal prompts related to each theme were provided with Poisson distribution, with the exception of
by the facilitators to support participants in sharing %CIU, which used Fisher’s exact test (two tailed).
memories with the group. Supports such as name Performance on everyday genres across the two time
badges, and the provision of a booklet for each par- points were compared using the same analyses, with
ticipant to read along aimed to overcome mem- Fisher’s exact tests also used to calculate the presence
ory loss. of significant differences in baseline and post-inter-
Intervention sessions were video-recorded using a vention scores on cognition and QoL measures for
Sony HDR-XR100E Camcorder to enable treatment individual participants. SPSS version 25 (IBM,
fidelity to be monitored. Fifty per cent of these ses- 2017), online calculator singlims.exe, and the reliable
sions were randomly selected and reviewed by a change index calculator were used for statis-
member of the supervisory team experienced with tical analysis.
delivering RT. Performance against the protocol was
evaluated for type and level of questioning, follow-up Result
of participants’ responses, engagement of the partici-
pants, use of the props and overall pace of the session, Results from individual participant’s pre-intervention
with weekly monitoring sessions scheduled with both everyday discourse were compared to healthy adult
group facilitators to ensure continued adherence to controls to determine whether performance was sig-
the intervention protocol. nificantly different from control data. Stability of
everyday discourse and nostalgic recounts across
Data and statistical analysis multiple baseline samples was also examined. Within
and across genre changes were then measured.
To determine any difference from healthy adults on
everyday discourse genres, the macrostructure ele-
Comparison of everyday discourse measures
ments of orientation, body, and conclusion from the
with normative data
CUDP were compared across all nine everyday dis-
course samples at baseline to data from 30 healthy Descriptive statistics for everyday discourse macro-
adult speakers (Whitworth et al., 2015) using a structure elements (orientation, body, and conclu-
Crawford and Howell to t-test (Crawford, sion) are reported for each participant in this study
Garthwaite, & Porter, 2010). Control data were not compared to 30 healthy adult speakers (Whitworth
available for comparison of nostalgic recounts. et al., 2015) (see Table III; graphical representation
Estimates of stability for everyday and nostalgic dis- of macrostructure is provided in Supplementary
course and their 95% confidence intervals were calcu- Figure 1). While consistently lower numerical scores
lated using intra-class correlations (ICC) during the were seen for all participants in comparison to per-
baseline and the post-intervention phases. Samples of formance of healthy controls, during the baseline

Table III. Performance of each participant on everyday discourse macrostructure elements (orientation, body, conclusion) during base-
line in comparison to healthy adult speakers.
Control (n ¼ 30) Participant 1 Participant 2 Participant 3 Participant 4
Discourse element Mean SD Raw score p Value Raw score p Value Raw score p Value Raw score P Value
Macrostructure: 29.1 10.1 11 0.088 16 0.212 18 0.289 15 0.180
Orientation
Macrostructure: 94.3 39.8 59 0.390 63 0.445 32 0.134 33 0.141
Body
Macrostructure: 4.9 2.02 15 <0.001 5 0.961 1 0.068 9 0.055
Conclusion
Crawford and Howell t-test.
Nostalgic Recounts and Reminiscence Therapy 7

0.024
0.001
<0.001
Table IV. Comparison of nostalgic recount macrostructure (orientation, body, conclusion), richness, quantity (no. of words), rate (WPM), efficiency (CIU/min), and effectiveness (%CIU) at baseline
assessment phase in the number of introductory and

p Value

0.440

0.916
body macrostructure elements used, these were not

1
significantly different to control data. One participant
(P1) provided significantly higher conclusion ele-

Post-intervention
ments in the macrostructure (score ¼ 15, p <0.001)

Participant 4
than the healthy adult speaker controls (M ¼ 4.9, SD

88.32
78.81

85.09
298.78
¼ 2.02), and P4’s higher usage of conclusion state-

105
24
54
8
ments approached significance (score ¼ 9, p
¼ 0.055).

Baseline

139.22
88.29
Stability of discourse measures at baseline

76.5

86.3
8
32
8
56
Stability of discourse, measured using ICCs during
the baseline and the post-intervention phases, showed

<0.001
<0.001
<0.001
p Value

0.742

0.547

0.926
0.546

0.757
that three discourse measures had significant levels of
stability during these two phases in either or both nos-
talgic or everyday discourse genres (WPM, CIU/min
and %CIUs) with variability seen across other indi-

Post-intervention
Participant 3
ces. Only baseline stability is reported here (see

372.11

75.15

74.69
Supplementary Table III) (post-intervention stability

103.2
20
93
153
7
data is available in Supplementary Table IV). For
nostalgic recounts, measures showing significant sta-
bility at baseline were WPM (ICC ¼ 0.840, p ¼
0.008), CIU/min (ICC ¼ 0.792, p ¼ 0.020), and

Baseline

83.77

81.65
103.33
100.88

Indicates statistically significant difference between baseline and post-intervention (a>0.05), Fisher’s exact (two tailed).
richness (ICC ¼ 0.712, p ¼ 0.033). While stability of

WPM: Words/minute; CIU/min: correct information units per minute; %CIU: percentage of correct information units.
17
33
4
38
macrostructure elements was not significant, body
elements showed moderate stability (ICC ¼ 0.710, p
0.017


<0.001
¼ 0.072) and stability of conclusion elements was
p Value

0.683
<0.001

0.477

0.709
1.0
good (ICC ¼ 0.760, p ¼ 0.091). For everyday dis-

1
course, those behaviours that showed significant sta-
bility at baseline were conclusion elements (ICC ¼
Post-intervention

0.860, p ¼ 0.020) and WPM (ICC ¼ 0.820, p ¼


Participant 2

61.25
50.74

81.74
185.78

0.014), with other measures showing poor stability or


20
69
4
60

were unable to be analysed due to violation of statis-


tical assumptions.

Therapy effectiveness on nostalgic recounts


Baseline

359.11
70.42
50.47

73.76

(within genre change)


20
101
117
2

Macrostructure: Two participants (P3 and P4)


showed significant improvement in the macrostruc-
p Value

0.028



0.268

0.093
0.493

0.016

0.046

ture of their nostalgic recounts post-intervention,


1.0

0.9

while two participants (P1 and P2) showed significant


reduction in macrostructure over the same period
Post-intervention

(see Table IV). P3 demonstrated no significant differ-


Participant 1

ence in the frequency of orientation and conclusion


45.91

44.73
650.44
102.8

elements of the macrostructure, but demonstrated a


134
16
43
5

significant improvement in the body element


(z ¼ 5.25, p <0.001) and richness (z ¼ 8.24, p
<0.001). P4 also showed no significant difference in
Baseline

624.89

73.08

73.68

frequency of orientation or conclusion elements, but


24
67
6
164

100

demonstrated a significant improvement in body ele-


ments (z ¼ 2.26, p ¼ 0.024,) and also in richness
and post-intervention.

(z ¼ 3.78, p < 0.001). No significant difference in


Discourse measure

orientation or conclusion elements, nor in richness,


No. of Words
Macrostructure:

Informativeness
Orientation

Conclusion

was seen for P1, however, a significant reduction in


CIU/min

body elements (z ¼ 2.19, p ¼ 0.028) was seen. P2


%CIU
WPM
Efficiency
Quantity
Richness
Body

also demonstrated no significant difference in orienta-


Rate

tion or conclusion elements, and a significant


8 N. Rose et al.

reduction in body elements (z ¼ 2.38, p ¼ 0.017) and

<0.001
Table V. Comparison of everyday discourse macrostructure (orientation, body, conclusion, and richness), quantity (no. of words), rate (WPM), efficiency (CIU/min), and effectiveness (%CIU) calcula-

p Value

0.051
0.663
0.651

0.877
0.891
richness (z ¼ 4.21, p <0.001).

1
Quantity, rate, efficiency and informativeness:
P3 and P4 demonstrated a significant increase in the
quantity of words (z ¼ 12.27, p <0.001; z ¼ 7.57, p

Post-intervention
Participant 4
<0.001, respectively) but did not change in rate, effi-

183.78
88.88
79.52
83.97
ciency and informativeness of nostalgic recounts

14
52
12
24
post-intervention (see Table IV). The only other sig-
nificant change was a reduction by P1 in CIU/min
(z ¼ 2.42, p ¼ 0.016) and %CIU (z ¼ 2.57, p
¼ 0.010).

Baseline

91.96

82.55
76.8
104
15
33
9
20
Therapy generalisation on everyday discourse
(across genre change)
Macrostructure: Variability was found in macro-

p Value

0.024
0.265
0.221
0.540

0.253
0.894
0.395
0.520
structure between baseline and post-intervention,
however, the two participants who had shown
improvement in nostalgic recounts also made gains in
everyday genres (see Table V). P3 demonstrated sig-

Participant 3

intervention

74.33
96.56

78.61
nificant improvement in the body of her macrostruc-

Post-

11
54
10

77
5
ture of her everyday discourse post-intervention
(z ¼ 2.26, p ¼ 0.024), while P4 approached signifi-
cance in the frequency of body elements (z ¼ 1.95, p

WPM: Words per minute; CIU/min: Correct information units per minute; %CIU: percentage of correct information units.
¼ 0.051). Two participants (P1 and P2) demon-

Baseline

99.43
65.84
66.69
strated significantly lower scores in macrostructure.

18
32
14

90
1
P1 demonstrated no change in orientation or body
elements, nor in richness, but a significant reduction

Statistically significant difference between baseline and post-intervention (a>0.05), Fisher’s exact (two tailed).
in conclusion elements (z ¼ 2.01, p ¼ 0.044). P2

0.004

<0.001


p Value

0.102
0.773

0.344
0.592
0.008
demonstrated no change in any macrostructure ele-
1

ments, but a significant reduction in richness


(z ¼ 2.91, p ¼0.004).
Rate, efficiency and informativeness: Two par-
Participant 2

intervention

ticipants (P1 and P4) demonstrated a significant


86.11
56.33
49.11
88.42
Post-

increase in the quantity of words (z ¼ 2.0, p ¼ 0.046;


16
45
7
15

z ¼ 4.64, p <0.001, respectively), while P2 showed a


significant decline (z ¼ 8.48, p <0.001) (see Table V).
No changes were evident, however, in rate, efficiency
Baseline

67.89
42.97
47.26
240.55

or informativeness across all participants, with the


16
63
5
37

exception of P2 who demonstrated a significant


increase in informativeness (z ¼ 3.45, p ¼0.008).
p Value

0.046
0.089
0.044
0.111

0.935
0.066
0.183

Impact of therapy on cognition and quality


of life
1

Cognition: No significant differences in cognition,


Participant 1

intervention

as measured by the MoCA screening tool, were seen


45.12
44.04
425.89
103.96
Post-

individually after the intervention (see Table VI), rep-


10
41
5
46

resented graphically in Supplementary Figure 2.


tions at baseline and post-intervention.

Quality of life: No significant differences in scores of


QoL, as measured by the DEMQOL, were seen fol-
Baseline

lowing intervention for individuals (see Table VI),


65.45
62.58
368.44
106.14
11
59
15
31

represented graphically in Supplementary Figure 3.

Discussion
Discourse measure

No. of words

This exploratory study aimed to explore whether par-


Macrostructure:

Informativeness
Orientation
Conclusion

ticipation in a structured reminiscence group, with a


CIU/min
%CIU
WPM

focus on recall and sharing of memories, would influ-


Efficiency
Quantity
Richness
Body

ence the planning, organisation and content of spo-


Rate

ken production in people with memory difficulties


Nostalgic Recounts and Reminiscence Therapy 9

p Value
arising from MCI or dementia. Conceptualising the

0.936
1.0
genre of memory recall as nostalgic recounts, direct
treatment effectiveness were measured through any
changes to discourse during nostalgic recounts, with

Post-intervention
generalisation to untreated genres monitored in

Participant 4
everyday recounts, procedures, expositions. Notably,

15
79
this study did not provide targeted input on discourse
structure as has been carried out with other discourse
level studies (Whitworth, Cartwright, et al., 2018),
but sought to monitor any changes in discourse as a
Baseline

result of repeated opportunity to practice spoken


16
76

recall within a group therapy setting.


Table VI. Comparison of cognition (MoCA) and quality of life (DEMQOL) scores for each participant at baseline and post-intervention using Fisher’s exact test.

Comparison of everyday discourse measures


p Value

with normative data


0.916
1.0

Prior to intervention, the everyday discourse per-


formance of the four participants in the study was
Post-intervention

compared to a healthy adult control group to ascer-


Participant 3

tain whether the participants were within normal


100
16

range in macrostructure, informativeness and effi-


ciency. While all participants were numerically lower
than healthy controls in introductory and body
macrostructure elements before intervention, these
differences were not significant, a finding likely to be
Baseline
15
104

influenced by the wide standard deviation within the


healthy adult population. This finding has been repli-
cated in studies with people with cognitive-communi-
cation impairment (Whitworth, Ng, Timms, &
p Value

0.571

Power, 2020) where impaired discourse profiles were


1.0

not reflected in performance that was significantly dif-


ferent to healthy controls. In contrast, P1 provided
Post-intervention

significantly more conclusion elements in the macro-


Participant 2

structure of everyday discourse, while P4 approached


22
63

significance. This was considered to reflect a difficulty


in efficiently ending the monologue rather than being
MoCA: Montreal cognitive assessment; DEMQOL: Dementia quality of life scale.

related to providing beneficial detail to the coherence


of the macrostructure. Interestingly, this behaviour
was also reported in one participant with cognitive-
Baseline
23
73

communication difficulties in the earlier study


(Whitworth et al., 2020), highlighting difficulties in
planning rather than higher performance. While the
p Value

consistent trend in reduced overall macrostructure


0.749
1.0

across all four participants remains noteworthy, it


also highlights the challenges in assessing discourse
when healthy speakers demonstrate such wide vari-
Post-intervention

ability, reinforcing the importance using each partici-


Participant 1

pant as their own control in the study design. Future


19
83

comparison of participants with an age matched con-


trol group would also enable greater exploration of
this issue, providing insight into whether older adults
with impaired cognition may be significantly different
Baseline

to older adults with normal cognition.


18
76

Stability of discourse measures


While establishing stable baselines in discourse per-
DEMQOL

formance can be challenging, this is also critical to


MoCA

monitor; where too variable, less confidence is pre-


sent when comparing pre- and post-intervention
10 N. Rose et al.

measures. Following the high stability levels seen in and therefore had greater capacity to enhance their
the Beales, Whitworth, Cartwright, Panegyres, and output with practice. Interestingly, no improvements
Kane (2018) study when the CUDP was measured were seen in the CIU analysis of nostalgic recounts
over three time points, this study collected samples of that monitored rate, efficiency, and informativeness
all genres across three sessions, both prior to and fol- of nostalgic recounts for any participant.
lowing intervention, and including the novel nostalgic While the significant gains seen in planning and
recounts developed for the study. In this study, 50% content of macrostructure for two participants would
of the measures showed moderate to good stability seem to provide promising evidence of the potential
during the baseline phase, with four measures reach- for RT to improve the discourse coherence of people
ing significance. These measures included WPM in with cognitive impairment, with the variation being
both nostalgic recounts and everyday discourse, rich- possibly accounted for by the severity of the neuro-
ness and CIU/min stable within nostalgic recounts, cognitive condition, one issue that arose during the
and the conclusion element of macrostructure stable study related to engagement with the assessment pro-
in everyday discourse. Other measures showed mod- cess. As no discourse assessment protocol for nostal-
erate to good stability but did not reach significance. gic recounts was available, nine topics were
Natural variability was expected to some degree given developed for this preliminary study to ensure suffi-
that the topics used within each of the sampling ses- cient samples to monitor stability at baseline and to
sions were different, however, this may also reflect the provide comparability with the everyday genres
smaller sample size in the current study in compari- within the CUDP. Participants, however, demon-
son to Beales et al. (2018). Both continued refine- strated less engagement during post-assessment, with
ment of the assessment tool and sampling a larger some participants reporting that they provided less
number of participants with cognitive impairment is than optimal responses due to not wanting to repeat
needed to develop greater confidence with the con- topics. This raises the inherent challenge in sampling
tent of discourse assessment in this population and recall of memories and the use of the current protocol
obtain a greater understanding of variability. as an outcome measure. Refining the protocol, poten-
tially through reducing the number of topics and
Therapy effectiveness on nostalgic recounts identifying matched topics, remains a priority. The
(within genre change) lower performance of some participants post-inter-
vention may therefore reflect the assessment task and
Central to the aims of the study was the exploration
not accurately have captured ability.
of within genre change, indicating therapy effective-
ness, evidenced by significant increases in discourse
Therapy generalisation on everyday discourse
coherence (macrostructure and richness) and signifi-
(across genre change)
cant increases in rate, efficiency, and informativeness
of nostalgic recounts. Variability in response to the In the event of within genre gains, a second objective
intervention was seen across participants, a factor was to establish across genre change, indicative of
that was not surprising given the different aetiologies therapy generalisation and evidenced by significant
and discourse profiles of the participants. P3 and P4, increases in discourse coherence (macrostructure)
both who had diagnoses of MCI, showed significant and significant increases in rate, efficiency, and
improvements in the body of the macrostructure, cor- informativeness of everyday discourse (everyday
responding with significant increases in the amount recounts, procedures, expositions). P3 made signifi-
of language spoken overall. Both P3 and P4 also dem- cant improvements in the body of her macrostructure
onstrated striking, and significant, gains in the novel of everyday genres, with P4 also making gains and
richness elements that were coded in the study, rein- approaching significance. Given that increases in
forcing the inclusion of these novel codes as an body elements were also seen in nostalgic recounts
important outcome measure to capture change rele- for both participants, these gains point to likely evi-
vant to this genre. In contrast, both P1 and P2 dem- dence of generalisation of increased abilities to plan
onstrated weaker performance when reassessed after and produce macrostructure in other genres. The
therapy on a number of measures. Both participants pattern demonstrated by P1 in showing significant
produced fewer body elements in nostalgic discourse, reduced informativeness and efficiency in nostalgic
with P2 also reducing in the richness of her produc- recounts was also seen numerically in everyday gen-
tion and overall output, and P1 reducing in efficiency. res, although these trends did not reach significance.
Notably, P1 and P2 had diagnoses of Alzheimer’s dis- Again, this may reflect a decline in general skills or a
ease, such that the more advanced diagnoses may be lower engagement with the repeated nature of the
indicative of poorer discourse outcomes. P2 did, how- assessment tasks. Interestingly, P2, who had declined
ever, present with the highest MoCA scores in the significantly in richness elements in nostalgic
group. It was also notable that P3 and P4 had poorer recounts, also declined significantly in richness within
discourse profiles (e.g. less body elements) than P1 everyday recounts (recounts were the only everyday
and P2 during baseline assessment, a factor that may genre where this was measured). The performance of
have been associated with their diagnoses of anxiety, all participants do lend support for common
Nostalgic Recounts and Reminiscence Therapy 11

processes being involved across the different genres, potential distinctiveness of nostalgic recounts as a dis-
with repeated practice in one genre impacting on pro- course genre. The continued development of ways in
duction within other genres and, ultimately, enhanc- which richness can be measured is considered
ing communication capacity. Findings, however, also important in progressing our understanding of the
lend support for the use of evaluative language, coded theoretical relationship between evaluative language,
within the unique ‘richness’ code, as being more rele- reminiscence and emotive topics.
vant to reminiscent discourse that everyday genres, a
finding that warrants further investigation. Limitations

Impact of therapy on cognition and quality A number of limitations were identified with this
of life study. Due to both the nature of the intervention, the
logistics of working within the schedule of the resi-
No significant differences were seen in outcomes dential care facility, and the early withdrawal of par-
related to cognition or QoL. Given the consistent evi- ticipants due to ill health, the group was limited to
dence of gains in these areas in previous studies four participants. While this resulted in a small sam-
(Pinquart & Forstmeier, 2012), this may be indicative
ple size to pilot the approach, the group size did
of a number of factors. This may be due to a lack of
enable a detailed exploration of the discourse behav-
sensitivity of the measures used, or that the focus of
iour of the participants; it does, however, warrant rep-
the intervention did not explicitly target the cognitive
lication on a larger scale. The impact of comorbidity
skills assessed on the MoCA or the QoL domains
of mental health conditions on pre-intervention com-
assessed via the DEMQOL. It is possible that concur-
munication was also a factor not explored in the
rent mental health conditions (anxiety, depression,
study, primarily due to limited guidance available as
bi-polar disorder) which were also present for three of
the participants may also underlie the lack of change to the potential impact on the discourse measures
in QoL, suggesting further investigation is needed under investigation. This highlights the need for fur-
where comorbid mental health conditions exist. ther research into the impact of mental health disor-
Qualitative questionnaires, which were taken after ders on discourse, facilitating a greater understanding
each intervention session (not reported here), did of changes seen in behaviour within this population.
however indicate high levels of enjoyment from all The inclusion of participants with sensory deficits
participants after each session, but may not have (blindness, hearing deficits) was also considered a
impacted on other facets of life. These data do, how- risk for participants in engaging with the multi-sen-
ever, provide some support for increased general cog- sory prompts, despite being prevalent within the
nition not being responsible for the gains in discourse population at the residential care centre. This necessi-
seen in this study. tated attention to appropriate strategies and aids and,
notably, the participant who was blind achieved sig-
Clinical and theoretical implications nificant gains following the group. The participants of
this study were also Australian born women drawn
Despite the variability in the results, and the possible
from the same residential care facility. The external
influence of severity of the neurocognitive disorder
validity and generalisability of the outcomes of this
and the impact of coexisting mental health condi-
study must therefore be considered in the light of the
tions, this study offers promising evidence that RT
homogeneous nature of the participants, and caution
has the potential to improve spoken communication.
The focus on the nostalgic recount as a unique genre taken in extrapolating the application of outcomes to
to capture the sharing of memories emerged as a a more diverse population.
viable method for conceptualising the preparation A number of considerations are also present in
and planning of sharing memories, and of measuring relation to the assessment measures. Refinement of
change after exposure to multiple opportunities to the nostalgic recount stimuli questions will be import-
access memories. With the exception of the richness ant for future studies to maximise salience of topics
codes, the significant gains seen in those participants and ensure memories are likely to be positive and
who increased their ability to produce nostalgic framed in nostalgia. As noted earlier, the develop-
recounts were mirrored in the spoken discourse of ment of a tool with a reduced number of topics may
everyday genres, highlighting the potential for a focus be more appealing to participants and ensure max-
on reminiscence to have direct impact to spoken out- imum engagement with post-intervention sampling.
put more generally. The language of appraisal, which With respect to the novel nature of the codes used to
was largely communicated using evaluative language measure evaluative language, a decision was made to
(expressions of attitudes, feelings, and opinions) discuss all discrepancies between raters and agree
reflected a difference between nostalgic recounts and changes. While this was useful in the development
everyday recounts. The significant increase seen for sense for a new set of codes, it would be critical in
two participants in the richness of nostalgic recounts future studies to identify and report the inter-rater
was not evident in everyday recounts, highlighting the reliability of these.
12 N. Rose et al.

Conclusion unilateral brain damage. Brain and Language, 42, 153–164.


doi:10.1016/0093-934X(92)90122-U
This research provides promising evidence that RT Bluck, S. (2003). Autobiographical memory: Exploring its func-
has the potential to directly improve the communica- tions in everyday life. Memory, 11, 113–123. doi:10.1080/
tion of people with neurocognitive disorders and, 741938206
Bourgeois, M., Brush, J., Douglas, N., Khayum, R., & Rogalski,
potentially, with MCI in particular. Nostalgic
E. (2016). Will you still need me when I’m 64, or 84, or 104?
recounts may provide an explicit context in which The importance of speech-language pathologists in promoting
speech-language pathologists can actively facilitate the quality of life of aging adults in the United States in the
the planning of spoken production in people with future. Seminars in Speech and Language, 37, 185–200. doi:10.
MCI and dementia, and have the potential to impact 1055/s-0036-1583544
Bourgeois, M., & Hickey, E. (2009). Dementia: From diagnosis to
on speaking in everyday contexts. Notably, this study
management: A functional approach. New York: Psychology
did not aim to draw on targeted discourse interven- Press.
tions that have demonstrated improved discourse in Budson, A. (2014). Memory systems in dementia. In B.
adults with communication difficulties. Combining Dickerson & A. Atri (Eds.), Dementia: Comprehensive principles
RT with discourse level interventions would likely be and practice. New York: Oxford University Press.
Burns, A., & Iliffe, S. (2009). Dementia. BMJ, 338, b75. doi:10.
a promising line of future research to maximise gains,
1136/bmj.b75
exploring whether certain participant profiles are Cahana-Amitay, D., & Albert, M.L. (2014). Brain and language:
more likely benefit from such an approach. Further Evidence for neural multifunctionality. Behavioural Neurology,
research in this area with larger cohorts and different 2014, 1–16. doi:10.1155/2014/260381
participant groups is warranted in order to enhance Crawford, J. R., Garthwaite, P. H., & Porter, S. (2010). Point and
interval estimates of effect sizes for the case-controls design in
our understanding of the theoretical relationship
neuropsychology: Rationale, methods, implementations, and
between autobiographical memory and dis- pro-posed reporting standards. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 27,
course production. 245–260. doi:10.1080/02643294.2010.513967
De Simone, M.S., Fadda, L., Perri, R., Aloisi, M., Caltagirone,
C., & Carlesimo, G.A. (2016). Does retrieval frequency
Acknowledgements account for the pattern of autobiographical memory loss in
early Alzheimer’s disease patients? Neuropsychologia, 80,
The authors would like to thank the participants who 194–200. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.11.024
gave their time, energy and commitment to the study, Duru Aşiret, G., & Kapucu, S. (2016). The effect of reminis-
Jan Bottrell and Elinor Pharoah, who assisted with cence therapy on cognition, depression, and activities of daily
delivery and coding, and the staff at the residential living for patients with Alzheimer disease. Journal of Geriatric
Psychiatry and Neurology, 29, 31–33. doi:10.1177/
care facility who recruited participants and negotiated
0891988715598233
the daily practicalities. Folkerts, A., Roheger, M., Franklin, J., Middelst€adt, J., & Kalbe,
E. (2017). Cognitive interventions in patients with dementia
Supplementary material living in long-term care facilities: Systematic review and
meta-analysis. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 73,
Supplemental data for this article can be accessed at 204–221. doi:10.1016/j.archger.2017.07.017
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17549507.2020.1747542 Fromm, D., Holland, A., Armstrong, A., Forbes, M.,
MacWhinney, B., Risko, A., & Mattison, M. (2011). “Better
but no cigar”: Persons with aphasia speak about their speech.
ORCID Aphasiology, 25, 1431–1447. doi:10.1080/02687038.2011.
608839
Anne Whitworth http://orcid.org/0000-0002- Hamdy, R.C., Lewis, J.V., Copeland, R., Depelteau, A., Kinser,
4653-0782 A., Kendall-Wilson, T., & Whalen, K. (2018). Repetitive
Sharon Smart http://orcid.org/0000-0002- questioning exasperates caregivers. Gerontology & Geriatric
5882-4673 Medicine, 4, 233372141773891. doi:10.1177/
2333721417738915
Jade Cartwright http://orcid.org/0000-0002- Hopper, T., & Bayles, K. (2008). Management of neurogenic
6381-6184 communication disorders associated with dementia. In R.
Chapey (Ed.), Language intervention strategies in aphasia and
related neurogenic communication disorders (5th ed., pp.
References
64–162). Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statis- Koo, T., & Li, M. (2016). A guideline of selecting and reporting
tical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5V R ). Philadelphia: intraclass correlation coefficients for reliability research.
American Psychiatric Pub. Journal of Chiropractic Medicine, 15, 155–163. doi:10.1016/j.
Armstrong, E., Ciccone, N., Godecke, E., & Kok, B. (2011). jcm.2016.02.012
Monologues and dialogues in aphasia: Some initial compari- Lanzi, S., Burshnic, S., & Bourgeois, S. (2017). Person-centered
sons. Aphasiology, 25, 1347–1371. doi:10.1080/02687038. memory and communication strategies for adults with
2011.577204 dementia. Topics in Language Disorders, 37, 361–374. doi:10.
Beales, A., Whitworth, A., Cartwright, J., Panegyres, P., & 1097/TLD.0000000000000136
Kane, R. (2018). Determining stability in connected speech Levelt, W. (1999). A Blueprint of the speaker. In C. Brown & P.
in primary progressive aphasia and Alzheimer’s disease. Hagoort (Eds.), The neurocognition of language. Oxford:
International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 20, Oxford Press.
361–370. doi:10.1080/17549507.2018.1442498 Levelt, W.J.M. (1995). The ability to speak: From intentions to
Bloom, R., Borod, J., Obler, L., & Gerstman, L. (1992). Impact spoken words. European Review, 3, 13–23. doi:10.1017/
of emotional content on discourse production in patients with S1062798700001290
Nostalgic Recounts and Reminiscence Therapy 13

Leyhe, T., M€ uller, S., Milian, M., Eschweiler, G.W., & Saur, R. RCSLT position paper 2014. London, UK: RCSLT. Retrieved
(2009). Impairment of episodic and semantic autobiograph- from https://www.rcslt.org/members/publications/publica-
ical memory in patients with mild cognitive impairment and tions2/dementia_position_paper2014
early Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2464–2469. Schweitzer, P. (2011). Innovative approaches to reminiscence:
doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.04.018 remembering yesterday, caring today. In H. Lee & T. Adams
Martin, J., & White, P. (2005). The language of evaluation: (Eds.), Creative approaches in dementia care (pp. 174–194).
Appraisal in English. Houndmills, Basingstoke, Hampshire; Houndmills, Basingstoke, Hampshire; New York: Palgrave
New York: Palgrave Macmillan. Macmillan.
Merriam, S. (1989). The structure of simple reminiscence. Smith, S.C., Lamping, D.L., Banerjee, S., Harwood, R.H.,
Gerontologist, 29, 761–767. Retrieved from https://watermark. Foley, B., Smith, P., … Knapp, M. (2007). Development of
silverchair.com/ doi:10.1093/geront/29.6.761 a new measure of health-related quality of life for people with
Miller, J., & Iglesias, A. (2016). Systematic Analysis of Language dementia: DEMQOL. Psychological Medicine, 37, 737–746.
Transcripts (SALT), Student Version 2016 [Computer Software]. doi:10.1017/S0033291706009469
Middleton, WI: SALT Software, LLC. Westerhof, G., & Bohlmeijer, E. (2014). Celebrating fifty years
Nasreddine, Z.S., Phillips, N.A., B~a#Dirian, VA#r., ~ of research and applications in reminiscence and life review:
Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., … Chertkow, H. State of the art and new directions. Journal of Aging Studies,
(2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A brief 29, 107–114. doi:10.1016/j.jaging.2014.02.003
screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the Whitworth, A., Budiman, P., Beales, A., Wynn, S., Cartwright,
American Geriatrics Society, 53, 695–699. doi:10.1111/j.1532- J., & Claessen, M. (2018). Just how stable is our discourse? A
5415.2005.53221.x hurdle for measuring therapeutic change (or not).
Nicholas, L., & Brookshire, R. (1993). A system for quantifying Aphasiology, 32, 246–247. doi:10.1080/02687038.2018.
the Informativeness and efficiency of the connected speech of 1493427
adults with aphasia. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 36, Whitworth, A., Cartwright, J., Beales, A., Leit~ao, S., Panegyres,
338–350. doi:10.1044/jshr.3602.338 P., & Kane, R. (2018). Taking words to a new level: A prelim-
O’Rourke, J., Tobin, F., O’Callaghan, S., Sowman, R., & inary investigation of discourse intervention in primary pro-
Collins, D. (2011). YouTube: A useful tool for reminiscence gressive aphasia. Aphasiology, 32, 1226–1284. doi:10.1080/
therapy in dementia? Age and Ageing, 40, 742–869. doi:10. 02687038.2017.1390543
1093/ageing/afr100 Whitworth, A., Claessen, M., Leit~ao, S., & Webster, J. (2015).
Okumura, Y., Tanimukai, S., & Asada, T. (2008). Effects of Beyond narrative: Is there an implicit structure to the way in
short-term reminiscence therapy on elderly with dementia: A which adults organise their discourse? Clinical Linguistics &
comparison with everyday conversation approaches. Phonetics, 29, 455–481. doi:10.3109/02699206.2015.1020450
Psychogeriatrics, 8, 124–133. doi:10.1111/j.1479-8301.2008. Whitworth, A., Ng, N., Timms, L., & Power, E. (2020).
00236.x Exploring the viability of NARNIA with cognitive-communi-
Pinquart, M., & Forstmeier, S. (2012). Effects of reminiscence cation difficulties: A pilot study. Seminars in Speech and
interventions on psychosocial outcomes: A meta-analysis. Language, 41, 83–98. doi:10.1055/s-0039-3400512
Aging & Mental Health, 16, 541–558. doi:10.1080/13607863. Woods, B., Spector, A., Jones, C., Orrell, M., & Davies, S.
2011.651434 (2005). Reminiscence therapy for dementia. Cochrane
Royal College of Speech and Language Therapists. (2014). Database of Systematic Reviews, 2000, CD001120. doi:10.
Speech and language therapy provision for people with dementia: 1002/14651858.CD001120.pub2

You might also like