Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 16

Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Vol.

161 (3rd series)


Clinical Neurophysiology: Diseases and Disorders
K.H. Levin and P. Chauvel, Editors
https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-64142-7.00062-X
Copyright © 2019 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved

Chapter 25

Clinical neurophysiology of REM parasomnias


AMBRA STEFANI, EVI HOLZKNECHT, AND BIRGIT HOGL* €
Department of Neurology, Medical University of Innsbruck, Innsbruck, Austria

Abstract
Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep behavior disorder (RBD), sleep paralysis, and nightmare disorder are
the three REM sleep parasomnias outlined by the International Classification of Sleep Disorders. In this
review we address the clinical neurophysiology of these disorders. The majority of neurophysiologic
studies have been conducted in RBD, and fewer studies have evaluated patients with nightmare disorder
or isolated sleep paralysis. Neurophysiologic studies of REM sleep parasomnias mostly used polysomno-
graphy (PSG), or were performed on animals to shed light on the pathophysiology of these disorders.
Fewer studies used electoencephalography or electromyography outside the context of PSG, evoked
potentials, or autonomic neurophysiologic studies. In this chapter, the main neurophysiologic findings
in REM sleep parasomnias are described and their implications and relevance are discussed.

INTRODUCTION REM SLEEP BEHAVIOR DISORDER


REM sleep behavior disorder (RBD), sleep paralysis, RBD is characterized by abnormal behaviors emerging
and nightmare disorders are the parasomnias of REM during REM sleep. These RBD behaviors manifest as
sleep, according to the International Classification of an enactment of dreams that are often unpleasant,
Sleep Disorders, Third Edition (ICSD-3) (American action-filled, and violent. RBD can cause sleep disruption
Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2014). We performed and can also cause injuries to the patient or to their bed
a systematic literature search concerning the clinical partner (American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2014).
neurophysiology of these disorders, using the search The current diagnostic criteria for RBD were
terms REM sleep behavior disorder (RBD), sleep published in the International Classification of Sleep
paralysis, nightmare disorders, and polysomnography Disorders, Third Edition, American Academy of Sleep
(PSG), electroencephalography (EEG), electrooculo- Medicine (2014) and require repeated episodes of
graphy (EOG), rapid eye movements (REMs), evoked REM sleep–related vocalization and/or complex motor
potentials, electromyography (EMG), REM sleep behaviors as well as pathologically increased EMG
without atonia, nerve conduction velocity, event- activity during REM sleep, as demonstrated by video-
related potentials, evoked potentials, visual evoked PSG (American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2014).
potentials, sensory evoked potentials, motor evoked RBD can be idiopathic or associated with neurologic
potentials, brainstem reflex, blink reflex, autonomic disorders, mostly a-synucleinopathies (Parkinson’s
dysfunction, transcranial magnetic stimulation, and disease [PD], dementia with Lewy bodies [DLB], or,
cortico-muscular coherence. The results are presented rarely, multiple system atrophy [MSA]). RBD can also
in this chapter. Given that these disorders are sleep be associated with autoimmune diseases (e.g., narco-
disorders, the most used neurophysiologic method is lepsy, Anti-IgLON5 disease) (Sabater et al., 2014;
polysomnography. RBD is the best-studied disorder H€ogl et al., 2015) or with other neurologic disorders
neurophysiologically. (H€ogl and Iranzo, 2017). The clinical relevance of

*Correspondence to: Birgit H€ogl, Department of Neurology, Medical University of Innsbruck, Anichstrasse 35, 6020 Innsbruck,
Austria. Tel: +43-512-504-81172, +43-512-504-23811, Fax: +43-512-504-23842, E-mail: birgit.ho@i-med.ac.at
382 A. STEFANI ET AL.
idiopathic RBD (iRBD) has been reconsidered during study in neonatal rats established that muscle twitches
recent years due to increasing evidence that the vast during REM sleep drive the activity and development
majority of patients with iRBD develop a neurodegen- of the motor cortex (Fraigne et al., 2015). Twitches dur-
erative disease over time, namely an a-synucleinopathy. ing REM sleep are “self-generated events” that have been
The risk of conversion is over 80% in longitudinal stud- proposed to function to promote motor learning and
ies with follow-up over 10 years (Postuma et al., 2012; development. An inhibitory drive onto skeletal motor
Schenck et al., 2013; Iranzo et al., 2016). Moreover, a neurons produces a temporally coordinated pattern of
recent study showed that even in iRBD patients who muscle twitches during REM sleep. Muscle twitches in
did not convert after a follow-up of 10 years or more, adult rats follow a well-defined temporal trajectory: they
biomarkers of a-synuclein–related neurodegeneration are largely absent during REM initiation but increase
are present (Iranzo et al., 2017). In light of these data, steadily thereafter, peaking toward REM termination.
iRBD is now considered an early stage a-synucleinopa- A GABA and glycine drive onto motor neurons prevents
thy, manifesting in some cases many years before first twitch activity during REM initiation, but progressive
appearance of motor symptoms during wakefulness or weakening of this drive functions to promote twitch
cognitive manifestations. Therefore, the term “isolated” activity during REM termination (Brooks and Peever,
RBD has been suggested as more appropriate, reflecting 2016). These findings in animal studies may explain
the continuum of progression (H€ ogl et al., 2018). the observation that during a REM behavioral episode
the movement often starts with single muscle twitches,
similar to physiologic twitches in REM sleep, and
Neurophysiologic studies in animals to
then extends to more muscles, and finally terminates
investigate the pathophysiology of RBD
in a typical RBD movement, i.e., complex acting-out-
Several animal studies have reproduced, in different of-dreams movements. In line with the data reported
ways, the cardinal features of RBD. At the neuroana- in animals by Brooks and Peever (Brooks and
tomic level, sublaterodorsal tegmental nucleus neurons Peever, 2016) in RBD patients, a progressive weaken-
specifically activated during paradoxical sleep send des- ing of the GABA and glycine motor neuron inhibition
cending efferents to glycine/GABA neurons within the seems to occur, promoting twitch activity during
ventral medulla, but not ascending projections to the REM termination.
intralaminar thalamus. This suggests a crucial role of
sublaterodorsal tegmental nucleus neurons in muscle
atonia rather than in paradoxical sleep generation. In line Neurophysiology of RBD
with this hypothesis, 30 days after adeno-associated POLYSOMNOGRAPHY
virus injections into the sublaterodorsal tegmental
nucleus, rats display a 30% decrease in daily paradoxical During PSG, there are EEG, electrooculogram (EOG),
sleep quantity and a significant increase of muscle tone and EMG findings that have been investigated in RBD.
during paradoxical sleep concomitant with a large
increase in abnormal motor dream–enacting behaviors
EEG
(Valencia Garcia et al., 2017). Glutamate sublaterodorsal
tegmental nucleus neurons generate muscle atonia dur- Studies of sleep architecture and power spectral
ing paradoxical sleep, most likely through descending analyses by fast Fourier transforms in iRBD and healthy
projections to glycine/GABA premotor neurons in the controls have shown heterogeneous findings. Some
ventral medulla (Valencia Garcia et al., 2017). studies have reported a greater percentage of slow
Caudal ventral mesopontine junction lesioned animals wave sleep in iRBD compared with healthy controls
show an increase in phasic motor activity in REM sleep, (Massicotte-Marquez et al., 2005; Ferri et al., 2017);
resembling that seen in human RBD (Lai et al., 2008). others showed no differences in sleep parameters
Transgenic mice with deficient glycine and GABA (Fantini et al., 2003; Latreille et al., 2011; O’Reilly
transmission have a behavioral, motor, and sleep pheno- et al., 2015); some have shown EEG slowing (Fantini
type typical of RBD, including REM motor behaviors, et al., 2003; Sasai et al., 2013) when analyzing EEG
non-REM muscle twitches, sleep disruption, and EEG spectral power; and one study reported an increase in
slowing (Brooks and Peever, 2011). delta and theta power in central regions only in RBD
Chemogenetic activation of glutamatergic neurons of patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI)
the red nucleus produces excessive muscle twitching (Iranzo et al., 2010). In line with this, a correlation
during REM sleep. The function of this brief muscle acti- between increased delta power during REM sleep and
vation during REM sleep has been investigated, and a MCI has been reported (Sasai et al., 2013). One study
CLINICAL NEUROPHYSIOLOGY OF REM 383
analyzed sleep spindles, showing smaller densities of artifacts from RBD-related tonic EMG activity in routine
fast spindles but larger densities of slow spindles in recording settings, and because of the reduced sensitivity
iRBD compared with controls (O’Reilly et al., 2015). of chin EMG alone (Fernandez-Arcos et al., 2017), the
SINBAR (Sleep Innsbruck Barcelona) group has recom-
mended the addition of upper limb muscle EMG chan-
EOG
nels in suspected RBD (Frauscher et al., 2012a,
REM sleep can be subdivided into REM sleep with and 2014a). Upper extremity muscles (biceps and flexor digi-
without rapid eye movements (REMs). It has been torum superificialis) combined with chin EMG have
reported that REM sleep with REMs acts as a function- been shown to better discriminate RBD and controls
ally isolated and close intrinsic loop, whereas REM sleep according to phasic EMG activity in these muscles dur-
without REMs seems to be a state of reduced alertness, ing REM sleep, compared with leg or sternocleidomas-
suggesting that these two components of REM sleep toid muscles (Frauscher et al., 2012a). Phasic and
have different functions (Wehrle et al., 2007). other EMG activity in the tibialis anterior muscle is
REMs have been analyzed in RBD, and density, frequent in elderly subjects, such as from excessive
index, and complexity have not been shown to differ fragmentary myoclonus (Frauscher et al., 2014a;
between RBD patients and controls (Leclair-Visonneau Fraigne et al., 2015; Raccagni et al., 2016). The useful-
et al., 2010). However, RBD behaviors occur more fre- ness of recording upper-limb EMG was demonstrated in
quently during REM sleep with REMs than without a study showing that 18.4% of iRBD patients would
(Frauscher et al., 2009; Manni et al., 2009; Leclair- have been misdiagnosed using only the mentalis
Visonneau et al., 2010). EMG (Fernandez-Arcos et al., 2017). The SINBAR
montage also allows detection of EMG activity when
patients display abnormal behaviors only in the limbs,
EMG
a common situation in iRBD. The need for both
Persistence of tonic EMG activity in the chin during upper and lower extremity EMG monitoring to fully
REM sleep in patients with Parkinson’s disease was first evaluate RBD is noted in the ICSD-3 (American
described in 1969 (Traczynska-Kubin et al., 1969) and Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2014). Evidence-based
confirmed in 1975 (Mouret, 1975). In the 1970s different cut-off data using any chin EMG activity combined
names were used to describe REM sleep without atonia with bilateral phasic activity of the flexor digitorum
(RWA). In the Japanese literature it was called “stage-1 superificialis muscles (SINBAR EMG montage) enable
REM with tonic EMG activity,” (Tachibana et al., distinguishing RBD patients from controls (Frauscher
1975) whereas Guilleminault et al. described it as et al., 2012a).
“sleep stage 7” (Guilleminault et al., 1976). Human It has also been shown that RWA as an EMG finding
RBD was first described in Schenck et al. (1986) in five during REM sleep in RBD has a remarkable stability
patients showing variable loss of chin and limb EMG over several nights (Ferri et al., 2013), so that in most
atonia during REM sleep (Schenck et al., 1986). RBD patients a single night of PSG is sufficient to
The first scoring method to quantify RWA was capture RWA. However, a percentage of patients may
published in Lapierre and Montplaisir (1992). Since not have enough REM sleep during the first night study
then, different classification systems have been pub- for adequate assessment. An alternative to EMG is
lished (Eisensehr et al., 2003; Bliwise et al., 2006; actigraphy using an accelerometer, which can be useful
Frauscher et al., 2012a). Scoring guidelines as suggested in identifying subjects with iRBD (Stefani et al.,
for diagnosis of RBD in the ICSD-3 (American Academy 2018). Periodic leg movements of sleep (PLMSs) have
of Sleep Medicine, 2014) are defined in the AASM been evaluated in RBD patients. In Caucasian RBD
Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events patients, PLMSs have been reported to be common
(American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2016). (70% (Fantini et al., 2002) to 85%) (Manconi et al.,
Classically, RWA has been evaluated in the chin 2007). One study reported a higher PLMS index in
EMG, as this channel is routinely used in the PSG. While RBD compared to controls only during REM sleep
several studies have shown that EMG activity of the (Fantini et al., 2002). Another study found higher PLMS
mentalis and submentalis muscles can distinguish RBD indices in both NREM and REM sleep in RBD; how-
from controls, it has also been shown that 65% of the ever, in this study PLMSs also occurred more frequently
behavioral events would have been missed if an upper during REM sleep in patients with RBD (Manconi et al.,
extremity had not been recorded (Iranzo et al., 2011). 2007). In another study of Japanese iRBD patients, only
Because it is difficult to distinguish EMG activity 33% had a PLMS index 15/h (Sasai et al., 2011).
derived from increased respiratory efforts or snoring In iRBD patients a reduction of cardiac and EEG
384 A. STEFANI ET AL.
activation associated with PLMS has been reported, similar to that of DLB and Parkinson’s disease–dementia
suggesting the presence of an impaired cortical (Iranzo et al., 2010). Another study showed waking EEG
and autonomic reactivity to internal stimuli (Fantini slowing in the posterior cortical regions only in RBD
et al., 2002). patients with MCI, and suggested that this could be a
marker for increased risk of conversion to DLB or PD
AUTOMATIC ANALYSIS OF NEUROPHYSIOLOGIC (Rodrigues Brazete et al., 2013). In summary, EEG slow-
PSG PARAMETERS ing has been frequently reported in iRBD, and some
studies report the value of EEG as an early indicator of
Several algorithms for automatic sleep stage classifica-
conversion.
tion (Schaltenbrand et al., 1996; Koley and Dey, 2012;
Christensen et al., 2014; Wang et al., 2015; Hassan
and Bhuiyan, 2017) and automatic detection of REMs AUTONOMIC DYSFUNCTION IN iRBD
have been developed (Tsuji et al., 2000; Suzuki et al., Objective autonomic function tests (tilt table examination,
2001; Yetton et al., 2016). However, expert manual orthostatic standing test, Valsalva maneuver) show that
scoring remains the gold standard. the degree of autonomic dysfunction in iRBD patients
To overcome cumbersome manual quantification of is intermediate between controls and PD patients
muscle activity, computerized programs have been (Frauscher et al., 2012b). Autonomic cardiac function
developed (Ferri et al., 2008, 2010; Frauscher et al., during sleep (Ferini-Strambi et al., 1996; Postuma et al.,
2014a; Frandsen et al., 2015). This methodology requires 2010; Sorensen et al., 2012, 2013) showed reduced tonic
high-quality technical recordings and visual exclusion of (vagal activity) and phasic (sympathetic activity) heart rate
epochs with confounding EMG activity and artifacts. variability, and reduced heart rate response to leg move-
Most computerized approaches are based on analysis ments. One study reported autonomic cardiovascular
of chin EMG activity and are subject to the same limita- dysfunction on the basis of peripheral cardiac/vascular
tions as manual quantification of chin EMG; only a few denervation rather than central autonomic dysregulation
methods include automatic analysis of upper-extremity (Dahms et al., 2016).
EMG (Frauscher et al., 2014a).
EVOKED POTENTIALS
WAKING EEG FINDINGS
Studies have assessed psychophysiologic parameters in
Most studies have reported waking EEG slowing in iRBD using auditory evoked potentials (active oddball
patients with iRBD. The first study performed by Fantini P300 paradigm and passive oddball paradigm) (Raggi
et al. showed EEG slowing and impaired cortical activa- et al., 2007). The authors found no differences between
tion during both wakefulness and REM sleep in iRBD patients and matched controls, leading them to conclude
patients compared with controls (Fantini et al., 2003). that cognitive evoked potentials are not sensitive to pre-
Another study reported more severe and more global or subclinical cognitive abnormalities in iRBD, at least at
EEG slowing, indicating dysfunction of cortical activa- the early-middle stages of the disease. As cognitive def-
tion similar to that found in neurodegenerative diseases icits have been described in RBD, event related poten-
(Massicotte-Marquez et al., 2008). Another study of tials using a cognitive task have been evaluated in
patients with iRBD with a mean follow-up of 3.5 years patients with PD with and without RBD. An occipital
showed that waking EEG slowing predicted the onset positive wave, the P2, appeared in patients with PD
of a neurodegenerative disease (Rodrigues Brazete and RBD, but it was nearly absent in controls. PD
et al., 2016). A study of iRBD patients measuring patients without RBD were not significantly different
functional connectivity from EEG recordings showed from PD patients with RBD, and the PD group without
decreased delta-band functional connectivity in the RBD did not differ significantly from the control group,
frontal regions that correlated with verbal fluency perfor- suggesting that RBD accentuates cerebral dysfunctions
mance (Sunwoo et al., 2017). EEG slowing during wake in PD (Gaudreault et al., 2013).
and REM sleep was confirmed in a study of iRBD Motor evoked potentials, somatosensory evoked
patients correlating RBD with mild cognitive impair- potentials, and blink reflex have not been investigated
ment (MCI) (Sasai et al., 2013). A study performed in RBD.
EEG spectral analysis in patients with iRBD without
MCI, RBD and MCI, and healthy controls, finding
QUANTITATIVE SENSORY TESTING
marked EEG slowing in those with RBD and MCI in
central and occipital regions during wakefulness and Somatosensory function is altered in PD with iRBD.
REM sleep, compared with controls and to a lesser extent Quantitative sensory testing revealed increased thermal
with those with iRBD without MCI. This pattern was detection thresholds in iRBD compared to healthy
CLINICAL NEUROPHYSIOLOGY OF REM 385
controls (Strobel et al., 2018). The authors hypothesized definite diagnosis of RBD, video PSG (vPSG) is required
that thermal sensory impairment might be due to func- (American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2014), and is
tional affection of the small nerve fibers. the only instrument enabling differential diagnosis of
RBD “mimics.”
TRANSCRANIAL MAGNETIC STIMULATION
Antidepressants and REM without atonia
A transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) protocol
relying on short latency afferent inhibition (SAI) of RBD observed in patients on antidepressant treatment
the motor cortex (Tokimura et al., 2000) allows evalu- was initially classified as “medication-induced second-
ation of cholinergic circuits. SAI refers to the suppres- ary RBD” (Schenck et al., 1992). However, later studies
sion of the amplitude of a motor evoked potential showed that cessation of antidepressant treatments did
produced by a conditioning afferent electrical stimulus, not resolve the RBD. The International RBD Study
usually applied to the median nerve at the wrist Group reported that patients with PSG-confirmed iRBD
approximately 20 ms prior to transcranial magnetic (n ¼ 318) were more likely to report a history of depres-
stimulation of the hand area of the contralateral motor sion and antidepressant use (due to selective serotoniner-
cortex (Tokimura et al., 2000). Nardone et al. evaluated gic reuptake inhibitors, OR 3.6) than matched controls
SAI testing in iRBD, showing that mean SAI was (n ¼ 318) (Frauscher et al., 2014b). Lifetime antidepres-
reduced in iRBD as compared to controls, and SAI sant use was more frequent than comorbid depression
values correlated with disease duration and also with (Frauscher et al., 2014b). Frequent antidepressant use
episodic verbal memory and executive functions (60%) was also reported in another study of 115 RBD
(Nardone et al., 2012). patients (60% iRBD, 48% taking antidepressants at the
time of the diagnosis) (Ju et al., 2011). In another iRBD
CORTICO-MUSCULAR COHERENCE cohort (n ¼ 100) 27% of patients who were taking
antidepressants had abnormalities on 12/14 tested neuro-
Cortico-muscular coherence (CMC) reflects functional
degenerative markers and were indistinguishable from
connectivity between the primary motor cortex, mea-
iRBD patients not taking antidepressants (Postuma
sured by EEG or magnetoencephalography, and a contra-
et al., 2013). In light of these findings, the development
lateral effector muscle during isometric contraction. Jung
of RBD with antidepressants is now seen as an early
et al. compared CMC values measured by EEG of RBD
sign of an underlying neurodegenerative disease
patients and healthy controls. In healthy subjects CMC
(Postuma et al., 2013; Postuma et al., 2015), suggesting
was significantly lower during REM sleep compared to
that these patients should also be followed up over the
wakefulness, but this was not the case in the RBD
long term as they are at risk of developing a neurode-
patients. CMC during REM sleep was significantly
generative disorder (Wing et al., 2015). However, one
higher in RBD patients than in controls, suggesting that
study showed a lower risk of conversion among those
cortical locomotor drive during REM sleep is increased
with antidepressant-induced RBD, although patients
in RBD (Jung et al., 2012).
demonstrated biomarkers of neurodegeneration (motor
impairment, olfactory deficit, color vision impairment,
BRAINSTEM REFLEXES
symptoms of autonomic dysfunction, cognitive defi-
The startle response is an alerting reaction to sudden and cits) (Postuma et al., 2013). Further studies with
unexpected sensory stimuli probably originating in the longer follow-up are needed to fully understand this
brainstem (Brown et al., 1991). A patient with RBD association.
was reported to have an excessive startle response to
visual stimuli, probably caused by a pontine lesion SLEEP PARALYSIS
(Peter et al., 2008).
Sleep paralysis is a transient, generalized, inability to
move and to speak occurring in the transitional period
NEUROPHYSIOLOGY IN THE DIFFERENTIAL
between wakefulness and sleep (Nielsen and Zadra,
DIAGNOSIS OF RBD
2000). It is characterized by the complete inability to
Several large clinical case series show that self- move in a subjectively awake person. It affects all
awareness of RBD is low (Frauscher et al., 2010; somatic (Chase and Francisco, 1994) muscles under
Boeve et al., 2013; Fernandez-Arcos et al., 2016). Accu- voluntary control (Nielsen and Zadra, 2000) except the
rate diagnosis is a challenge, as by history alone RBD is diaphragm, external eye muscles, and the stapedius,
easily confused with sleep apnea, non-REM parasomnia, similar to the physiologic muscle atonia experienced
periodic limb movement disorder, epilepsy, and noctur- in REM sleep (Kryger et al., 1994). Auxiliary respira-
nal wandering (Fernandez-Arcos et al., 2016). For a tory muscles (e.g., the intercostal muscles) are also
386 A. STEFANI ET AL.
paralyzed, but the diaphragm is not affected, so that Neurophysiology of sleep paralysis
sleep paralysis is often accompanied by feelings of suf-
The few neurophysiologic studies performed in patients
focation. Sleep paralysis can last up to several minutes,
with isolated sleep paralysis have been based on PSG or
disappears spontaneously or upon external stimulation
multiple sleep latency tests (MSLT). Some studies have
(Hishikawa and Shimizu, 1995) and often occurs in
applied specific signal analysis, such as fast Fourier
association with hypnopompic hallucinations (McCarty
transforms of EEG data. Only a few studies have focused
and Chesson, 2009).
on other neurophysiologic techniques.
Sleep paralysis can occur in isolation, or as an
PSG and MSLT recordings obtained during sponta-
element of the clinical tetrad of narcolepsy, where it
neously occurring or provoked sleep paralysis have
has been reported in as many as 50% of patients
demonstrated characteristic changes in EEG, EOG,
(Frauscher et al., 2013). It can be accompanied by hyp-
and chin/extremity muscle activity. Different types of
nagogic or hypnopompic hallucinations (H€ ogl and
manipulations of the slow wave sleep cycle have been
Iranzo, 2017). The experience of a complete inability
applied to induce sleep paralysis and are described in
to move despite subjective alertness is overwhelmingly
following text.
unpleasant and frightening, especially when it occurs
for the first time.
The diagnostic criteria for recurrent isolated sleep
POLYSOMNOGRAPHY
paralysis, all of which must be met, include: recurrent
inability to move the trunk and all of the limbs at sleep Early studies showed that auditory arousing stimuli pre-
onset or upon awakening from sleep; each episode lasts sented repeatedly with increasing intensity during sleep
seconds to a few minutes; episodes cause clinically onset REM periods were correctly perceived in normal
significant distress including bedtime anxiety or fear individuals, but in patients with narcolepsy the subjective
of sleep; and the disturbance is not better explained response was not accompanied by a correct motor behav-
by another sleep disorder (especially narcolepsy), ioral response (pressing a button on an electrical switch).
mental disorder, medical condition, medication, or This was attributed to false awakenings with REM-related
substance use. paralysis (Nan’no et al., 1970). Sleep paralysis has been
observed after acute reversal of the sleep–wake cycle
(Weitzman et al., 1970) and after massive manipulation
Clinical features of sleep paralysis
of the human sleep–wake cycle, such as a 90-min day
The pathophysiology of sleep paralysis is reflected in (Carskadon and Dement, 1975; Takeuchi et al., 1992).
neurophysiologic manifestations, and includes atonia According to these early studies, sleep paralysis was
of striated muscles under voluntary control, except the linked to the occurrence of sleep onset REM period.
diaphragm, external eye muscles, and stapedius, similar In a PSG study, Takeuchi et al. performed sleep inter-
to the atonia in REM sleep. Muscle twitches also occur. ruptions during nocturnal PSG in healthy subjects, and
Isolated sleep paralysis is often mistaken for other 9.4% of these interruptions resulted in isolated sleep
neurologic disorders (H€ ogl and Iranzo, 2017) and neuro- paralysis. The reported subjective perception included
physiologic studies can help distinguish it from con- inability to move while recognizing the sleep laboratory
founders. Sleep paralysis plays a role in the writing of surroundings, often accompanied by auditory and visual
great novelists, including Dostoevsky (Stefani et al., hallucinations. PSG recordings during this state were
2017) and Maupassant (“The Horla”) (Miranda and characterized by dissociation between REM sleep and
Hogl, 2013; Miranda and Bustamante, 2015). In the fine wakefulness, demonstrating abundant alpha EEG or a
arts, Johann Heinrich F€ussli’s (1775) painting The Night- mixed pattern of alpha waves and low voltage slow
mare may reflect sleep paralysis with a hypnagogic waves, and persistence of muscle atonia (Takeuchi
hallucination (Baumann et al., 2007). The frequent et al., 1992). Eye blinks were also noted. The authors
occurrence of isolated sleep paralysis in healthy persons concluded that isolated sleep paralysis occurs in the tran-
has led to popular designations for the phenomenon in sition between REM and wakefulness. Lucid dreaming
various parts of the world (e.g., in Mexico sleep paralysis occurred during unequivocal REM sleep; whereas, iso-
is called Se me subio un muerto [“A cadaver climbed on lated sleep paralysis occurred during ambiguous REM
me”] (Stefani et al., 2017). In Hong Kong, it is called “the sleep or in the transition between REM and wakefulness.
ghost oppression phenomenon” (Wing et al., 1994; The authors highlight the close relationship between
Stefani et al., 2017), reflecting the inability to move sleep onset REM periods and isolated sleep paralysis
and the presence of hallucinations. Sleep paralysis can (Takeuchi et al., 1992).
also be induced by sleep deprivation or chronically insuf- In patients with narcolepsy, sleep paralysis with or
ficient sleep (McCarty and Chesson, 2009). without hypnagogic hallucinations is frequent but rarely
CLINICAL NEUROPHYSIOLOGY OF REM 387
occurs during sleep attacks or short daytime naps, prob- EMG activity, has been noted during sleep onset REM
ably because the uncomfortable sleeping position, such periods, supporting the concept that sleep paralysis is dis-
as standing or sitting, prevents the occurrence of sleep tinct from hypnagogic hallucinations and results from a
onset REM episodes (Hishikawa and Shimizu, 1995). dissociation between the level of consciousness and the
somatic muscle and reflex activities (Hishikawa and
PSG, MSLT and fast Fourier transform signal Shimizu, 1995).
analysis
Further studies on sleep paralysis involving PSG
In a study of provoked cataplexy in patients with narco-
lepsy, unprovoked sleep paralysis was observed (Dyken In one PSG study of patients with obstructive sleep apnea
et al., 1996). During the episode, PSG revealed a pattern (OSA), 38.3% were reported to have experienced sleep
compatible with REM sleep, and the PSG REM sleep paralysis (Hsieh et al., 2010). The observation was based
pattern was electrophysiologically indistinguishable only on a self-reported sleep questionnaire, so nonres-
from that recorded during cataplexy, sleep paralysis, torative sleep and early morning sleepiness causation
and hypnagogic hallucinations (Dyken et al., 1996). cannot be excluded. It was observed that OSA patients
This study was based on visual analysis of four EEG with report of isolated sleep paralysis had higher
channels and chin EMG and a single lower extremity Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS) and Pittsburgh Sleep
channel only. Quality Index (PSQI) scores. This study highlights
In a vPSG and MSLT study, it was reported that how PSG is valuable in ruling out other causes of sleep
incomplete sleep paralysis can be the first symptom fragmentation, such as OSA, in patients with suspected
of narcolepsy (Buskova et al., 2013). In this case sleep sleep paralysis (Hsieh et al., 2010).
paralysis occurred during an MSLT. EEG during the Sleep paralysis is consistent with the concept of dis-
episode showed recurrent alpha waves intermingled sociated states discussed by Mahowald and Schenck
with low-voltage fast activity; chin EMG showed inter- (2005)). Terzahgi et al. have also discussed the possible
mittent tonic and phasic muscle activity (Buskova presence of simultaneously coexisting regional states of
et al., 2013). wakefulness and sleep as local brain phenomena, previ-
Another case report demonstrated sleep paralysis in a ously documented in confusional arousal (Terzaghi
patient with narcolepsy (Terzaghi et al., 2012). The sleep et al., 2009).
paralysis episode occurred on awakening from sleep Another neurophysiologic study of narcoleptic
onset REM (SOREM) during the MSLT, and miosis, dys- patients using 24-h sleep monitoring reported lower
arthric speech, and inability to protrude the tongue were coherences and delta power and higher muscle atonia
described. EEG showed recurrent alpha-like frequency in lucid dreamers (those experiencing being aware of
intermingled and irregularly alternating with low-voltage dreaming while asleep and continuing to dream) (Dodet
fast frequency activity, persistence of REM bursts, and a et al., 2015). The occurrence of sleep paralysis did not
chin EMG pattern characterized by muscle atonia mixed distinguish lucid and nonlucid dreamers.
together with episodes of loss of atonia (Terzaghi et al.,
2012). The patient reported remembering details of the NEUROPHYSIOLOGIC STUDIES IN ANIMALS TO
conversation with the neurologist during sleep paralysis, INVESTIGATE THE PATHOPHYSIOLOGY OF SP AND
but wondered if it was a dream. EEG frequency domain RELATED CONDITIONS
analysis and fast Fourier transform frequency spectra
Electrophysiologic studies in rats after specific lesions or
calculations showed normal wake activity peaking at
substance administration have been used to elucidate
9.5 Hz and REM activity in the 19-Hz range. In contrast,
mechanisms of physiologic REM sleep paralysis. They
the sleep paralysis episode presented two peaks in the
have shown that trigeminal motor neurons and masseter
9.5-Hz and 17- to 18.5-Hz range. When the amplitudes
muscles are switched off during REM sleep in rats
of wake with eyes closed and REM spectra were
(Brooks and Peever, 2012).
summed, a new spectrum was constructed that matched
Carbachol injections in the nucleus pontis oralis pro-
very closely the sleep paralysis episode. The authors
duced carbachol-induced REM sleep or a waking state
concluded that the patient was in an intermediate state
with muscle atonia (representing cataplexy or sleep
between wake and REM sleep during the paralysis
paralysis induced by carbachol), depending on the state
(Terzaghi et al., 2012).
in which the injection was performed (Torterolo et al.,
2016). The waking state showed high gamma coherence,
EMG and nerve conduction velocity studies
similar to what is seen during active wakefulness.
In narcolepsy patients with sleep paralysis, suppression In narcoleptic mice, orexin gene transfer into the
of the H-reflex, in addition to suppression of tonic chin zona incerta neurons suppressed muscle paralysis, and
388 A. STEFANI ET AL.
analysis of nuchal muscle tone and EEG activity versions of the ICSD3 and DSM 5 criteria (American
suggested that zona incerta neurons can serve as a target Psychiatric Association, 2013; American Academy of
of drugs to control muscle paralysis in narcolepsy (Liu Sleep Medicine, 2014).
et al., 2011). It was suggested that this technique could Nightmares can occur as a primary complaint without
be used to identify target brain areas for drug develop- other comorbidity (H€ogl and Iranzo, 2017) or in the
ment (Liu et al., 2011). context of other sleep disorders, psychiatric disorders,
or systemic diseases. Nightmares are frequent in RBD
(Schenck et al., 1987; Valli et al., 2012, 2015), narco-
NEUROPHYSIOLOGY IN THE DIFFERENTIAL
lepsy, and sleep apnea, according to some reports. They
DIAGNOSIS OF SLEEP PARALYSIS
can occur in the context of neurologic diseases, including
Video-EEG and PSG should be used to assess for focal certain forms of epilepsy, and can be medication/
epileptic seizures that may mimic sleep paralysis substance-induced. Nightmares and nightmare disorder
(Galimberti et al., 2009). One patient who met criteria need to be distinguished from sleep terrors, hypnagogic
for sleep paralysis demonstrated focal epileptic seizures hallucinations with or without sleep paralysis, and
during sleep paralysis attacks captured on video-EEG nocturnal panic attacks.
and PSG. EEG showed onset over the right posterior
temporal regions with fast sharp recruiting activity
Historic neurophysiologic research
spreading to both parietal and occipital regions and fol-
in nightmares
lowed by a sudden suspension of deltoid and anterior
tibialis EMG activity bilaterally, whereas EMG activity In the first decades of polysomnographic sleep research,
of chin and sternocleidomastoid muscles was preserved. nightmares associated with REM sleep, sleep terrors, and
Later, the EEG activity stopped abruptly. The video nightmares associated with non-REM sleep were not
showed abrupt cessation of body movement coincident well differentiated. Broughton reasoned that nightmares
with discharge onset. Later, the patient reported a typical occur in “confusional states of arousal,” not in “dreaming
sleep paralysis attack. After levetiracetam was prescribed, sleep” (Broughton, 1968). Typical nightmare attacks
the attacks ceased (Galimberti et al., 2009). Sleep paralysis during abrupt arousal from slow-wave sleep and one
needs to be differentiated from epilepsy and this can be following arousal from slow-wave sleep were recorded
achieved by performing vPSG or video-EEG during the in normal subjects and sleepwalkers, and none of these
attack. attacks were associated with REM sleep or detailed
dreams (Broughton, 1968).
Fisher et al. conducted a PSG study identifying three
NIGHTMARE DISORDER
types of nightmares: “major stage four nightmares,”
The diagnosis of nightmare disorder consists of three “REM anxiety dreams,” and “nightmares associated with
criteria, all of which must be met: spontaneous awakening out of sleep stage 2 sleep during
the initial nonREM sleep cycle” (Fisher et al., 1970).
1. Repeated occurrence of extended, extremely dys-
The authors also recorded REM dreams in subjects in
phoric, and well-remembered dreams that usually
whom anxiety was present, finding dream content that
involve threats to survival, security, or physical
was “not as panicky as in the nonREM nightmare”
integrity.
(Fisher et al., 1970). However, this study was later
2. On awakening from the dysphoric dreams, the
criticized (Nielsen and Zadra, 2000) for the presence
person rapidly becomes oriented and alert.
of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in some patients.
3. The dream experience, or the sleep disturbance
In these historic studies, nightmares always occurred
produced by awakening from it, causes clinically
during REM sleep, usually during particularly long REM
significant distress or impairment in social, occupa-
periods late during the night and sometimes during REM
tional, or other important areas of functioning
sleep periods with increased eye movements (Hartmann,
(American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2014).
1984). It was reported that in nightmares arising from
Nightmares are frequent, occurring in an estimated REM sleep there was an increase in the cardiorespiratory
6.6% of the general population (Ohayon and Schenck, rate and a marked increase in the number of eye move-
2010) and in 16% of a 700-patient cohort with sleep ments (Fisher et al., 1970).
disorders (Krakow, 2006). In a subsequent psychophysiologic study of night-
There has been some controversy about whether mares and night terrors, transcriptions of interviews from
a diagnosis of nightmare disorder requires awakening spontaneous non-REM awakenings were analyzed
from the nightmare, or fear of returning to sleep. This (mainly out of Rechtschaffen and Kales Stage 3 and 4
distinction is no longer explicitly stated in the current sleep) (Fisher et al., 1973). The interviews were taken
CLINICAL NEUROPHYSIOLOGY OF REM 389
during several years from a series of subjects with night These findings suggested that increased REM propensity
terrors. The authors reported that there was a 58% rate of did not contribute to the occurrence of nightmares,
dream recall, mainly out of stage 3–4 sleep. However, the but the study only mildly increased REM propensity.
clinical differentiation of nightmares and terrors was not
well established. Only later was the terminology differ- AUTONOMIC CHANGES IN NIGHTMARES
entiated to separate nightmares and night terrors, the
Increases in respiratory and heart rate were observed
first occurring during REM sleep and the latter during
with severe nightmares in an early study, although a con-
non-REM sleep (Newell et al., 1992).
founding with sleep terrors cannot be excluded (Fisher
Nightmare disorder is currently classified among the
et al., 1970).
REM-related parasomnias associated with REM sleep.
In nine patients with nightmare disorder without
However, in adults dream-like imagery can be associated
psychiatric comorbidity, heart and respiratory rate
with some non-REM sleep parasomnias. This is also true
recordings during nightmare and nonnightmare REM
for night terrors (Uguccioni et al., 2013).
sleep episodes confirmed earlier reports of sympathetic
arousal during some nightmares (Nielsen and Zadra,
More recent PSG studies in nightmares
2000). Mean heart rate for REM sleep with nightmare
A PSG study compared patients suffering from posttrau- was elevated during 3 min before awakening, but respi-
matic nightmares and those suffering from idiopathic ratory rate was not affected. Most subjects (78%) showed
nightmares with a healthy control group and reported that heart-rate acceleration during nightmare sleep, whereas
patients who suffered from posttraumatic nightmares the same number showed heart-rate deceleration during
exhibited significantly more nocturnal awakenings than nonnightmare REM (Nielsen and Zadra, 2000).
patients with idiopathic nightmares or control subjects Abnormal reduced heart-rate variability has also
(Germain and Nielsen, 2003). been reported in a wide spectrum of anxiety disorders,
including PTSD and panic disorder, conditions frequently
Other neurophysiologic studies of associated with intense nightmares (Nielsen and Zadra,
nightmares: EEG, evoked potentials 2000; Nielsen et al., 2010).
Only a few studies have been performed to assess other
OTHER PSG AND EEG SIGNAL ANALYSIS STUDIES
neurophysiologic aspects of patients with nightmare dis-
order. In one study, 60-min EEG studies and auditory An EEG study of 17 young individuals with frequent
evoked potentials (AEP or BAEPs) were performed on nightmares without neurologic, psychiatric, or sleep
subjects afflicted with recurrent nightmares (Newell disorders showed reduced sleep efficiency, increased
et al., 1992). They reported that all had normal EEGs. wakefulness, less slow-wave sleep, and increased noc-
BAEPs and AEPs were also within clinical norms, with turnal awakenings from N2 sleep compared with controls
amplitudes of N100, P160, and N200 not significantly (Simor et al., 2012). A longer duration of REM sleep was
different from controls (Newell et al., 1992). Nightmare reported to be mediated by heightened negative effect.
sufferers were reported to have significantly less sleep, The sleep architecture was clearly disturbed in the night-
more awakenings, and reduced slow-wave sleep during mare group in spite of adequate subjective sleep quality
overnight sleep recording, compared to published sleep reported by the subjects. The authors discuss that frag-
norms (Newell et al., 1992). mented sleep could trigger the appearance of dysphoric
Using EEG power spectra, during nightmares higher dreams, especially in comorbid trait-like effects of affec-
absolute and relative alpha-power and higher fast beta tive dysregulation or state-like effects of current stress, or
power over frontotemporal regions, as well as a distinct that the high number of awakenings and more superficial
amplification of classic posterior alpha were seen, com- sleep could promote the recall of dreams including the
pared with frontal brain sites (Nielsen and Zadra, 2000). dysphoric ones, concluding that impaired sleep continu-
In a later study of subjects with frequent nightmares and ity is an integral part of nightmare disorder (Simor et al.,
healthy controls, REM sleep propensity and increased 2012). The same group investigated relative spectral
REM sleep pressure were studied after partial REM sleep power values for non-REM and REM sleep in individ-
deprivation. The partial REM deprivation mildly reduced uals with nightmare disorder and found increased high
REM percentage from 17.8% to 13.1% N2 (P ¼ 0.002) alpha (10–14.5 Hz) and frontocentral increases in high
in nightmare sufferers, and from 19.0% to 13.2% delta (3–4 Hz) power during REM sleep and a trend
in controls (P ¼ 0.001). Subjects with nightmares toward increased frontocentral low alpha (7.75–9 Hz)
showed less evidence of REM rebound. Nightmare suf- power in non-REM sleep, independent of waking
ferers had signs of lower-than-normal REM propensity emotional distress (Simor et al., 2013b). They concluded
for deprivation sleep as well as during recovery sleep. that high REM alpha and low non-REM alpha power
390 A. STEFANI ET AL.
are strongly related in nightmare, suggesting the conductance correlated with time to fall asleep and panic
possible presence of wake-like features during REM and upon awakening. The authors demonstrated that auto-
signs of increased alertness during non-REM (Simor nomic reactions were the most reliable correlates of pure
et al., 2013b). sleep and health and concluded that autonomic reactions
In a further study the same group reported increased to nightmare imagery appear related to sleep disturbance
alpha power and no increased heart-rate variability and health symptomatology. Reducing nightmare-
during pre-REM periods in subjects with nightmares, related autonomic arousal could improve sleep and
indicating compromised non-REM/REM transition in health (Rhudy et al., 2008).
these subjects (Simor et al., 2014). Another study in individuals with frequent night-
A subsequent study concluded that non-REM sleep mares but not PTSD demonstrated similar sleep distur-
microstructure is altered even during nonsymptomatic bances compared to those with PTSD (Germain and
nights in patients with frequent nightmares and that Nielsen, 2003).
disrupted sleep seems to be related to abnormal arousal PSGs performed on patients with PTSD and comorbid
processes (Simor et al., 2013a). Another group found panic and nightmare did not show the normal differences
that patients with idiopathic nightmares had significantly in respiratory rates between non-REM and REM sleep;
less microarousals from N3 sleep and significantly PTSD patients with trauma-related nightmare complaints
more microarousals from REM sleep compared to exhibited higher sleep respiration rates in both REM and
patients with sleepwalking. The total REM percentage non-REM sleep (Woodward et al., 2003).
did not differ between the two groups (Perogamvros
et al., 2015).
PTSD AND PSG
Nightmares in posttraumatic stress disorder In combat-exposed military veterans, eveningness influ-
enced PTSD symptoms (greater lifetime PTSD symp-
EVOKED POTENTIALS toms) and sleep, with more disturbed sleep, and more
A study of combat veterans with PTSD investigated frequent and intense nightmares. (Hasler et al., 2013).
the P1 midlatency auditory evoked potential and its In women with PTSD, the heart-rate response signif-
habituation with a paired stimulus (Gillette et al., icantly correlated with Clinician-Administered PTSD
1997). Compared to various control groups, PTSD Scale (CAPS) total score and with CAPS nightmares.
subjects exhibited significantly diminished habituation The authors discussed that the association of night-
of the P1 potential, which is correlated with the intensity mares with greater heart-rate response in the absence
of PTSD symptoms, such as trauma-related nightmares of association with larger skin conductance response
and flashbacks. Based on these findings, the authors likely reflects reduced parasympathetic tone (Tanev
suggested the presence of a sensory gating defect at et al., 2017).
the brainstem level in PTSD (Gillette et al., 1997). The association of PTSD and sleep apnea was inves-
In a group of combat-veteran patients with PTSD tigated in PTSD. Overall nightmare severity was similar
studied with PSG, trauma-related nightmare complaints in PTSD with and without OSA (van Liempt et al., 2011).
were not associated with an effect on sleep latency but The frequency of an apnea-hypopnea index (AHI) > 10/h
instead with effects on wake after sleep onset. In the was similar among all groups, but the mean CAPS score
discussion, the authors suggest that in their sample of was significantly higher in patients with sleep apnea
chronic, severe, combat-related PTSD patients, trauma- compared to those without sleep apnea. These findings
related nightmares were associated with increased wake suggested that comorbid OSA leads to an increase in
after sleep onset, whereas other types of nightmares were PTSD complaints (van Liempt et al., 2011).
not (Woodward et al., 2000). A study of patients with PTSD, frequent nightmares,
and sleep disturbance after administration of prazosin
found that prazosin compared with placebo increased
AUTONOMIC NEUROPHYSIOLOGIC STUDIES
total sleep time by 94 min, and increased REM sleep time
PTSD
IN NIGHTMARES WITH
and mean REM period duration without altering sleep
Assessing arousal response, heart rate, skin conductance, onset latency (Taylor et al., 2008). A study using a rodent
and facial EMG reactions (corrugator and lateralis model of PTSD found acute increases in REM sleep and
frontalis muscle) in trauma-exposed individuals suffer- transition to REM sleep, which significantly correlated
ing from chronic nightmares, investigators found facial with PTSD severity. They also reported that reductions
EMG reactions and heart rate were associated with in delta and sigma pent power during transition to
increased global sleep problems, fewer hours slept per REM sleep also correlated with impaired fear-associated
night, and panic upon waking (Rhudy et al., 2008). Skin memory processing in rodents. The author suggested
CLINICAL NEUROPHYSIOLOGY OF REM 391
that changes in REM sleep and transition to REM OTHER COMORBIDITIES
sleep may serve as sleep biomarkers to identify
Nightmares appear to be significantly more prevalent in
increased susceptibility to PTSD following trauma
both narcolepsy type 1 and 2, while PSG showed more
exposure (Vanderheyden et al., 2015).
wakefulness and a higher percentage of non-REM stage
N1 sleep in narcolepsy type 1 (Pisko et al., 2014).
Nightmares in other disorders The term trauma associated sleep disorder (TASD)
has been proposed to describe disruptive nocturnal
Nightmares also occur in other settings such as physio-
behaviors and nightmares following traumatic experi-
logic dreaming and lucid dreaming. Disturbed dreaming
ences (Mysliwiec et al., 2014). In one PSG study,
is also observed in sleep terrors, terrifying hypnagogic
REM without atonia was present in 4 patients with
hallucinations, nightmares in PTSD, dreams in narco-
TASD, and the SINBAR EMG activity index ranged
lepsy, sleep paralysis, and RBD (reviewed in Nielsen
from 13.7% to 37.6%, with only one patient exceeding
and Zadra, 2000).
the cut-off for RBD (Frauscher et al., 2012a). TSAD
In a series of 20 patients with temporal lobe epilepsy
could be distinguished as a unique sleep disorder because
screening positive for parasomnias and undergoing
patients were young, had a precipitating event, and had
PSG, 16 had confusional arousals, 14 had recurrent
nightmares with a specific topic. In contrast to RBD,
nightmares, 2 had sleepwalking episodes, and 4 had
in TASD an overdrive phenomenon appears to occur,
night terrors in different combinations (Silvestri and
resulting in loss of REM atonia and loss of sympathetic
Bromfield, 2004). Half the patients had documented
suppression (Mysliwiec et al., 2014).
episodes during PSG (again confusional arousals, night
terrors, sleepwalking, and nightmare out of stage 2).
REFERENCES
SLEEP APNEA AND NIGHTMARES American Academy of Sleep Medicine (2014). The interna-
tional classification of sleep disorders: diagnostic & coding
One study of patients with PSG-confirmed OSA found
manual (ICSD-3), American Academy of Sleep Medicine,
that respiratory parameters, as a measure of sleep apnea
Westchester, IL.
severity, did not correlate with nightmare frequency, American Academy of Sleep Medicine (2016). AASM manual
thereby refuting a simple oxygen hypothesis for night- for the scoring of sleep and associated events. 2.3.
mares (Schredl et al., 2006). American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and
Another study investigated clinical and PSG charac- statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5, American
teristics and response to positive airway pressure therapy Psychiatric Association.
in 99 patients with comorbid OSA and nightmares BaHammam AS, Al-Shimemeri SA, Salama RI et al. (2013).
(BaHammam et al., 2013). The study found that OSA Clinical and polysomnographic characteristics and response
patients with nightmares had a significantly higher to continuous positive airway pressure therapy in obstructive
AHI during REM sleep compared with the OSA patients sleep apnea patients with nightmares. Sleep Med 14:
149–154.
without nightmares (51.7 vs 39.8); REM AHI and inter-
Baumann C, Lentzsch F, Regard M et al. (2007). The halluci-
rupted sleep were independent predictors of nightmares
nating art of Heinrich Fussli. Front Neurol Neurosci 22:
in the OSA patients. Nightmares disappeared in 91% of 223–235.
the patients who accepted CPAP treatment, compared Bliwise DL, He L, Ansari FP et al. (2006). Quantification of
with 36% of those who refused CPAP (BaHammam electromyographic activity during sleep: a phasic electro-
et al., 2013). myographic metric. J Clin Neurophysiol 23: 59–67.
CPAP-therapy reduced PTSD-associated nightmares Boeve BF, Silber MH, Ferman TJ et al. (2013).
and PTSD symptoms in a group of patients with OSA, Clinicopathologic correlations in 172 cases of rapid eye
PTSD, and nightmares (Tamanna et al., 2014). movement sleep behavior disorder with or without a
A PSG study investigated female patients with coexisting neurologic disorder. Sleep Med 14: 754–762.
borderline personality disorder, with comorbid post- Brooks PL, Peever JH (2011). Impaired GABA and glycine
transmission triggers cardinal features of rapid eye move-
traumatic stress response in 33%. Borderline personality
ment sleep behavior disorder in mice. J Neurosci 31:
patients compared with controls had only slightly
7111–7121.
decreased sleep efficiency, but the number of awaken- Brooks PL, Peever JH (2012). Identification of the transmitter
ings, time awake, and number of arousals were signi- and receptor mechanisms responsible for REM sleep paral-
ficantly increased. The patients also had shorter ysis. J Neurosci 32: 9785–9795.
REM latencies, higher REM densities, and more non- Brooks PL, Peever J (2016). A temporally controlled inhibi-
REM or awake epochs during REM periods (Schredl tory drive coordinates twitch movements during REM
et al., 2012). sleep. Curr Biol 26: 1177–1182.
392 A. STEFANI ET AL.
Broughton RJ (1968). Sleep disorders: disorders of arousal? Ferri R, Rundo F, Manconi M et al. (2010). Improved compu-
Enuresis, somnambulism, and nightmares occur in confu- tation of the atonia index in normal controls and patients
sional states of arousal, not in dreaming sleep. Science with REM sleep behavior disorder. Sleep Med 11:
159: 1070–1078. 947–949.
Brown P, Rothwell JC, Thompson PD et al. (1991). New obser- Ferri R, Fulda S, Manconi M et al. (2013). Night-to-night var-
vations on the normal auditory startle reflex in man. Brain iability of periodic leg movements during sleep in restless
114 (Pt 4): 1891–1902. legs syndrome and periodic limb movement disorder: com-
Buskova J, Pisko J, Dostalova S et al. (2013). Incomplete sleep parison between the periodicity index and the PLMS index.
paralysis as the first symptom of narcolepsy. Sleep Med 14: Sleep Med 14: 293–296.
919–921. Ferri R, Rundo F, Silvani A et al. (2017). REM sleep EEG
Carskadon MA, Dement WC (1975). Sleep studies on a instability in REM sleep behavior disorder and clonazepam
90-minute day. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol effects. Sleep 40 (8).
39: 145–155. Fisher C, Byrne J, Edwards A et al. (1970).
Chase MH, Francisco R (1994). The control of motoneurons A psychophysiological study of nightmares. J Am
during sleep. In: MH Kryger, T Roth, WC Dement Psychoanal Assoc 18: 747–782.
(Eds.), Principles and practice of sleep medicine, 2nd ed. Fisher C, Kahn E, Edwards A et al. (1973).
Saunders, Philadelphia; London. A psychophysiological study of nightmares and night ter-
Christensen JA, Zoetmulder M, Koch H et al. (2014). Data- rors. I. Physiological aspects of the stage 4 night terror.
driven modeling of sleep EEG and EOG reveals character- J Nerv Ment Dis 157: 75–98.
istics indicative of pre-Parkinson’s and Parkinson’s dis- Fraigne JJ, Torontali ZA, Snow MB et al. (2015). REM sleep at
ease. J Neurosci Methods 235: 262–276. its core–circuits, neurotransmitters, and pathophysiology.
Dahms C, Guenther A, Schwab M et al. (2016). Dysautonomia Front Neurol 6: 123.
in prodromal alpha-synucleinopathy: peripheral versus Frandsen R, Nikolic M, Zoetmulder M et al. (2015). Analysis
central autonomic degeneration. Eur J Neurol 23: 878–890. of automated quantification of motor activity in REM sleep
Dodet P, Chavez M, Leu-Semenescu S et al. (2015). Lucid behaviour disorder. J Sleep Res 24: 583–590.
dreaming in narcolepsy. Sleep 38: 487–497. Frauscher B, Gschliesser V, Brandauer E et al. (2009). The
Dyken ME, Yamada T, Lin-Dyken DC et al. (1996). relation between abnormal behaviors and REM sleep
Diagnosing narcolepsy through the simultaneous clinical microstructure in patients with REM sleep behavior
and electrophysiologic analysis of cataplexy. Arch disorder. Sleep Med 10: 174–181.
Neurol 53: 456–460. Frauscher B, Gschliesser V, Brandauer E et al. (2010). REM
Eisensehr I, Linke R, Tatsch K et al. (2003). Increased muscle sleep behavior disorder in 703 sleep-disorder patients:
activity during rapid eye movement sleep correlates with the importance of eliciting a comprehensive sleep history.
decrease of striatal presynaptic dopamine transporters. Sleep Med 11: 167–171.
IPT and IBZM SPECT imaging in subclinical and clini- Frauscher B, Iranzo A, Gaig C et al. (2012a). Normative EMG
cally manifest idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder, values during REM sleep for the diagnosis of REM sleep
Parkinson’s disease, and controls. Sleep 26: 507–512. behavior disorder. Sleep 35: 835–847.
Fantini ML, Michaud M, Gosselin N et al. (2002). Periodic leg Frauscher B, Nomura T, Duerr S et al. (2012b). Investigation
movements in REM sleep behavior disorder and related of autonomic function in idiopathic REM sleep behavior
autonomic and EEG activation. Neurology 59: 1889–1894. disorder. J Neurol 259: 1056–1061.
Fantini ML, Gagnon JF, Petit D et al. (2003). Slowing of elec- Frauscher B, Ehrmann L, Mitterling T et al. (2013). Delayed
troencephalogram in rapid eye movement sleep behavior diagnosis, range of severity, and multiple sleep comorbid-
disorder. Ann Neurol 53: 774–780. ities: a clinical and polysomnographic analysis of 100
Ferini-Strambi L, Oldani A, Zucconi M et al. (1996). Cardiac patients of the Innsbruck narcolepsy cohort. J Clin Sleep
autonomic activity during wakefulness and sleep in REM Med 9: 805–812.
sleep behavior disorder. Sleep 19: 367–369. Frauscher B, Gabelia D, Biermayr M et al. (2014a).
Fernandez-Arcos A, Iranzo A, Serradell M et al. (2016). The Validation of an integrated software for the detection
clinical phenotype of idiopathic rapid eye movement sleep of rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder. Sleep
behavior disorder at presentation: a study in 203 consecu- 37: 1663–1671.
tive patients. Sleep 39: 121–132. Frauscher B, Jennum P, Ju YE et al. (2014b). Comorbidity and
Fernandez-Arcos A, Iranzo A, Serradell M et al. (2017). medication in REM sleep behavior disorder: a multicenter
Diagnostic value of isolated mentalis versus mentalis plus case-control study. Neurology 82: 1076–1079.
upper limb electromyography in idiopathic rem sleep Galimberti CA, Ossola M, Colnaghi S et al. (2009). Focal
behavior disorder patients eventually developing a neuro- epileptic seizures mimicking sleep paralysis. Epilepsy
degenerative syndrome. Sleep 40 (4). Behav 14: 562–564.
Ferri R, Manconi M, Plazzi G et al. (2008). A quantitative Gaudreault PO, Gagnon JF, Montplaisir J et al. (2013).
statistical analysis of the submentalis muscle EMG ampli- Abnormal occipital event-related potentials in Parkinson’s
tude during sleep in normal controls and patients with REM disease with concomitant REM sleep behavior disorder.
sleep behavior disorder. J Sleep Res 17: 89–100. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 19: 212–217.
CLINICAL NEUROPHYSIOLOGY OF REM 393
Germain A, Nielsen TA (2003). Sleep pathophysiology in Koley B, Dey D (2012). An ensemble system for automatic
posttraumatic stress disorder and idiopathic nightmare sleep stage classification using single channel EEG signal.
sufferers. Biol Psychiatry 54: 1092–1098. Comput Biol Med 42: 1186–1195.
Gillette GM, Skinner RD, Rasco LM et al. (1997). Combat vet- Krakow B (2006). Nightmare complaints in treatment-seeking
erans with posttraumatic stress disorder exhibit decreased patients in clinical sleep medicine settings: diagnostic and
habituation of the P1 midlatency auditory evoked potential. treatment implications. Sleep 29: 1313–1319.
Life Sci 61: 1421–1434. Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC (1994). Principles
Guilleminault C, Raynal D, Takahashi S et al. (1976). and practice of sleep medicine, Saunders, Philadelphia;
Evaluation of short-term and long-term treatment of the London.
narcolepsy syndrome with clomipramine hydrochloride. Lai YY, Hsieh KC, Nguyen D et al. (2008). Neurotoxic
Acta Neurol Scand 54: 71–87. lesions at the ventral mesopontine junction change
Hartmann E (1984). The nightmare: the psychology and biol- sleep time and muscle activity during sleep: an animal
ogy of terrifying dreams, Basic Books, New York. model of motor disorders in sleep. Neuroscience 154:
Hasler BP, Insana SP, James JA et al. (2013). Evening-type 431–443.
military veterans report worse lifetime posttraumatic stress Lapierre O, Montplaisir J (1992). Polysomnographic features
symptoms and greater brainstem activity across wakeful- of REM sleep behavior disorder: development of a scoring
ness and REM sleep. Biol Psychol 94: 255–262. method. Neurology 42: 1371–1374.
Hassan AR, Bhuiyan MI (2017). Automated identification Latreille V, Carrier J, Montplaisir J et al. (2011).
of sleep states from EEG signals by means of ensemble Non-rapid eye movement sleep characteristics in idio-
empirical mode decomposition and random under sampling pathic REM sleep behavior disorder. J Neurol Sci 310:
boosting. Comput Methods Programs Biomed 140: 201–210. 159–162.
Hishikawa Y, Shimizu T (1995). Physiology of REM sleep, Leclair-Visonneau L, Oudiette D, Gaymard B et al.
cataplexy, and sleep paralysis. Adv Neurol 67: 245–271. (2010). Do the eyes scan dream images during rapid
H€ogl B, Iranzo A (2017). s behavior disorder and other rapid eye movement sleep? Evidence from the rapid eye
eye movement parasomnias. Continuum 23 (4): 1017–1034. movement sleep behaviour disorder model. Brain 133:
H€ogl B, Heidbreder A, Santamaria J et al. (2015). IgLON5 1737–1746.
autoimmunity and abnormal behaviours during sleep. Liu M, Blanco-Centurion C, Konadhode R et al. (2011).
Lancet 385: 1590. Orexin gene transfer into zona incerta neurons suppresses
H€ogl B, Stefani A, Videnovic A (2018). Idiopathic REM muscle paralysis in narcoleptic mice. J Neurosci 31:
sleep behaviour disorder and neurodegeneration—an 6028–6040.
update. Nat Rev Neurol 14 (1): 40–55. https://doi.org/ Mahowald MW, Schenck CH (2005). Insights from studying
10.1038/nrneurol.2017.157. human sleep disorders. Nature 437: 1279–1285.
Hsieh SW, Lai CL, Liu CK et al. (2010). Isolated sleep paral- Manconi M, Ferri R, Zucconi M et al. (2007). Time structure
ysis linked to impaired nocturnal sleep quality and health- analysis of leg movements during sleep in REM sleep
related quality of life in Chinese-Taiwanese patients with behavior disorder. Sleep 30: 1779–1785.
obstructive sleep apnea. Qual Life Res 19: 1265–1272. Manni R, Terzaghi M, Glorioso M (2009). Motor-behavioral
Iranzo A, Isetta V, Molinuevo JL et al. (2010). episodes in REM sleep behavior disorder and phasic events
Electroencephalographic slowing heralds mild cognitive during REM sleep. Sleep 32: 241–245.
impairment in idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder. Massicotte-Marquez J, Carrier J, Decary A et al. (2005).
Sleep Med 11: 534–539. Slow-wave sleep and delta power in rapid eye movement
Iranzo A, Frauscher B, Santos H et al. (2011). Usefulness of the sleep behavior disorder. Ann Neurol 57: 277–282.
SINBAR electromyographic montage to detect the motor Massicotte-Marquez J, Decary A, Gagnon JF et al. (2008).
and vocal manifestations occurring in REM sleep behavior Executive dysfunction and memory impairment in
disorder. Sleep Med 12: 284–288. idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder. Neurology 70:
Iranzo A, Santamaria J, Tolosa E (2016). Idiopathic rapid eye 1250–1257.
movement sleep behaviour disorder: diagnosis, manage- McCarty DE, Chesson Jr AL (2009). A case of sleep paralysis
ment, and the need for neuroprotective interventions. with hypnopompic hallucinations. Recurrent isolated sleep
Lancet Neurol 15: 405–419. paralysis associated with hypnopompic hallucinations, pre-
Iranzo A, Stefani A, Serradell M et al. (2017). Characterization cipitated by behaviorally induced insufficient sleep syn-
of patients with longstanding idiopathic REM sleep behav- drome. J Clin Sleep Med 5: 83–84.
ior disorder. Neurology 89: 242–248. Miranda M, Bustamante ML (2015). Depiction of parasomnia
Ju YE, Larson-Prior L, Duntley S (2011). Changing demo- in the arts. Somnologie (Berl) 19: 248–253.
graphics in REM sleep behavior disorder: possible effect Miranda M, Hogl B (2013). Guy de Maupassant and
of autoimmunity and antidepressants. Sleep Med 12: his account of sleep paralysis in his tale, "the Horla".
278–283. Sleep Med 14: 578–580.
Jung KY, Cho JH, Ko D et al. (2012). Increased corticomus- Mouret J (1975). Differences in sleep in patients with
cular coherence in idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder. Parkinson’s disease. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol
Front Neurol 3: 60. 38: 653–657.
394 A. STEFANI ET AL.
Mysliwiec V, O’Reilly B, Polchinski J et al. (2014). Trauma Raggi A, Manconi M, Consonni M et al. (2007). Event-related
associated sleep disorder: a proposed parasomnia encom- potentials in idiopathic rapid eye movements sleep behav-
passing disruptive nocturnal behaviors, nightmares, and iour disorder. Clin Neurophysiol 118: 669–675.
REM without atonia in trauma survivors. J Clin Sleep Rhudy JL, Davis JL, Williams AE et al. (2008). Physiological-
Med 10: 1143–1148. emotional reactivity to nightmare-related imagery in
Nan’no H, Hishikawa Y, Koida H et al. (1970). A neurophys- trauma-exposed persons with chronic nightmares. Behav
iological study of sleep paralysis in narcoleptic patients. Sleep Med 6: 158–177.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 28: 382–390. Rodrigues Brazete J, Montplaisir J, Petit D et al. (2013).
Nardone R, Bergmann J, Kunz A et al. (2012). Cortical afferent Electroencephalogram slowing in rapid eye movement
inhibition is reduced in patients with idiopathic REM sleep sleep behavior disorder is associated with mild cognitive
behavior disorder and cognitive impairment: a TMS study. impairment. Sleep Med 14: 1059–1063.
Sleep Med 13: 919–925. Rodrigues Brazete J, Gagnon JF, Postuma RB et al. (2016).
Newell SA, Padamadan H, Drake Jr ME (1992). Electroencephalogram slowing predicts neurodegeneration
Neurophysiologic studies in nightmare sufferers. Clin in rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder. Neurobiol
Electroencephalogr 23: 203–206. Aging 37: 74–81.
Nielsen TA, Paquette T, Solomonova E et al. (2010). Sabater L, Gaig C, Gelpi E et al. (2014). A novel non-rapid-
REM sleep characteristics of nightmare sufferers before eye movement and rapid-eye-movement parasomnia with
and after REM sleep deprivation. Sleep Med 11 (2): sleep breathing disorder associated with antibodies to
172–179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2008.12.018. IgLON5: a case series, characterisation of the antigen,
Nielsen TA, Zadra A (2000). Dreaming disorders. In: and post-mortem study. Lancet Neurol 13: 575–586.
MH Kryger, T Roth, WC Dement (Eds.), Principles and Sasai T, Inoue Y, Matsuura M (2011). Clinical significance
practice of sleep medicine, third edn. Saunders, Philadelphia. of periodic leg movements during sleep in rapid eye
Ohayon MM, Schenck CH (2010). Violent behavior during movement sleep behavior disorder. J Neurol 258:
sleep: prevalence, comorbidity and consequences. Sleep 1971–1978.
Med 11: 941–946. Sasai T, Matsuura M, Inoue Y (2013). Electroencephalographic
O’Reilly C, Godin I, Montplaisir J et al. (2015). REM findings related with mild cognitive impairment in
sleep behaviour disorder is associated with lower fast idiopathic rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder.
and higher slow sleep spindle densities. J Sleep Res 24: Sleep 36: 1893–1899.
593–601. Schaltenbrand N, Lengelle R, Toussaint M et al. (1996). Sleep
Perogamvros L, Aberg K, Gex-Fabry M et al. (2015). stage scoring using the neural network model: comparison
Increased reward-related behaviors during sleep and between visual and automatic analysis in normal subjects
wakefulness in sleepwalking and idiopathic nightmares. and patients. Sleep 19: 26–35.
PLoS One 10: e0134504. Schenck CH, Bundlie SR, Ettinger MG et al. (1986). Chronic
Peter A, Hansen ML, Merkl A et al. (2008). REM sleep behav- behavioral disorders of human REM sleep: a new category
ior disorder and excessive startle reaction to visual stimuli of parasomnia. Sleep 9: 293–308.
in a patient with pontine lesions. Sleep Med 9: 697–700. Schenck CH, Bundlie SR, Patterson AL et al. (1987). Rapid
Pisko J, Pastorek L, Buskova J et al. (2014). Nightmares eye movement sleep behavior disorder. A treatable para-
in narcolepsy: underinvestigated symptom? Sleep Med somnia affecting older adults. JAMA 257: 1786–1789.
15: 967–972. Schenck CH, Mahowald MW, Kim SW et al. (1992).
Postuma RB, Lanfranchi PA, Blais H et al. (2010). Cardiac Prominent eye movements during NREM sleep and REM
autonomic dysfunction in idiopathic REM sleep behavior sleep behavior disorder associated with fluoxetine treat-
disorder. Mov Disord 25: 2304–2310. ment of depression and obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Postuma RB, Bertrand JA, Montplaisir J et al. (2012). Rapid Sleep 15: 226–235.
eye movement sleep behavior disorder and risk of dementia Schenck CH, Boeve BF, Mahowald MW (2013). Delayed
in Parkinson’s disease: a prospective study. Mov Disord 27: emergence of a parkinsonian disorder or dementia in
720–726. 81% of older men initially diagnosed with idiopathic
Postuma RB, Gagnon JF, Tuineaig M et al. (2013). rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder: a 16-year
Antidepressants and REM sleep behavior disorder: update on a previously reported series. Sleep Med 14:
isolated side effect or neurodegenerative signal? Sleep 744–748.
36: 1579–1585. Schredl M, Schmitt J, Hein G et al. (2006). Nightmares and
Postuma RB, Iranzo A, Hogl B et al. (2015). Risk factors for oxygen desaturations: is sleep apnea related to heightened
neurodegeneration in idiopathic rapid eye movement sleep nightmare frequency? Sleep Breath 10: 203–209.
behavior disorder: a multicenter study. Ann Neurol 77: Schredl M, Paul F, Reinhard I et al. (2012). Sleep and
830–839. dreaming in patients with borderline personality disorder:
Raccagni C, Loscher WN, Stefani A et al. (2016). a polysomnographic study. Psychiatry Res 200: 430–436.
Peripheral nerve function in patients with excessive Silvestri R, Bromfield E (2004). Recurrent nightmares and dis-
fragmentary myoclonus during sleep. Sleep Med 22: orders of arousal in temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain Res Bull
61–64. 63: 369–376.
CLINICAL NEUROPHYSIOLOGY OF REM 395
Simor P, Horvath K, Gombos F et al. (2012). Disturbed dream- Terzaghi M, Sartori I, Tassi L et al. (2009). Evidence of disso-
ing and sleep quality: altered sleep architecture in subjects ciated arousal states during NREM parasomnia from an
with frequent nightmares. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin intracerebral neurophysiological study. Sleep 32: 409–412.
Neurosci 262: 687–696. Terzaghi M, Ratti PL, Manni F et al. (2012). Sleep paralysis in
Simor P, Bodizs R, Horvath K et al. (2013a). Disturbed dream- narcolepsy: more than just a motor dissociative phenome-
ing and the instability of sleep: altered nonrapid eye move- non? Neurol Sci 33: 169–172.
ment sleep microstructure in individuals with frequent Tokimura H, Di Lazzaro V, Tokimura Y et al. (2000). Short
nightmares as revealed by the cyclic alternating pattern. latency inhibition of human hand motor cortex by somato-
Sleep 36: 413–419. sensory input from the hand. J Physiol 523: 503–513.
Simor P, Horvath K, Ujma PP et al. (2013b). Fluctuations Torterolo P, Castro-Zaballa S, Cavelli M et al. (2016).
between sleep and wakefulness: wake-like features indi- Neocortical 40 Hz oscillations during carbachol-induced
cated by increased EEG alpha power during different sleep rapid eye movement sleep and cataplexy. Eur J Neurosci
stages in nightmare disorder. Biol Psychol 94: 592–600. 43: 580–589.
Simor P, Kormendi J, Horvath K et al. (2014). Traczynska-Kubin D, Atzef E, Petre-Quadens O (1969).
Electroencephalographic and autonomic alterations in sub- Sleep in parkinsonism. Acta Neurol Psychiatr Belg 69:
jects with frequent nightmares during pre-and post-REM 727–733.
periods. Brain Cogn 91: 62–70. Tsuji Y, Satoh H, Itoh N et al. (2000). Automatic detection of
Sorensen GL, Kempfner J, Zoetmulder M et al. (2012). rapid eye movements by discrete wavelet transform.
Attenuated heart rate response in REM sleep behavior dis- Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 54: 276–277.
order and Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 27: 888–894. Uguccioni G, Golmard JL, de Fontreaux AN et al. (2013).
Sorensen GL, Mehlsen J, Jennum P (2013). Reduced sympa- Fight or flight? Dream content during sleepwalking/sleep
thetic activity in idiopathic rapid-eye-movement sleep terrors vs. rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder.
behavior disorder and Parkinson’s disease. Auton Sleep Med 14: 391–398.
Neurosci 179: 138–141. Valencia Garcia S, Libourel P-A, Lazarus M et al. (2017).
Stefani A, Iranzo A, Santamaria J et al. (2017). Description of Genetic inactivation of glutamate neurons in the rat subla-
sleep paralysis in the brothers Karamazov by Dostoevsky. terodorsal tegmental nucleus recapitulates REM sleep
Sleep Med 32: 198–200. behaviour disorder. Brain 140: 414–428.
Stefani A, Heidbreder A, Brandauer E et al. (2018). Screening Valli K, Frauscher B, Gschliesser V et al. (2012).
for idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder: usefulness of Can observers link dream content to behaviours in
actigraphy. Sleep. 41 (6): zsy053. rapid eye movement sleep behaviour disorder? A cross-
Strobel AV, Tankisi H, Finnerup NB et al. (2018). sectional experimental pilot study. J Sleep Res 21:
Somatosensory function is impaired in patients with idio- 21–29.
pathic REM sleep behaviour disorder. Sleep Med 42: 83–89. Valli K, Frauscher B, Peltomaa T et al. (2015). Dreaming furi-
Sunwoo JS, Lee S, Kim JH et al. (2017). Altered functional ously? A sleep laboratory study on the dream content of
connectivity in idiopathic rapid eye movement sleep people with Parkinson’s disease and with or without rapid
behavior disorder: a resting-state EEG study. Sleep 40 eye movement sleep behavior disorder. Sleep Med 16:
(6): zsx058. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/zsx058. 419–427.
Suzuki H, Matsuura M, Moriguchi K et al. (2001). Two auto- van Liempt S, Westenberg HG, Arends J et al. (2011).
detection methods for eye movements during eyes closed. Obstructive sleep apnea in combat-related posttraumatic
Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 55: 197–198. stress disorder: a controlled polysomnography study. Eur
Tachibana M, Tanaka K, Hishikawa Y (1975). A sleep study of J Psychotraumatol 2.
acute psychotic states due to alcohol and meprobamate Vanderheyden WM, George SA, Urpa L et al. (2015). Sleep
addiction. In: ED Weitzman (Ed.), Advances in sleep alterations following exposure to stress predict fear-
research, Spectrum, New York. associated memory impairments in a rodent model of
Takeuchi T, Miyasita A, Sasaki Y et al. (1992). Isolated sleep PTSD. Exp Brain Res 233: 2335–2346.
paralysis elicited by sleep interruption. Sleep 15: 217–225. Wang Y, Loparo KA, Kelly MR et al. (2015). Evaluation of an
Tamanna S, Parker JD, Lyons J et al. (2014). The effect automated single-channel sleep staging algorithm. Nat Sci
of continuous positive air pressure (CPAP) on nightmares Sleep 7: 101–111.
in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and Wehrle R, Kaufmann C, Wetter TC et al. (2007). Functional
obstructive sleep apnea (OSA). J Clin Sleep Med 10: 631–636. microstates within human REM sleep: first evidence from
Tanev KS, Orr SP, Pace-Schott EF et al. (2017). Positive asso- fMRI of a thalamocortical network specific for phasic
ciation between nightmares and heart rate response to loud REM periods. Eur J Neurosci 25: 863–871.
tones: relationship to parasympathetic dysfunction in Weitzman ED, Kripke DF, Goldmacher D et al. (1970). Acute
PTSD nightmares. J Nerv Ment Dis 205: 308–312. reversal of the sleep-waking cycle in man. Effect on sleep
Taylor FB, Martin P, Thompson C et al. (2008). Prazosin stage patterns. Arch Neurol 22: 483–489.
effects on objective sleep measures and clinical symptoms Wing YK, Lee ST, Chen CN (1994). Sleep paralysis in
in civilian trauma posttraumatic stress disorder: a placebo- Chinese: ghost oppression phenomenon in Hong Kong.
controlled study. Biol Psychiatry 63: 629–632. Sleep 17: 609–613.
396 A. STEFANI ET AL.
Wing YK, Lam SP, Zhang J et al. (2015). Reduced striatal FURTHER READING
dopamine transmission in REM sleep behavior disorder
comorbid with depression. Neurology 84: 516–522. American Academy of Sleep Medicine (2005). International
Woodward SH, Arsenault NJ, Murray C et al. (2000). classification of sleep disorders; diagnostic and coding
Laboratory sleep correlates of nightmare complaint in manual, American Academy of Sleep Medicine,
PTSD inpatients. Biol Psychiatry 48: 1081–1087. Westchester, IL.
Woodward SH, Leskin GA, Sheikh JI (2003). Sleep respiratory American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and
concomitants of comorbid panic and nightmare complaint statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV, American
in post-traumatic stress disorder. Depress Anxiety 18: Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC.
198–204.
Yetton BD, Niknazar M, Duggan KA et al. (2016). Automatic
detection of rapid eye movements (REMs): a machine
learning approach. J Neurosci Methods 259: 72–82.

You might also like