Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

ARTICLE IN PRESS

Tourism Management 29 (2008) 146–150


www.elsevier.com/locate/tourman

Virtual experience vs. brochures in the advertisement of scenic spots:


How cognitive preferences and order effects influence advertising effects
on consumers
Wen-Bin Chioua,, Chin-Sheng Wanb, Hsin-Yi Leec
a
Center for Teacher Education Program, National Sun Yat-Sen University, 70, Lien-Hai Road, Kaohsiung 804, Taiwan, ROC
b
General Education Center, National Kaohsiung Hospitality College, 1, Sung-Ho Rd., Kaohsiung 812, Taiwan, ROC
c
Graduate School of Business Administration, National Chiayi University, Chiayi 580, Taiwan, ROC
Received 4 August 2006; received in revised form 8 March 2007; accepted 20 March 2007

Abstract

Virtual experience has begun to play a significant role in the marketing and promotion for the tourism industry. This article
demonstrates that the advertising effects of traditional brochures vs. virtual experience would be contingent on consumers’ cognitive
preferences. The traditional use of brochures in advertising would be more effective for verbalizers, whereas the virtual experience mode
would be more effective for visualizers. Under a hybrid of the two advertising modes, a recency effect was found indicating that the
subsequent or more recently experienced advertising mode would generate a greater impact. Moreover, the recency effect of traditional
brochures was more apparent for verbalizers, whereas the recency effect of virtual experience was more pronounced for visualizers. The
findings provide insights into the contingent use of traditional brochures, the use of virtual experience, and the use of hybrid advertising.
r 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Advertising effect; Cognitive preference; Order effects; Virtual experience

1. Introduction market, people have used multiple channels to gain travel


information. People may have received tour information
Traditionally, advertising has been defined as a form of from relatives, friends, brochures, or travel agents in the
controlled communication that attempts to persuade past. However, through the progress of media technology
consumers, using strategies and appeals, to buy or use a in recent years, most consumers can now view the photos
particular product or service (Defleur & Dennis, 1996). It is and read information about scenic spots through the
becoming abundantly clear that the media environment Internet in order to experience the sights in advance (Klein,
into which advertising is placed is changing, and because of 1998). Traditionally, most tourism-oriented industries
this trend, the nature of advertising is changing as well. used brochures to promote travel-related products (Hollo-
Many new channels of mass communication were devel- way & Plant, 1992; Yamamoto & Gill, 1999), but
oped during the latter part of the 20th century that exposed brochures can only supply short and limited introductions.
the public to an ever-increasing number of mediated Direct product experiences have consistently been shown to
messages (Fitzgerald, 1999; Lombard et al., 1997). lead to stronger beliefs and attitudes than advertising
Tourism and leisure have become prevalent activities in (Marks & Kamins, 1988; Smith & Swinyard, 1988). More
modern life. With the rapid expansion of the tourism and more industries (e.g., hotels, exhibitions and travel
destinations) now provide virtual experience, such as
Corresponding author. Tel.: +886 7 5252000 5884; panoramic views, animation, and interactive photos, so
fax: +886 7 5255892.
that consumers can get a direct experience without actually
E-mail addresses: wbchiou@mail.nsysu.edu.tw (W.-B. Chiou), being there. Clearly, virtual experience does more for the
won@mail.nkhc.edu.tw (C.-S. Wan), jennyhylee@gmail.com (H.-Y. Lee). tourism industry than does just print information; virtual

0261-5177/$ - see front matter r 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.tourman.2007.03.016
ARTICLE IN PRESS
W.-B. Chiou et al. / Tourism Management 29 (2008) 146–150 147

experience actually gives consumers the chance to get a feel belief updating: primacy and recency (Haugtvedt &
of their travel destinations on the Internet. Therefore, the Wegener, 1994; Hogarth & Einhorn, 1992). Generally, an
preferred medium for advertising scenic spots has gradually order effect exists when an outcome, based on a fixed set of
turned from the traditional brochure to virtual experience phenomena, varies with the order in which the phenomena
(Fang & Lie, 2006). occur. Primacy is obtained when the relative weight
accorded to the first piece of the evidences is the greatest.
2. Review of literature and hypotheses Recency is obtained when the relative weight accorded to
the last piece of the evidences is the greatest.
2.1. Cognitive preference as a moderator Previous researchers (Haugtvedt & Wegener, 1994;
Hogarth & Einhorn, 1992) have stated that the presenta-
Previous studies on virtual experience have focused tion order of advertising information would affect con-
mostly on how virtual experience affects consumers (Li, sumers’ information processing. The primacy effect refers
Daugherty, & Biocca, 2001). However, consumer research- to previous information exhibiting greater effect than
ers have been increasingly interested in how cognitive subsequent information, whereas the recency effect refers
personality factors influence various aspects of consumer to subsequent information generating greater effect than
behavior. One cognitive personality factor in particular, earlier information (Curtis & Duane, 1994). Therefore,
cognitive preference (visualizers vs. verbalizers), has been when marketers in the tourism industry advertise products
useful in understanding selected aspects of consumer by adopting both virtual experience and brochures, they
behavior (Paivio, 1991). Cognitive preference refers to an should consider message order effects. Moreover, the order
individual’s style of or inclination in information proces- effect would be contingent on consumers’ cognitive
sing, which is closely related to personal preference but not preferences, under the hypothesis that depicts the interplay
to intellectuality (Riding & Douglas, 1993). In principle, of advertising mode and cognitive preference.
cognitive personality research classifies consumers into two The first study generally examined whether the differ-
groups: visualizers (consumers who prefer visual informa- ential advertising effects between virtual experience and
tion and products that stress the visual, such as member- traditional brochures would be contingent on consumers’
ship in a videotape club) or verbalizers (consumers who cognitive preferences. The second study, aimed to deter-
prefer written or verbal information and products, such as mine whether the cognitive preferences of consumers would
membership in book clubs or audiotape clubs). influence the order effects when virtual experience and
To compare the effects of visualizing and verbalizing traditional brochures were used consecutively in advertis-
styles on memory, previous studies have supported the idea ing scenic spots.
that people differ in their predisposition to remember
details from verbal or visual information (Riding & 3. Experiment 1
Ashmore, 1980; Riding, Burton, Rees, & Sharratt, 1995).
Some people use fewer images and rely mostly on verbal 3.1. Participants and design
material, whereas others remember better from visual
material (Riding & Douglas, 1993). Because of the In the recruitment of participants, Singh, Lessig, Kim,
differences in the consumers’ cognitive preferences, mar- Gupta, and Hocutt (2000) suggested that the effects of
keters should consider how cognitive preference moderates advertising on students and the common masses are
the effects of verbal and visual materials, so that they may similar. Hence, it was appropriate to use college students
know what mode of advertising to choose in order to as participants in this research. In Study 1, the Ping-Tung
achieve better advertising results. The findings about the Indigenous People Cultural Park in Taiwan was used as the
moderating effect of consumers’ cognitive preferences on advertising target. Respondents were 104 college students
advertising effects may provide further insights into the who participated in a 2 (cognitive preference: verbalizers
interplay of cognitive preference and advertising mode vs. visualizers)  2 (advertising mode: traditional brochure
(virtual experience vs. traditional brochure). More specifi- vs. virtual experience) between-subjects design.
cally, it was predicted that visualizers, when compared with
verbalizers, would be more influenced by virtual experience 3.2. Independent variables
than by traditional brochures. On the other hand,
verbalizers, when compared with visualizers, would be Cognitive preference was treated as a subject variable in
more affected by traditional brochures than by virtual this experimental study. The verbal and visual cognitive
experience. styles scales (CSS) developed by Kirby, Moore, and
Schofield (1988) were not employed in the present research,
2.2. The order effect and hybrid advertising using both because this self-report measure is relatively subjective.
brochures and virtual experience Participants’ cognitive preference was determined by a
free recall task that was designed to differentiate their
Researchers in marketing, psychology, and many other relative advantage in information processing between
disciplines have identified two types of order effects in visual items and verbal items. Participants’ recall rate
ARTICLE IN PRESS
148 W.-B. Chiou et al. / Tourism Management 29 (2008) 146–150

regarding the visual- and verbal-item task was used to seven-point scale (from totally disagree to totally agree).
represent their relative advantage in processing visual or The purchase intent that consisted of three items was
verbal information. measured on a seven-point scale (from totally disagree to
For the classification of consumers’ cognitive prefer- totally agree). After viewing the ad, participants were asked
ences, participants whose recall rate in the visual-item task to rate the advertising effect on the three scales. High
was higher than that in the verbal-item task by at least half scores represent greater advertising attitude, brand atti-
of the standard deviation of rate differences were placed in tude, and purchase intent.
the ‘‘visualizers’’ group, whereas participants who per-
formed better in the verbal-item task were placed in the
3.4. Results
‘‘verbalizers’’ group. Those participants who did not show
a significant rate difference between the two kinds of recall
To test our hypotheses, participants’ responses were
tasks were excluded from the subject pool. Among the
submitted to a 2 (cognitive preference: verbalizers vs.
participants, there were 52 verbalizers and 52 visualizers.
visualizers)  2 (advertising mode: brochure vs. virtual
The contrasted-group method was used to test whether the
experience) between-subjects MANOVA (Table 1).
recall task was valid to discriminate between participants’
The main effect of the advertising mode was obtained,
cognitive preferences. The visualizers (M ¼ 3.46, SD ¼
F(3, 98) ¼ 4.18, po0.01. The advertising effect under the
0.92) scored higher on the visual subscale of the CCS with a
virtual experience mode was significantly greater than that
five-point scale than did the verbalizers (M ¼ 2.44,
under the traditional brochure mode. More importantly,
SD ¼ 0.80), with t(102) ¼ 6.03, po0.001. In contrast, the
MANOVA revealed a significant interaction of cognitive
verbalizers (M ¼ 3.63, SD ¼ 0.82) scored higher on the
preference and advertising mode, F(3, 98) ¼ 22.66, po
verbal subscale of the CCS than did the visualizers
0.001. For the participants who were visualizers, the
(M ¼ 2.56, SD ¼ 0.85), with t(102) ¼ 6.59, po0.01. Re-
advertising effect of virtual experience was more prominent
sults showed that the recall task employed in the present
than that of traditional brochure, F(3, 48) ¼ 9.17,
research was satisfactory for measuring participants’
po0.001. On the other hand, the advertising effect of
cognitive preferences.
traditional brochures was more pronounced than that of
The advertising mode was manipulated between subjects.
virtual experience for those who were verbalizers,
Both verbalizers and visualizers were randomly assigned to
F(3, 48) ¼ 19.93, po0.001.
either the traditional brochure condition or the virtual
experience condition. Half of the participants received the
virtual experience advertising, whereas the other half 4. Experiment 2
received the traditional brochure advertising.
4.1. Methodology
3.3. Dependent measures
In the second study, respondents were 136 college
The dependent measure was the advertising effect as students who participated in this study for course credit.
determined by the advertising attitude scale developed by They were assigned to a 2 (cognitive preference: visualizers
Gardner (1985), the brand attitude scale developed by vs. verbalizers)  2 (presentation order: virtual experience
Lafferty and Goldsmith (1999), and the purchase intent followed by traditional brochure vs. traditional brochure
scale developed by McKenzie, Lutz, and Belch (1986). followed by virtual experience) between-subjects design.
Both the advertising attitude scale and the brand attitude Among the participants were 52 verbalizers and 84
scale that consisted of four items each were rated on a visualizers.

Table 1
Advertising effect of cognitive preference by advertising mode

Cognitive preference by advertising mode Advertising effect

Advertising attitude Brand attitude Purchase intent

M SD M SD M SD

Verbalizers (n ¼ 52)
Traditional brochure 5.13 0.83 4.89 1.02 5.04 1.08
Virtual experience 3.86 0.95 4.19 0.81 4.41 1.15
Visualizers (n ¼ 52)
Traditional brochure 3.81 0.70 3.84 0.68 4.00 0.96
Virtual experience 4.95 0.84 5.22 0.78 5.64 0.93

Note: Each experimental condition consisted of 26 participants. Advertising attitude, brand attitude, and purchase intent were all rated on a seven-point
scale.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
W.-B. Chiou et al. / Tourism Management 29 (2008) 146–150 149

Participants’ cognitive preferences were determined by the 5. Concluding comments


same method used in the first study. The presentation order
was manipulated between subjects. Half of the participants In principle, verbalizers tend to use fewer images, but
received the traditional brochures followed by the virtual rely mostly on verbal material, whereas visualizers tend to
experience mode, whereas the other half received the two remember better from visual material and not from verbal
advertising modes in the opposite order. The dependent material. The findings of the first study were in accordance
measures were similar to those employed in the first study. with the predictions. The results also suggest that
consumers who are verbalizers would more likely be
affected by the ads with traditional brochure, whereas
4.2. Results
those who are visualizers would more likely be influenced
by the ads with virtual experience.
To examine whether the advertising effect would depend
In the second study, the results first indicated that the
on the interaction of presentation order and cognitive
recency effect was more prominent than the primacy effect
preference, participants’ responses were submitted to a 2
when hybrid advertising was employed. This finding was
(cognitive preference: visualizers vs. verbalizers)  2 (pre-
consistent with a classical study conducted by Insko (1964).
sentation order: virtual experience followed by traditional
Thus, when receivers were asked to rate their responses
brochure vs. traditional brochure followed by virtual
immediately after a hybrid presentation, the recency effect
experience) MANOVA (Table 2).
was more apparent. Furthermore, the second study also
The main effect of the presentation order was significant,
revealed that the advertising effect was greater for
F(3, 130) ¼ 3.52, po0.05. The advertising effect under the
verbalizers when the subsequent advertising mode was
traditional brochure followed by virtual experience condition
traditional brochure, whereas the advertising effect was
was greater than that under the virtual experience followed by
greater for visualizers when the subsequent advertising
traditional brochure condition. Results revealed that the
mode was virtual experience. Thus, it suggests that in
subsequent advertising mode (i.e., virtual experience) exhib-
hybrid advertising, the recency effect would depend on
ited greater advertising effect than the previous advertising
participants’ cognitive preferences. In the practice of
mode (i.e., traditional brochure). This further indicated that,
contemporary marketing, potential tourists are likely to
in hybrid advertising, the recency effect might generate a
be subjected to both advertising modes. Future research
greater impact than the primacy effect. Moreover, a
may investigate the complementarity of the different modes
significant interaction of cognitive preference and presenta-
of advertising.
tion order was found, F(3, 130) ¼ 16.46, po0.001. For
In conclusion, the findings showed that consumers’
participants who were verbalizers, the advertising effect under
cognitive preferences played a critical role in determining
the virtual experience followed by traditional brochure
the advertising effect of virtual experience vs. traditional
condition was greater than that under the traditional
brochures. A match hypothesis that refers to the con-
brochure followed by virtual experience condition, F(3, 48) ¼
gruence between the advertising mode and consumers’
3.66, po0.05. Among visualizers, the advertising effect under
cognitive preferences was supported by the research
the traditional brochure followed by virtual experience
findings. Furthermore, the findings in the second study
condition was greater than that under the virtual experience
suggest that when marketers want to employ both virtual
followed by traditional brochure condition, F(3, 48) ¼ 20.59,
experience and traditional brochures in advertising their
po0.001.
products, the virtual experience mode should be presented
to visually oriented consumers after the traditional
Table 2
Advertising effect of cognitive preference by the presentation order of
brochure mode, whereas the traditional brochure mode
hybrid advertising should be introduced after the virtual experience mode for
verbally oriented consumers.
Cognitive Advertising effect It should be noted that there are limitations to the
preference by
presentation Advertising Brand attitude Purchase intent present research. First, participants in the present research
order attitude were Taiwanese college students. Future research may
employ multiple samples with different demographic
M SD M SD M SD
backgrounds to expand the external validity of the
Verbalizers (n ¼ 52) arguments addressed in this article. Second, the advertising
TB–VR 4.23 0.64 4.21 0.67 4.45 1.05 effects assessed in this article were self-report measure-
VR–TB 4.77 0.82 4.76 0.84 5.08 1.07 ments that focused on attitudes and purchase intent.
Visualizers (n ¼ 84) However, recognition and recall, which are commonly
TB–VR 5.31 0.85 5.29 0.87 5.41 0.92 used as indicators of advertising effectiveness (Singh &
VR–TB 3.87 0.89 4.28 0.94 4.41 1.14 Churchill, 1987), may also determine the presence of the
Note: TB, traditional brochure; VR, virtual experience. Advertising
advertising effects. Future research may examine the
attitude, brand attitude, and purchase intent were all measured on a advertising effects by adopting this measurement ap-
seven-point scale. proach. Finally, only one scenic spot was employed to test
ARTICLE IN PRESS
150 W.-B. Chiou et al. / Tourism Management 29 (2008) 146–150

the hypotheses in this research. Future research may utilize Kirby, J. R., Moore, P. J., & Schofield, N. J. (1988). Verbal and visual
different types of scenic spots and examine whether the learning styles. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 13, 169–184.
Klein, L. (1998). Evaluating the potential of interactive media through a
advertising effects of traditional brochures vs. virtual
new lens: Search vs. experience goods. Journal of Business Research,
experience would be contingent upon the types of scenic 41(3), 195–203.
spots involved. Lafferty, B. A., & Goldsmith, R. E. (1999). Corporate credibility’s role in
In sum, this article enhances our understanding of the consumers’ attitude and purchase intentions when a high versus a low
usage of virtual experience in advertising products in the credibility endorser is used in the ad. Journal of Business Research,
tourism industry. Consumers’ cognitive preferences might 44(2), 109–116.
Li, H., Daugherty, T., & Biocca, F. (2001). Characteristics of virtual
interact with the advertising modes to determine the experience in electronic commerce: A protocol analysis. Journal of
advertising effects. Marketers may benefit from the Interactive Marketing, 15(3), 13–30.
applications of the findings observed in the present Lombard, M., Snyder, J., Campanella, C., Kaynak, M. S., Pemrick, J.,
research, particularly when deciding on the appropriate Linder et al. (1997). The cluttering of television. In Paper presented at
presentation order of hybrid advertising according to the the Mass Communication division at the Annual conference of the
international communication association, Montreal, Canada.
interplay of consumers’ cognitive preferences and advertis- Marks, L., & Kamins, M. (1988). The use of product sampling and
ing modes. advertising: Effects of sequence of exposure. Journal of Marketing
Research, 25, 266–282.
Mckenzie, S. B., Lutz, R. J., & Belch, G. E. (1986). The role of attitude
References toward the ad as a mediator of advertising effectiveness: A test of
competing explanations. Journal of Marketing Research, 23(2),
Curtis, P. H., & Duane, T. W. (1994). Message order effects in persuasion: 130–143.
An attitude strength perspective. Journal of Consumer Research, 21(2), Paivio, A. (1991). Images in mind: The evolution of a theory. New York:
205–218. Harvester Wheatsheaf.
DeFleur, M. L., & Dennis, E. E. (1996). Understanding mass communica- Riding, R. J., & Ashmore, J. (1980). Verbaliser–imager learning style and
tion. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Company. children’s recall of information presented in pictorial versus written
Fang, C. L., & Lie, T. (2006). Assessment of internet marketing and form. Educational Studies, 6, 141–145.
competitive strategies for leisure farming industry in Taiwan. Journal Riding, R. J., Burton, D., Rees, G., & Sharratt, M. (1995). Cognitive style
of American Academy of Business, 8(2), 296–300. and personality in 12-year-old children. British Journal of Educational
Fitzgerald, K. (1999). Picking through the clutter, MediaCom bids for Psychology, 65, 113–124.
flawless. Advertising Age, 70(8), 2. Riding, R. J., & Douglas, G. (1993). The effect of cognitive style and mode
Gardner, M. P. (1985). Dose attitude toward the ad affect brand attitude of presentation on learning performance. British Journal of Educational
under a brand evaluation set. Journal of Marketing Research, 22(2), Psychology, 63, 297–307.
192–198. Singh, S. N., & Churchill, G. A. (1987). Response-bias-free recognition
Haugtvedt, C. P., & Wegener, D. T. (1994). Message order effects in tests to measure advertising effects. Journal of Advertising Research,
persuasion: An attitude strength perspective. Journal of Consumer 27(3), 23–36.
Research, 21(1), 205–218. Singh, S. N., Lessig, V. P., Kim, D., Gupta, R., & Hocutt, M. A. (2000).
Hogarth, R. M., & Einhorn, H. J. (1992). Order effects in belief updating: Does your ad have too many pictures? Journal of Advertising Research,
The belief-adjustment model. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 1–55. 40(1), 11–27.
Holloway, J. C., & Plant, R. V. (1992). Marketing for tourism (2nd ed.). Smith, R. E., & Swinyard, W. R. (1988). Cognitive response to advertising
London: Pitman. and trial: Belief strength, belief confidence and product curiosity.
Insko, C. H. (1964). Primacy vs. recency in persuasion as a function of the Journal of Advertising, 17(3), 3–14.
timing of argument and measures. Journal of Abnormal and Social Yamamoto, D., & Gill, A. M. (1999). Emerging trends in Japanese
Psychology, 64(4), 621–623. package tourism. Journal of Travel Research, 38(2), 134–143.

You might also like