Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/331058587

Note-taking habits of 21st Century college students: implications for student


learning, memory, and achievement

Article  in  Memory · February 2019


DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2019.1569694

CITATIONS READS

30 9,622

5 authors, including:

Kayla Morehead Katherine A Rawson


Kent State University Kent State University
8 PUBLICATIONS   172 CITATIONS    147 PUBLICATIONS   6,594 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Note-taking View project

CLJs and self-regulation View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Kayla Morehead on 15 February 2019.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Memory

ISSN: 0965-8211 (Print) 1464-0686 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pmem20

Note-taking habits of 21st Century college


students: implications for student learning,
memory, and achievement

Kayla Morehead, John Dunlosky, Katherine A. Rawson, Rachael Blasiman &


R. Benjamin Hollis

To cite this article: Kayla Morehead, John Dunlosky, Katherine A. Rawson, Rachael Blasiman
& R. Benjamin Hollis (2019): Note-taking habits of 21st Century college students: implications for
student learning, memory, and achievement, Memory

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2019.1569694

Published online: 12 Feb 2019.

Submit your article to this journal

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=pmem20
MEMORY
https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2019.1569694

Note-taking habits of 21st Century college students: implications for student


learning, memory, and achievement
Kayla Moreheada, John Dunloskya, Katherine A. Rawsona, Rachael Blasimanb and R. Benjamin Hollisc
a
Department of Psychological Sciences, Kent State University, Kent, OH, USA; bDepartment of Psychology, Kent State University, Salem;
c
Office of Continuing & Distance Education, Kent State University, Kent, OH, USA

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


Students rely on their notes to memorise and learn critical course content, and recent studies of Received 6 June 2018
note-taking state that most students take notes, citing a survey published in 1974. Over the past Accepted 6 January 2019
four decades, classrooms and note-taking technologies have evolved: students can take notes
KEYWORDS
on electronic devices, and some classes are entirely online. Despite these changes, few studies Note-taking; note-taking
have investigated their impact on note-taking habits. Given that note-taking is critical for habits; survey; technology;
student learning, the purpose of the present study was to update the literature on students’ online courses
general note-taking behaviour and investigate the impact of these newer technological
advances on note-taking. We surveyed 577 college students, using questions from prior
surveys and new questions relevant to technology. As per prior studies, students still
reported taking notes in classrooms, and as important, students reported flexibility in their
note-taking by (a) using a notebook or a laptop depending on course demands and (b) often
deciding not to take notes in online courses. Thus, students are flexible in their note-taking
behaviour, but may not always make the best decisions about how and when to take notes.

With the exception of three reported studies concerning the considered best practices. To provide a context for the
utility of notetaking in the fifties … , little other recent attention present research, we first discuss what has changed since
in the area of student practices has been evident. The effort
reported here attempts to assess current student attitudes
1974 and then describe an empirical framework for evalu-
and practices in the area of notetaking, and to compare ating the effectiveness of students’ note-taking habits.
these to earlier findings. (Palmatier & Bennett, 1974, p. 215) Then, we briefly review prior survey studies (in the
context of the framework) and end with an overview of
This quote comes from one of the most recent large-scale
the present research.
surveys focused on student note-taking, which was pub-
lished in 1974. Noting the lack of relevant research from
Changes since 1974 that may Influence note-taking
the 1950s to the 1970s, Palmatier and Bennett raised a
habits
concern that “the appearance of the spiral-bound note-
book” (p. 216) would affect student note-taking. We So, what has happened since 1974 that may be relevant to
suspect that most researchers will agree that the techno- note-taking and the degree to which students are taking
logical advances introduced in education since 1974 far effective notes and learning from them? With respect to
exceed the advance of spiral-bound notebooks. Moreover, taking notes, unlike the first laptop computer (which was
recent survey research indicates that when students are introduced in 1981, had no battery, and weighed close to
preparing for high-stakes exams, they largely rely on mem- 25 pounds), modern laptop computers are lightweight
orising their notes (Karpicke, Butler, & Roediger, 2009; and can be readily used to take notes in the classroom.
Morehead, Rhodes, & DeLozier, 2016). Given that the More recently, eWriters (e.g., Boogie Board™ eWriters,
content of notes is foundational to what students learn, it which function as electronic paper) and other tablets
is important to understand when and where students (e.g., iPads, which have eWriting capabilities as well as
take notes as well as the strategies students use when other functions) now allow students to take notes long-
studying their notes to enhance memory and learning of hand without using paper. As for teaching technologies,
course content. Accordingly, the main goals of the many advances have been made that could impact
present research were to provide an extensive and up-to- whether and how students take notes. PowerPoint was
date survey of student note-taking habits – both for lec- introduced in 1990 and is now used by many teachers to
tures in classrooms and on-line courses – as well as to present course content during lectures. The internet has
compare student note-taking habits against what is led to enrollment in online courses in which students

CONTACT Kayla Morehead kmorehea@kent.edu Department of Psychological Sciences, Kent State University, Kent, OH 44240, USA
© 2019 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
2 K. MOREHEAD ET AL.

view lectures and course content online without being in a Concerning the encoding function, two aspects of note-
classroom. taking are arguably most relevant: (a) what method stu-
Given these advances in note-taking and instructional dents use to take notes and (b) whether students
technologies, one could safely argue that the need to attempt to organise their notes as they take them. With
update our knowledge about students’ note-taking behav- respect to note-taking methods, students can take notes
iour is greater now than in 1974 when the largest impact by longhand in a notebook or using an electronic device
on note-taking was the invention of the spiral notebook. (e.g., a tablet or eWriter), or they can type notes on a
Given that notes themselves represent the focal stimuli rel- laptop computer or tablet. In empirical research of which
evant to a great deal of student learning, understanding method is best, taking notes longhand has generally pro-
how students take notes and how they go about studying duced as good or better performance outcomes when
them when preparing for exams will have important impli- investigated in real-life courses and in the laboratory
cations for memory investigators who seek to improve (Carter, Greenberg, & Walker, 2017; Luo, Kiewra, Flanigan,
student achievement. Accordingly, we conducted a large- & Peteranetz, 2018; Mueller & Oppenheimer, 2014; for
scale survey of students’ note-taking that both connects one exception, see Bui, Myerson, & Hale, 2013). Using a
with earlier research (by using several of the survey ques- laptop in a classroom may be especially disruptive,
tions from prior research) and extends it (by surveying stu- because computer use (typing, access to the internet)
dents about note-taking with respect to the while taking notes can be distracting for the laptop user
aforementioned technological advances). Furthermore, and potentially for students sitting near them (e.g., Sana,
the present survey included more detailed questions Weston, & Cepeda, 2013). Thus, key questions concerning
about students’ note-taking beyond whether they take the encoding function of note-taking include, Will the
notes, so as to help memory investigators generate majority of students report taking notes longhand, or do
hypotheses and research ideas based on students’ many now use laptops? And, do any students use both
reported note-taking behaviour. note-taking methods, and if so, why?
The encoding function of note-taking may also be ben-
efitted by organising and transforming notes, versus just
copying a lecture verbatim. The idea here is that transform-
Empirical framework for interpreting students’
ing a lecture into one’s own words and organising (or inte-
note-taking habits
grating) the lecture content can boost performance, partly
Students largely self-regulate their note-taking. Teachers because such generative processes are more effortful than
may offer scaffolds (e.g., an outline of a lecture or copies passively copying a lecture. Better organisation (e.g., using
of PowerPoint slides) to help students take notes, but it is outlines or the Cornell note-taking method) may support
typically up to each student to decide how and when to more effective restudy and subsequently improve the
take notes. Unfortunately, the literature on self-regulated storage function. Thus, to investigate their approach to
learning has revealed that how students regulate their note-taking in the present survey, students who indicated
learning – including decisions about which strategies to taking notes also reported whether they tended to copy
use and when to use them – often does not match empiri- notes or organise them in some manner.
cal evidence about how best to regulate learning (Bjork, The effectiveness of the storage function is largely
Dunlosky, & Kornell, 2013). For instance, many students reflected by how students study their notes, and some
report preferring to prepare for exams by rereading their study strategies are better than others. For instance, to
textbooks instead of testing themselves (Gurung, enhance the storage function, students should test them-
Weidert, & Jeske, 2010; Hartwig & Dunlosky, 2012; More- selves on the content of notes instead of merely rereading
head et al., 2016), whereas evidence indicates that reread- them (e.g., Dunlosky et al., 2013; Roediger & Butler, 2011).
ing is less effective than self-testing (for reviews, see General surveys of students’ study behaviour suggest
Dunlosky, Rawson, Marsh, Nathan, & Willingham, 2013; that they tend to overly rely on less effective strategies
Roediger & Butler, 2011). (e.g., rereading), and outcomes from the present survey
Accordingly, to provide a framework for interpreting the provide converging evidence about how students use
outcomes from students’ reports of their note-taking their notes to prepare for exams.
habits, we first consider the relevant empirical evidence Finally, one novel aspect of the present research per-
concerning best practices. We also organised the frame- tains to revealing how students approach taking notes in
work on the distinction between the two main functions online courses, which could have implications for both
of note-taking: encoding and storage. The encoding func- the encoding and storage function of note-taking. One
tion concerns the degree to which the act of taking possibility is that some students will not take notes, with
notes improves student learning of the target materials, the idea that they will merely view the online lectures
whereas the storage function concerns how students sub- and supporting materials to prepare for exams. Recent
sequently attempt to learn from their notes and its impact research suggests that students who rely on reviewing
on test performance (for a review, see Kiewra, 1989; online lectures perform poorly (e.g., Liles, Vuk, & Tariq,
Kobayashi, 2005; Kobayashi, 2006). 2018). But, is it common for students to rely less on
MEMORY 3

note-taking for online than in-class lectures? To answer this such studies by Van Meter, Yokoi, and Pressley (1994) did
question, we compared reports of note-taking habits for so by interviewing 252 students. All students reported
those students who were enrolled in both kinds of taking notes. Most students reported that their note-
course when taking the survey. taking was directed towards the goal of doing well in a
Because students may not always make the best choices course. They reported taking notes for several reasons;
about how to take notes, we included several other ques- the most common reasons included paying attention
tions on the survey to provide possible insight into why during lectures, organising information from lectures, or
they might make sub-optimal decisions (e.g., Were they using notes as a study aid. They reported reviewing their
taught how to take notes?) and whether they are inter- notes by rereading or organising them. Consistent with
ested in learning how to improve their note-taking skills. this finding, Nandagopal and Ericsson (2012) reported
that 100% of students reported reviewing their notes,
and Tran and Lawson (2001) reported that reference and
Historical overview of survey research on note-
repetition were the most popular review strategies (44%
taking habits
and 76% respectively). Based on the aforementioned
The first two surveys on note-taking were conducted by empirical framework of best practices, many students par-
Charters (1924, 1925). The purpose of these studies was ticipating in this survey were not fully capitalising on the
to investigate student study behaviour, including note- storage function of note-taking.
taking. In both survey, female college students (44 in the A more recent survey of student note-taking, adminis-
first survey, 258 in the second) were interviewed about tered between 2012 and 2013 involving 435 students,
their study habits. In the first survey, most women reported reported that 99.6% of students took notes at least some
taking notes because it was often required and that note- of the time with 93.8% reporting taking notes often or
taking was useful for organising information in a brief always (as reported in a general chapter on note-taking
amount of time. Participants in the second survey reported by Peverly & Wolf, 2019). In the same survey, 96.5% of stu-
that they often kept their notes, thought notes were useful dents reported taking notes on paper at least sometimes
for review, and believed they would have done poorer on and 53.4% reported taking notes on a computer at least
exams if they had not taken notes. Despite believing note- sometimes. In another survey by Aguilar-Roca, Williams,
taking was important, 68% reported receiving no instruc- and O’Dowd (2012), 50% of students reported taking
tion on how to take notes. The next survey (Danskin & notes on paper and about 22% reported taking notes on
Burnett, 1952) investigated what study techniques superior a laptop. Although a smaller percentage reported using
students used, including how they took notes. The laptops, as noted in the framework above, these students
researchers interviewed 37 students who were in the top may be at a disadvantage, and so might students sitting
of their class. Eighty-nine percent of students reported near them during in-class lectures.
taking notes, and 86% reported organising their notes Of course, these outcomes support the anecdotal obser-
while taking them. No survey research on note-taking vations of anyone who has recently taught (or taken) an in-
behaviour was conducted again until 1974 when Palmatier class lecture in college: most students take notes in some
and Bennett administered the first large-scale survey (N = fashion or another. But, do they also take notes for lectures
223) pertaining specifically to student note-taking. They during online courses, and have they begun to adopt
asked participants how they took notes and about their newer technologies (e.g., eWriters or tablets) while taking
use of various note-taking techniques. Ninety-nine notes? Given that most students take and review notes,
percent of students reported taking notes during lectures, how they engage in note-taking with technology has
and 96% believed that note-taking was essential for important consequences for classroom learning.
success in college. Students reported taking notes in
spiral notebooks, on loose-leaf paper, or on legal pads.
Overview of present survey of note-taking habits
Since this survey, few other studies of note-taking
behaviour have been conducted, and these typically did Given that few large-scale surveys have focused exclusively
not include questions relevant to recent technological on students’ note-taking (with one of the largest scale
advances.1 Nevertheless, we discuss the subset of these surveys being published over forty years ago), and only a
surveys from which we adapted questions for the few surveys have addressed the impact of technology on
present survey. Hartley and Davies (1978) administered a note-taking (e.g., Aguilar-Roca et al., 2012; Fried, 2008;
survey to investigate why students take notes. They sur- Peverly & Wolf, 2019), our goal for the present investigation
veyed 52 American students about their note-taking was to update the literature by surveying students about
beliefs and skills. Ninety-eight percent of participants their note-taking and the impact of technology on their
believed note-taking was important, 56% reported receiv- note-taking, including the use of electronic devices in
ing instruction on note-taking, and 67% wished they had class and note-taking in online courses. We recruited a
better note-taking skills. large sample by collecting data from students at Kent
Other research has attempted to develop theories of State University on the main campus, on the regional cam-
note-taking based on student perceptions. The first of puses, and enrolled in online courses.
4 K. MOREHEAD ET AL.

To develop survey questions, we used questions from time completing the survey were excluded from analyses.
prior surveys and developed new questions to assess the Data for one participant who did not finish the survey
use of technology in the classroom and note-taking in were also excluded.
online courses. Many questions focused on how students Demographic information was similar across all samples
take and review notes to emphasise the encoding function (see Table 1), as were general results. Because our aim was
(taking notes) and storage function (reviewing notes) as to collect a large sample and results for all three samples
described above. Given that many experimental studies were similar, their data were combined for a total sample
have investigated these functions of note-taking, we size of 577.2
asked students several questions about how they
engaged in both functions. We also asked participants
Materials
about their note-taking skills and whether they had
received training on note-taking. We describe these ques- The survey consisted of 55 questions (see Appendix for a
tions more fully in the Method section. list of all questions asked), but some questions were con-
To summarise, our main goals were to provide an ditional based on answers to prior questions. For
extensive and up-to-date survey of student note-taking instance, if students reported not taking notes in class,
habits. Based on prior evidence, we expected most stu- they did not answer questions about note-taking behav-
dents to report taking notes in a class and studying iour in class. Therefore, most participants did not answer
them in a relatively ineffective manner. Our empirical fra- every question. In the next sections, we discuss the
mework also emphasises the importance of understand- origin of and rationale for the questions used in the
ing the use of technology, because its ineffective use present survey.
could result in poor learning. For instance, we expected
at least some students to report using laptops (which
Table 1. Demographic Information from All Samples.
can undermine performance), but only a few surveys
Main
have investigated how widespread laptops are being Question Choices Campus Regional Online
used in classrooms. Because online courses provide Gender (Q1)a Male 24 19 41
video content that students can review if they would Female 75 78 58
like, we expected some students to report not taking Other 1 3 1
notes in these courses. Again, not taking notes would Age (Q2) Under 18 0 5 0
be expected to limit their learning, but how widespread 18–22 97 73 95
this potential problem may be is currently unknown. 23–25 2 5 3
25–30 0 5 2
30–40 0 5 0
40+ 0 5 0
Method
Ethnicity (Q3) Hispanic/ 2 0 2
Participants Latino
Native 0 0 0
Participants were recruited from three groups of under- American
Asian 3 0 4
graduate students at Kent State University. All students African 10 8 13
completed the survey for course credit. The first group American
(N = 312) consisted of students enrolled in psychology Middle Eastern 2 3 4
Caucasian 83 89 77
courses on the main campus. The second group (N = 37)
consisted of students enrolled in psychology courses on 1st Language English Yes 97 100 91
the regional campuses. Although students in these (Q4) No 3 0 9
cohorts may have been enrolled in online courses and Year (Q7) Freshmen 47 35 60
hence could answer questions about their note-taking in Sophomore 18 24 27
this venue, we also wanted to ensure enough students Junior 17 22 10
Senior 18 19 3
who participated were enrolled in online courses. Thus, a
third group (N = 238) was sampled from students who High School GPA (Q8) 1.0–1.9 0 0 0
were enrolled in at least one web-enhanced course 2.0–2.9 7 11 13
3.0–3.5 46 24 45
where lectures were online. All students signed up for 3.6–4.0 47 65 43
the survey through the university’s SONA system. Although
the first two groups consisted of students enrolled in psy- College GPA (Q9) 0.0–0.9 0 0 0
1.0–1.9 0 5 1
chology courses, most students in the sample were not 2.0–2.9 18 16 14
psychology majors. Across all three groups, only 17% of 3.0–3.5 28 27 30
participants (101) were psychology majors. 3.6–4.0 17 27 10
1st Semester 37 24 44
Nine students completed the survey through both the
Note. Numbers reported are percentages of total N per sample. aQ refers to
main campus and online group. Because these students the actual questions to which participants responded, which are presented
completed the survey twice, their data from the second in the Appendix.
MEMORY 5

Encoding and storage then answered questions about note-taking including


Concerning encoding, students were asked about their questions about the encoding function of notes (in class-
note-taking behaviour during both in-class and online lec- rooms, during online classes, and in general), questions
tures. Some questions were adapted from prior surveys. about the storage function of notes, and questions about
These questions included: whether students took notes their note-taking skills and training.
in class, if they believed that note-taking was essential for
effective learning, if they took notes differently depending
Results
on the lecture, and if they tried to organise their notes
(questions adapted from Bonner & Holliday, 2006; We present responses to survey questions in Tables 2–8,
Danskin & Burnett, 1952; Dunkel & Davy, 1989; Fried, and we consider the outcomes in relation to the aforemen-
2008; Hartley & Davies, 1978; Palmatier & Bennett, 1974; tioned empirical framework in the Discussion section.
Van Meter et al., 1994). Other questions were developed Given that not all students answered every question
by the authors for the present survey and included what (because the presentation of some questions was con-
devices students used to take notes, if they took notes ditional on prior answers), we also included the number
differently in smaller classes, and if having PowerPoint out- of respondents (in parentheses) for each question. Impor-
lines affected their note-taking behaviour. Critically, we tantly, in most tables, we included not only responses
developed questions about note-taking behaviour during from the present survey but also the responses from any
online lectures to parallel questions about note-taking prior surveys that included similar questions about note-
during in-class lectures to investigate whether note- taking. Results from research published in 2000 or later
taking behaviour differed for online classes. The final set are presented under columns labelled “twenty-first
of items consisted of a series of true/false statements Century”, and results from research published before
adapted from Hartley and Davies (1978) and Dunkel and 2000 are presented under columns labelled “twentieth
Davy (1989). These items were statements about note- Century”. Accordingly, a quick scan of these tables provides
taking behaviour, and students were asked to decide a comparison across survey studies and highlights those
whether the statements were true of themselves. We also questions and responses that are unique to the present
created a new item about whether students played survey. In the rest of the Results section, we highlight a
games or were on the internet on their laptops during few of the notable outcomes. We encourage readers to
class. review the tables for further details.
Concerning storage, students were asked if they
reviewed their notes and believed that reviewing their
Encoding
notes was important for learning. If they reported review-
ing their notes, they were asked how they reviewed Classroom lectures
them. For this question, they were given several options Results for questions pertaining to note-taking in class-
from which to choose, such as rereading notes, highlight- rooms are presented in Table 2. Consistent with prior
ing or marking them, or testing themselves on their research (Bonner & Holliday, 2006; Dunkel & Davy, 1989;
notes (adapted from Badger, White, Sutherland, & Haggis, Hartley & Davies, 1978; Palmatier & Bennett, 1974), most
2001; Hartwig & Dunlosky, 2012; Morehead et al., 2016; students reported taking notes in class (96%), whereas
Tran & Lawson, 2001; Van Meter et al., 1994). 88% of participants reported that taking notes was necess-
ary for effective learning. Most students reported taking
notes longhand in a notebook (86%), but some (46%)
Note-taking skills and training
reported taking notes on a laptop. Thirty-two percent of
Finally, students were asked if they believed they took
the students reported taking notes both in a notebook
good notes and if they wished they had better note-
and on a laptop (not presented in Table 2); these students
taking skills. They were then asked if they had received
were asked to explain why they used more than one
training on how to take notes (questions adapted from
device. When asked to explain, they reported using a
Dunkel & Davy, 1989; Hartley & Davies, 1978; Palmatier &
laptop when the instructor spoke quickly (20%), using a
Bennett, 1974). If they responded yes to this question,
laptop when PowerPoint slides were available (20%), or
they were asked to explain when they were taught, who
that it depended on the class (20%; values not reported
taught them, and for how long they were taught.
in Table 2).
Fifty-six percent of students reported trying to organise
their notes rather than merely copying what the teacher
Procedure
said. Seventy-four percent reported taking notes differently
This survey was approved by the university Institutional based on the class. When asked to explain, these student
Review Board. Participants completed the survey at their reported changing their note-taking behaviour depending
convenience by following a link to the survey on Qualtrics. on the teacher’s style (21%), the discipline (e.g., math vs.
Participants took nine minutes on average to complete the psychology; 18%), or whether outlines or PowerPoint
survey. They first completed demographic information, slides were available online (17%; values not in Table 2).
6 K. MOREHEAD ET AL.

Table 2. Note-taking in Classroom Lectures.


Question Choices Present Survey Twenty-first Century Twentieth Century
Do you take notes? (Q10) Yes 96 (552) 91–99.63,12 89–1004,11,14

Do you feel that taking notes is necessary? (Q12) Yes 88 (510) -- 94–985,7,11

What do you use to take notes? (Q15) Notebook 86 (477) 50–971,12 10011
Laptop 46 (255) 22–641,6,12 --
Tablet 1 (7) 612 --
eWriter 0 (1) -- --
Other 1 (8) -- --

Do you try to organise your notes or copy what the Organise 56 (307) 783 864
teacher says? (Q19) Copy 30 (164) -- --
Depends 15 (81) -- --

How do you organise your notes? (Q20) Rephrase 36 (178) -- --


Bulleted list/Outline 85 (412) -- --
Cornell Notes 4 (19) -- --
Mapping 6 (27) -- --
Other 3 (15) -- --

Do you take notes differently depending on the class? (Q21) Yes 74 (408) -- 83–965,7

Do you take more notes in smaller classes? (Q23) Yes 30 (163) -- --

Do you take notes in some lectures, but not others? (Q11) Yes 37 (212) -- --

What else do you take notes on? (Q24) Textbook Readings 86 (477) -- --
Other Readings 46 (255) -- --
In-class Activities 1 (7) -- --
Class Videos 0 (1) -- --
Other 1 (8) -- --
Note. Main values are percentages, and the numbers in parentheses are the number of respondents. The last two columns present data from prior
research reflecting questions similar to those asked in the present survey. Superscripts denote which studies were pooled for values in the table.
Only studies reporting quantitative outcomes were included. For some questions, responses do not add to 100% because students could select mul-
tiple options. We did not include the percentage responding no for yes/no options.

Online lectures students who enroll in both kinds of course do not take
Thirty-eight percent of participants (n = 220) reported notes differently in general, but take notes substantially
being enrolled in a course where most or all of the lectures less often in their online courses than in their in-class
were online. Results for questions pertaining to note-taking lectures.
during online lectures are presented in Table 3. Unlike in-
class lectures, only about half of the students reported
General encoding questions
taking notes during online lectures (49%), but 58%
reported that note-taking was necessary for effective learn- Results for general encoding questions are reported in
ing. When asked why they did not take notes, 67% of these Table 5. Most students reported that their teachers pro-
students reported not taking notes because the lecture vided PowerPoint slides (70%), and over half of those
material is available online as either video lectures or students reported that they took notes differently
slides, so they believed taking notes was unnecessary. when PowerPoint slides were provided than when they
Another 20% reported not taking notes because they were not, either reporting that they took notes on the
were bored or lazy (not reported in Table 3). Of those provided handouts (49%) or did not take notes at all
who reported taking notes, most reported using a note- (16%). They also reported that when PowerPoint slides
book (86%; 20% reported using a notebook and a laptop) were provided they took the same amount of notes
and trying to organise their notes (70%). (48%) or less notes (41%). Also, the majority (66%) of stu-
It is possible that students enrolled in online classes dents reported not learning about Cornell notes,
differ from those who only attend in-class lectures. suggesting that this organisational technique is not
Hence, we also explored how students enrolled in online being widely used in this cohort.
classes took notes in their in-class lectures. Results are pre- Responses to the true/false statements adapted from
sented in Table 4, which includes in-class and online Dunkel and Davy (1989) and Hartley and Davies (1978)
lecture outcomes from only students enrolled in online are presented at the bottom of Table 5. Most results
classes. Note that results for in-class lectures are similar were similar across surveys. However, results differed for
to results for all participants (Table 2), indicating that two items in that, compared to prior research, more
MEMORY 7

Table 3. Note-taking in Online Lectures. Storage function of note-taking


Online
Question Choices Lectures Results for questions pertaining to the storage function of
Do you take notes? (Q26) Yes 49 (107) notes are presented in Table 6. Most students reported
reviewing their notes (93%), and most of these students
Do you feel that taking notes is Yes 58 (127)
(92%) reported doing so by reading their notes. Although
necessary? (Q28)
less popular than rereading, about half of the students
Do you feel that reviewing notes is Yes 80 (175) reported testing themselves on their notes.
necessary? (Q29)

What do you use to take notes? (Q30) Notebook 86 (92)


Laptop 32 (34) Note-taking skills and training
Tablet 2 (2)
eWriter 0 (0) Responses to questions pertaining to note-taking skills and
Other 1 (1) training are presented in Table 7. Most students reported
taking good notes (89%), although 58% reported
Do you try to organise your notes or Organise 70 (75)
copy what the teacher says? (Q33) Copy 25 (27) wanting better note-taking skills. Forty-eight percent of
Depends 5 (5) students reported being taught note-taking skills, and of
these students, most reported being taught note-taking
How do you organise your notes? (Q34) Rephrase 42 (45)
Bulleted list/ 83 (89) skills in middle school or high school by a teacher.
Outline
Cornell Notes 1 (1)
Mapping 6 (6) Gender differences in note-taking
Other 2 (2)
Prior research has revealed gender differences in note-
What else do you take notes on? (Q35) Textbook Readings 72 (68)
Other Readings 32 (30)
taking behaviour. For example, Reddington, Peverly, and
Online 46 (44) Block (2015) reported that females recorded significantly
demonstrations more information in their notes than did males. Because
Other videos 33 (31)
Websites 25 (24)
gender differences have been reported in prior research,
Note. Main values are percentages, and the numbers in parentheses are the
we investigated them in the present research. In particular,
number of respondents. we compared responses on some questions that may differ
between males and females based on prior research. These
results are presented in Table 8, and for interested readers, a
students in the present investigation reported (1) that they table of comparisons for all questions can be obtained from
would not take notes if they did not have to take exams the first author. As shown in Table 8, a few differences
and (2) that they took notes to avoid reading the textbook. occurred. Taking notes during in-class lectures was reported
more often by females than males (χ2 (2, n = 577) = 7.99, p
Table 4. In-class and Onine Lecture results for Students who Reported taking
< .05), and believing that note-taking during in-class lec-
Online Classes. tures is necessary for effective learning was reported more
In-class Online often by females than males (χ2 (2, n = 577) = 24.48, p
Question Choices Lectures Lectures < .001). When PowerPoint slides were provided, females
Do you take notes during Yes 96 (210) 49 (107) were more likely than males to say they took notes on hand-
lectures?
outs and less likely to say they took fewer notes (χ2 (2, n =
Do you feel that taking notes Yes 88 (193) 58 (127) 565) = 13.07, p < .05). Finally, as compared to males, more
is necessary for effective females reported taking good notes (χ2 (2, n = 577) =
learning?
14.82, p < .01). Although these results were exploratory,
What do you use to take Notebook 86 (180) 86 (92) they are consistent with prior research on gender differ-
notes? Laptop 55 (115) 31.8 (34) ences in note-taking.
Tablet 2 (4) 1.9 (2)
eWriter 0 (0) 0 (0)
Other 2 (4) 0.9 (1)
Discussion
Do you try to organise your Organise 58 (121) 70 (75)
notes or copy what the Copy 30 (63) 25 (27) To overview, the present research investigated students’
teacher says? Depends 12 (26) 5 (5) note-taking behaviour both for traditional in-class lectures
and online lectures. Most students reported taking notes
If you organise your notes, Rephrase 27 (56) 21 (45)
how do you do so? Bulleted list/ 74 (156) 42 (89) and believing note-taking was essential for effective learn-
Outline ing. Moreover, when taking notes, the majority of students
Cornell Notes 2 (5) 0.5 (1) reported organising them during in-class lectures (56%,
Mapping 6 (13) 3 (6)
Other 2 (5) 1 (2) Table 2) and online lectures (70%, Table 3). Although
Note. Main values are percentages, and the numbers in parentheses are the such organisation may not always support better perform-
number of respondents. ance (see discussion of the empirical framework in the
8 K. MOREHEAD ET AL.

Table 5. General Encoding Questions.


Twenty-first Twentieth
Question Choices Present Survey Century Century
Do your professors provide PowerPoint slides? (Q36) Yes 70 (402) -- --
No 2 (12) -- --
Some do 28 (163) -- --

Do you take notes differently when PowerPoint Yes, I take notes on handouts. 49 (277) -- --
slides are available? (Q37) Yes, I do not take notes. 16 (90) -- --
No 35 (198) -- --

Do you take the same amount of notes when PowerPoint Yes 48 (270) -- --
slides are available? (Q38) No, I take less notes 41 (231) -- --
No, I take more notes 11 (64) -- --

Have you heard of Cornell notes? (Q39) Yes 34 (197) -- --


No 66 (380) -- --

I sometimes find my note-taking interferes with my immediate True 67 (384) -- 54–605,7


understanding of the lecture. (Q53)

If I did not have to take exams, I would not take notes. (Q54) True 52 (297) -- 29–385,7

I take notes so that I will not have to read the textbook. (Q55) True 46 (266) -- 10–155,7

I often doodle instead of taking notes. (Q51) True 28 (161) -- 25–305,7

I am often on the internet or playing games instead of taking notes. (Q52) True 22 (124) -- --
Note. Main values are percentages, and the numbers in parentheses are the number of respondents.

Introduction), it does suggest that most students attempt students reported flexibility in their note-taking. Some stu-
to actively process lecture content versus passively copy. dents reported using both notebooks and laptops to take
And, as indicated by directly comparing the present out- notes, and most students who reported using both also
comes to those of prior surveys (Tables 2 and 3, Tables indicated that they chose which method to use based on
5–7), these reports are consistent with the general note- how quickly the instructor spoke or whether PowerPoints
taking literature that demonstrates how much students slides were available. These results suggest that at least
value taking notes during in-class lectures and reviewing some students attempt to regulate their note-taking so
them to prepare for exams. as to increase the chances that their notes are complete.
Other results are relatively unique to the present survey, These responses also suggest that the relative efficacy of
and we will highlight those outcomes with respect to the note-taking devices may depend on the lecturer style
empirical framework. Concerning the encoding function, and course material. That is, using a laptop to take notes
what method do students report using while taking may be beneficial in some circumstances, such as when
notes? A key outcome of the present research is that lectures do not include visuals (that would need to be

Table 6. Storage Function of Note-taking.


Questions Choices Present Survey Twenty-first Century Twentieth Century
Do you review notes? (Q42) Yes 93 (539) 10010 4814

How do you review notes? (Q43) Rereading 92 (497) 28–762,13 --


Make flashcards 56 (303)
13
Highlight/mark them 56 (302) 9 --
Test myself 56 (300)
Copy them 36 (193) 28–332,8,9 1214
Summarise notes 29 (157) -- --
Add questions 9 (47) -- --
Other 2 (10) -- --

If you take notes, why do you do so? (Q41) So I can review them later 85 (490) 2113 96–985.7
To aid learning 79 (454) -- --
It helps me learn by writing 63 (365) -- --
Because I have to take exams 63 (362) 1513 297
So I pay attention during the lecture 62 (356) -- --
Because it is expected 21 (123) -- --
Because the teacher can see me 6 (34) -- --
Other 1 (6) -- --
I do not take notes 2 (9) -- --
Note. Main values are percentages, and the numbers in parentheses are the number of respondents.
MEMORY 9

Table 7. Note-taking Skills and Training


Twenty-first Twentieth
Question Choices Present Survey Century Century
Do you take good notes? (Q40) Yes 89 (511) 9412 --

Do you wish you had better note-taking skills? (Q48) Yes 58 (335) -- 59–775,7

Were note-taking skills taught to you? (Q44) Yes 48 (276) -- 17–565,7,11

When were you taught note-taking skills? (Q45) Elementary school 26 (72) -- --
Middle school 65 (180) -- --
High school 76 (210) -- --
Community college 2 (6) -- --
College 18 (50) -- --
Other 1 (3) -- --

Who taught you note-taking skills? (Q46) Teacher 96 (266) -- --


Professor 18 (49) -- --
Parent or Guardian 23 (64) -- --
Other 3 (7) -- --

For how long were you taught note-taking skills? (Q47) Part of a class period 31 (85) -- --
An entire class period 18 (49) -- --
A few class periods 45 (124) -- --
A month 4 (10) -- --
A semester 13 (36) -- --
Note. Main values are percentages, and the numbers in parentheses are the number of respondents.

illustrated by hand) or when a lecturer speaks quickly. because they believed that it was unnecessary to do so
Given that some students reported using multiple note- given that the lectures are always available, suggesting
taking methods, future research should investigate the that they believe that notes only function as records of
degree to which students’ choices are adaptive – that is, lecture content that would be lost otherwise. However,
do they choose the most effective method for a particular some note-taking studies have reported at least some
context? benefit for taking notes compared to listening to a
Students reported flexibility in note-taking behaviour, lecture (see Kiewra, 1989; Kobayashi, 2005 for review). By
but is this flexibility always beneficial? For instance, about choosing not to take notes in online courses, students
half of the participants reported not taking notes in may be negatively impacting their subsequent memory
online courses even though most of these students for the course content, especially if they do not review
reported taking notes during in-class lectures (Tables 3 the video lectures (e.g., Liles et al., 2018). Even if they do,
and 4). They often explained that they did not take notes merely viewing the video again may be analogous to

Table 8. Responses by Gender.


Question Choices Male Female Other
Do you take notes in class? (Q10) Yes 92 (163) 97 (383) 100 (6)

Do you feel that note-taking is necessary for effective learning? (Q12) Yes 79 (139) 93 (365) 100 (6)

Do you take notes differently when PowerPoints are available? (Q37) Yes, I take notes on handouts 39 (66) 54 (209) 33 (2)
Yes, I do not take notes 22 (37) 13 (52) 17 (1)
No 40 (68) 33 (127) 50 (3)

Do you take good notes? (Q40) Yes 82 (145) 92 (362) 67 (4)

What do you use to take notes in class? (Q15) Notebook 90 (146) 85 (326) 83 (5)
Laptop 39 (63) 49 (189) 50 (3)
Tablet 2 (4) 1 (3) 0 (0)
eWriter 1 (1) 0 (0) 0 (0)
Other 0 (0) 2 (7) 17 (1)

Do you take notes online? (Q26) Yes 39 (24) 52 (82) 50 (1)

Do you feel note-taking online is necessary for effective learning? (Q28) Yes 53 (32) 60 (94) 50 (1)

Do you feel reviewing notes online is necessary for effective learning? (Q29) Yes 72 (43) 82 (130) 100 (2)

Do you review your notes? (Q42) Yes 90 (160) 95 (373) 100 (6)
Note. Main values are percentages, and the numbers in parentheses are the number of respondents.
10 K. MOREHEAD ET AL.

rereading and hence would be a relatively ineffective strat- note-taking? For instance, is using an eWriter to take notes
egy (e.g., Dunlosky et al., 2013); moreover, reviewing a comparable to taking notes longhand on paper for both
video may be less efficient than studying one’s notes, encoding and storage functions? Third, over half of students
assuming that not all of the content in the lecture is critical reported never being taught note-taking skills, and many
material. One straightforward implication is that many stu- who did report training indicated that they received instruc-
dents will need to be instructed to take notes while watch- tion in middle school and high school when note-taking
ing to-be-tested content from online courses. may not be critical in many courses. So, how and when
Concerning the storage function (Table 6), when stu- should students be taught note-taking skills?
dents review their notes, the majority of students (92%) A limitation of the present research is that some changes
report rereading them. Although this strategy is relatively in reported note-taking habits that have arisen since the
ineffective on its own, over half of the students also 1970s may not be due to historical cohort effects per se
reported using flashcards (56%) or testing themselves but may have resulted from other differences (e.g., demo-
(56%), which are normatively more effective strategies. Of graphic characteristics of the samples) across the survey
course, students must use self-testing appropriately to studies. Note, however, that the similarities in reported
improve achievement. For instance, with respect to flash- note-taking habits across decades seem more prominent
cards, students should continue testing themselves in a than do the differences, with the latter largely arising from
session until they can correctly recall answers and return the development of new technologies (e.g., laptops) that
to re-learn them (through testing and feedback) in were not available in the 1970s. Another limitation is that
another session (for an overview of this use of self- the survey was conducted at one large state-supported uni-
testing, called successive relearning, see Dunlosky & versity in the midwest of the United States. A question arises
Rawson, 2015). Unfortunately, in flashcard research using about whether students’ note-taking habits would be sub-
authentic materials, it has taken students considerable stantively different in other contexts, such as at private uni-
amount of practice to learn and correctly recall a small versities, in different countries and cultures, and so forth.
set of conceptual definitions (e.g., Dunlosky & Rawson, The present survey could be adapted to explore these poss-
2015; Janes, Dunlosky, & Rawson, 2018). So, when students ible differences in future survey research.
use flashcards to test themselves on difficult concepts
during a brief, single session, they benefit little to none
from doing so (e.g., Lin, McDaniel, & Miyatsu, 2018). Closing Remarks
Future research should be aimed at exploring the details
In closing, classrooms have changed in many ways since
of how (and how often) students use self-testing to study
1974, with electronic note-taking devices, PowerPoint,
their notes and whether they effectively use instantiations
and online courses all potentially affecting student note-
of self-testing (e.g., successive relearning) in a manner that
taking. For the most part, students take notes now as
can enhance their learning and achievement.
they did in the 1970s (even though more technology is
Finally, it is perhaps not too surprising that some stu-
available for note-taking than ever before). However, stu-
dents’ reported note-taking habits do not match best prac-
dents reported flexibility in their note-taking, as they
tices, because about half (52%) of the students reported
reported taking notes differently (or not at all) based on
that they had never been taught note-taking skills. And,
characteristics of their classes, and many students reported
for those who did receive training, the amount of training
not taking notes during online lectures even if they
was often meager – just a part of a class or in only a few
reported taking notes during in-class lectures. How will
class periods (Table 7). Fortunately, close to 60% of the stu-
the classroom change over the next forty years? What
dents indicated that they wished they had better note-
note-taking devices will be available to students, and
taking habits, so many students would likely be receptive
how will they affect how and when students take notes?
to further instruction and training.
Or – more problematic given that less than half of students
report taking notes in online courses – as more content
Future directions and Limitations becomes available online, will students take notes at all?
Given the importance of effective note-taking to memory
Results from the present survey suggest other future direc-
for and learning of course content, continuing to
tions for note-taking research. First, as noted above, stu-
examine student note-taking behaviour as technologies
dents reported flexibility in their note-taking, but their
change will be an important avenue for future research.
decisions on how to take notes may not always be
effective. For example, students reported using laptops
when the lecturer spoke quickly or covered a lot of Notes
material, but no research has investigated whether using
laptops to take notes is advantageous under these circum- 1. Surveys that included only a few questions about note-taking
are not discussed here (Carrier, Williams, & Dalgaard, 1988;
stances. Second, few students reported using eWriters or Isaacs, 1994; Kay & Lauricella, 2014).
tablets for note-taking, but as these devices become more 2. Outcomes separated by sample are available from the first
popular, how will they affect the amount and quality of author.
MEMORY 11

Acknowledgements techniques: Promising directions from cognitive and educational


psychology. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 14, 4–58.
This research was supported by a National Science Foundation (NSF) doi:10.1177/1529100612453266
grant, STTR Phase II: Digital e-Writer for the Classroom, Grant 6
Fried, C. B. (2008). In-class laptop use and its effects on student learn-
Number 413328. Any opinions, findings, conclusions or recommen- ing. Computers & Education, 50, 906–914. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.
dations expressed in this material are those of the author(s) and do 2006.09.006
not necessarily reflect the views of the NSF. The authors have no Gurung, R. A. R., Weidert, J., & Jeske, A. (2010). Focusing on how stu-
financial or non-financial interest in the materials discussed in this dents study. Journal of the Scholarship of Teaching and Learning,
manuscript. Many thanks to Asad Khan, Annette Kratcoski, Duane Mar- 10, 28–35.
hefka, Erica Montbach, and Todd Packer for support and encourage- 7
Hartley, J., & Davies, I. K. (1978). Note-taking: A critical review.
ment with this project. Programmed Learning and Educational Technology, 15, 207–224.
doi:10.1080/0033039780150305
8
Hartwig, M. K., & Dunlosky, J. (2012). Study strategies of college stu-
Disclosure statement dents: Are self-testing and scheduling related to achievement?
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 126–134. doi:10.3758/s13423-
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
011-0181-y
Isaacs, G. (1994). Lecturing practices and note-taking purposes. Studies
in Higher Education, 19, 203–216.
Funding Janes, J. L., Dunlosky, J., & Rawson, K. A. (2018). How do students use
This work was supported by National Science Foundation (NSF): [Grant self-testing across multiple study sessions when preparing for a
Number 413328]. high-stakes exam? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and
Cognition, 7, 230–240. doi:10.1016/j.jarmac.2017.11.003
Karpicke, J. D., Butler, A. C., & Roediger IIIH. L. (2009). Metacognitive
strategies in student learning: Do students practise retrieval when
References
they study on their own? Memory (Hove, England), 17, 471–479.
1
Aguilar-Roca, N. M., Williams, A. E., & O’Dowd, D. K. (2012). The impact doi:10.1080/09658210802647009
of laptop-free zones on student performance and attitudes in large Kay, R. H., & Lauricella, S. (2014). Investigating the benefits and
lectures. Computers & Education, 59, 1300–1308. doi:10.1016/j. challenges of using laptop computers in higher education.
compedu.2012.05.002 Canadian Journal of Learning and Technology, 40, 1–25. doi:10.
2
Badger, R., White, G., Sutherland, P., & Haggis, T. (2001). Note perfect: 21432/T2VC76
An investigation of how students view taking notes in lectures. Kiewra, K. A. (1989). A review of note-taking: The encoding-storage
System, 29, 405–417. doi:10.1016/S0346-251X(01)00028-8 paradigm and beyond. Educational Psychology Review, 1, 147–172.
Bjork, R. A., Dunlosky, J., & Kornell, N. (2013). Self-regulated learning: doi:10.1007/BF01326640
Beliefs, techniques, and illusions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, Kobayashi, K. (2005). What limits the encoding effect of note-taking? A
417–444. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143823 meta-analytic examination. Contemporary Educational Psychology,
3
Bonner, J. M., & Holliday, W. G. (2006). How college science students 30, 242–262. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2004.10.001
engage in note-taking strategies. Journal of Research in Science Kobayashi, K. (2006). Combined effects of note-taking/reviewing on
Teaching, 43, 786–818. doi:10.1002/tea.20115 learning and the enhancement through interventions: A meta-ana-
Bui, D. C., Myerson, J., & Hale, S. (2013). Note-taking with computers: lytic review. Educational Psychology, 26, 459–477. doi:10.1080/
Exploring alternative strategies for improved recall. Journal of 01443410500342070
Educational Psychology, 105, 299–309. doi:10.1037/a0030367 Liles, J., Vuk, J., & Tariq, S. (2018). Study habits of medical students: An
Carrier, C. A., Williams, M. D., & Dalgaard, B. R. (1988). College students’ analysis of which study habits most contribute to success in the
perceptions of notetaking and their relationship to selected learner preclinical years. MedEdPublish, 7, 1–16. doi:10.15694/mep.2018.
characteristics and course achievement. Research in Higher 0000061.1
Education, 28, 223–239. Lin, C., McDaniel, M. A., & Miyatsu, T. (2018). Effects of flashcards
Carter, S. P., Greenberg, K., & Walker, M. S. (2017). The impact of com- on learning authentic materials: The role of detailed versus con-
puter usage on academic performance: Evidence from a random- ceptual flashcards and invidiaul differences in structure-building
ized trial at the United States Military Academy. Economics of ability. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 7,
Education Review, 56, 118–132. doi:1016/j.econedurev.2016.12.005 529–539.
Charters, J. A. (1924). Methods of study used by college women. The Luo, L., Kiewra, K. A., Flanigan, A. E., & Peteranetz, M. S. (2018). Laptop
Journal of Educational Research, 10, 344–355. doi:10.1080/ versus longhand note taking: Effects on lecture notes and achieve-
00220671.1924.10879513 ment. Instructional Science, 1–25. doi:10.1007/s11251-018-9458-0
9
Charters, J. A. (1925). How two hundred and fifty-eight junior college Morehead, K., Rhodes, M. G., & DeLozier, S. (2016). Instructor and
women study. The Journal of Educational Research, 11, 41–48. student knowledge of study strategies. Memory (Hove, England),
doi:10.1080/00220671.1925.10879527 24, 257–271. doi:10.1080/09658211.2014.1001992
4
Danskin, D. G., & Burnett, C. W. (1952). Study techniques of: Those Mueller, P. A., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2014). The pen is mightier than
superior students. Journal of Counseling & Development, 31, 181– the keyboard: Advantages of longhand over laptop note taking.
186. doi:10.1002/j.2164-4918.1952.tb01433.x Psychological Science, 25, 1159–1168. doi:10.1177/
5
Dunkel, P., & Davy, S. (1989). The heuristic of lecture notetaking: 0956797614524581
10
Perceptions of American & international students regarding the Nandagopal, K., & Ericsson, K. A. (2012). An expert performance
value & practice of notetaking. English for Specific Purposes, 8, 33– approach to the study of individual differences in self-regulated
50. doi:10.1016/0889-4906(89)90005-7 learning activities in upper-level college students. Learning
Dunlosky, J., & Rawson, K. A. (2015). Practice tests, spaced practice, and and Individual Differences, 22, 597–609. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2011.11.
successive relearning: Tips for classroom use and for guiding stu- 018
11
dents’ learning. Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Palmatier, R. A., & Bennett, J. M. (1974). Notetaking habits of college
Psychology, 1, 72–78. students. Journal of Reading, 18, 215–218. Retrieved from http://
Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Marsh, E. J., Nathan, M. J., & Willingham, D. T. www.jstor.org/stable/40009958.
(2013). Improving students’ learning with effective learning
12 K. MOREHEAD ET AL.

12
Peverly, S. T., & Wolf, A. D. (2019). Note-taking. In J. Dunlosky & K. A. Sana, F., Weston, T., & Cepeda, N. J. (2013). Laptop multitasking hinders
Rawson (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Cognition and Education classroom learning for both users and nearby peers. Computers &
(pp. 320–355). New York: Cambridge University Press. Education, 62, 24–31. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2012.10.003
13
Reddington, L. A., Peverly, S. T., & Block, C. J. (2015). An examination of Tran, T. A. T., & Lawson, M. (2001). Students’ procedures for reviewing
some of the cognitive and motivation variables related to gender lecture notes. International Education Journal, 2, 278–293.
14
differences in lecture note-taking. Reading and Writing, 28, 1155– Van Meter, P., Yokoi, L., & Pressley, M. (1994). College students’ theory
1185. doi:10.1007/s11145-015-9566-z of note-taking derived from their perceptions of note-taking.
Roediger, H. L., & Butler, A. C. (2011). The critical role of retrieval prac- Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 323–338. doi:10.1037/0022-
tice in long-term retention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 20–27. 0663.86.3.323
doi:10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.003

Appendix
Instructions and all survey questions

Instructions: “For this study, you will be asked to fill out a brief online survey about note-taking in in-person and/or online classes.”
Demographics
Q1. What is your gender? (male; female; other)
Q2. How old are you? (Under 18; 18–22; 23–25; 25–30; 30–40; 40+)
Q3. What is your ethnicity? (Hispanic/Latino; Native American; Asian; Native Hawaiian or Other Pacific Islander; African American; Middle Eastern; White)
Q4. Is English your first language? (Yes; No)
Q5. What is your major?a
Q6. Which University do you attend? (Kent State Main Campus; Ashtabula; East Liverpool; Geauga Campus; Salem; Stark; Trumbull; Tuscarawas; Other)
Q7. What year are you in school? (Freshman; Sophomore; Junior; Senior)
Q8. What was your high school GPA (on the 4.0 scale)? (0.0–0.9; 1.0–1.9; 2.0–2.9; 3.0–3.5; 3.6–4.0)
Q9. What is your current college GPA? (0.0–0.9; 1.0–1.9; 2.0–2.9; 3.0–3.5; 3.6–4.0; First Semester, do not have one yet)
Classroom Lectures
Instructions: “The next few questions pertain only to lectures you attend in a classroom in-person. As you answer the following questions, consider how you
take notes while in a classroom.”
Q10. Do you take notes during in-class lectures? (Yes; No)
Q11. Do you take notes in some lecture classes but not others? (Yes. Please Explain; No)
Q12. Do you feel that taking notes during in-class lectures is necessary for effective learning? (Yes; No)
Q13. Do you audio record lectures? (Yes; No)
Q14. If yes, do you take written notes as well as audio recording? (Yes; No)
The following questions were only presented if participants selected Yes on Q10
Q15. What do you use to take notes?b (Notebook or loose-leaf paper (i.e., handwrite notes); Laptop; Tablet (e.g., Apple iPad, Amazon Fire, Samsung Galaxy
Tab); eWriter that is specific for writing (e.g. Boogie Board Sync); Other. Please Explain)
Q16. If you selected more than one note-taking device, please explain.a
Q17. If you use a tablet to take notes, do you use a note-taking app? (Yes; No)
Q18. If yes, what app do you use?a
Q19. As you are writing, do you try to organise your notes, or do you copy what the teacher says? (I take organised Notes; I copy what the teacher says;
Depends. Please Explain)
Q20. If you organise your notes, how do you do so?b (Rephrase into my own words; Create a bulleted list or outline; Cornell notes; Mapping; Other. Please
Explain)
Q21. Do you take notes differently depending on the class? (Yes; No)
Q22. If yes, how so?a
Q23. Do you take more notes when you are in a smaller class where the teacher is likely to see you? (Yes; No)
Q24. Besides in-class lectures, what else do you take notes on?b (Textbook readings; Other class readings; In-class activities; Class videos; Other. Please Explain)
Online Classes
Instructions: “The next few questions pertain to online courses. As you answer the following questions, consider how you take notes in an online course. This
includes flipped courses where some of the content is in-class and some is online. However, only answer for the portion of the class that is online.”
Q25. Are you currently enrolled in a class where most or all lectures are online? (Yes; No)
The Next questions were only presented if participants selected Yes on Q25
Q26. While viewing a lecture online, do you take notes? (Yes; No)
Q27. If no, why not?a
Q28. Do you feel that taking notes during online lectures is necessary for effective learning in online courses? (Yes; No)
Q29. Do you feel that reviewing your notes is necessary for effective learning in online courses? (Yes; No)
The following questions were only presented if participants selected Yes on Q26
Q30. What do you use to take notes when viewing a lecture online?b (Notebook or loose-leaf paper (i.e., handwrite notes); Laptop; Tablet (e.g., Apple iPad,
Amazon Fire, Samsung Galaxy Tab); eWriter that is specific for writing (e.g. Boogie Board Sync); Other. Please Explain)
Q31. If you use a tablet to take notes, do you use a note-taking app? (Yes; No)
Q32. If yes, what app do you use?a
Q33. While taking notes during an online lecture, do you try to organise your notes, or do you just copy what the teacher says? (I take organised Notes; I copy
what the teacher says; Depends. Please Explain)
Q34. If you take organised notes, how do you organise your notes?b (Rephrase into my own words; Create a bulleted list or outline; Cornell notes; Mapping;
Other. Please Explain)
Q35. Besides during online lectures, what else do you take notes on?b (Textbook readings; Other class readings; Online demonstrations; Other videos, not
lectures by the teacher; Websites; Other. Please explain)
MEMORY 13

General Note-taking
Instructions: “The following questions pertain to note-taking in general. For the next questions, please consider how you take notes in general regardless of
whether your classes are in-person or online.”
Q36. Do your professors provide PowerPoint slides? (Yes; No; Some do. Please Explain)
The next two questions were presented if participants did not select No to Q36.
Q37. Do you take notes differently when PowerPoint slides are made available? (Yes, I take notes on the handouts; Yes, I do not take notes; No)
Q38. Do you take the same amount of notes when PowerPoint slides are made available? (Yes; No, I take less notes; No, I take more notes)
Q39. Have you heard of Cornell notes? (Yes; No)
Q40. Do you take good notes? (Yes; No)
Q41. If you take notes, why do you take notes?b (To aid Learning; Because it is expected; Because the teacher can see me; It helps me learn by writing as the
teacher is speaking; So I can review them later; Because I have to take exams; So I pay attention during the lecture; Other. Please Explain; I do not take notes)
Instructions and All Survey Questions (continued)
Q42. Do you review your notes? (Yes; No)
Q43. If yes, how do you review your notes?b (Rereading; Highlight or mark them; Add questions; Make flashcards from notes; Summarise notes; Test myself on
them; Copy them from memory; Other. Please explain)
Q44. Were note-taking skills taught to you? (Yes; No)
The next three questions were only presented if participants selected Yes on Q44
Q45. When were they taught?b (Elementary school; Middle school; High school; Community college; College; Other. Please explain)
Q46. Who taught you note-taking skills?b (Teacher; Professor; Parent or Guardian; Other. Please explain)
Q47. For how long were you taught note-taking skills?b (Part of a class period; An entire class period; A few class periods; A month; A semester)
Q48. Do you wish you have better note-taking skills? (Yes; No)
The following question was only asked of students at the Main Campus
Q49. Are you in the 1st year experience course? (Yes; No)
Q50. If yes, do you have learning modules? (Yes; No)
Questions Taken Directly from Prior Research
Instructions: “The next questions are a series of statements. For each statement, decide if it is true of you. If it is, select true. If not, select false.”
The response options for the following questions are True or False.
Q51. I often doodle instead of taking notes.
Q52. I am often on the internet or playing games instead of taking notes.
Q53. I sometimes find my note-taking interferes with my immediate understanding of the lecture.
Q54. If I did not have to take exams, I would not take notes.
Q55. I take notes so that I will note have to read the textbook.
Note. aResponse was open-ended. bParticipants could select more than one answer.

View publication stats

You might also like