Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

279

Relationships between prolactin, LH and broody behaviour


in bantam hens
P. J. Sharp, M. C. Macnamee, R. J. Sterling, R. W. Lea and H. C. Pedersen*
AFRC Institute of Animal Physiologyand GeneticsResearch, Edinburgh ResearchStation, Roslin,
Midlothian EH25 9PS
*Department of Zoology, Universityof Trondheim, N-7055 Dragvoll, Norway
(M. C. Macnameeis now at EndocrinologyDepartment, Bourn Hall Clinic, Bourn Hall, Bourn,
Cambridge CB3 7TR)
(R. W. Lea is now at School of Applied Biology,LancashirePolytechnic,Preston PRI 2TQ)
RECEIVED 21 December 1987

ABSTRACT
The interactions between broody behaviour and time when plasma prolactin decreased to baseline
changesin concentrations of plasma prolactin and LH values. Administration of ovine prolactin depressed
were investigatedin bantam hens. Adoption of newly (P <0.01) the initial increasein plasma LH after nest
hatched chicks caused incubating hens to leave their deprivation, but repeated administration of prolactin
nests and prevented plasma prolactin decreasing as for up to 72h failed to suppress plasma LH to the
rapidly as in hens deprivedof their nests and not given values found in incubating hens. Repeated adminis-
chicks. Further, the hens allowed to rear chickscame tration of ovineprolactin at 5-to 8-hintervalsfor 72h
back into lay later (P < 0-001) than the hens not maintained readiness -to incubate in nest-deprived
allowed chicks. Plasma prolactin decreased and hens.
plasma LH increased in hens deprived of their nests: It is concluded that the secretion of prolactin in
these changes were reversedwhen the hens re-nested. broody hens is facilitated by the presence of chicks
The changes in plasma LH and prolactin in nest- and that increasedconcentrationsof plasma prolactin
deprived and re-nesting birds were not always syn- maintain incubation behaviour. In incubating hens
chronous; this was particularly clear immediately the secretionof LH and prolactin may be partly regu-
after nest deprivation when the increasein plasma LH lated independently. In addition, LH secretion may
preceded the decreasein the plasma prolactin. Readi- also be inhibited by increasedplasma prolactin.
ness to incubate disappeared between 48 and 72h J. Endocr. (1988)118, 279-286
after nest deprivation and corresponded with the

INTRODUCTION 1984) but not in precocial (Goldsmith & Williams,


1980;Wentworth, Proudman, Opelet al. 1983;Hector
The maintenance of high concentrations of plasma & Goldsmith, 1985)speciesmaintains high levels of
prolactin in birds incubating eggs appears to be a plasma prolactin. It is not known in precocial species
direct result of incubation behaviour. Removal of the whether plasma prolactin is as depressed after the
nest results in a rapid decreasein plasma prolactin (EI hatching of young as after the removalof the nest and
Halawani, Burke & Dennison, 1980; Lea & Sharp, eggs.
1982; Goldsmith, Burke & Prosser, 1984; Ramsey, The increasedconcentration of plasma prolactin in
Goldsmith & Silver, 1985;Lea, Vowles& Dick, 1986) birds incubating eggs appears to maintain readiness
which is reversed if the nest is returned and the bird to incubate and to depress plasma gonadotrophins.
resumes incubation in the turkey (E1Halawani et aL Thus, injections of prolactin maintain incubation in
1980)and canary (Goldsmith et al. 1984)but not in ring doves (Lehrman & Brody, 1964;Janik & Buntin,
the ring dove (R. W. Lea, unpublished observation). 1985; Lea et al. 1986) and stimulate incubation in
The presence of chicks in altricial (Buntin, 1979; turkeys(ElHalawani, Silsby,Behnke& Fehrer, 1986).
Dawson & Goldsmith, 1982; Silverin & Goldsmith, However, the role of prolar ':n in the maintenance of
J. Endocr. (1988) 118, 279-286 © 1988 Journal of Endocrinology Ltd Printed in Great Britain
0022-0795/88/0118-0279 $02.00/0
280 P. J. SHARP and others • Hormones and broody behaviour

incubation has been questioned by the observation they were brooding chicks and compared with pro-
that the initiation of re-nesting in nest-deprived lactin and LH concentrations when the birds were
turkeys and canariesdoesnot appear to depend on the laying or incubating. The rate of decrease in plasma
maintenance of plasma prolactin levelsabove baseline prolactin after hatch was compared with that in the
(EI Halawani et al. 1980;Goldsmith et al. 1984). incubating hens deprived of their nestsin experiments
A depressiveeffectof prolactin on gonadotrophin 2 and 3. The onset of broodiness was synchronizedas
secretionis suggestedby the association betweenhigh closelyas possibleby transferring 20 layingbirds with
concentrations of plasma prolactin and low concen- previous experienceof broodiness from a stimulatory
trations of plasma luteinizing hormone (LH) in (14L:10D) to a non-stimulatory (8L:16D) daylength
broody birds (e.g.Sharp, Scanes,Williamset al. 1979; for 6 weeks to put them out of lay. They were trans-
Burke & Dennison, 1980; Goldsmith & Williams, ferredback to 14L:10Dto stimulatethe resumptionof
1980;Goldsmith, Edwards, Koprucu & Silver, 1981; lay and kept in floor pens with nest boxes. Eggswere
Lea, Dods, Sharp & Chadwick, 1981; Dawson & removedfor 5-6 weeksafter comingback into lay, but
Goldsmith, 1982;Goldsmith et al. 1984).The finding subsequently the birds were allowed to accumulate
in bantam hens that plasma prolactin increasesbefore clutchesof eggsto encourage broodiness.The 11hens
plasma LH falls at the onset of incubation, and that selectedfor the study became broody between 12and
treatment of broody bantam hens with anti-prolactin 16days after being allowed to accumulateclutchesof
serum increasesplasma LH, suggeststhat the inverse eggs. After the birds had been incubating for 18-21
relationship betweenplasma prolactin and LH may be days, all the eggswere replaced with day-old bantam
causal (Lea et al. 1981).This view is consistent with chicks hatched from eggs which had been incubated
the ability of exogenousprolactin to induce ovarian artificially.The normal length of incubation in ban-
regression in poultry (e.g. Opel & Proudman, 1980) tam hens is 21 days. Each hen was given four chicks.
and ring doves (Buntin & Tesch, 1985; Janik & This was done to make it easier to take blood samples
Buntin, 1985).However, there is evidencein broody at precise intervals after the birds began to brood
mallards (Goldsmith & Williams, 1980),turkeys (E1 chicks.Blood sampleswere taken directlyfrom a wing
Halawani, Silsby, Fehrer & Behnke, 1983),canaries veinat 3-to 4-dayintervalsstarting after the birds had
(Goldsmith et al. 1984)and doves(R. W. Lea, unpub- resumed laying and ending after they had incubated
lished observation) that changes in plasma LH con- eggs,reared chicksand started layingagain.
centrations are not always associated with inverse
changesin plasma prolactin.
Experiment2
This series of experiments in bantam hens was
designed to increase our understanding of the inter- This experiment was carried out to determine the
actions betweenbroody behaviour, prolactin and LH. temporal relationships between changes in plasma
The possibility that the presence of young chicks prolactin and LH in incubating birds deprivedof their
might influenceplasma prolactin after hatching was nest for 24 or 48 h. A second purpose was to establish
investigated, as was the putative role of prolactin in whether there are differencesin plasma prolactin con-
the maintenance of incubation behaviour. Finally, an centrations between birds which re-nest after nest
attempt was made to determine whether injections of deprivation and those that do not. The hens had been
prolactin suppressthe concentration of plasma LH. incubatingfor between8 and 14 days.The experimen-
tal birds were transferred at 08.00h from their nests
and placed one bird to a cage, in wire battery cages,
MATERIALS AND METHODS for 24h (sevenhens) or 48 h (ten hens) and were then
returned to their nests. Serial blood samples were
Laying bantam hens, 18- to 30 months old, from the
-
taken directly from a wing vein. Birds deprived of
laboratory breedingflock, were kept in groups of two their nests for 24 or 48 h were sampled at 0, 3, 6, 12,
to six in floor pens (4 x 1m) with nest boxes or indi- 18,24, 27, 30, 36and 48 h or 0, 6, 12,24, 36,42,48, 51,
vidually in wire battery cages. The lighting schedule 54,60 and 72h respectively,after being transferred to
was 14h light:10h darkness (14L:10D;lights on from cages.Groups of fivecontrol birds incubatingeggsin
07.00-21.00h) and the birds had free access to food their nest boxeswere sampled at the same time.
and water.
Experiment 1 Experiment3
This experiment used broody hens to determine the This experimentwas carried out to determinewhether
effectof the presenceof chicks on the concentrations the administration of ovine prolactin would maintain
of plasma prolactin and LH. These hormones were readinessto incubate in incubating bntams deprived
measured in 11 bantam hens throughout the period of their nests for 72h. A 72-h nest Aeprivationperiod
.1".Endocr. (1988) 118, 279-286
Hormones and broody behaviour • P. J. SHARP and others 281

was chosen becausethe result from experiment2 indi- concentrations for each experimentwere measured in
cated that 50% of the hensdid not re-nestafter 48h of single assays to eliminate interassay variation. The
nest deprivation. It was anticipated that a further 24-h inter- and intra-assay coefficientsof variation for the
nest deprivation would result in the disappearance of chicken prolactin assay were 4.5 and 3%, and for the
readinessto incubate in the majority of the hens. LH assay 6 and 4% respectively.
The hens had been incubating eggsfor 24+ 3 (s.D.)
days. They were transferred to battery cages, and Statistics
eight experimentalhens were injectedi.p. at 5- to 8-h
intervals with 25 IU ovine prolactin (Lot 123F-0320; Friedman's analysis of variance and Wilcoxon's tests
Sigma Chemical Company, Poole, Dorset, U.K.) in were used for serial samples unless otherwise stated
1ml of an alkaline saline solution (0.15mol NaC1/1 (Conover, 190). Comparisons between groups were
made alkaline with 0.01mol Na0H/; pH 8.5) begin- made using the Mann—WhitneyU test.
ning 3 h after transfer to cages. Eight control hens
were injected at the same time with alkaline saline
vehicle. In total, each bi-rd received 12 injections RESULTS
spread over 72h. Blood samples were taken at 12-h
intervals to demonstrate that the treatment had Effect of the presenceof chicks on concentrationsof
resulted in the expected decrease in plasma con- plasma prolactinandLH
centrations of chicken prolactin. The effect of i.p. At the onset of incubation the concentration of
injections of ovine prolactin on plasma levelsof this plasma LH decreasedwhile that of plasma prolactin
hormone was investigatedin experiment4. After the increased (Fig. 1). After the eggs were replaced
last injection,at 08.00h, the birds werereturned to the by chicks (designated day 0 from hatch) the con-
floor pens containing their nest boxes. The birds were centration of plasma prolactin began to decrease
then observed continuously for 1h, and spot checks (P < 0.05)within 3 days (Fig. 1).However, it was still
were made 12, 54, 78 and 102h later to observe 50% above baselinebetween6 and 9 days after hatch
nestingbehaviour. and did not fall to baselinevaluesuntil 10days before
the resumption of egg production. At this time the
Experiment4 plasma concentration of LH began to increase
This experimentwas carried out to determinewhether (P < 0-05).The hensbeganto lay 30+ 2 days after they
injectionsof ovineprolactin depressthe increasein the had been givennewlyhatched chicks.
concentration of plasma LH seen after nest depri- The hens took a closeinterest in the newlyhatched
vation. Groups of eight experimentaland eight con- chicks, brooding them, encouragingthem to feed and
trol hens, which had been incubatingeggsfor between drink, and defending them against intruders. As the
8 and 13 days, were transferred to battery cages for chicks grew, this intensematernal response gradually
24h. The experimental birds were injected i.p. with declined, so that 2-3 weeks after hatching brooding
25IU ovine prolactin (Lot 123F-0320;Sigma)at the was infrequent and the chicks were spending more
time of transfer and at 6, 12, 18 and 22h thereafter. time away from their mothers.
Control birds were injectedat the same timeswith the In experiments in which incubating hens were
alkaline saline vehicle. Blood samples were taken deprived of their nests the plasma concentration of
from a wingvein immediatelybefore each injectionof prolactin fell more rapidly than in incubating hens
ovine prolactin for measurement of ovine prolactin given day-old chicks (Figs 2b, 3a and 4b). In hens
and chicken LH. deprived of their nests, plasma prolactin returned to
Additional information on the effectof ovine pro- baseline values after 36-48 h and had decreased to
lactin on plasma LH was obtained by measuring 50% above baseline after about 18-24h (Figs 2, 3
plasma LH and chicken prolactin levels in samples and 4). Control hens deprived of their nests in the
taken in experiment3. experiment shown in Fig. 4 came back into lay after
12+ 3 days. This was significantlyearlier (P < 0-001;
Hormone assays Student's t-test) than in the hens allowed to rear
chicks.
Plasma concentrations of chicken prolactin and
LH were measured using the homologous assays
described by Lea, Sharp & Chadwick (1982) and Relationshipsbetweenplasma prolactinand LH in
Sharp, Dunn & Talbot (1987) respectively. Ovine nest-deprivedandre-nestinghens
prolactin was measured as described by McNeilly& Plasma concentrations of prolactin decreased signifi-
Andrews (1974) in a single assay. The intra-assay cantly (P <0.05) after 12h of nest deprivation in all
coefficientof variation was 4.2%. Prolactin and LH hens deprived of their nests for 24h (Fig. 2b) and in
J. Endocr. (1988) 118, 279-286
282 P. J. SHARP and others • Hormones and broody behaviour

105 1.5
90 3

90 1.0

75 0•5
J_
60
(lig/1)
LH 3

1 (p.g/1)
, Plasma LH
30
2
Plasma

-40 -20 0 +20 30 1


Time since hatch (days)
-20 6 ' +20
Time since onset of t nest removed t nest returned
lay (days)
0
0 24 48
FIGURE 1. Changesin the concentrationsof plasmaprolactin
(0) and LH (•) (means + s.E.M.) in 11bantam hensduring Time (h)
a broody cycle.Layinghenswereallowedto accumulate FIGURE 2. Changesin the concentrationsof plasmaprolactin
clutchesof eggs,incubatethem, rear chicksand return to (0) and LH (•) (means + s.E.M.) in incubatingbantam
lay. After the henshad beenincubatingfor 18-21days,the hens (a) not deprived(n=5) or (b)deprivedof their nests
eggswerereplacedwithday-oldbantam chicks(day0 from (n= 7)for 24h.
hatch). The henswerelayinguntil about 25daysbefore
hatch: the onsetof incubationbehaviouriscorrelatedwith
the steepincreasein plasmaprolactin and the decreasein Of the ten hens deprived of their nests for 48 h, six
plasma LH at 18-21daysbeforehatch. The hensresumed resumed nesting when given the opportunity. Changes
laying30+ 2 daysafter beingprovidedwithday-oldchicks. in plasma concentrations of LH and prolactin were
plotted separately for the birds that re-nested and
six out of ten hens deprived of their nests for 48 h (Fig. those which did not re-nest (Fig. 3). Plasma concen-
3a). In the latter, plasma prolactin had re'turned to trations of prolactin fell significantly (P < 0-05) during
baseline values 48 h after nest deprivation (Fig. 3b). the 48-h period of nest deprivation in both groups.
This observation was confinned in a subsequent However, in birds that re-nested plasma prolactin was
experiment in which nest-deprived hens were injected higher than in those which did not re-nest, at the start
with ovine prolactin or saline vehicle (Fig 4b). of the experiment, when the nests were removed
Plasma concentrations of LH increased significantly (P < 0.05) and again when the nests were returned 48 h
(P <0-05) within 3 h of nest deprivation and reached later (P < 0.001) (Fig. 3a). Concentrations of plasma
maximum values after 6 h (Fig. 2b). Plasma LH was LH and changes in plasma LH during the 48-h period
also found to be significantly increased (P < 0-001) 6 h of nest deprivation were similar in hens which
after nest deprivation in hens deprived of their nests re-nested and in those which did not (Fig. 3b).
for 48 h (Fig. 3b) or deprived of their nests and
injected with saline vehicle (Fig. 4a). Maintenanceof readinessto incubatein nest-deprived
All the hens deprived of their nests for 24 h resumed hens treated with ovine prolactin
nesting immediately after being allowed access to their Five out of eight hens deprived of their nests for 72 h
nests. Re-nesting was associated with an immediate and injected with ovine prolactin returned to their
decrease in plasma LH, which was significant nests within 10 min of being allowed to do so. One of
(P < 0-01) after 3 h, and an increase in plasma pro- the eight control hens injected with saline vehicle also
lactin which was significant (P < 0.05) after 6 h (Fig. re-nested within 10 min. After 1 h of being allowed to
2b). Concentrations of plasma LH and prolactin re-nest, seven of the eight nest-deprived hens injected
returned to values characteristic of incubating hens with prolactin had re-nested. The hen which had been
12 h after re-nesting (Figs 2b, 3a and b). injected with prolactin and did not re-nest after 1 h
J. Endocr, (1988) 118, 279-286
Hormones and broodv behaviour P. J. SHARP and others 283

120 Repeated injectionsof ovine prolactin for more than


12h after nest deprivation failed to depress the con-
centration of plasma LH below that in the control
J.
hens (Fig. 4).
90
I 11 The repeated injections of ovine prolactin resulted
in circulating concentrations of more than about
300gg/1(Fig. 4a). The concentration of chicken pro-
lactin decreased progressivelyto the baseline value
60 over 36h, irrespective of whether the hens were
c7. 1.. injectedwith ovine prolactin or alkaline salinevehicle
(Fig. 4b).
.....................
30
4 (b) DISCUSSION

3 Previous studies (for review see Goldsmith, 1983)


...... ),„ ............... suggest that concentrations of plasma prolactin are
LH 2 ' maintained after hatching in species with altricial
- —6—
young but not in those with precocial young. The
Plasma implication that precocial young do not affect pro-
t nest removed nest returned lactin secretion in their mothers is not supported by
0
0 24 48 72 the present study. The fall in plasma prolactin
Time (h) observedafter incubatingbantam hens wereprovided
FIGURE 3. Changesin the concentrationof (a) plasmapro- with day-old chicks was not as rapid as in bantam
lactin and (b) plasma LH in incubatingbantam hensnot hens deprived of their nests and not provided with
deprived(11;n=5) or deprivedof their nestsfor 48 h (open chicks.This associationbetweenpartially maintained
symbols).Hens deprivedof theirnestsfor 48h weresubdi- concentrations of plasma prolactin and broody
videdinto those whichre-nested(0; n= 6) and thosewhich behaviour could be causal sinceinjection of prolactin
did not re-nest(A; n=4). Valuesrepresentmeans + S.E.M. inducesmaternalbehaviourin domesticfowl(Burrows
& Byerly, 1938; Nalbandov, 1945; Nalbandov &
Card, 1945;Saeki&Tanabe, 1955;Opel& Proudman,
raised her feathers and emittedcallscharacteristicof a 1980).Further, in willow ptarmigan, administration
broody hen when approached. Twelve hours after of prolactin enhances the intensity of parental care
being allowed to re-nest this hen had returned to its (Pedersen, 1988). However, it is likely that the
nest but in contrast with the sevenother hens injected presence of chicks does not stimulate prolactin
with prolactin, left her nest when an observer placed secretion but tends to maintain it. This can be
his hand under her. Sevenof the eight hens injected deduced from the observation that plasma prolactin
with prolactin and one of the eight control hens does not increase in laying or non-laying hens in
injectedwith salinevehiclewereincubatingeggs54,78 which parental behaviour was induced by forced
and 102h after being allowedto re-nest. adoption of newly hatched chicks (Richard-Yris,
Leboucher,Chadwick& Garnier, 1987).
Effect of injectionof ovineprolactinon concentrations The relative importance of visual, auditory and
of plasma LH in hens deprivedof their nests tactile stimuli provided by the chicks in maintaining
Plasma concentrations of LH increased significantly brooding behaviour is unknown. Bantam hens will
(P <0-01) in hens deprived of their nests irrespective 'incubate' sittingon an empty nest if they are provided
of whether they were injectedwith ovine prolactin or with a clutch of eggswhich they can seebut not touch
alkaline saline vehicle(Fig. 4). A peak in the concen- (Steen & Parker, 1981). This observation demon-
tration of LH was observedbetween 6 and 12h after strates the importanceof visualstimuliin maintaining
nest removal in the control hens but not in hens incubation behaviour, and it is possible that newly
injectedwith prolactin. A similar peak in plasma LH hatched chicks also provide a visual stimulus which
was also observed in the 48-h (Fig. 3b) but not in the maintains parental behaviour. On the other hand
24-h (Fig. 2b) nest deprivation experiments. The tactile contact between hen and chicks has been
absence of a peak concentration of plasma LH for shown to be necessaryin the domestic hen to main-
12h after nest deprivation in hens injectedwith ovine tain full maternal brooding behaviour (Maier, 1963;
prolactin resed in plasma concentrations of LH Richard-Yris & Leboucher, 1986). Since the fall in
being significantlylowerthan in control hens (Fig. 4). plasma prolactin during the 2-3 weeksafter hatch was
J. Endocr. (1988) 118, 279-286
284 P. J. SHARP and others • Hormones and broody behaviour

(ps/1)
2
LI-I

?-
Plasma

0 H HHHH
300

72. 200 1;
)

•- (1) 0
cp>100

0
0 12 24 0 12 24 36 48 60 72
Time (h)
FIGURE 4. Effect of repeated i.p. injections of 25 IU ovine prolactin (0) or saline
vehicle (•) in groups of eight incubating bantam hens for (a) 24 h or (b) 72 h after
nest deprivation on concentrations of plasma LH and ovine or chicken (ch)
prolactin. The arrows indicate the times of injection of ovine prolactin or vehicle.
The values represent means + S.E.M. *P< 0.05, **P< 0.001 compared with
saline-injected control birds (Student's t-test).

associated with a progressive reduction in brooding preceded the fall in plasma prolactin after nest depri-
behaviour, it is possible that prolactin secretion is vation (Figs 2b and 3). A similar relationship was
maintained in hens with chicks by tactile stimuli. seen in incubating turkeys treated with the anti-
The rapid fall in plasma prolactin after nest depri- serotoninergic drug, p-chlorophenlyalanine, to dis-
vation and the increase after renesting confirms the rupt broodiness (EI Halawani et al. 1980). These
finding in other species that prolactin secretion is observations,togetherwith the asynchronouschanges
maintained by incubation behaviour (see Introduc- in plasma LH or prolactin seen at the onset of
tion). The 48- to 72-hperiod required after nest depri- incubation or during re-nesting in other species
vation for the bantam hens to lose their readiness to (Goldsmith & Williams,1980;Goldsmith et al. 1984),
incubate is similar to that required by nest-deprived suggests that the secretion of these hormones is not
turkey hens (ElHalawani et al. 1980)and corresponds entirely interdependent.
with the time required for plasma prolactin to return The observation that the administration of pro-
to baselinevalues.It thus appears that in thesespecies lactin attenuated the intitial increase in plasma LH
readinessto incubate cannot be maintained after con- after nest deprivation is consistent with an inhibitory
centrations of plasma prolactin have returned to these action of prolactin on LH release. A similar conclu-
values. sion was drawn from the finding that the adminis-
The temporal relationship betweenplasma LH and tration of anti-prolactin serum to incubating bantam
prolactin after removal and return of nests suggests hens results in an increase in plasma LH (Lea et al.
that changes in plasma LH are not always inversely 1981).An inhibitory action of increased plasma pro-
related to concentrations of plasma prolactin. Most lactin on gonadotrophin secretionis also suggestedby
striking was the steep increase in plasma LH which the fact that hens allowed to rear chicks returned to
J. Endocr. (1988)118, 279-286
Hormones and broody behaviour • P. J. SHARP and dthers 285

lay significantlylater (30+ 2 days versus 12+ 3 days; and is initiatedby progesteroneactingon a neural sub-
P <0.001) than hens deprived of their nests and not strate previouslyprimed with oestrogen (Wood-Gush
given chicks. It seems that the maintenance of & Gilbert, 1973, 1975).It seems that at the onset of
increased concentrations of plasma prolactin in hens incubation an increasein plasma prolactin facilitates
brooding chicks may have delayed the resumption of this steroid-dependent behaviour. The initial depen-
ovarian folliculogenesis. dencyof incubation behaviour on an ovarian steroid-
The failure of repeated administration of ovine primed neural substrate seems to disappear after the
prolactin for 72h to depress plasma LH to the values behaviour becomes fully established. Thus, well-
found in incubating hens argues against high concen- established incubation behaviour is not disrupted
trations of plasma prolactin being the only factor after ovariectomy(Sharp, Sterling& Pedersen, 1986).
responsible for inhibiting LH release in these birds. It seemsthat the successor failure of earlier attempts
It is possible that the failure of ovine prolactin to to demonstrate a role for prolactin in incubation
suppressconcentrations of plasma LH to those found behaviour in domesticfowlmay reflectappropriate or
in incubating hens is due to differences in species inappropriate balances between circulating ovarian
specificity between chicken and ovine prolactin. steroids and the dose of prolactin administered. In
Alternatively, the blood—brainbarrier might have addition, it is also likelythat the onset of incubation is
restricted the access of the administered ovine pro- influencedby poorly defined non-hormonal environ-
lactin to target sites in the brain (Buntin & Ruzychi, mental factors suchas the availabilityof an acceptable
1987).It is probable that prolactin enters the brain via nest site. It is not surprising, therefore, that a role for
the choroid plexusas has beendemonstrated in the rat prolactin in incubation behaviour can be more readily
by Walsh, Slaby & Posner (1987). demonstrated in nest-deprived hens in which the
The maintenance of readiness to incubate in nest- behaviour is not directly dependent on ovarian
deprived bantam hens after repeated injections of steroidsor undefinedenvironmentalstimuli.
prolactin confirms similar observations in the ring
dove (Lehrman & Brody, 1964;Janik & Buntin, 1985;
Lea et al. 1986).Similarly,El Halawani et al. (1986) ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
showed that nesting behaviour induced in ovari-
ectomized turkeys by treatment with oestrogen and We thank Mr R. T. Talbot and Mr S. Anderson for
progesterone is advanced to incubation behaviour help with the immunoassays and Drs A. Chadwick
after the administration of prolactin. and A. S. McNeillyfor the supply of reagents for the
The contrast between the marked effects of ovine prolactin radioimmunoassay.
prolactin on the maintenanceof incubation behaviour
and its modestly depressive effect on the concen-
tration of plasma LH suggestsdifferent mechanisms REFERENCES
of action. Studies on the ring dove show that intra-
cerebroventricular injections of ovine prolactin Buntin,J. D. (1979).Prolactinreleasein parent ringdovesafter
depress testicular weight (Buntin & Tesch, 1985)and briefexposureto theiryoung. Journal of Endocrinology 82,
plasma LH (Buntin, Lea & Figge, 1988)but do not 127-130.
maintain readiness to incubate (Buntin & Tesch, Buntin,J. D., Lea,R. W. & Figge,G. R. (1988).Reductionsin
plasmaLH concentrationand testicularweightin ringdovesfol-
1985).It seemsthat prolactin acts, at least in part, on lowingintracranialinjectionof prolactinor growthhormone.
peripheral tissues to maintain incubation behaviour. Journal of Endocrinology 118, 33 40.
-

A similar differencein the sites of action of prolactin Buntin,J. D. & Ruzychi,E. (1987).Characteristicsof prolactin
in the bantam hen may explain the contrast between bindingsitesin the brain of the ringdove (Steptopelia risoria).
its marked effecton incubation behaviour but modest General and Comparative Endocrinology 65, 243 253.-

Buntin,J . D. &Tesch,D. (198,5).Effectsof intracranialprolactin


effecton LH secretion. administrationon maintenanceof incubationreadiness,ingestive
Previous attempts to show that prolactin plays a behavior,and gonadalconditionin ringdoves.Hormones and
role in incubation behaviour in the domestic fowl Behavior 19, 188 203.
-

have produced conflicting results. Early studies Burke,W. H. & Dennison,P. T. (1980).Prolactinand luteinizing
showed that injections of mammalian prolactin hormonelevelsin femaleturkeys (Meleagris gallopavo) duringa
photoinducedreproductivecycleand broodiness.General and
induces incubation behaviour in laying hens (Riddle, Comparative Endocrinology 41, 92 100.
-

Bates & Lahr, 1935;Saeki & Tanabe, 1955),but this Burrows,W. H. & Byerly,T. C. (1938).The effectof certaingroups
was not confirmed in more recent work using better of environmentalfactorsupon expressionof broodiness.Poultry
Science 17, 324 330.
purifed preparations of ovine prolactin (Opel & -

Conover,W.J. (1980).Practical Nonparametric Statistics, edn 2.


Proudman, 1980;R. W. Lea and P. J. Sharp, unpub- NewYork, Chichester,Brisbane,Toronto:John Wiley& Sow
lished obserw tions). Nesting behaviour normally Dawson,A. & Goldsmith,A. R. (1982).Prolactinand gonadotro-
occurs in laying hens around the time of oviposition pin secretionin wildstarlings(Sturnus vulgaris) duringthe annual
J. Endocr. (1988)118, 279-286
286 P. J. SHARP and others • Hormones and broody behaviour

cycleand in relationto nesting,incubationand rearingyoung. Nalbandov,A. V.&Card, L. E. (1945).Endocrineidentificationof


General and Comparative Endocrinology 48, 213 221. - the broodygenotypeofcocks.Journal of Heredity 36,35-39.
El Halawani,M. E., Burke,W. H. & Dennison,P.T. (1980).Effects Opel,H. & Proudman,J. A. (1980).Failureof mammalianpro-
of nestdeprivationon serumprolactinlevelin nestingfemale lactinto induceincubationbehaviourin chickensand turkeys.
turkeys. Biology of Reproduction 23, 118 123. - Poultry Science 59, 2550 2558.
-

El Halawani,M. E., Silsby,J. L., Behnke,E. J. & Fehrer,S.C. Pedersen,H.-C.(1988).Effectsof exogenousprolactinon parental
(1986).Hormonc,,:inductionof incubationbehaviorin ovari- behaviourin free-livingfemaleWillowptarmigan Lagopus L.
ectomizedfemaleturkeys (Meleagris gallopavo). Biology of lagopus. Animal Behaviour. (In Press.)
Reproduction 35,59 67. - Ramsey,S. M., Goldsmith,A. R. & Silver,R. (1985).Stimulus
ElHalawani,M. E., Silsby,J. L., Fehrer, S.C. & Behnke,E. J. requirementsforprolactinand LH secretionin incubatingring
(1983).Reinitiationof ovulatorycyclesin incubatingfemale doves.General and Comparative Endocrinology 59, 246 256.
-

turkeysby an inhibitorof serotoninsynthesis,p-chlorophenylala- Richard-Yris,N.-A.& Leboucher,G. (1986).Comportêment


nine. Biology of Reproduction 28, 221 228.- maternelinduitchezla pouledomestique:influenced'une
Goldsmith,A. R. (1983).Prolactinin avianreproductivecycles.In separationpartielleoå totalesur lemaintiende la réactivité
Hormones and Behavior in Higher Vertebrates, pp. 375 387.Eds - maternelle.Compte Rendu Hebdotnadaire des Seances de
J. Balthazart,E. Prove& R. Gilles,Berlin,Heidelberg,New l'Academie des Sciences (III) 10,387 390.
-

York, Toronto: Springer-Verlag. Richard-Yris,N.-A.,Leboucher,G., Chadwick,A. &Garnier,


Goldsmith,A. R., Burke,S.&Prosser,J. M. (1984).Inverse D. H. (1987).Inductionofmaternal behaviourin incubatingand
changesin plasmaprolactinand LH concentrationsin female non-incubatinghens:influenceof hormones. Physiology and
canariesafter disruptionand reinitiationofincubation.Journal of Behaviour 40, 193 199.
-

Endocrinology 103, 251 256. - Riddle,0., Bates,R. W.& Lahr, E. L. (1935).Prolactininduces


Goldsmith,A. R., Edwards,C., Koprucu,M. & Silver,R. (1981). broodinessin fowl.American Journal of Physiology 111, 352 360.
-

Concentrationsof prolactinand luteinizinghormonein plasma Saeki,Y. &Tanabe,Y. (1955).Changesin prolactincontentof fowl


of dovesin relationto incubationand developmentof thecrop pituitary glandduringbroodyperiodsand someexperimentson
gland. Journal of Endocrinology 90, 437 443. - the inductionof broodiness.Poultry Science 34, 909-919.
Goldsmith,A. R. &Williams,D. M. (1980).Incubationin mallards Sharp,P. J., Dunn, I. C. &Talbot, R. T. (1987).Sexdifferencesin
(Anas platyrhynchos): changesin plasmalevelsof prolactinand the LH responsesto chickenLHRH-I and -II in thedomestic
luteinizinghormone.Journal of Endocrinology 86, 371-379. fowl.Journal of Endocrinology 115, 323 331.
-

Hector,J. A. L. &Goldsmith,A. R. (1985).The roleofprolactin Sharp,P. J., Scanes,C. G., Williams,J. B.,Harvey,S.&Chadwick,
duringincubation:comparativestudiesof threeDiomedea A. (1979).Variationsin theconcentrationsof prolactin,luteiniz-
albatrosses.General and Comparative Endocrinology 60, 236-243. inghormone,growthhormoneand progesteronein theplasmaof
Janik, D. S.& Buntin,J. D. (1985).Behaviouraland physiological broodybantams (Gallus domesticus). Journal of Endocrinology
effectsof prolactinin incubatingringdoves.Journal of 80, 51-57.
Endocrinology 105, 201 209. - Sharp,P. J., Sterling,R. J. &Pedersen,H.-C.(1986).An assessment
Lea,R. W., Dods,A. S.M., Sharp,P. J. &Chadwick,A. (1981). of the rolesof prolactinand ovarianhormonesin themainten-
The possibleroleof prolactinin theregulationof nesting anceof lowplasmaLH levelsin broody bantams. Journal of
behaviourand the secretionof luteinizinghormonein broody Endocrinology 108 (Suppl.),AbstractNo. 93.
bantams. Journal of Endocrinology 91, 89 97. - Silverin,B.& Goldsmith,A. (1984).The effectsof modifying
Lea,R. W. & Sharp,P. J. (1982).Plasmaprolactinconcentrationsin incubationon prolactinsecretionin free-livingpiedflycatchers.
broody turkeys:lackof agreementbetweenhomologouschicken General and Comparative Endocrinology 55, 239 244.
-

and turkeyprolactinradioimmunoassays.British Poultry Science Steen,J. B.& Parker,H. (1981).The egg-'numerostae.A new
23, 451-459. conceptin the regulationof clutchsize. Ornis Scandinavica 12,
Lea, R. W., Sharp,P. J. & Chadwick,A. (1982).Dailyvariationsin 109-110.
the concentrationsof plasmaprolactinin broodybantams. Walsh,R. J., Slaby,F. &Posner,B.T. (1987).A receptor-mediated
General and Comparative Endocrinology 48, 254 275. - mechanismfor thetransport of prolactinfrombloodto
Lea,R. W.,Vowles,D. M. &Dick,H. R. (1986).Factorsaffecting cerebrospinalfluid.Endocrinology 120, 1846-1850.
prolactinsecretionduringthe breedingcycleof the ringdove Wentworth,B.C., Proudman,J. A., Opel,H., Wineland,N. J.,
(Spreptopelia risoria) and its possiblerolein incubation.Journal Zimmermann,N. G. &Lapp,A. (1983).Endocrinechangesin
of Endocrinology 110, 447 458. - incubatingand broodingturkey hen. Biology of Reproduction 29,
Lehrman,D. S.& Brody,P. N. (1964).Effectof prolactinon 87-92.
establishedincubationbehaviourin the ringdove.Journal of Wood-Gush,D. G. M. & Gilbert,A. B.(1973).Somehormones
Comparative and Physiological Psychology 57,161 165. - involvedin the nestingbehaviorof hens. Animal Behavior 21,
McNeilly,A. S.&Andrews,P. (1974).Purificationand characteri- 98--103.
zationof caprineprolactin. Journal of Endocrinology 60, 357-367. Wood-Gush,D. G. M. &Gilbert,A. B.(1975).The physiological
Maier, R. A. (1963).Maternalbehaviourin thedomestichen:the basisof a behaviourpattern in the domestichen. In Avian
role ofphysicalcontact. Journal of Comparative Physiology and Physiology, pp. 261-276.Ed. M. Peaker. London,NewYork:
Psychology 56, 357 361.
- AcademicPress.
Nalbandov,A. V.(1945).A studyof theeffectof prolactinon
broodinessand on cocktestis. Endocrinology 36, 251-258.

J. Endocr. (1988)118,279-286

You might also like