Bastos & Haddad, 1996 - Breeding Activity of The Neotropical Treefrog Hyla Elegans

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Society for the Study of Amphibians and Reptiles

Breeding Activity of the Neotropical Treefrog Hyla elegans (Anura, Hylidae)


Author(s): Rogério P. Bastos and Célio F. B. Haddad
Source: Journal of Herpetology, Vol. 30, No. 3 (Sep., 1996), pp. 355-360
Published by: Society for the Study of Amphibians and Reptiles
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1565172
Accessed: 06/11/2009 09:03

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless
you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you
may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.

Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at
http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=ssar.

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed
page of such transmission.

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Society for the Study of Amphibians and Reptiles is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend
access to Journal of Herpetology.

http://www.jstor.org
Vol. 30, No. 3, pp. 355-360, 1996
Journalof Herpetology,
Copyright1996Societyfor the Study of Amphibiansand Reptiles

Breeding Activity of the Neotropical Treefrog


Hyla elegans (Anura, Hylidae)
ROGERIOP. BASTOS'AND CELIO F. B. HADDAD2

de BiologiaGeral,ICB,UFG,CaixaPostal131,74001-970Goidnia,Goids,Brasil,and
'Departamento
de Zoologia,UNESP,CaixaPostal199, 13506-900RioClaro,SaoPaulo,Brasil
2Departamento

ABSTRACT. - Breeding of Hyla elegans was monitored from August 1991to July 1992at a temporarypond
in Ubatuba, State of Sao Paulo, southeastern Brazil. Males began to call as they entered the chorus, and
defended their calling sites from other males, at times with physical interactions. Females, however, were
not aggressive toward either males or other females. We found a positive correlation between the numbers
of females and males in the chorus, but no significant correlation between OSR (number of reproducing
females/number of reproducing males) and the number of males present. OSR was highly male-biased;
on average, there were 10 males for each female; this low OSR may explain low average mating success
of males. Females chose males as mates freely, and males did not attempt to intercept females approaching
other males. Males in amplexus were larger and heavier than unmated, calling males. In addition, snout-
vent lengths of males and females in amplexus were positively correlated, and males were, on average,
0.81 the length of females. Experimentally paired males and females with smaller or larger ratios of SVLs
had a lower percent of fertilization than pairs near the population average.

During the breeding season, many anurans to Sao Paulo, Brazil (Lutz, 1973; Frost, 1985),
aggregate in certain areas to reproduce (Wells, and from sea level to approximately 800 m (Lutz,
1977). At these aggregations, social interactions 1973). The present study describes the breeding
are frequent, and reproductive behavior can be behavior of H. elegans, in southeastern Brazil,
complex (Arak, 1983). In many species, mating with emphasis on sexual selection.
occurs nonrandomly with respect to variation Our aims were to: (1) quantify the reproduc-
in male traits (see Cherry, 1992). For example, tive parameters of both sexes (temporal distri-
males in amplexus can be larger than solitary bution, sex ratio, size, amplexus, and clutches),
ones (e.g., Lee and Crump, 1981; Ryan, 1983), (2) determine whether matings are non-random
or there can be positive assortative mating by in relation to male size, (3) verify the occurrence
size or mass (e.g., Davies and Halliday, 1977; of assortative mating (i.e., if females select males
Robertson, 1986). that are proportional in size to themselves), and
The two mechanisms of sexual selection pro- (4) verify if relative sizes of mates influence
posed by Darwin (1871), male-male competition fertilization.
and female choice, are involved in the genesis
of nonrandom mating patterns (Olson et al., MATERIALS AND METHODS

1986). In anurans, the intensity of each mech- Hyla eleganswas observed in a temporary pond
anism of sexual selection depends, in part, on at Nuicleo Picinguaba (44?50'W and 23?23'S),
the duration of breeding period (Wells, 1977; Parque Estadual da Serra do Mar, in the mu-
Ryan, 1985). In anurans with prolonged breed- nicipality of Ubatuba, State of Sao Paulo, Brazil,
ing seasons, females generally arrive at breed- between August 1991 and July 1992. The area
ing sites asynchronously, having the opportu- of the pond was approximately 2600 m2. Due
nity to choose males (Arak, 1983) that defend to irregular rainfall during 1991 and 1992, the
territories for vocalization and/or oviposition pond dried more than once. We visited the study
(Wells, 1977; Haddad and Cardoso, 1992). In pond either fortnightly or monthly, monitoring
contrast to prolonged breeders, female anurans the pond for 2-10 nights during each visit. Field
that breed explosively have little opportunity observations were conducted from 1730 h until
to choose males (Arak, 1983). In such a pattern, 2400 to 0200 h, depending on activity of tree-
characteristics that confer greater advantage in frogs. We monitored the pond for a total of 278
male-male competition will be more important h in 73 visits.
for reproductive success (Wells, 1977; Halliday, We marked 330 males and 75 females by toe
1983). clipping (Martof, 1953), representing approxi-
We conducted a long-term study of the tree- mately 40% of the population at the pond (Bas-
frog Hyla elegans Wied-Neuwied, a species that tos, 1993). We measured the snout-vent length
has a wide geographical distribution along the (SVL) of individuals to the nearest 0.1 mm with
Atlantic Forest, from the States of Pernambuco a caliper ruler and weighed individuals to the
356 R. P. BASTOS AND C. F. B. HADDAD

TABLE1. Comparisons (t-values) of mean snout-vent length and mean mass of males and females of Hyla
elegans. * Female mass after oviposition.

Males Females
N Mean + SD Range N Mean + SD Range t df P
SVL (mm) 258 25.74 + 1.14 22.1-29.6 49 32.34 + 1.55 28.8-35.7 28.16 57 <0.01
Mass (g) 223 1.17 ? 0.20 0.7-1.8 59 2.01 + 0.26* 1.3-2.6* 21.23 61 <0.01

nearest 0.1 g with a balance. We counted the number of calling males decreased (Fig. 1). The
total number of males and females in the pond first females arrived at the chorus one or two
and recorded the location of these individuals, hours after sunset (x + SD = 81 min ? 35, range
territorial encounters, matings, and number of = 50-131, N = 6 nights).
clutches. The operational sex ratio (or OSR, defined as
All pairs found in amplexus were collected the number of reproducing females/number of
and placed in plastic bags containing water from reproducing males) was highly male-biased
the pond until females laid eggs. The clutches during the entire study period; estimates of OSR
obtained were preserved in 5%formalin. Males varied from 0.024 to 0.224 (x + SD = 0.083 +
and females were weighed, measured, and re- 0.05; N = 23 nights). Number of mating females
leased on the following night. was positively correlated with the number of
To test the hypothesis that females choose males (r = 0.818, P < 0.01; N = 23 nights), being
males that maximize fertilization success, we on average 10 males:1 female. Estimates of OSR
captured pairs in amplexus and unmated, call- were not significantly correlated with the num-
ing males. These individuals were transported ber of males present (r = 0.307, P > 0.05; N =
to the laboratory, where the original male of a 23 nights), indicating that females were not pro-
pair in amplexus was exchanged with another portionally more abundant in larger choruses
of greater or smaller SVL. Clutches produced (Fig. 2).
by the experimental pairs were maintained in Male and FemaleBehaviorin the Chorus.-Male
the laboratory for two days, after which the H. elegans defended calling sites by calling (see
number of embryos was counted. Bastos and Haddad, 1995) and through physical
For the statistical analyses of the results we combat. These territories were not used for ovi-
used the Student's t-test or the Pearson corre- position or shelter.
lation coefficient (Zar, 1984). Data of relative Few males (N = 4) adopted a crouched pos-
sizes of mates influencing fertilization were fit- ture, did not emit calls (their vocal sacs were
ted to a second-degree polynomial regression not filled), and remained between 10 to 30 cm
(Zar, 1984). The significance level considered from the calling males. There was no correla-
was 0.05. tion between the size of the chorus and the
number of these non-calling males. Aggressive
RESULTS interactions between females were not ob-
Adult Characteristics.-Females were larger and served, and females adopted a crouched posture
heavier after oviposition than males (Table 1). similar to that displayed by non-calling males.
Males in amplexus were larger and heavier than Gravid females observed on six occasions
unmated males (Table 2). The average (+SD) moved slowly among the territories of calling
mass of clutches was 0.74 ? 0.23 g (N = 14), males, keeping a distance of about 8 to 10 cm
which represents 35% of mean females' mass from the males. Other females (N = 3) perched
after oviposition. on branches of emergent vegetation, maintain-
Chorus Structure.-Choruses usually lasted ing a distance of about 20 to 70 cm from groups
from sunset until approximately six hours. New of 4 to 5 males. Females sometimes sat near
males continued to arrive at the chorus until males for as long as three hours, and in some
about three hours after sunset, after which the cases spent up to one hour near one male before

TABLE 2. Comparisons (t-values) of mean snout-vent length and mean mass of mated and unmated males
of Hyla elegans.

Matedmales Unmatedmales
N Mean + SD N Mean+ SD t df P
SVL (mm) 53 26.35 ? 1.16 205 25.58 + 1.09 4.5 256 <0.01
Mass (g) 52 1.23 ? 0.12 171 1.15 + 0.21 3.2 154 <0.01
REPRODUCTION IN HYLA ELEGANS 357

80' 0.3-

.
60
U) 0.2-
a
z U
0
u. 40 U
O
0.1- U
U * *s
LU
m U
*b
- 20 U
.
Z *

on. I I
I 50 10 150 200
CHORUSSIZE
0 1 2 3 4 5 6
HOURSAFTER SUNSET FIG.2. Relationshipbetween number of males of
FIG.1. Mean numbers of calling males (?SD) of Hyla elegansin the chorus and OSRin 23 nights.
Hyla elegansat different times. Averages calculated
for 10 nights.
mass of females after oviposition (r = 0.425, P
< 0.01, N = 47). There was a higher percentage
of fertilization when the male SVL/female SVL
moving toward others. However, no female was rate was closer to the mean value (x = 0.81)
observed approaching closely all the males
available at the pond on any particular night. recorded for amplectant pairs found in the field
We also did not observe any male intercepting (Fig. 4). Deviations from this mean rate value
a female that was moving toward a calling male. resulted in a reduction in the percentage of fer-
We observed the initiation of amplexus nine tilization (98% at the mean value found in the
times. Amplexus is axillary and only occurred field; Fig. 4).
when the female remained very close to a call- Six gravid females were marked and recap-
tured once; the average number of days be-
ing male (<5 cm), although no physical contact tween successive captures was 55.67 + 40.48
between the pair was observed before clasping.
Most males (N = 279) were not observed in
amplexus, 50 were seen clasping once, and only 30-
three males were observed twice in amplexus
(one of these males was captured in amplexus a

on two consecutive nights). 28- * * *


E *
We found a positive correlation (r = 0.415, E U
U
P < 0.01; N = 51) between SVL of males and > 26- *. s
*

*I
females in amplexus, indicating positive as- u)
U . ?? ?
*
sortative mating for body size (Fig. 3A). On mmmm
?
average, SVL of males was 0.81 (SD = 0.04) of 24-
the length of females. There was no assortative
mating for mass (Fig. 3B; r = 0.189, P > 0.05; 22 I I v
N= 51). B 30 32 34 36
FEMALE
SVL (mm)
Oviposition.-After entering amplexus, the pair
remained at the male's calling site until neigh-
boring males ceased their calling or left the site. 1.6'
The durations between the start of amplexus U
and the beginning of oviposition for two pairs * U.. U U
1.4-
were 200 and 215 min, respectively. The am- 0
ua
plectant female walked in circles on the aquatic
U.... U

vegetation and periodically showed abdominal < 1.2- ? * a


??? .
contractions, arching her back and expelling -I
1.
? a ?U ?
her eggs, which were fertilized directly in the
water. The female freed herself from the am- 1.0o ? ?

plectant male by dislodging him with her hind


legs. Ovipositions ranged from 31 to 53 min (x 0.8 .
+ SD = 41 ? 9.38, N = 4). 1.0 1.5 2.0 2.5 3.0
Females deposited an average of 300 + 60 FEMALE
MASS(g)
eggs (N = 47). We found a significant positive FIG.3. Relationship between the snout-vent
correlation between clutch size and SVL (r = lengths (above) and masses (below) of males and fe-
0.382, P < 0.01; N = 46), and clutch size and males of Hyla elegans,capturedin amplexus.
358 R. P. BASTOS AND C. F. B. HADDAD

100-
a higher probability of mating than males in
smaller choruses.
80-
Male H. elegansbegan calling activity and ter-
z
o 60- ritory establishment before the arrival of fe-
< males, as reported for Hyla minuta(Haddad and
40- Cardoso, 1992). The earlier arrival of males may
t be related to competition for calling sites (Had-
.I
20- dad and Cardoso, 1992). By obtaining a suitable
call site, a male increases the distance over which
his calls are broadcast.
SVL MALE/SVL FEMALE
As observed for other species of anurans
(Kluge, 1981; Perrill, 1984; Telford and Dyson,
FIG.4. Curvilinearrelationshipbetween percent- 1988), male H. elegans can breed on consecutive
age of eggs fertilized and the ratioof male to female nights. Male H. elegans that consistently attend
body size for pairs of Hyla elegansmated artificially choruses might increase their chances of repro-
[Y = -3,673.0 + 9,620.3X - 6,137.3X2, R2= 0.85, P < duction (see for other species: Given, 1988; Ritke
0.001].The arrow indicates the mean ratio (0.81) of and Semlitsch, 1991).
male to female body size for pairsfound in the field.
Male H. elegans did not wait for contact by
the female in order to enter in amplexus; similar
observations were made by Morris (1989) on
(range = 12-112). The numbers of eggs depos-
ited by two of these females were counted at Hyla chrysoscelis.Arnold (1976) suggested that
first capture and recapture. One female pro- males do not wait for physical contact by fe-
duced 301 and 241 eggs at first capture and re- males to prevent furtive clasping by satellite
males. The number of supposed satellite males
capture, respectively; the other, 538 and 266. observed for H. elegans was low, which appar-
ently had little influence on the behavior of the
DISCUSSION individuals. We did not observe males of H.
Hyla eleganshas a prolonged breeding season, elegans intercepting gravid females moving to-
and males aggregate several nights throughout wards a calling individual. However, males may
the year. These aggregations can be classified attempt to intercept pairs as they move toward
as leks because oviposition occurred away from oviposition sites, since females in amplexus only
territories defended by selected males, and males began to move toward the oviposition site after
provided only sperm to females (Emlen and Or- the nearest calling males ceased their activities
ing, 1977; Sullivan and Hinshaw, 1992). The or left the vicinity. Such a hypothesis is based
arrival of males and females at the breeding site on the possibility of interception of the pair and
throughout the study was not synchronized. the displacement of the males already in am-
Nightly estimates of OSRs were strongly male- plexus.
biased; on average, ten times as many males as Female H. elegans were not amplexed by the
females were present each night. This estimate first male they encountered. However, any fe-
is close to those reported for other species with male mate choice was limited and females could
prolonged breeding seasons (e.g. Howard and not analyse all the males from the chorus, due
Kluge, 1985; Dyson et al., 1992). Such low OSRs to distance of displacement involved. Never-
may account for why only 17% of the males theless, it is reasonable to assume that female
succeeded in mating one or more times. choice may occur at the level of local male group
The number of males was positively corre- and probably within a single night, in a similar
lated with the number of females present at the manner observed for H. chrysoscelis (Morris,
chorus, as has also been observed for many anu- 1989) and H. minuta(Haddad and Cardoso, 1992).
rans (e.g., Morris, 1989; Sullivan and Hinshaw, Assortative mating in some species of anu-
1992; Tejedo, 1993). This correlation may simply rans with axillary amplexus results in higher
indicate that the chorus composition is related fertilization success due to the greater precision
to environmental conditions (Green, 1990), be- of juxtaposition of the cloacae of the partners
ing explained by causes not related to sexual (Licht, 1976). Smaller males would be incapable
selection (Tejedo, 1993). Because the temporary of adequately reaching the cloaca (and may not
pond studied dried several times and could have have enough sperm to fertilize the whole
been the only local suitable site for breeding, clutch); larger males would not be able to keep
the aggregations of H. elegans could also be a a constant juxtaposition of cloacae throughout
consequence of such transient conditions. There the oviposition (Robertson, 1990; Bourne, 1993).
was no significant correlation between number In H. elegans, the observation that a greater per-
of males present and estimates of OSR, indi- centage of eggs were fertilized when the ratio
cating that males in larger choruses do not have of male and female body size, in experimentally
REPRODUCTION IN HYLA ELEGANS 359

paired individuals, was closer to the average bystomamaculatum,A. tigrinumand Plethodonjor-


observed for natural pairings supports Licht's dani.Z. Tierpsychol.42:247-300.
hypotesis. We did not observe egg fertilization BASTOS,R. P. 1993. Reproductive biology of Hyla
in one instance in which the ratio of male to elegans (Anura, Hylidae), in Ubatuba, State of Sao
Paulo. Master's Thesis, Universidade Estadual
female body size was high (0.90; Fig. 4). This
Paulista, Rio Claro, SP, Brasil (in Portuguese).
result is difficult to explain; however, similar ,AND C. F. B. HADDAD. 1995. Vocalizations
results were obtained for Uperoleia laevigata and acoustic interactions in Hyla elegans (Anura,
(Robertson, 1990). Hylidae) during the reproductive activity. Natur-
Although females of H. elegans may breed alia 20:165-176 (in Portuguese).
more than once in a season, they seem to require BERVEN, K. A. 1988. Factors affecting variation in
an interval of approximately two weeks be- reproductive traits within a population of Wood
tween laying of two consecutive clutches. Such Frogs (Rana sylvatica). Copeia 1988:605-615.
an interval is similar to that reported for other BOURNE, G. R. 1993. Proximatecosts and benefits of
mate acquisition at leks of the frog Ololygon rubra.
anurans (Wells, 1976; Perrill and Daniel, 1983). Anim. Behav. 45:1051-1059.
Considering the methodology used in this study, CHERRY,M. J. 1992. Sexual selection in the leopard
the interval could be even shorter, since marked toad, Bufo pardalis. Behaviour 120:164-175.
females may have laid eggs in days not included DARWIN,C. 1871. The Descent of Man, and Selection
in our samples in the field. The ability to pro- in Relation to Sex. Princeton Univ. Press, Prince-
duce more than one clutch during the repro- ton, New Jersey.
ductive season seems to be restricted to species DAVIES,N. B., AND T. R. HALLIDAY.1977. Optimal
of prolonged breeding (Howard, 1978; Telford mate selection in the toad Bufo bufo. Nature 269:
and Dyson, 1990). 56-58.
DYSON, M. L., N. I. PASSMORE, P. J. BISHOP, AND S. P.
Clutch size in H. elegans was positively cor- HENZI. 1992. Male behavior and correlates of
related with both the SVL and the mass of fe-
mating success in a natural population of African
males, as has been found in other species of painted reed frogs (Hyperolius marmoratus). Her-
anurans (Berven, 1988; Martins, 1988). Within- petologica 48:236-246.
individual variation in the size of successive EMLEN,S. T., AND L. W. ORING. 1977. Ecology, sexual
clutches observed in our study has been re- selections and the evolution of mating systems.
ported for other anurans (Perrill and Daniel, Science 197:215-223.
1983; Ritke et al., 1990), and probably is related FROST,D. R. (ed.) 1985. Amphibian species of the
to the ability of females to obtain resources world. A taxonomy and geographical reference.
Allen Press and Association Systematics Collec-
needed to produce another clutch (Kluge, 1981),
tions, Lawrence, Kansas.
in addition to possible temperature influences
GREEN,A. J. 1990. Determinants of chorus partici-
(Kaplan, 1987). pation and the effects of size, weight and com-
Our data on the reproductive behavior of petition on advertisement calling in the tungara
males and females of H. elegans indicate that frog, Physalaemus pustulosus (Leptodactylidae).
some individuals can breed more than once Anim. Behav. 39:620-638.
during the year. This behavior may be highly GIVEN, M. F. 1988. Territoriality and aggressive in-
teractions of male carpenter frogs, Rana virgatipes.
adaptive in coping with unpredictable abiotic
(e.g., rain), and biotic (e.g., competition and pre- Copeia 1988:411-421.
HADDAD, C. F. B., AND A. J. CARDOSO.1992. Female
dation; Morin et al., 1990) factors. choice in Hyla minuta (Amphibia: Anura). Acta.
Acknowledgments.-We thank A. S. Abe, J. P. Zool. Liolloana 41:81-91 (in Spanish).
Pombal, Jr., M. J. Ryan, B. K. Sullivan, and two HALLIDAY, T. R. 1983. The study of mate choice. In
anonimous reviewers for critical reading of the P. Bateson (ed.), Mate Choice, pp. 3-32, Cambridge
University Press, Cambridge
manuscript. A. d'Heursel for the help with the HOWARD, R. D. 1978. The evolution of mating strat-
English. J. P. Pombal Jr., E. C. P. Pombal, L. P.
C. Morellato for field assistance. We also thank egies in bullfrogs, Rana catesbeiana.Evolution 32:
850-871.
the Instituto Florestal of the State of Sao Paulo
, AND A. G. KLUGE. 1985. Proximate mecha-
for permiting access to the study area and CNPq nisms of sexual selection in wood frogs. Evolution
for fellowships and financial support (C. F. B. 39:260-277.
H. processes no 301281/91-2 and 403221/91-9). KAPLAN, R. H. 1987. Developmental plasticity and
maternal effects of reproductive characteristics in
the frog, Bombinaorientalis. Oecologia (Berlin) 71:
LITERATURE CITED 273-279.
ARAK,A. 1983. Male-male competition and mate KLUGE,A. G. 1981. The life history, social organi-
choice in anuran amphibians. In P. Bateson (ed.), zation, and parental behavior of Hyla rosenbergi
Mate Choice, pp. 181-210, Cambridge University Boulenger, a nest-building gladiator frog. Misc.
Press, Cambridge. Publ. Mus. Zool., Univ. Mich. 160:1-170.
ARNOLD,S. J. 1976. Sexual behaviour, sexual inter- LEE,J. C., AND M. L. CRUMP. 1981. Morphological
ference and sexual defence in the salamanders Am- correlates of male mating success in Triprionpe-
360 R. P. BASTOS AND C. F. B. HADDAD

tasatusand Hyla marmorata(Anura: Hylidae). Oec- ROBERTSON, J. G. M. 1986. Female choice, male strat-
ologia (Berlin) 50:153-157. egies and the role vocalizations in the Australian
LICHT,L. E. 1976. Sexual selection in toads (Bufo frog Uperoleiarugosa.Anim. Behav. 34:773-784.
americanus).Can. J. Zool. 54:1277-1284. . 1990. Female choice increases fertilization
Lurz, B. 1973. Brazilian Species of Hyla. Univ. Texas success in the Australian frog, Uperoleialaevigata.
Press, Austin. Anim. Behav. 39:639-645.
MARTINS,M. 1988. Reproductive biology of Lepto- RYAN,M. J. 1983. Sexual selection and communi-
dactylus fuscus in Boa Vista, Roraima (Amphibia: cation in a Neotropical frog, Physalaemuspustulo-
Anura). Rev. Brasil. Biol. 48:969-977 (in Portu- sus. Evolution 37:261-272.
guese). . 1985. The Tungara Frog: a Study in Sexual
MARTOF, B. S. 1953. Territoriality in the green frog, Selection and Communication. Univ. Chicago
Rana clamitans.Ecology 34:165-173. Press, Chicago, Illinois.
MORIN,P. J., S. P. LAWLER, ANDE. A. JOHNSON.1990. SULLIVAN, B. K., ANDS. H. HINSHAW.1992. Female
Ecology and breeding phenology of larval Hyla choice and selection on male calling behavior in
andersonii:the disadvantages of breeding late. Ecol- the grey treefrog Hyla versicolor.Anim. Behav. 44:
ogy 71:1590-1598. 733-744.
MORRIS, M. R. 1989. Female choice of large males in TEJEDO, M. 1993. Do male natterjack toads join larger
the treefrog Hyla chrysoscelis:the importance of breeding choruses to increase mating success? Co-
identifying the scale of choice. Behav. Ecol. So- peia 1993:75-80.
ciobiol. 25:275-281. TELFORD, S. R., ANDM. L. DYSON. 1988. Some deter-
OLSON,D. H., A. R. BLAUSTEN, AND R. K. O'HARA. minants of the mating system in a population of
1986. Mating pattern variability among western painted reed frogs (Hyperoliusmarmoratus).Behav-
toad (Bufo boreas)populations. Oecologia (Berlin) iour 37:29-32.
70:351-356. . 1990. The effect of rainfall on interclutch
PERRILL, S. A. 1984. Male mating behavior in Hyla interval in painted reed frog (Hyperoliusmarmor-
regilla. Copeia, 1984:727-732. atus). Copeia 1990:644-648.
, ANDR. E. DANIEL.1983. Multiple egg clutch- WELLS,K. D. 1976. Multiple egg clutches in the green
es in Hyla regilla, H. cinereaand H. gratiosa.Copeia frog (Rana clamitans). Herpetologica 32:85-87.
1983:513-516. . 1977. The social behaviour of anuran am-
RITKE, M. E., J. G. BABB,ANDM. K. RITKE.1990. Life phibians. Anim. Behav. 25:660-693.
history of gray treefrog (Hyla chrysoscelis)in west- ZAR,J. H. 1984. Biostatistical Analysis. 2a ed. Pren-
ern Tennessee. J. Herpetol. 24:135-141. tice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey.
,AND R. D. SEMLITSCH. 1991. Mating behavior
and determinants of male mating success in the Accepted: 24 April 1996.
gray treefrog, Hyla chrysoscelis.Can. J. Zool. 69:
246-250.

You might also like