Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts

© 2018 American Psychological Association 2018, Vol. 1, No. 999, 000


1931-3896/18/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/aca0000193

Linking Trauma to Posttraumatic Growth and Mental Health Through


Emotional and Cognitive Creativity

Hod Orkibi and Neta Ram-Vlasov


University of Haifa
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

The association between adverse life events and creativity has almost exclusively been investigated
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

qualitatively and with eminent creators. Also, the mediating roles of emotional creativity (the ability to
experience novel and appropriate emotions), divergent thinking (the cognitive ability to think of multiple
ideas), and creative self-efficacy (CSE; one’s confidence in their ability to be creative) have not been
explored simultaneously or in this context. The goal of the present study was to test a multiple mediation
model which theorized that exposure to traumatic events would be associated with posttraumatic growth
(PTG) and mental health symptoms through emotional creativity, divergent thinking, and CSE in the
general population. Findings from a sample of 252 Israeli adults (73% females, aged 19 –58), of whom
64% had been exposed to war as civilians, showed that exposure to a greater number of traumatic events
was related to higher CSE scores, emotional creativity, and overall divergent thinking. A path analysis
to test indirect effects indicated that emotional creativity (but not divergent thinking) followed by CSE
mediated the positive association between exposure and PTG as well as the negative association between
exposure and mental health symptoms. CSE mediated the association between emotional creativity and
divergent thinking to both PTG and mental health symptoms. The results may provide a better
understanding of possible paths through which exposure to traumatic events relates to psychological
outcomes, highlighting the role of CSE as a mediator that may account for how emotional and cognitive
creative abilities are associated with PTG and mental health.

Keywords: trauma, posttraumatic growth, divergent thinking, emotional creativity, creative self-efficacy

Many people are exposed to potentially traumatic events during ertzel, Goertzel, & Goertzel, 1978; Piirto, 1998; Simonton, 2010),
their lives (Benjet et al., 2016; Sledjeski, Speisman, & Dierker, this association has been largely unexplored quantitatively and in
2008). These are defined as events that are directly experienced, noneminent people. The present study investigated the adversity-
witnessed, or experienced indirectly, that involve death, serious creativity association in a sample drawn from the general popula-
injury, or threat to the self or others (American Psychiatric Asso- tion in Israel. We examined a multiple mediation model (i.e.,
ciation, 2013). In a nationally representative sample of U.S. adults, where several mediator variables operate in parallel and in se-
68% reported experiencing more than one potentially traumatic quence), theorizing that the exposure to potentially traumatic
event (Goldstein et al., 2016). Among the general population in events could be associated with posttraumatic growth (PTG) and
Israel, 9% of all adults reported a lifetime prevalence of posttrau- mental health through emotional creativity, divergent thinking, and
matic stress (Bleich, Gelkopf, & Solomon, 2003; Hobfoll, Canetti- creative self-efficacy (CSE).
Nisim, & Johnson, 2006), with more symptoms during or imme-
diately after a military conflict or terror event (Hobfoll et al., 2008;
Trauma and Posttraumatic Growth
Neria, Besser, Kiper, & Westphal, 2010).
Can such adverse life events be related to positive outcomes? Considerable research indicates that exposure to traumatic
Can creativity play a role in this relationship? Whereas adverse events has a negative impact on physical and mental health (Benjet
and potentially traumatic events have been qualitatively associated et al., 2016). Certain individuals exposed to traumatic events may
with creativity in eminent creators (Csikszentmihalyi, 1996; Go- nevertheless experience PTG—positive psychological changes ex-
perienced following a traumatic or highly stressful life event
(Tedeschi & Calhoun, 2004). PTG theories generally suggest that
an experience of adversity shatters individuals’ pretrauma beliefs
about the benevolence, justice, and controllability of the world,
Hod Orkibi and Neta Ram-Vlasov, School of Creative Arts Therapies,
University of Haifa.
and may cause them to create new beliefs and views that integrate
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Hod the negative and positive consequences of the traumatic event, thus
Orkibi, School of Creative Arts Therapies, University of Haifa, 199 Aba enabling a more elaborated and complex understanding of the self
Khoushy Avenue, Mount Carmel, Haifa 31905, Israel. E-mail: horkibi@ and the world (C. L. Park, 2004). According to Tedeschi and
univ.haifa.ac.il Calhoun (2004), there are five domains of PTG: an increase in

1
2 ORKIBI AND RAM-VLASOV

perceived new possibilities, an appreciation of life, acknowledg- negative emotional experiences can facilitate creative performance
ment of personal strength, spirituality, and positive relationships in (Baas, De Dreu, & Nijstad, 2008; Davis, 2009). In a different
terms of becoming closer to friends and family. PTG thus involves study, the lifetime number of events was a significant predictor of
a creative reconstruction of an individual’s life narrative in an perceived creative growth, but with a small effect size (Forgeard,
effort to make sense of the adversity, find new meaning in the 2013). A more recent quantitative study indicated that performing
experience, and create a new strand of continuity that incorporates artists who reported being exposed to a greater number of types of
the event into a coherent life narrative (Jirek, 2017; Neimeyer, childhood adversity also reported more intense creative experi-
2001, 2004; Tedeschi & Calhoun, 2004). PTG has been positively, ences compared to performers with minimal or no childhood
albeit weakly, associated with self-reported creative growth, the adversity, with a medium effect size (Thomson & Jaque, 2018).
perceived increase in people’s own creativity as a result of a In terms of well-being, most studies to date have focused on the
traumatic event (Forgeard, 2013). However, a number of alterna- associations between creativity and psychopathology rather than
tive explanations of PTG are less positive, and view people’s creativity and health (Thys, Sabbe, & De Hert, 2014). However,
reports of growth after adversity as self-enhancing positive illu- creativity has long been identified as an important human quality
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

sions, avoidance-based defense or coping strategy, and maladap- in the humanistic (Maslow, 1962; Rogers, 1961) and positive
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tive reality distortions (Chopko, Palmieri, & Adams, 2018; Jaya- psychology literature (Peterson & Seligman, 2004). In the creativ-
wickreme & Blackie, 2014; Lahav, Solomon, & Levin, 2016). It ity literature, Cropley (1990) noted that creativity can be both a
has been suggested that PTG may only be related to lesser psy- means of artistic expression and a conscious or unconscious force
chological distress when actual growth-related actions are in- affecting personal growth. This suggests that creative people can
volved, rather than merely growth-related cognitions (Hobfoll et use their creative skills in coping with and adapting to life. In an
al., 2007). This points to the need to further explore the associa- extensive discussion of health and creativity, Runco (2007) re-
tions between traumatic events, creativity, and indicators of well- viewed empirical evidence on the relationships between creativity
being and health. and both psychological and physical health. Notably, creativity
was associated with adaptability, which Runco identified as “one
of the most powerful concepts in the creativity literature” (p. 161).
Trauma, Creativity, and Well-Being
Cohen’s (2012, p. 8) similar concept of creative adaptiveness
Exposure to a potentially traumatic event disrupts the homeo- refers to the “ability to adjust flexibly to conditions or environ-
static resting state and triggers a series of responses intended to ments in developing new ideas or products while adhering to what
enable the organism to adjust to the altered condition (Zemishlany, is approved or permitted in a given cultural context.” Relatedly,
2012). Runco (1999) argued that adverse or traumatic experiences creative thinking that is new and helpful was posited to be a
create tension and disequilibrium, which may in turn intrinsically process variable that may promote adaptive future-thinking and
lead to creative adaptation in terms of reinterpreting the experience psychological flexibility in clinical interventions (Forgeard & El-
through new emotions and cognitions. Similarly, diversifying ex- stein, 2014). Some recent empirical studies have shown that ev-
perience theory posits that highly unusual and unpredictable events eryday creative activities have small to moderate associations with
or situations, including adverse and potentially traumatic events, feeling happy and active (Conner & Silvia, 2015; Silvia et al.,
can foster unconventional ideas by disrupting conventional and/or 2014), experiencing more positive emotions and flourishing in
fixed patterns of thinking and pushing individuals to break bound- terms of feelings of purpose and meaning in life, engagement, and
aries and see new perspectives (Damian, 2017; Damian & Simon- social connectedness (Conner, DeYoung, & Silvia, 2018).
ton, 2015). Historiometric research, biographical case studies, and In summary, the qualitative evidence on the adversity-creativity
qualitative data suggest that creative individuals tend to have association has focused on highly creative individuals and it re-
experienced higher rates of adversity, including early family mains unclear how this specific relationship links to PTG and
trauma and loss (Csikszentmihalyi, 1996; Goertzel et al., 1978; health in the general population. This study sought to quantita-
Piirto, 1998; Simonton, 2010). In particular, the childhood psy- tively examine a theoretical model postulating that exposure to
chological stress associated with social isolation has been defined potentially traumatic events may relate to PTG and mental health
as a stressful life event that “can provide the freedom and moti- through three mediators discussed below: emotional creativity,
vation to observe, create, and fantasize” (Aldwin & Sutton, 1998, divergent thinking, and CSE.
p. 53).
Aside from these studies, however, there are few quantitative
Emotional and Cognitive Creativity
examinations of the association between adversity and creativity.
In one study, Averill (1999) found that a higher number and Averill’s conceptualization of emotional creativity is “basically
greater severity of prior traumatic events were moderately associ- unidimensional” but factor analysis has suggested that it consists
ated with greater emotional creativity. A survey study showed of the three facets of preparedness, novelty, and a combination of
relatively small linear associations between creativity (as a char- effectiveness/authenticity (Averill, 1999, 2000, 2009; Sundarara-
acter strength), the number of potentially traumatic events and jan & Averill, 2007, p. 209). Preparedness refers to the under-
PTG (Peterson, Park, Pole, D’Andrea, & Seligman, 2008). A few standing of emotions and the willingness to explore emotions.
experimental studies have reported moderate to strong associations Novelty refers to experiencing new emotional reactions in com-
between the stressful experience of social rejection and better parison to a person’s past reactions or to the reference group.
performance on creative thinking tasks (Akinola & Mendes, 2008; Effectiveness refers to an emotional response that has potential
S. H. Kim, Vincent, & Goncalo, 2013). These findings are some- value and benefit to the person or the group. Authenticity refers to
what consistent with studies suggesting that under some conditions an emotional response that is self-originated and expresses a
TRAUMA, CREATIVITY, AND PTG 3

person’s own values and beliefs. Note that emotional creativity and Creative Self-Efficacy
emotional intelligence are two distinct sets of abilities: whereas the
former involves the ability to generate a novel and appropriate Another mediator in our theorized model is CSE, which refers to
a person’s confidence in his or her ability to be creative (Tierney
emotional response that diverges from the individual’s typical
& Farmer, 2002). CSE is rooted in Bandura’s (1997) social cog-
response, the latter involves the ability to converge to the best
nitive theory, which posits that strong efficacious beliefs enhance
solution to an emotionally related problem (Ivcevic, 2007).
human achievement and well-being because people who have high
Emotional creativity was shown to be significantly and posi-
confidence in their capabilities perceive difficulties as challenges
tively related to the early experience of traumatic events, hassles,
to be mastered rather than as threats to be avoided. Bandura argued
and minor disappointments during childhood and adolescence
that successful mastery experiences are the most effective way to
(Averill, 1999). Higher scores on the Effectiveness and Authen-
build strong efficacy beliefs and that “innovativeness requires an
ticity dimensions of emotional creativity were reported to be
unshakeable sense of efficacy to persist in creative endeavors” (p.
moderately associated with the greater perceived benefit of that
239). CSE is thought to influence whether a person will engage
event (Averill, 1999), which is consistent with the conceptualiza-
with (or avoid) a particular creative endeavor, sustain an effort, or
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tion of PTG. Whereas emotional creativity was positively corre- perform at a particular level of creative achievement (Beghetto &
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

lated with students’ more frequent experience of positive emotions Karwowski, 2017). CSE has been measured as both a domain-
in the classroom during a semester (love, gratitude, and hope; general (Karwowski, 2016) and a domain-specific factor (Be-
Oriol, Amutio, Mendoza, Da Costa, & Miranda, 2016) it correlated ghetto, Kaufman, & Baxter, 2011; Karwowski, Gralewski, &
negatively with students’ self-reported alexithymia, both with me- Szumski, 2015).
dium effects (Fuchs, Kumar, & Porter, 2007). The emotional To the best of our knowledge, no studies have examined the
creativity of young adults was also positively correlated with more association between CSE and emotional creativity, but one study
frequent involvement in creative leisure activities such as writing, indicated that general self-efficacy had a small positive correlation
painting, composing music, performing drama, and do-it-your-self with the emotional creativity of high school students (Zareie,
home improvement, with small to medium effects (Trnka, Zahrad- 2014). The empirical findings on the associations between CSE
nik, & Kuška, 2016). and divergent thinking are inconsistent: some studies have re-
Cognitive creativity (i.e., creative ideations or thinking) gener- ported no correlations (Puente-Diaz & Arroyo Judith, 2016;
ally refers to the ability to generate multiple creative ideas or Puente-Díaz & Cavazos-Arroyo, 2017b; Reiter-Palmon et al.,
alternative solutions to a problem (Guilford, 1967; Torrance, 1998) 2012) whereas others have reported small to medium correlations
and is traditionally measured on divergent thinking tasks that ask (Mathisen & Bronnick, 2009; Pretz & McCollum, 2014; Puente-
people to generate ideas in response to verbal or figural prompts. Díaz & Cavazos-Arroyo, 2017a). This inconsistency may result
Divergent thinking tasks are scored according to four ideational from different operationalizations and measures of CSE as well as
outcomes: fluency, which represents the ability to produce many unexplored intervening variables.
ideas; flexibility, which represents the ability to produce ideas Consistent with Bandura’s reasoning, CSE is thought to emerge
from different conceptual categories; originality, which represents and build on successful creative performance. It has been sug-
the ability to produce unique and statistically infrequent ideas; and gested as a possible mediator in the relationship between creative
elaboration, which represents the ability to produce detailed ideas. potential and creative achievement (Karwowski & Barbot, 2016).
The scoring of a divergent thinking task is performed manually by CSE has also been proposed as a mediating process variable that
trained raters and some outcomes are strongly interrelated; for explains how personal and/or contextual factors contribute to cre-
example, more responses (fluency) lead to more categories (flex- ative outcomes (Choi, 2004; Karwowski, 2016; Puente-Díaz &
ibility; Kaufman, Plucker, & Baer, 2008). Divergent thinking tasks Cavazos-Arroyo, 2017b; Qiang, Han, Guo, Bai, & Karwowski,
are frequently used to estimate the potential for creative thinking 2018). In a recent review of studies on the mediating role of CSE,
and future creative achievements (K. H. Kim, 2008; Runco & the authors concluded there is “a solid base of empirical evidence
Acar, 2012; Runco, Millar, Acar, & Cramond, 2010; Zeng, Proc- for CSE as an important avenue through which other factors can
tor, & Salvendy, 2011). The correlations between divergent think- build creativity” (Farmer & Tierney, 2017, p. 39). In this same
ing scores (externally rated) and self-reported perceived creativity vein, CSE was reported to mediate the relationship between cre-
vary in the literature, and range typically from zero to moderately ative mindsets and creative problem-solving (Royston & Reiter-
positive (Pretz & McCollum, 2014; Reiter-Palmon, Robinson- Palmon, 2017) as well as the relationship between intrinsic moti-
Morral, Kaufman, & Santo, 2012; Silvia, Wigert, Reiter-Palmon, vation and creative achievement, and between intrinsic motivation
& Kaufman, 2012). and well-being indicators, albeit with relatively modest effect sizes
Finally, there is evidence that divergent thinking components (Forgeard & Benson, 2017).
are moderately predictive of coping abilities in young children
(Carson, Bittner, Cameron, Brown, & Meyer, 1994) and that
The Present Study
divergent thinking flexibility is associated with reduced stress and Although qualitative studies have suggested that the experience
higher life satisfaction among hurricane survivors, with medium of adversities and traumatic events can predict creative achieve-
effects (Metzl, 2009). Self-reported flexibility was moderately ment (Damian & Simonton, 2015; Goertzel et al., 1978; Piirto,
associated with better well-being among earthquake survivors (Fu 1998; Simonton, 2010), only a few creativity constructs have been
& Chow, 2017), as well as with lower levels of posttraumatic quantitatively associated with PTG (Forgeard, 2013). The present
stress symptoms and higher levels of PTG among veterans (Keith, cross-sectional study sought to extend the literature by (a) quan-
Velezmoro, & O’Brien, 2015). titatively examining the empirical links between trauma and cre-
4 ORKIBI AND RAM-VLASOV

ativity, (b) examining a sample drawn from the general population, experienced by individuals following a traumatic event. Two items
(c) simultaneously examining the three types of creativity in one represent each of the five original subscales of growth: new
model, and (d) evaluating the possible mediating role of emotional possibilities, relating to others, personal strength, spiritual change
creativity, cognitive creativity, and CSE. and appreciation of life (Tedeschi & Calhoun, 1996). Items are
We tested a multiple mediation model positing three-path me- scored on a 6-point scale ranging from 0 (not at all/never experi-
diations where two mediators intervene sequentially (i.e., in a enced) to 5 (very much) and the global growth score is calculated
chain) between the independent variable and the dependent vari- as the sum of all responses. The short form was used to minimize
able. Hypothesis 1 posited that exposure to traumatic events should the burden on participants. The validity of the short version was
relate to PTG and mental health through two sequential mediators: confirmed by a strong correlation (r ⫽ .94) with the long version
emotional creativity followed by CSE. Hypothesis 2 posited that and good internal consistency reliability with a Cronbach’s alpha
exposure to traumatic events should relate to PTG and mental of .86 (Daphna-Tekoah, 2017). In this study, Item 7 (“I have
health through two sequential mediators: divergent thinking fol- stronger religious faith”) was deleted following an internal con-
lowed by CSE. sistency reliability analysis. The final Cronbach’s alpha was .87
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

for the nine-item scale.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Method Mental health symptoms. The 12-item General Health Ques-


tionnaire is a self-report measure of the severity of psychological
distress in the last month, with a main focus on depression and
Participants anxiety symptoms (Goldberg & Williams, 1988). The question-
An initial sample of 274 adults was recruited from three colleges naire consists of six positively worded items (e.g., “felt capable of
in northern Israel and through online social networks. Of this making decisions about things”), and six negatively worded items
initial sample, 22 respondents who failed an inattentiveness check (e.g., “lost much sleep over worry”). Participants indicate whether
item were excluded, a recommended procedure “which should they experienced each symptom in the last month on a scale
improve data quality, power, and reproducibility” (McKibben & ranging from 0 (never) to 3 (always). Positively worded items are
Silvia, 2017, p. 12). The final sample was composed of 252 recoded so that high scores represent worse health. Validity has
participants (73% females) aged 19 –58 (M ⫽ 29, SD ⫽ 7.6). The been reported through significant correlations with many health
majority of the sample (60%) identified as Jewish, 30% as Arabs, and well-being measures and good internal consistency reliability
and 10% as “other.” Similarly, 60% identified their native lan- was demonstrated with Cronbach’s alphas of .82–.86 (Goldberg &
guage (i.e., mother tongue) as Hebrew and 40% as “other.” Most Williams, 1988). In this study, the Cronbach’s alpha was .85.
participants (90%) were bachelor’s-degree students in the social Cognitive creativity. The Abbreviated Torrance Test for
sciences and 10% were master’s-degree students. Fifty-five per- Adults (ATTA; Goff & Torrance, 2002) assesses four divergent
cent reported their financial situation was “average,” 40% reported thinking abilities: fluency, originality, elaboration, and flexibility.
“above average,” and 5% “below average.” We used Verbal Task 1 for fluency and originality and Visual Task
3 for elaboration and flexibility that have been translated to He-
brew. To establish interrater reliability, two independent raters
Measures
with thorough training in rating the ATTA scored 40 tasks, of
Traumatic events. The Traumatic Events Questionnaire which 10 were randomly selected for each subscale, after which
(Vrana & Lauterbach, 1994) asks participants to state whether they their results were compared. Differences were discussed and the
have experienced nine traumatic events such as a serious accident, raters either came to an agreement or corrected errors. To ensure
death of someone close, physical or sexual abuse, as well as other consistency, one rater continued to grade all tests while discussing
events they are asked to list. As in previous studies in Israel (Amir questions with the other rater to achieve consensus (Althuizen,
& Sol, 1999), military and terror-related traumatic events were Wierenga, & Rossiter, 2010). The interrater reliability coefficients
added to the original scale, such as serious training accidents, (interclass correlations, ICCs) for the four tasks were Fluency Task
combat in wars or military operations, missile attacks, and terror 1 (ICC ⫽ .98), Originality Task 1 (ICC ⫽ .91), Elaboration Task
bombings. The total number of events was 30. For each event, 3 (ICC ⫽ .88), and Flexibility Task 3 (ICC ⫽ .88), indicating
participants were asked to indicate how traumatic the event was for excellent reliability (Shrout & Fleiss, 1979). Raw subscale scores
them on a scale of 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely). Consistent with were analyzed in this study because the standard scores in the
previous studies in Israel, this 5-point scale was recoded into a ATTA manual are from a sample precluding comparison. A com-
dichotomous exposure score: no exposure was coded 0 for rates 1 posite ATTA score was computed given our interest in the overall
to 2 and exposure was recoded 1 for rates 3 to 5 (Lev-Wiesel, role of divergent thinking (i.e., “cognitive creativity”), the need for
Daphna-Tekoah, & Hallak, 2009; Stolovy, Lev-Wiesel, & Witz- parsimony, and as suggested in the ATTA manual (Goff & Tor-
tum, 2015). Then, to obtain a trauma exposure score, according to rance, 2002, p. 5). We converted each fluency, flexibility, elabo-
the impact on the person, the number of events reported by the ration, and originality score into a z-score and then summed them
individual was summed; additional points were added if an event (see also Runco et al., 2010).
occurred more than once and if it occurred during childhood. The Emotional creativity. The Emotional Creativity Inventory
original questionnaire has shown good validity (Crawford, Lang, (ECI; Averill, 1999) is a 30-item self-report measure that includes
& Laffaye, 2008) and good test–retest reliability coefficients rang- three subscales representing components of emotional creativity:
ing from .72 to .99 (Vrana & Lauterbach, 1994). preparedness, novelty, and effectiveness/authenticity. Respondents
Posttraumatic growth. The 10-item Posttraumatic Growth are asked to rate statements concerning the way they think, feel,
Inventory Short Form measures self-reports of positive changes and react in a variety of situations, and their opinions on a variety
TRAUMA, CREATIVITY, AND PTG 5

of issues. Items are scored on a scale ranging from 1 (much less) .01 and CSE, r ⫽ .25, p ⬍ .001, and negatively but weakly
to 5 (much more). The ECI’s validity was confirmed in several correlated with mental illness symptoms, r ⫽ ⫺.15, p ⬍ .05.
studies that have also showed a test–retest reliability coefficient of Educational level was positively but weakly correlated with the
r ⫽ .91 and internal consistency reliability with a Cronbach’s composite ATTA score, r ⫽ .15, p ⬍ .05 and emotional creativity,
alpha of .91 for the ECI total score (Averill, 1999). In this study, r ⫽ .22, p ⬍ .01.
the Cronbach’s alpha was .92 for the total score, which was used A multivariate analysis of variance indicated there was no
given our primary interest in the global construct of “emotional statistically significant difference in the study variables based on
creativity” as a parallel to “cognitive creativity.” participants’ financial situation (Wilk’s ␭ ⫽ .928, F(12, 454) ⫽
Creative self-efficacy. The six-item self-report CSE scale is 1.43, p ⫽ .15, partial ␩2 ⫽ .037). A t test for independent samples
part of the creative self-concept measure that also includes a indicated one statistically significant difference based on partici-
5-item scale of creative personal identity which was not used here pants’ native language (Hebrew vs. other). Participants with He-
(Karwowski, 2014). Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses brew as a native language scored significantly higher (M ⫽ .63,
suggested the two subscales are correlated but measure distinct SD ⫽ 2.88) than other participants (M ⫽ ⫺.98, SD ⫽ 2.58) on the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

constructs. A sample item is “I trust my creative abilities.” Partic- ATTA, t(250) ⫽ 4.50, p ⱕ .001, with a Cohen’s d ⫽ 0.58
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ipants rate items on a scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to indicating a medium effect size (J. Cohen, 1992). Thus, because
5 (strongly agree), with high scores reflecting high CSE. Validity the divergent thinking test includes a verbal task, participants’
was demonstrated through positive correlations with creative abil- native language was included in the subsequent mediation analysis
ities and self-report originality, and internal consistency reliability to control for its potential effect on ATTA scores.
was confirmed with a Cronbach’s alpha of .89 (Karwowski, 2014). A t test for independent samples indicated there were statisti-
In this study, the Cronbach’s alpha for the CSE scale was .83. cally significant gender differences for two variables. Females
(M ⫽ 1.06, SD ⫽ .35) scored significantly high than males (M ⫽
Procedure .90, SD ⫽ .35) on mental health symptoms, t(250) ⫽ ⫺3.25, p ⱕ
Permission to conduct the study was obtained from the Univer- .001, with a Cohen’s d ⫽ 0.46 indicating a medium effect size.
sity’s Human Research Ethics Committee. All participants signed Males (M ⫽ 3.93, SD ⫽ .66) scored higher than females (M ⫽
an informed consent form where it was clarified that participation 3.65, SD ⫽ .59) on CSE, t(250) ⫽ 3.24, p ⱕ .001, with a Cohen’s
in the study was on a voluntary basis and it was explicitly ex- d ⫽ 0.45 indicating a medium effect size. Given these results, the
plained that participants had the right to refuse to participate or to model’s invariance across gender was explored.
withdraw from the study at any time, without any penalty or
prejudice to their interests. Data were collected using pen and Descriptive Statistics and Correlation Analysis
paper for the divergent thinking test and Internet-based survey We measured the lifetime prevalence of 30 traumatic events. As
software. All participants were asked to provide their age, gender, can be seen in Figure 1, the most prevalent events in this sample,
religion, native language, education level, and socioeconomic sta- in descending order, were: exposure to war as a civilian (63%), car
tus. accident (60%), unexpected sudden death of a significant other
(55%), and a life threatening condition in a close person (42%).
Data Analysis Table 1 presents the means and standard deviations for all
First, the prevalence of each traumatic event in the sample and variables with higher scores indicating higher levels of that psy-
differences in the study variables according to demographics were chological construct. Note that the CSE mean score in the present
examined. Second, bivariate correlations for all the variables were sample was similar to that reported in other studies (e.g., Kar-
explored. Third, path analysis tested the theoretical model using wowski, 2014). As can also be seen in Table 1, the sum of
IBM’s AMOS 23 (Byrne, 2016). The model’s fit to the data was traumatic events correlated significantly and positively with emo-
evaluated using the criteria of ␹2/df ⬍3, a comparative fit in- tional creativity, the composite ATTA score, and three divergent
dex ⱖ0.95, the Tucker–Lewis index ⱖ0.95, and the root mean thinking abilities (fluency, originality, and flexibility) but not with
square error of approximation ⬍0.80 (Schreiber, Nora, Stage, elaboration. Traumatic events were also significantly positively
Barlow, & King, 2006). The bootstrap method for testing indirect correlated with CSE, suggesting that exposure to a larger number
effects (i.e., mediation) was used with the confidence level set at of events may be associated with having a higher sense of CSE.
0.95 and bootstrap bias-corrected samples set at 5,000. When zero Moreover, CSE positively correlated with emotional creativity and
is not within the 95% confidence interval (CI), the indirect effect all divergent thinking measures except flexibility. CSE also posi-
is significantly different from zero at p ⬍ .05 (two-tailed; Preacher tively correlated with PTG and negatively correlated with mental
& Hayes, 2004, p. 722). Maximum likelihood imputation was used health symptoms.
to adjust for missing values (Byrne, 2016). Finally, the model’s
invariance across gender was explored with a procedure for mul- Theorized Mediation Model
tigroup analysis (Byrne, 2004). Based on theoretical reasoning, the error terms of PTG and
mental health symptoms were allowed to covary. Emotional cre-
Results ativity and ATTA were also allowed to covary, but because the
Preliminary Analysis of Demographics relationship was not significant (p ⫽ .057), the covariance was
removed. The path analysis indicated that the theorized model
A preliminary analysis indicated that age was positively but depicted in Figure 2 provided a good fit to the data on the fit
weakly correlated with the composite ATTA score, r ⫽ .18, p ⬍ indices shown in Table 2 (top row).
6 ORKIBI AND RAM-VLASOV
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 1. Prevalence of traumatic events.

For the emotional creativity path (Hypothesis 1), two three- possible to reject the null hypothesis of no mediation, and
path mediation sequences were tested, each with two mediators indicating the existence of an indirect effect (Taylor et al.,
intervening in a sequence between the independent and depen- 2008). Similarly, the analysis confirmed a significant indirect
dent variable. We calculated a separate z-score for each specific association between traumatic events and mental health symp-
three-path mediation (Taylor, MacKinnon, & Tein, 2008). The toms through emotional creativity followed by CSE as two
analysis confirmed a significant indirect association between mediators intervening in a sequence (z ⫽ ⫺ 2.50, p ⬍ .05,
traumatic events and PTG through emotional creativity fol- indirect effect of ␤ ⫽ ⫺0.021, SE ⫽ 0.01).
lowed by CSE as two mediators intervening in a sequence (z ⫽ In addition, CSE, as single mediator, significantly mediated the
2.15, p ⬍ .05, indirect effect of ␤ ⫽ 0.013, SE ⫽ 0.01). This positive association between emotional creativity and PTG (95%
specific three-path mediation was confirmed because the CI [.014, .086], p ⬍ .01, indirect effect of ␤ ⫽ 0.042, SE ⫽ 0.02).
z-score fell outside the range of ⫺1.96 and ⫹ 1.96, making it CSE, as single mediator, also mediated the negative association
TRAUMA, CREATIVITY, AND PTG 7

Table 1
Descriptive Statistics and Correlations

Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

1. Traumatic events —
2. Emotional creativity .31ⴱⴱ —
3. Fluency .19ⴱⴱ .18ⴱⴱ —
4. Originality .20ⴱⴱ .12 .82ⴱⴱ —
5. Flexibility .13ⴱ .14ⴱ .31ⴱⴱ .33ⴱⴱ —
ⴱⴱ
6. Elaboration .12 .07 .17 .18ⴱⴱ .33ⴱⴱ —
7. ATTA .23ⴱⴱ .18ⴱ .80ⴱⴱ .81ⴱⴱ .68ⴱⴱ .58ⴱⴱ —
8. CSE .24ⴱⴱ .28ⴱⴱ .16ⴱ .15ⴱ .09 .20ⴱⴱ .21ⴱⴱ —
9. PTG .19ⴱⴱ .39ⴱⴱ .07 .12 –.06 –.03 .04 .29ⴱⴱ —
10. MHS .08 .04 –.06 –.02 –.00 .00 –.03 –.26ⴱⴱ –.16ⴱⴱ —
M (SD) 5.57 (4.60) 2.80 (.63) 5.81 (3.07) 3.48 (2.51) 3.17 (1.40) 10.61 (7.28) .00a (.72) 3.72 (.62) 3.05 (1.18) 1.01 (.35)
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Note. ATTA ⫽ Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults composite score; CSE ⫽ creative self-efficacy; PTG ⫽ posttraumatic growth; MHS ⫽ mental health
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

symptoms. Interpretation of the magnitude of correlations according to Cohen (1992): small ⱖ.10, medium ⱖ.30, and large ⱖ.50.
a
z-Score mean equals 0.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01.

between emotional creativity and mental health symptoms (95% mediator, mediated the positive association between ATTA and
CI [⫺.126, ⫺.030], p ⬍ .001, indirect effect of ␤ ⫽ ⫺0.068, SE ⫽ PTG (95% CI [.004, .066], p ⬍ .05, indirect effect of ␤ ⫽ 0.028,
0.02). Note that because the CI values did not include zero, the SE ⫽ 0.01). CSE, as single mediator, also mediated the negative
indirect effect was significantly different from zero and the null association between ATTA and mental health symptoms (95% CI
hypothesis of no mediation could be rejected for each of the single [⫺.094, ⫺.008], p ⬍ .05, indirect effect of ␤ ⫽ ⫺0.043, SE ⫽
mediator paths (Preacher & Hayes, 2004). 0.02). Again, because the CI values did not include zero, the
For the cognitive creativity path, a three-path mediation se- indirect effect was significantly different from zero and the null
quence was not confirmed (Hypothesis 2). However, the analysis hypothesis of no mediation could be rejected for each of the single
yielded a significant indirect association where CSE, as a single mediator paths (Preacher & Hayes, 2004).

Figure 2. Hypothesized model linking traumatic events to posttraumatic growth and mental health symptoms.
Standardized regression weights and multiple square correlations are presented. Solid lines indicate path
coefficients significantly different from zero and dashed paths indicate nonsignificant path coefficients. ATTA ⫽
Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults composite score.
8 ORKIBI AND RAM-VLASOV

Table 2 .05, indirect effect of ␤ ⫽ ⫺0.054, SE ⫽ 0.02). As seen in Table


Goodness-of-Fit Indices for the Five Models 2, the AIC that is used for model comparison, with lower values
reflecting better fit (Schreiber et al., 2006), indicated that the
Model ␹2 df ␹2/df CFI TLI AIC RMSEA alternative model with elaboration as a mediator was superior to all
ATTA 11.49 6 1.92 .97 .90 55.49 .06 other models (AIC ⫽ 50.32).
Elaboration 6.32 6 1.05 .99 .99 50.32 .02
Originality 8.02 6 1.33 .99 .95 52.02 .04
Flexibility 9.33 6 1.56 .98 .92 53.34 .05 Discussion
Fluency 9.82 6 1.64 .97 .91 53.82 .05
The purpose of this study was to quantitatively examine the
Note. ATTA ⫽ Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults composite score; roles of emotional creativity, cognitive creativity, and CSE in the
CFI ⫽ comparative fit index; TLI ⫽ Tucker–Lewis index; AIC ⫽ Akaike relationship between exposure to traumatic events and psycholog-
information criterion; RMSEA ⫽ root mean square error of approximation.
The four alternative mediation models can be seen in Figure 3. ical outcomes in the general population. The findings suggest that
exposure to a greater number of traumatic events was positively
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

related to higher CSE, emotional creativity, and overall divergent


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

thinking. The positive relationship between exposure and PTG as


In summary, although we found two three-path mediation chains
well as the negative relationship between exposure and mental
in the emotional creativity path (from traumatic events to both
health symptoms were statistically mediated by emotional creativ-
PTG and mental health symptoms), there was no similar three-path
ity followed by CSE. In addition, CSE, as a single mediator,
mediation in the cognitive creativity path. However, in both the
mediated the relationship between emotional creativity and diver-
emotional and cognitive creativity paths, CSE mediated the asso-
gent thinking with both PTG and mental health symptoms. The
ciation between creativity and psychological outcomes as a single
theorized model was not significantly different for female and
mediator. Overall, the R2 values in Figure 2 indicate that about
male participants and out of four alternative models examined
10% of the variance in PTG and 20% of the variance in mental
(which replaced the composite ATTA score by each one of its
health symptoms were explained by the model, with the corre-
components), the alternative model with elaboration as a mediator
sponding medium effect sizes of f 2 ⫽ 0.11 and f 2 ⫽ 0.25 (J.
was superior to all other models. The next section discusses these
Cohen, 1988).
findings in light of theories and previous research.

Theorized Model Invariance Across Gender


Traumatic Experiences and Creativity
Given the abovementioned significant gender differences, we
conducted a post hoc multigroup analysis to test whether the Our findings lend quantitative support to historiometric and
theorized mediation model differed for females (n ⫽ 184) versus biographical case studies that have probed the association between
males (n ⫽ 68). Two models were constructed for comparison: an adverse life events and creativity (Damian & Simonton, 2015;
unconstrained model that posited a distinct model for each gender Goertzel et al., 1978; Piirto, 1998; Simonton, 2010). The positive
group, and a fully constrained model that posited equality (i.e., correlations found here suggest that the greater the number of
invariance) on all regression weights between the gender groups potentially traumatic events people experience, the more likely
(Byrne, 2004, 2016). The results of the model comparison showed they are to score higher on emotional creativity and overall diver-
that the constrained and unconstrained models did not significantly gent thinking (i.e., a composite ATTA score). These findings are
differ (⌬␹2 ⫽ 21.308, ⌬df ⫽ 14, p ⫽ .094). The Akaike informa- conceptually consistent with Runco’s (1999) claim that adverse or
tion criterion (AIC) that is used for model comparison, with lower traumatic experiences may create tension and disequilibrium that
values reflecting better fit (Schreiber et al., 2006), also indicated may in turn intrinsically motivate individuals toward creative
that the constrained model (100.755) was superior to the uncon- adaptation in terms of reinterpreting the experience through new
strained model (107.447). These findings indicate that overall, the cognitions and emotions. This observation is also in line with the
model was not significantly different for female and male partic- abovementioned diversifying experience theory (Damian, 2017;
ipants in this study. Damian & Simonton, 2015), which posits that exposure to adverse
experiences may equip individuals with the ability to generate
unconventional ideations and emotional responses. As Averill
Alternative Mediation Models
(1999) noted, “Prior traumatic experiences may lead a person to
We conducted a post hoc path analysis of four alternative think about and be more innovative in emotional response . . .” or
models where only the composite ATTA score was replaced with a person who “is innovative emotionally may better remember or
each divergent thinking subscale score to pinpoint its specific role interpret prior events as traumatic . . . [although] a retrospective
in the model (see Figure 3). Exposure to traumatic events was reinterpretation of events is not likely to be the entire story” (p.
significantly and directly associated with fluency, originality, and 364). Given the correctional nature of the present data this question
flexibility, but only marginally significantly associated with elab- remains be answered in future studies.
oration (p ⫽ .054). These findings are consistent with the corre- Exposure to trauma has been associated with “the cognitive
lations presented in Table 1. Second, a significant indirect effect flexibility necessary for coming up with creative ideas” (Damian
was only found for the elaboration model, with CSE as a single & Simonton, 2015, p. 625), with an outflow of intrusive cognitions
mediator between elaboration and PTG (95% CI [.009, .078], p ⬍ (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), and with deliberate
.05, indirect effect of ␤ ⫽ 0.034, SE ⫽ 0.02) and between elabo- cognitive processes that signal “initial attempts to reconstruct core
ration and mental health symptoms (95% CI [⫺.108, ⫺.016], p ⬍ beliefs disrupted by the traumatic event” (Cann et al., 2011;
TRAUMA, CREATIVITY, AND PTG 9
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 3. Four alternative mediation models linking traumatic events to posttraumatic growth and mental
health symptoms. Standardized regression weights and multiple square correlations are presented. Solid lines
indicate path coefficients significantly different from zero and dashed paths indicate nonsignificant path
coefficients.

Hanley, Garland, & Tedeschi, 2017, p. 526). Conceptually, this Potential Mechanisms of Change
may provide an initial explanation as to why experiencing a greater
number of potentially traumatic events may be directly associated The current findings also contribute to a better understanding of
with cognitive fluency, originality, and flexibility but less so with the ways in which exposure to trauma may relate to positive
elaboration. Whereas fluency, originality, and flexibility have been psychological outcomes. Specifically, we found that emotional
shown to be interrelated (Zeng et al., 2011), elaboration is different, in creativity followed by CSE (as two sequential mediators) statisti-
that producing many original and different ideas in a given timeframe cally mediated a positive association between exposure and PTG
may limit individuals’ the ability to embellish all of these ideas with as well as a negative association between exposure and mental
details. The difference between elaboration and other divergent think- health symptoms. Surprisingly, although the direct association
ing abilities has also emerged in creativity studies examining Kirton’s between exposure to traumatic events and overall divergent think-
adaptor–innovator theory (Kirton, 1978, 1994) where innovators are ing (i.e., a composite ATTA score) was significant in the model
posited to create change by wildly shifting the existing paradigm, (see Figure 2), we did not find a similar three-path mediation in the
whereas adaptors create change by restrainedly refining and extending cognitive creativity path. This may suggest that a different mech-
it by means of introducing novelty into the paradigm in a way that is anism may exist for the emotional and the cognitive creativity
creative and generally supportive of the existing paradigm. Studies paths but further studies are needed to test this hypothesis.
generally show that fluency and originality load on the same Another significant theoretical contribution is the role of CSE as
factor of the Innovative creative style, whereas elaboration an underlying mediator through which emotional creativity, over-
loads on the Adaptive creative style (K. H. Kim, Cramond, & all divergent thinking, and elaboration relate to psychological
Bandalos, 2006; Krumm, Aranguren, Arán Filippetti, & Lemos, outcomes. Our findings are consistent with earlier studies linking
2016; Krumm, Lemos, & Filippetti, 2014). Taken together, our emotional creativity to general self-efficacy (Zareie, 2014), overall
findings lend further credence to the view that each divergent divergent thinking to CSE (Mathisen & Bronnick, 2009; Pretz &
thinking ability may have an independent meaning and thus McCollum, 2014; Puente-Díaz & Cavazos-Arroyo, 2017a), and
should be interpreted separately (K. H. Kim, 2011). elaboration to CSE (Byrge & Tang, 2015). In line with previous
10 ORKIBI AND RAM-VLASOV

considerations of CSE as a mediator (Karwowski & Barbot, 2016; variations should also be examined in terms of emotional creativity
Puente-Díaz & Cavazos-Arroyo, 2017b; Royston & Reiter- (i.e., specifically with respect to the idea of generating new or
Palmon, 2017), our findings hint that CSE may account for how unusual and appropriate emotional responses), divergent thinking
emotional and cognitive creative abilities may associate with pos- abilities, and the self-rating of creativity (Glăveanu & Tanggaard,
itive psychological outcomes of greater PTG and better health. 2014; Kwan, Leung, & Liou, 2018). Last, 73% of our sample was
This view corresponds with Bandura’s (1997) principal hypothesis female. Studies have often suggested that women exhibit more
that strong efficacious beliefs lead to positive changes in well- emotional creativity (Averill, 1999) and PTG than men (Akbar &
being, which has been largely supported across domains (see Witruk, 2016). Thus gender should be taken into consideration
reviews in Maddux, 1995; Pajares & Urdan, 2006; Schwarzer, when interpreting the results and a more gender-balanced sample
1992). should be recruited in future studies, although other studies with
The findings that only overall divergent thinking and elabora- CSE have had similar gender distributions (e.g., Royston & Reiter-
tion as a single mediator significantly related to CSE may either Palmon, 2017).
suggest that together, the four divergent thinking abilities have an Despite these limitations, these findings underscore the impor-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

aggregated impact on CSE (i.e., an aggregation hypothesis) or that tance of examining whether creativity enhancement programs can
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

elaboration is a crucial factor in this relationship (i.e., a particu- lead to changes in perceptions of benefits in the aftermath of
larization hypothesis). Similarly, significant mediations were only traumatic events through changes in the variables measured in this
found in the elaboration model, with CSE mediating the positive study. In fact, studies have reported associations between partici-
indirect association between elaboration and PTG and with CSE pation in group (Byrge & Tang, 2015) and online (Robbins &
mediating the negative indirect association between elaboration Kegley, 2010) creativity training and an increase in divergent
and mental health symptoms. Through the lens of the adaptor– thinking components and CSE that were measured before and after
innovator theory, it makes sense that out of the four divergent the program. Therefore, the next step could be to investigate
thinking abilities, a significant indirect association was only found whether and how creativity training may help regulate distressing
for the elaboration model. As noted, PTG does not involve “wildly feelings and thoughts associated with adverse life events and
shifting the existing paradigm” in terms of relentlessly displacing trauma. Another important avenue would be to investigate which
the traumatic experience; rather it involves creating a new strand events in an individual’s history are the most highly associated
of continuity that incorporates the experience into a continuous with emotional creativity, each of the divergent thinking abilities,
and coherent life narrative (Jayawickreme & Blackie, 2016; Jirek, and CSE. Going beyond exposure to adversity, studies should
2017), with a more elaborated and complex understanding of the examine the unique association between different posttraumatic
self and the world (Calhoun & Tedeschi, 2006). Whereas elabo- stress symptoms and creativity, including hypervigilance (an en-
ration refers to the ability to generate detailed ideas (Goff & hanced state of sensory sensitivity and awareness), reexperiencing,
Torrance, 2002), the PTG process also includes creative elabora- nightmares, flashbacks, and so forth. It is worth nothing that the
tion of one’s narrative, feelings, and responses to the traumatic association between adversity and creativity differs from the as-
event (Neimeyer, 2004). sociation between psychopathology and creativity (Thys et al.,
2014): whereas the former is attributed to an external stressful or
traumatic event, the latter is attributed to an internal (i.e., personal)
Limitations, Future Directions, and Implications
psychiatric vulnerability. In terms of the Four Ps framework for
This study has several limitations that delineate directions for creativity (Rhodes, 1961), this distinction corresponds to studies
future research. First, the cross-sectional design in this study does on the Press (i.e., place or environment) that fosters creativity
not allow for causal inference; future studies should collect longi- versus studies on the Person performing the creative act.
tudinal data to examine the effect of time and the possibility of Clinically, most creativity-based interventions and protocols
reverse causation. Second, whereas divergent thinking was mea- have focused on processing traumatic experiences through art-
sured with an objective time-limited performance task, CSE and making (Rowe et al., 2016; Schouten, de Niet, Knipscheer, Kleber,
emotional creativity were measured using self-reports. Future stud- & Hutschemaekers, 2015), dance and movement (Ho, 2015;
ies should include peer or spouse evaluation of emotional creativ- Levine & Land, 2016), music (Bensimon, Amir, & Wolf, 2012;
ity to enhance the robustness of the results and overcome social Story & Beck, 2017) and drama (Hudgins & Toscani, 2013;
desirability and consistency motif biases that can influence partic- Sajnani & Johnson, 2014), whereas fewer have focused on foster-
ipants’ responses (N. K. Park, Chun, & Lee, 2016). Third, specific ing PTG (Garland, Carlson, Cook, Lansdell, & Speca, 2007; Kern
correlates of the five PTG dimensions were not examined because & Perryman, 2016). Creative approaches that aim to facilitate the
we used the Posttraumatic Growth Inventory Short Form to min- processing of a traumatic experience posit that creative, symbolic
imize the burden on participants. A more nuanced picture of the or direct self-expression of a passively experienced trauma pro-
links between creativity and PTG dimensions should be explored vides an opportunity to actively give aesthetic form, shape, make
in future studies. Fourth, although the association between PTG sense, and ultimately gain a sense of mastery over overwhelming
and mental health symptoms was significantly negative in this emotions, thoughts and bodily sensations (Rose, 1987). The exter-
study, future studies should examine the conditions in which nalization of an inner trauma enables it to be witnessed by the self
perceived PTG can be translated into growth-related actions and and others and thus be integrated into a coherent meaningful
actual positive functioning (Hobfoll et al., 2007). Fifth, PTG narrative (Richman, 2013). To date, clinical interventions that
dimensions should be examined for cultural variations because specifically aim to facilitate the process of PTG mostly follow an
values underlying the idea of human growth are likely to vary existential-narrative-cognitive framework employing creative nar-
across cultures (Vázquez, Pérez-Sales, & Ochoa, 2014). Cultural rative methods (Calhoun & Tedeschi, 2013; Tedeschi, Calhoun, &
TRAUMA, CREATIVITY, AND PTG 11

Groleau, 2015; Tedeschi & Moore, 2016). Along these lines, a Sciences, 217, 1036 –1043. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2016.02
related theoretical and clinical direction would be to examine .102
creativity variables as moderators that may buffer the trauma- Akinola, M., & Mendes, W. B. (2008). The dark side of creativity:
symptoms association or strengthen the trauma-PTG association. Biological vulnerability and negative emotions lead to greater artistic
creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1677–1686.
Conclusion http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167208323933
Aldwin, C. M., & Sutton, K. J. (1998). A developmental perspective on
Positive self-belief in one’s creative abilities may thus play a posttraumatic growth. In R. G. Tedeschi, C. L. Park, & L. G. Calhoun
significant role in the posttrauma growth process in that it reflects (Eds.), Posttraumatic growth: Positive changes in the aftermath of crisis
the perseverance and confidence that are essential to the develop- (pp. 43– 64). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
ment of PTG. As trauma often involves the disruption of funda- Althuizen, N., Wierenga, B., & Rossiter, J. (2010). The validity of two
mental beliefs about the world, internal resources may be more brief measures of creative ability. Creativity Research Journal, 22,
fundamental to PTG than external resources (Hanley et al., 2017). 53– 61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400410903579577
One central resource is the ability to make growth interpretations American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical man-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

that include a positive reappraisal of adverse events as beneficial ual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and/or meaningful. Evidence suggests that the ability to generate Amir, M., & Sol, O. (1999). Psychological impact and prevalence of
traumatic events in a student sample in Israel: The effect of multiple
novel and appropriate interpretations of negative pictures (i.e.,
traumatic events and physical injury. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 12,
“creative reappraisals”) promotes a more adaptive reaction to those
139 –154. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1024754618063
pictures in terms of reducing the unpleasant feelings that arose
Augustine, L. F., Vazir, S., Rao, S. F., Rao, M. V., Laxmaiah, A., & Nair,
from looking at them (Wu, Guo, Tang, Shi, & Luo, 2017). Simi-
K. M. (2011). Perceived stress, life events & coping among higher
larly, Runco (2006) argued that “the key mechanism in personal secondary students of Hyderabad, India: A pilot study. The Indian
creativity is the capacity to produce an original interpretation of Journal of Medical Research, 134, 61– 68.
experience” (p. 101). A perceived need for adaptation depends Averill, J. R. (1999). Individual differences in emotional creativity: Struc-
more on subjective personal interpretations of experiences than on ture and correlates. Journal of Personality, 67, 331–371. http://dx.doi
objective experiences (Runco, 1999). This idea is consistent with .org/10.1111/1467-6494.00058
evidence that subjective measures of perceived stress are more Averill, J. R. (2000). Intelligence, emotion, and creativity: From trichot-
often associated with health outcomes than objective measures of omy to trinity. In R. Bar-On & D. A. Parker (Eds.), Handbook of
stressful events (Augustine et al., 2011). Individuals who experi- emotional intelligence (pp. 277–298). San-Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
ence the same event can be affected differently so that while one Averill, J. R. (2009). Emotional creativity: Toward “spiritualizing the
person may see a problem, or interpret a situation as stressful, passions”. In S. J. Lopez & C. R. Snyder (Eds.), Oxford handbook of
another may not. Our findings suggest that in people exposed to positive psychology (2nd ed., pp. 249 –258). Oxford, United Kingdom:
potentially traumatic events, a potential key mechanism linking Oxford University Press.
both emotional and cognitive creativity to positive psychological Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). A meta-analysis of
outcomes (PTG and mental health) is having a positive subjective 25 years of mood-creativity research: Hedonic tone, activation, or reg-
belief in one’s own creative abilities. In short, CSE may reflect ulatory focus? Psychological Bulletin, 134, 779 – 806. http://dx.doi.org/
belief in one’s ability to generate novel and adaptive emotional and 10.1037/a0012815
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, NY:
cognitive responses to an adverse situation. However, this hypoth-
Freeman.
esis warrants further investigation with a CSE measure that is
Beghetto, R. A., & Karwowski, M. (2017). Toward untangling creative
specific to the PTG domain, unlike the general CSE measure used
self-beliefs. In M. Karwowski & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), Effect of beliefs,
in this study. self-efficacy, mindset, and identity (pp. 3–22). London, United King-
Overall, it is surprising that the literature has not investigated dom: Academic Press.
constructs that are well researched by creativity researchers (i.e., Beghetto, R. A., Kaufman, J. C., & Baxter, J. (2011). Answering the
CSE, divergent thinking, emotional creativity) as change process unexpected questions: Exploring the relationship between students’ cre-
variables that may point to possible mechanisms of change in ative self-efficacy and teacher ratings of creativity. Psychology of Aes-
clients during creativity-based interventions to increase PTG and thetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 5, 342–349. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
mental health. Future research could further advance the interdis- a0022834
ciplinary connections between trauma and creativity studies by Benjet, C., Bromet, E., Karam, E. G., Kessler, R. C., McLaughlin, K. A.,
examining personal (e.g., openness to experience and resilience) Ruscio, A. M., . . . Koenen, K. C. (2016). The epidemiology of traumatic
and environmental (e.g., availability of care and resources) factors event exposure worldwide: Results from the World Mental Health
that may reveal individual differences in the extent to which some Survey Consortium. Psychological Medicine, 46, 327–343. http://dx.doi
people grow from adverse experiences and others do not. We hope .org/10.1017/S0033291715001981
that the current findings will stimulate psychology, creativity and Bensimon, M., Amir, D., & Wolf, Y. (2012). A pendulum between trauma
creative arts therapies researchers to share knowledge and exper- and life: Group music therapy with post-traumatized soldiers. The Arts
tise that may help better determine how creativity and the arts can in Psychotherapy, 39, 223–233. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.aip.2012.03
benefit people coping with trauma. .005
Bleich, A., Gelkopf, M., & Solomon, Z. (2003). Exposure to terrorism,
stress-related mental health symptoms, and coping behaviors among a
References
nationally representative sample in Israel. Journal of the American
Akbar, Z., & Witruk, E. (2016). Coping mediates the relationship between Medical Association, 290, 612– 620. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.290
gender and posttraumatic growth. Procedia: Social and Behavioral .5.612
12 ORKIBI AND RAM-VLASOV

Byrge, C., & Tang, C. (2015). Embodied creativity training: Effects on Davis, M. A. (2009). Understanding the relationship between mood and
creative self-efficacy and creative production. Thinking Skills and Cre- creativity: A meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Deci-
ativity, 16, 51– 61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2015.01.002 sion Processes, 108, 25–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2008.04
Byrne, B. M. (2004). Testing for multigroup invariance using AMOS .001
Graphics: A road less traveled. Structural Equation Modeling, 11, 272– Farmer, S. M., & Tierney, P. (2017). Considering creative self-efficacy: Its
300. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15328007sem1102_8 current state and ideas for future inquiry. In M. Karwowski & J. C.
Byrne, B. M. (2016). Structural equation modeling with Amos: Basic Kaufman (Eds.), The creative self: Effect of beliefs, self-efficacy, mind-
concepts, applications, and programming (3rd ed.). New York, NY: set, and identity (pp. 23– 47). London, United Kingdom: Academic
Routledge. Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-809790-8.00002-9
Calhoun, L. G., & Tedeschi, R. G. (2006). Handbook of posttraumatic Forgeard, M. J. C. (2013). Perceiving benefits after adversity: The rela-
growth: Research & practice. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Publishers. tionship between self-reported posttraumatic growth and creativity. Psy-
Calhoun, L. G., & Tedeschi, R. G. (2013). Posttraumatic growth in clinical chology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7, 245–264. http://dx.doi
practice. New York, NY: Routledge. .org/10.1037/a0031223
Cann, A., Calhoun, L. G., Tedeschi, R. G., Triplett, K. N., Vishnevsky, T., Forgeard, M. J. C., & Benson, L. (2017). Extracurricular involvement and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

& Lindstrom, C. M. (2011). Assessing posttraumatic cognitive pro- psychological adjustment in the transition from adolescence to emerging
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

cesses: The Event Related Rumination Inventory. Anxiety, Stress, and adulthood: The role of mastery and creative self-efficacy. Applied De-
Coping, 24, 137–156. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2010.529901 velopmental Science. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10
Carson, D. K., Bittner, M. T., Cameron, B. R., Brown, D. M., & Meyer, .1080/10888691.2017.1288124
S. S. (1994). Creative thinking as a predictor of school-aged children’s Forgeard, M. J. C., & Elstein, J. G. (2014). Advancing the clinical science
stress responses and coping abilities. Creativity Research Journal, 7, of creativity. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 613.
145–158. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400419409534520 Fu, F., & Chow, A. (2017). Traumatic exposure and psychological well-
Choi, J. N. (2004). Individual and contextual predictors of creative perfor- being: The moderating role of cognitive flexibility. Journal of Loss and
mance: The mediating role of psychological processes. Creativity Re- Trauma, 22, 24 –35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15325024.2016.1161428
search Journal, 16, 187–199. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2004 Fuchs, G. L., Kumar, V. K., & Porter, J. (2007). Emotional creativity,
alexithymia, and styles of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 19,
.9651452
233–245. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400410701397313
Chopko, B. A., Palmieri, P. A., & Adams, R. E. (2018). Relationships
Garland, S. N., Carlson, L. E., Cook, S., Lansdell, L., & Speca, M. (2007).
among traumatic experiences, PTSD, and posttraumatic growth for
A non-randomized comparison of mindfulness-based stress reduction
police officers: A path analysis. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Re-
and healing arts programs for facilitating post-traumatic growth and
search, Practice, and Policy, 10, 183–189. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
spirituality in cancer outpatients. Supportive Care in Cancer, 15, 949 –
tra0000261
961. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00520-007-0280-5
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences
Glăveanu, V. P., & Tanggaard, L. (2014). Creativity, identity, and repre-
(2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates.
sentation: Towards a socio-cultural theory of creative identity. New
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159.
Ideas in Psychology, 34, 12–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.newidea-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155
psych.2014.02.002
Cohen, L. M. (2012). Adaptation and creativity in cultural context. Revista
Goertzel, M. G., Goertzel, V., & Goertzel, T. G. (1978). Three hundred
de Psicología, 30, 3–18.
eminent personalities. London, United Kingdom: Jossey-Bass.
Conner, T. S., DeYoung, C. G., & Silvia, P. J. (2018). Everyday creative
Goff, K., & Torrance, E. P. (2002). Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults:
activity as a path to flourishing. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 13,
Manual. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service.
181–189. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2016.1257049 Goldberg, D., & Williams, P. (1988). A user’s guide to the General Health
Conner, T. S., & Silvia, P. J. (2015). Creative days: A daily diary study of Questionnaire. Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson.
emotion, personality, and everyday creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Goldstein, R. B., Smith, S. M., Chou, S. P., Saha, T. D., Jung, J., Zhang,
Creativity, and the Arts, 9, 463– 470. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/aca H., . . . Grant, B. F. (2016). The epidemiology of DSM–5 posttraumatic
0000022 stress disorder in the United States: Results from the National Epidemi-
Crawford, E. F., Lang, A. J., & Laffaye, C. (2008). An evaluation of the ologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions-III. Social Psychiatry
psychometric properties of the traumatic events questionnaire in primary and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 51, 1137–1148. http://dx.doi.org/10
care patients. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 21, 109 –112. http://dx.doi .1007/s00127-016-1208-5
.org/10.1002/jts.20280 Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York, NY:
Cropley, A. J. (1990). Creativity and mental health in everyday life. McGraw-Hill.
Creativity Research Journal, 3, 167–178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ Hanley, A. W., Garland, E. L., & Tedeschi, R. G. (2017). Relating
10400419009534351 dispositional mindfulness, contemplative practice, and positive reap-
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity: Flow and the psychology of praisal with posttraumatic cognitive coping, stress, and growth. Psycho-
discovery and invention. New York, NY: HarperCollins. logical Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 9, 526 –536.
Damian, R. I. (2017). Where do diversifying experiences fit in the study of http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/tra0000208
personality, creativity, and career success? In G. J. Feist, J. C. Kaufman, Ho, R. T. H. (2015). A place and space to survive: A dance/movement
& R. Reiter-Palmon (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity and therapy program for childhood sexual abuse survivors. The Arts in
personality research (pp. 102–123). Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cam- Psychotherapy, 46, 9 –16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.aip.2015.09.004
bridge University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/9781316228036.007 Hobfoll, S. E., Canetti-Nisim, D., & Johnson, R. J. (2006). Exposure to
Damian, R. I., & Simonton, D. K. (2015). Psychopathology, adversity, and terrorism, stress-related mental health symptoms, and defensive coping
creativity: Diversifying experiences in the development of eminent among Jews and Arabs in Israel. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108, Psychology, 74, 207–218. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.74.2
623– 636. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000011 .207
Daphna-Tekoah, S. (2017). Hebrew version of the Post Traumatic Growth Hobfoll, S. E., Canetti-Nisim, D., Johnson, R. J., Palmieri, P. A., Varley,
Inventory Short Form. Manuscript in preparation. J. D., & Galea, S. (2008). The association of exposure, risk, and
TRAUMA, CREATIVITY, AND PTG 13

resiliency factors with PTSD among Jews and Arabs exposed to repeated Kim, S. H., Vincent, L. C., & Goncalo, J. A. (2013). Outside advantage:
acts of terrorism in Israel. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 21, 9 –21. Can social rejection fuel creative thought? Journal of Experimental
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jts.20307 Psychology: General, 142, 605– 611. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a00
Hobfoll, S. E., Hall, B. J., Canetti-Nisim, D., Galea, S., Johnson, R. J., & 29728
Palmieri, P. A. (2007). Refining our understanding of traumatic growth Kirton, M. (1978). Have adaptors and innovators equal levels of creativity?
in the face of terrorism: Moving from meaning cognitions to doing what Psychological Reports, 42, 695– 698. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1978
is meaningful. Applied Psychology, 56, 345–366. http://dx.doi.org/10 .42.3.695
.1111/j.1464-0597.2007.00292.x Kirton, M. (1994). Adaptors and innovators: Styles of creativity and
Hudgins, K., & Toscani, F. (2013). Healing world trauma with the ther- problem solving (Rev. ed.). New York, NY: Routledge.
apeutic spiral model: Psychodramatic stories from the frontlines. Lon- Krumm, G., Aranguren, M., Arán Filippetti, V., & Lemos, V. (2016).
don, United Kingdom: Jessica Kingsley. Factor structure of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking Verbal Form
Ivcevic, Z. (2007). Artistic and everyday creativity: An act-frequency B in a Spanish-speaking population. The Journal of Creative Behavior,
approach. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 41, 271–290. http://dx.doi 50, 150 –164. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jocb.76
.org/10.1002/j.2162-6057.2007.tb01074.x Krumm, G., Lemos, V., & Filippetti, V. A. (2014). Factor structure of the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Jayawickreme, E., & Blackie, L. E. R. (2014). Post-traumatic growth as Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking Figural Form B in Spanish-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

positive personality change: Evidence, controversies and future direc- speaking children: Measurement invariance across gender. Creativity
tions. European Journal of Personality, 28, 312–331. http://dx.doi.org/ Research Journal, 26, 72– 81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2013
10.1002/per.1963 .843908
Jayawickreme, E., & Blackie, L. E. R. (2016). Contemporary understand- Kwan, L. Y.-Y., Leung, A. K.-Y., & Liou, S. (2018). Culture, creativity,
ings of posttraumatic growth. In E. Jayawickreme & L. E. R. Blackie and innovation. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 49, 165–170.
(Eds.), Exploring the psychological benefits of hardship: A critical http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022117753306
reassessment of posttraumatic growth (pp. 7–17). Cham, Switzerland: Lahav, Y., Solomon, Z., & Levin, Y. (2016). Posttraumatic growth and
Springer. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47989-7_2 perceived health: The role of posttraumatic stress symptoms. American
Jirek, S. L. (2017). Narrative reconstruction and post-traumatic growth Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 86, 693–703. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
among trauma survivors: The importance of narrative in social work
ort0000155
research and practice. Qualitative Social Work: Research and Practice,
Levine, B., & Land, H. M. (2016). A meta-synthesis of qualitative findings
16, 166 –188. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1473325016656046
about dance/movement therapy for individuals with trauma. Qualitative
Karwowski, M. (2014). Creative mindsets: Measurement, correlates, con-
Health Research, 26, 330 –344. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/104
sequences. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8, 62–70.
9732315589920
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0034898
Lev-Wiesel, R., Daphna-Tekoah, S., & Hallak, M. (2009). Childhood
Karwowski, M. (2016). The dynamics of creative self-concept: Changes
sexual abuse as a predictor of birth-related posttraumatic stress and
and reciprocal relations between creative self-efficacy and creative per-
postpartum posttraumatic stress. Child Abuse & Neglect: The Interna-
sonal identity. Creativity Research Journal, 28, 99 –104. http://dx.doi
tional Journal, 33, 877– 887. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2009.05
.org/10.1080/10400419.2016.1125254
.004
Karwowski, M., & Barbot, B. (2016). Self-beliefs and creativity in devel-
Maddux, J. E. (1995). Self-efficacy, adaptation, and adjustment: Theory,
opmental context. In J. C. Kaufman & J. Baer (Eds.), Cambridge
research, and application. New York, NY: Plenum Press. http://dx.doi
companion to creativity in reason and development (pp. 302–326).
.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-6868-5
Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press. http://dx.doi
Maslow, A. H. (1962). Creativity in self-actualizing people. In A. H.
.org/10.1017/CBO9781139941969.016
Karwowski, M., Gralewski, J., & Szumski, G. (2015). Teachers’ effect on Maslow (Ed.), Toward a psychology of being (pp. 127–137). Princeton,
students’ creative self-beliefs is moderated by students’ gender. Learn- NJ: D. Van Nostrand. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10793-010
ing and Individual Differences, 44, 1– 8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j Mathisen, G. E., & Bronnick, K. S. (2009). Creative self-efficacy: An
.lindif.2015.10.001 intervention study. International Journal of Educational Research, 48,
Kaufman, J. C., Plucker, J. A., & Baer, J. (2008). Essentials of creativity 21–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2009.02.009
assessment. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. McKibben, W. B., & Silvia, P. J. (2017). Evaluating the distorting effects
Keith, J., Velezmoro, R., & O’Brien, C. (2015). Correlates of cognitive of inattentive responding and social desirability on self-report scales in
flexibility in veterans seeking treatment for posttraumatic stress disorder. creativity and the arts. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 51, 57– 69.
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 203, 287–293. http://dx.doi http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jocb.86
.org/10.1097/NMD.0000000000000280 Metzl, E. S. (2009). The role of creative thinking in resilience after
Kern, E., & Perryman, K. (2016). Leaving it in the sand: Creatively hurricane Katrina. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3,
processing military combat trauma as a means for reducing risk of 112–123. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0013479
interpersonal violence. Journal of Creativity in Mental Health, 11, Neimeyer, R. A. (2001). Meaning reconstruction and the experience of
446 – 457. loss. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. http://dx
Kim, K. H. (2008). Meta-analyses of the relationship of creative achieve- .doi.org/10.1037/10397-000
ment to both IQ and divergent thinking test scores. The Journal of Neimeyer, R. A. (2004). Fostering posttraumatic growth: A narrative
Creative Behavior, 42, 106 –130. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/j.2162-6057 elaboration. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 53–59.
.2008.tb01290.x Neria, Y., Besser, A., Kiper, D., & Westphal, M. (2010). A longitudinal
Kim, K. H. (2011). The creativity crisis: The decrease in creative thinking study of posttraumatic stress disorder, depression, and generalized anx-
scores on the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. Creativity Research iety disorder in Israeli civilians exposed to war trauma. Journal of
Journal, 23, 285–295. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2011.627805 Traumatic Stress, 23, 322–330.
Kim, K. H., Cramond, B., & Bandalos, D. L. (2006). The latent structure Oriol, X., Amutio, A., Mendoza, M., Da Costa, S., & Miranda, R. (2016).
and measurement invariance of scores on the Torrance tests of creative Emotional creativity as predictor of intrinsic motivation and academic
thinking–figural. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 66, engagement in university students: The mediating role of positive emo-
459 – 477. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0013164405282456 tions. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1243.
14 ORKIBI AND RAM-VLASOV

Pajares, F., & Urdan, T. C. (2006). Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents. nal of Creative Behavior. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/
Greenwich, CT: IAP. 10.1002/jocb.226
Park, C. L. (2004). The notion of growth following stressful life experi- Runco, M. A. (1999). Tension, adaptability, and creativity. In S. W. Russ
ences: Problems and prospects. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 69 –76. (Ed.), Affect, creative experience, and psychological adjustment (pp.
Park, N. K., Chun, M. Y., & Lee, J. (2016). Revisiting individual creativity 165–194). Philadelphia, PA: Taylor & Francis.
assessment: Triangulation in subjective and objective assessment meth- Runco, M. A. (2006). Reasoning and personal creativity. In J. C. Kaufman
ods. Creativity Research Journal, 28, 1–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ & J. Baer (Eds.), Creativity and reason in cognitive development (pp.
10400419.2016.1125259 99 –116). Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.
Peterson, C., Park, N., Pole, N., D’Andrea, W., & Seligman, M. E. P. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511606915.007
(2008). Strengths of character and posttraumatic growth. Journal of Runco, M. A. (2007). Creativity- Theories and themes: Research, devel-
Traumatic Stress, 21, 214 –217. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jts.20332 opment, and practice. Boston, MA: Elsevier Academic Press.
Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Character strengths and Runco, M. A., & Acar, S. (2012). Divergent thinking as an indicator of
virtues: A handbook and classification. Washington, DC: American creative potential. Creativity Research Journal, 24, 66 –75. http://dx.doi
Psychological Association. .org/10.1080/10400419.2012.652929
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Piirto, J. (1998). Understanding those who create (2nd ed.). Scottsdale, Runco, M. A., Millar, G., Acar, S., & Cramond, B. (2010). Torrance Tests
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

AZ: Gifted Psychology Press. of Creative Thinking as predictors of personal and public achievement:
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for A fifty-year follow-up. Creativity Research Journal, 22, 361–368. http://
estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Re- dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2010.523393
search Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 717–731. http://dx.doi Sajnani, N., & Johnson, D. R. (2014). Trauma-informed drama therapy:
.org/10.3758/BF03206553 Transforming clinics, classrooms, and communities. Springfield, IL:
Pretz, J. E., & McCollum, V. A. (2014). Self-perceptions of creativity do Charles C. Thomas.
not always reflect actual creative performance. Psychology of Aesthetics, Schouten, K. A., de Niet, G. J., Knipscheer, J. W., Kleber, R. J., &
Creativity, and the Arts, 8, 227–236. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0035597 Hutschemaekers, G. J. M. (2015). The effectiveness of art therapy in the
Puente-Diaz, R., & Arroyo Judith, C. (2016). An exploration of some treatment of traumatized adults: A systematic review on art therapy and
antecedents and consequences of creative self-efficacy: The role of trauma. Trauma, Violence & Abuse, 16, 220 –228. http://dx.doi.org/10
achievement goals, enjoyment and divergent thinking. Creativity: The- .1177/1524838014555032
ories, Research, Applications, 3, 19 –33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/ctra- Schreiber, J. B., Nora, A., Stage, F. K., Barlow, E. A., & King, J. (2006).
2016-0002 Reporting structural equation modeling and confirmatory factor analysis
Puente-Díaz, R., & Cavazos-Arroyo, J. (2017a). Creative self-efficacy: The results: A review. The Journal of Educational Research, 99, 323–338.
influence of affective states and social persuasion as antecedents and http://dx.doi.org/10.3200/JOER.99.6.323-338
imagination and divergent thinking as consequences. Creativity Re- Schwarzer, R. (1992). Self-efficacy: Thought control of action. Washing-
search Journal, 29, 304 –312. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2017 ton, DC: Hemisphere.
.1360067 Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in
Puente-Díaz, R., & Cavazos-Arroyo, J. (2017b). Creative self-efficacy: assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 420 – 428. http://
The role of self-regulation for schoolwork and boredom as antecedents, dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.86.2.420
and divergent thinking as a consequence. Social Psychology of Educa- Silvia, P. J., Beaty, R. E., Nusbaum, E. C., Eddington, K. M., Levin-
tion, 20, 347–359. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11218-017-9376-z Aspenson, H., & Kwapil, T. R. (2014). Everyday creativity in daily life:
Qiang, R., Han, Q., Guo, Y., Bai, J., & Karwowski, M. (2018). Critical An experience-sampling study of “little c” creativity. Psychology of
thinking disposition and scientific creativity: The mediating role of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8, 183–188. http://dx.doi.org/10
creative self-efficacy. The Journal of Creative Behavior. Advance online .1037/a0035722
publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jocb.347 Silvia, P. J., Wigert, B., Reiter-Palmon, R., & Kaufman, J. C. (2012).
Reiter-Palmon, R., Robinson-Morral, E. J., Kaufman, J. C., & Santo, J. B. Assessing creativity with self-report scales: A review and empirical
(2012). Evaluation of self-perceptions of creativity: Is it a useful crite- evaluation. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6, 19 –34.
rion? Creativity Research Journal, 24, 107–114. http://dx.doi.org/10 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0024071
.1080/10400419.2012.676980 Simonton, D. K. (2010). So you want to become a creative genius? You
Rhodes, M. (1961). An analysis of creativity. Phi Delta Kappan, 42, must be crazy. In D. H. Cropley, A. J. Cropley, J. C. Kaufman, & M. A.
305–310. Runco (Eds.), The dark side of creativity (pp. 218 –234). New York, NY:
Richman, S. (2013). Out of darkness: Reverberations of trauma and its Cambridge University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511
creative transformations. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 23, 362–376. http:// 761225.012
dx.doi.org/10.1080/10481885.2013.794647 Sledjeski, E. M., Speisman, B., & Dierker, L. C. (2008). Does number of
Robbins, T. L., & Kegley, K. (2010). Playing with Thinkertoys to build lifetime traumas explain the relationship between PTSD and chronic
creative abilities through online instruction. Thinking Skills and Creativ- medical conditions? Answers from the National Comorbidity Survey-
ity, 5, 40 – 48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2009.07.001 Replication (NCS-R). Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 31, 341–349.
Rogers, C. R. (1961). On becoming a person: A therapist’s view of http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10865-008-9158-3
psychotherapy. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin. Stolovy, T., Lev-Wiesel, R., & Witztum, E. (2015). Dissociation: Adjust-
Rose, G. J. (1987). Trauma and mastery in life and art. New Haven, CT: ment or distress? Dissociative phenomena, absorption and quality of life
Yale University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/j.ctt211qwjn among Israeli women who practice channeling compared to women with
Rowe, C., Watson-Ormond, R., English, L., Rubesin, H., Marshall, A., similar traumatic history. Journal of Religion and Health, 54, 1040 –
Linton, K., . . . Eng, E. (2016). Evaluating art therapy to heal the effects 1051. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10943-014-9885-4
of trauma among refugee youth: The burma art therapy program eval- Story, K. M., & Beck, B. D. (2017). Guided Imagery and Music with
uation. Health Promotion Practice, 18, 26 –33. http://dx.doi.org/10 female military veterans: An intervention development study. The Arts
.1177/1524839915626413 in Psychotherapy, 55, 93–102. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.aip.2017.05
Royston, R., & Reiter-Palmon, R. (2017). Creative self-efficacy as medi- .003
ator between creative mindsets and creative problem-solving. The Jour- Sundararajan, L., & Averill, J. R. (2007). Creativity in the everyday:
TRAUMA, CREATIVITY, AND PTG 15

Culture, self, and emotions. In R. Richards (Ed.), Everyday creativity Trnka, R., Zahradnik, M., & Kuška, M. (2016). Emotional creativity and
and new views of human nature (pp. 195–220). Washington: American real-life involvement in different types of creative leisure activities.
Psychological Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/11595-013 Creativity Research Journal, 28, 348 –356. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
Taylor, A. B., MacKinnon, D. P., & Tein, J.-Y. (2008). Tests of the 10400419.2016.1195653
three-path mediated effect. Organizational Research Methods, 11, 241– Vázquez, C., Pérez-Sales, P., & Ochoa, C. (2014). Posttraumatic growth:
269. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1094428107300344 Challenges from a cross-cultural viewpoint. In G. A. Fava & C. Ruini
Tedeschi, R. G., & Calhoun, L. G. (1996). The Posttraumatic Growth (Eds.), Increasing psychological well-being in clinical and educational
Inventory: Measuring the positive legacy of trauma. Journal of Trau- settings: Interventions and cultural contexts (pp. 57–74). Dordrecht, The
matic Stress, 9, 455– 471. Netherlands: Springer. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-8669-0_4
Tedeschi, R. G., & Calhoun, L. G. (2004). Posttraumatic growth: Concep- Vrana, S., & Lauterbach, D. (1994). Prevalence of traumatic events and
tual foundations and empirical evidence. Psychological Inquiry, 15, post-traumatic psychological symptoms in a nonclinical sample of col-
1–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli1501_01 lege students. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 7, 289 –302. http://dx.doi
Tedeschi, R. G., Calhoun, L. G., & Groleau, J. M. (2015). Clinical
.org/10.1002/jts.2490070209
applications of posttraumatic growth. In S. Joseph (Ed.), Positive psy-
Wu, X., Guo, T., Tang, T., Shi, B., & Luo, J. (2017). Role of Creativity in
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

chology in practice: Promoting human flourishing in work, health,


the Effectiveness of Cognitive Reappraisal. Frontiers in Psychology, 8,
education, and everyday life (2nd ed., pp. 503–518). Hoboken, NJ:
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

1598. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01598
Wiley. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9781118996874.ch30
Zareie, H. A. (2014). Structural modeling of the relationship between
Tedeschi, R. G., & Moore, B. A. (2016). The posttraumatic growth
emotional creativity, self-efficacy and academic motivation among stu-
workbook: Coming through trauma wiser, stronger, and more resilient.
Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publications. dents. Bulletin of Environment. Pharmacology and Life Sciences, 3,
Thomson, P., & Jaque, S. V. (2018). Childhood adversity and the creative 27–30.
experience in adult professional performing artists. Frontiers in Psychol- Zemishlany, Z. (2012). Resilience and vulnerability in coping with stress
ogy, 9, 111. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00111 and terrorism. The Israel Medical Association Journal, 14, 307–309.
Thys, E., Sabbe, B., & De Hert, M. (2014). Creativity and psychopathol- Zeng, L., Proctor, R. W., & Salvendy, G. (2011). Can traditional divergent
ogy: A systematic review. Psychopathology, 47, 141–147. http://dx.doi thinking tests be trusted in measuring and predicting real-world creativ-
.org/10.1159/000357822 ity? Creativity Research Journal, 23, 24 –37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
Tierney, P., & Farmer, S. M. (2002). Creative self-efficacy: Its potential 10400419.2011.545713
antecedents and relationship to creative performance. Academy of Man-
agement Journal, 45, 1137–1148.
Torrance, E. P. (1998). The Torrance tests of creative thinking norms- Received January 9, 2018
technical manual figural (streamlined) forms A and B. Bensenville, IL: Revision received May 20, 2018
Scholastic Testing Service, Inc. Accepted May 24, 2018 䡲

You might also like