Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Early Childhood Research Quarterly 26 (2011) 50–60

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Early Childhood Research Quarterly

The effects of syllable instruction on phonemic awareness in preschoolers


Teresa A. Ukrainetz ∗ , Janae J. Nuspl, Kimberly Wilkerson, Sarah Rose Beddes
University of Wyoming, Laramie, WY 82071, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Purpose: Preschooler instruction for speech sound awareness typically teaches a progression of speech
Received 2 February 2009 units from sentences to phonemes, ending at simple first phoneme activities. This study investigates the
Received in revised form 13 April 2010 effects of teaching advanced tasks of phoneme blending and segmenting with and without the larger
Accepted 21 April 2010
speech unit of the syllable.
Method: Thirty-nine 4–5-year-old typically developing children received twice-weekly small-group
Keywords:
instruction in three conditions: two weeks of syllable tasks then four weeks of multiple phoneme tasks
Phonemic awareness
(SP), four weeks of multiple phoneme tasks only (MP), or an active control condition of first phoneme
Phonological awareness
Preschool instruction
instruction (FP).
Results: The conditions SP and MP showed large significant gains on blending and segmenting and no sig-
nificant differences on first phoneme isolating compared to the FP condition. A comparison of SP and MP
did not show significant differences on phoneme blending and segmenting, but SP showed significantly
more confusion during early sessions of phoneme instruction.
Conclusion: This preliminary evidence suggests that preschoolers can improve understanding of phoneme
blending and segmenting, without first being taught syllable blending and segmenting, and with no nega-
tive effects on first sound awareness. These findings support a more efficient way of teaching preschoolers
awareness of the individual sounds of speech. Replication with a larger sample, including children at-risk
for literacy difficulties, is recommended before firm conclusions should be drawn.
© 2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Phonemic awareness, one of the central components of read- preschoolers fare in acquiring understanding of phoneme blend-
ing and writing in alphabetic print systems, is often arrived at ing and segmenting, and whether one stage in comprehensive
through teaching a presumed developmental order from the larger instruction, that of syllable awareness, provides added benefit in
units of words and syllables to the smallest units of phonemes the development of phonemic awareness.
(Gillon, 2006; Phillips, Clancy-Menchetti, & Lonigan, 2008; Schuele
& Boudreau, 2008). The challenging tasks of blending and seg-
menting words into their component phonemes are typically left 1. Phonemic and phonological awareness
until kindergarten, to be taught concurrently with formal reading
instruction. There is considerable evidence that such an instruc- Phonemic awareness is the understanding that spoken words
tional progression improves phonemic awareness and contributes can be separated and manipulated as minimally contrastive sound
to better word reading and spelling (e.g., Brady, Fowler, Stone, & units (e.g., bone into/b/-/o/-/n/). This awareness is one of the
Winbury, 1994; Lundberg, Frost, & Peterson, 1988). However, it strongest predictors of performance in word reading and spelling
is important to know whether such comprehensive instruction is (e.g., Hoien, Lundberg, Stanovich, & Bjaalid, 1995; Stanovich, 2000).
necessary or whether there are more efficient ways of arriving at It is needed for children to recognize the alphabetic principle and
the critical level of awareness of the phoneme. In addition, with to sound out printed words (Torgesen, Al Otaiba, & Grek, 2005).
increasing literacy expectations in preschool, empirical evidence A large body of research evidence shows the beneficial effects of
is needed on the effects of instruction in advanced tasks, such programs that teach phonemic awareness on phonemic awareness
as phoneme segmenting and blending. This study examines how itself, and on word reading and spelling for children of a range of
abilities and ages (see Ehri et al., 2001; National Institute on Child
Health and Human Development [NICHD], 2000). Direct instruction
∗ Corresponding author at: The Division of Communication Disorders, Dept. 3311,
in phoneme manipulation is considered an essential part of reading
1000 E. University Ave., University of Wyoming, Laramie, WY, United States.
instruction in kindergarten, especially for children at risk for read-
Tel.: +1 307 766 5576; fax: +1 307 766 6829. ing difficulties (Torgesen et al., 2005; Vaughn, Wanzek, Woodruff,
E-mail address: tukraine@uwyo.edu (T.A. Ukrainetz). & Linan-Thompson, 2008).

0885-2006/$ – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.04.006
T.A. Ukrainetz et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 26 (2011) 50–60 51

Phonemic awareness falls within the larger umbrella of phono- ommended simply engaging children in “listening to patterned,
logical awareness. Phonology is the science of speech sounds predictable texts while enjoying the feel of reading and lan-
(Merriam Webster, 1993). Phonological awareness involves both guage” (p. 4). The instructional climate has since shifted toward
the smallest unit of the phoneme and larger units of onset-rime more formal literacy lessons and higher expectations, including for
(e.g., b-atter and spl-atter) and syllables (e.g., ba-na-na and bigg-er). phonemic awareness (Phillips et al., 2008).
In addition, the term phonological has been applied to tasks that Despite the diversity of possible instructional approaches, if
require understanding of meaning, such as counting the words in a preschool instruction is present, some form of comprehensive
spoken sentence and dividing compound words into root words. instruction appears dominant. State standards include attention
It has even been applied to tasks involving identifying environ- to counting words and syllables (e.g., Wyoming Department of
mental sounds, which are familiar non-speech sounds, such as Education, 2003). Expert articles address teaching word, syllable,
door knocks and bird tweets. Sometimes, it has even erroneously and rhyme, as well as phoneme skills (e.g., Gillon, 2006; Phillips et
included letter-sound awareness (e.g., Robertson & Salter, 1997). al., 2008; Schuele & Boudreau, 2008). Manualized curricula detail
To further the confusion, the label of phonemic has been applied to how to teach awareness of non-speech sounds, words, syllables,
the teaching of phonological awareness. For example, in Ehri et al.’s rhymes, and phonemes (e.g., Adams, Foorman, Lundberg, & Beeler,
(2001) report of the beneficial effects of sound awareness instruc- 1998; Notari-Syverson, O-Connor, & Vadasy, 1998). Preschool web-
tion, the term phonemic was used and phonemic outcomes were sites promote this comprehensive instruction (e.g., http://www.
analyzed, but the meta-analysis included studies that taught larger teachers.net/lessons/posts; http://www.ehow.com; http://www.
units of sound such as syllables, words, and non-speech sound teacherquicksource.com/preschool). Norm-referenced phonologi-
events. cal awareness tests include units larger than the phoneme, such as
The phonemic versus phonological distinction is a critical issue rhyme, syllable, and compound word or sentence (e.g., Robertson
for this study and will be addressed further. Given that preschool & Salter, 1997; Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1999).
instruction is predominantly phonological rather than phonemic, The expert literature has taken an uncritical stance in support of
and phonological subsumes phonemic, this report refers to all vari- comprehensive instruction. For example, in their report on features
eties of sound awareness instruction as phonological awareness of successful phonological awareness instruction for preschoolers,
instruction, regardless of how cited published reports originally Phillips et al. (2008) recognize that speech units are not acquired
identified it. fully sequentially, that not all speech units are necessary for acquir-
ing phonemic awareness, that phoneme segmenting takes time to
teach systematically and supportively, and that not all speech units
2. Phonological awareness instruction merit equal teaching time. Despite these considerations, Phillips
et al. do not even raise the possibility of focusing exclusively at
Phonological awareness instruction has involved several the phoneme level. Instead, they recommend a flexible sequen-
approaches. These approaches differ on scope and sequence of tial order of comprehensive instruction from sentences through to
sound unit instruction. One approach is comprehensive, sequen- phonemes.
tial instruction. One skill is taught at a time, beginning with large
units of sound and progressing to smaller units. Some comprehen-
sive approaches skip steps, such as not teaching environmental 3. Syllable awareness as an entry into phoneme awareness
sounds, but all teach some tasks involving units bigger than the and reading
phoneme. This comprehensive approach has support from con-
trolled group research studies (e.g., Brady et al., 1994; DeBaryshe & This study focuses on the benefits of syllable awareness. The syl-
Gorecki, 2007; Lundberg et al., 1988). lable is the largest purely phonological unit. Clapping and marching
Other approaches initiate instruction directly at the phoneme to syllables in a rhythmic beat is more entertaining for young
level, without prior instruction in larger units of sound. The pro- children than the awkward parsing of a word into its compo-
cedure may be sequential, starting with distinctively different nent phonemes. Syllables are more pronounceable than many
phonemes that can be seen on the lips (e.g., /b/,/s/), and moving phonemes and often align with morphological units (e.g., na-tion-
to less salient phonemes, such as vowels or consonants present al). Awareness of syllables has been found to be part of a single
in clusters (e.g., /str/). Instruction moves from manipulating indi- overarching phonological awareness factor, along with phoneme
vidual phonemes to segmenting phonemes in short words to and rhyme awareness (Anthony et al., 2002; Wagner, Torgesen, &
segmenting progressively longer words. This sequential phoneme- Rashotte, 1994). Syllable and phoneme competencies may not even
only approach has empirical support from controlled group studies be separable on tests, as in the Comprehensive Test of Phonological
(e.g., Ball & Blachman, 1988; Torgesen, Morgan, & Davis, 1992). Processing (Wagner et al., 1999), where a single score on a sound
An alternate phoneme-only approach involves a less-structured, deletion task is obtained by combining performance on syllable and
simultaneous ordering in which different phoneme tasks and phoneme items.
different word shapes are addressed together within a lesson. Although syllables are part of phonological awareness, syllable
Learning support is provided through differential scaffolding (e.g., awareness appears to operate separately from phoneme awareness
more help for more difficult tasks) embedded in bookread- and to contribute little to reading. Hoien et al. (1995) found little
ing and shared writing activities (McFadden, 1998; Richgels, relationship between syllable and phoneme awareness (correla-
Poremba, & McGee, 1996; Ukrainetz, 2006a). This approach also tions of .1–.2) for kindergartners and first graders. Wood and Terrell
has support from controlled group research studies (Ukrainetz, (1998) showed that good and poor readers did not significantly
Cooney, Dyer, Kysar, & Harris, 2000; Ukrainetz, Ross, & Harm, differ on syllable segmentation. First grade syllable awareness
2009). explains less than 1% of unique variance in word decoding (Engen
In preschool, although the importance is recognized, phonolog- & Hoien, 2002; Hoien et al., 1995). Even for later reading, chunk-
ical awareness instruction of any kind is not yet commonly present ing into syllabic units often results in only partial word decoding
(Phillips et al., 2008). This lack of attention is not surprising con- which is associated with poorer reading performance (McGuiness,
sidering that, just over 10 years ago, the 1998 position statement 1997). Thus, syllables, while being part of phonological awareness,
issued by the International Reading Association and the National do not appear to be important in developing phonemic awareness
Association for the Education of Young Children (IRA/NAEYC) rec- or word reading.
52 T.A. Ukrainetz et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 26 (2011) 50–60

4. Development of phonemic awareness These studies suggest that, at least for kindergartners, it is
more efficient to begin instruction at the phoneme level than
One of the arguments for comprehensive instruction is that it teach a full array of sounds. For preschoolers, the comprehen-
reflects the developmental order of growing awareness from larger sive approach might still provide a better entry into phonemic
to smaller units of speech (e.g., Gillon, 2006; Phillips et al., 2008; awareness. Ukrainetz (2008) conducted a systematic review of the
Schuele & Boudreau, 2008). However, careful examination of the literature for preschoolers on teaching awareness of phonemes ver-
evidence suggests that the developmental ordering of phonological sus the full array of sounds. Only ten studies were located that
awareness accomplishments is much less linear and fixed than has involved peer-reviewed reports of group controlled comparisons.
been generally believed. None of the studies taught non-speech sound identification or sen-
Young preschoolers can demonstrate phonemic awareness tence manipulation. Five studies included attention to rhyme and
without direct instructions. Half of 3-year-olds can achieve begin- syllable. Five preschool studies taught only at the phoneme level.
ning phoneme isolation and alliteration (Chaney, 1992; Maclean, The largest gains were obtained for instruction in simple phoneme
Bryant, & Bradley, 1987), and a third of 4-year-olds can match by tasks, compared to instruction that included rhyme and syllable, or
first phoneme (Lonigan, Burgess, Anthony, & Barker, 1998). Fox and that included advanced phoneme tasks.
Routh (1975) found that segmenting sentences into words, words Only two instructional studies were located that inform prac-
into syllables, and syllables into phonemes all showed similar steep tice specifically on syllable instruction. Cary and Verhaeghe (1994)
growth trajectories between 3 and 4 years. With instruction, these demonstrated that teaching syllable segmentation and deletion to
phoneme achievements have been accelerated, with first sound kindergartners resulted in no significant improvement in phoneme
matching expected for graduating preschoolers (McGee, 2005) and segmentation and deletion. Direct instruction in phonemes was
segmenting words into phonemes expected by the end of kinder- required to gain improvement in phoneme awareness. In a retro-
garten (Good, Simmons, Kame’enui, Kaminski, & Wallin, 2002). spective study by Nancollis, Lawri, and Dodd (2005), preschoolers
A factor that has complicated understandings of the develop- were taught rhyme identification, syllable segmentation, and first
mental progression is that tasks vary in cognitive and memory sound isolation. Two years later, the treatment group showed
demands. For example, comparing only two pictured words for first moderately better rhyme and non-word spelling, but phoneme
sound matches is much easier than comparing one word to an array segmentation was significantly worse than the comparison group.
of three words. Segmenting the sounds in a very simple word like
me may be achieved before a task of matching phonemes from a
6. The current study
multiple non-pictured word array. Child-friendly instructions and
materials can reveal the competencies of 3-year-olds. When tasks
Both comprehensive instruction and phoneme-only instruction
are kept equivalent in cognitive and memory demands, there is very
are effective routes into phonemic awareness. Teaching recom-
little ordering within this multidimensional domain, with overlaps
mendations and practices support comprehensive instruction,
in achievements among rhyme, syllable, and phonemes (Anthony
although evidence suggests that phoneme-only instruction is a
et al., 2002; Anthony, Lonigan, Driscoll, Phillips, & Burgess, 2003;
more efficient approach. However, firm conclusions about relative
Fox & Routh, 1975; Yopp, 1988).
efficiency cannot be made due to the lack of research that directly
The developmental order of acquisition of phonological aware-
compares teaching approaches.
ness is, in fact, not very ordered. Phoneme awareness emerges as
A common step in comprehensive instruction is teaching sylla-
early as awareness of larger units of speech. Developmental order,
ble awareness. The syllable is a unit of speech that is easily learned
then, does not justify teaching comprehensively over teaching a
by preschoolers. Syllable blending and segmenting are considered
selective focus on phonemes.
part of the skills involved in reading and an entry into the more
challenging task of phoneme blending and segmenting. However,
there is little evidence in support and some evidence against these
5. Phoneme-only versus more comprehensive instruction
expectations. Syllable instruction may have no beneficial role in
developing phonemic awareness, may take valuable time away
There is research support for both comprehensive and
from phoneme instruction, and may even confuse children as they
phoneme-only instruction. However, no studies could be located
shift from syllables to phonemes.
that directly compared the two approaches. Alternative evidence
This study provides a preliminary direct comparison of
can be gained from a post hoc comparison of controlled studies.
phoneme awareness instruction with and without prior lessons
A comprehensive-approach study is that of Lundberg et al.
in syllable awareness for a small sample of typically achieving
(1988). Lundberg et al. taught eight months of daily 15-min whole
preschoolers. In addition, although recommendations are begin-
class lesson that sequentially addressed the full array of sound
ning to include teaching phoneme blending and segmenting, there
units, from non-speech sounds to segmenting phonemes. Results
is little empirical evidence on preschoolers’ response to such
for phoneme segmenting compared to the no-treatment condition
instruction—or how time spent on them may affect progress on
showed a significant moderate effect size (d = 0.69). This training
the first phoneme skills critical at entry to kindergarten. Thus, this
progression was replicated with a similar effect size in an 18-week
study also examines the effects of teaching advanced phoneme
version by Brady et al. (1994).
awareness skills. The research questions in this study were as fol-
In contrast, Ball and Blachman (1988) taught only at the
lows:
phoneme level, in a sequential approach for seven weeks in four
20-min a week small-group sessions. The segmentation condi-
tion showed significantly better phoneme segmenting and word 1. Will phoneme blending and segmenting instruction result in
reading skills with large effect sizes (d = 1.85, d = 1.67) compared improved phoneme blending and segmenting?
to the no-treatment control and to the language/letter treatment. 2. What is the effect of syllable awareness instruction on phoneme
Ukrainetz et al. (2000) taught phoneme isolating and segmenting blending and segmenting?
in three 30-min sessions per week for seven weeks, with sound 3. Is there learning confusion from syllable awareness instruction
talk embedded in shared book and writing activities. Large effect early in phoneme awareness instruction?
sizes were obtained for segmentation compared to no-treatment 4. What is the effect of phoneme blending and segmenting instruc-
controls (d = 1.37). tion on first phoneme isolating?
T.A. Ukrainetz et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 26 (2011) 50–60 53

The first hypothesis was that typically achieving preschoolers did not meet the language eligibility standard. Their parents were
could make significant gains, although not master, these advanced counseled about sources of speech-language support. One child
tasks. A positive response was a pre-condition to answer the syl- was too young to start kindergarten that fall. Five participants had
lable question—if these preschoolers could not learn phoneme scheduling difficulties or left the preschool prior to post-testing.
blending and segmenting, then the effect of syllable instruction This resulted in 39 participants.
would not be apparent. For the next two questions, the hypotheses
were that prior syllable instruction would provide no additional 7.3.2. Testing measures
benefit in phoneme awareness, and children would be confused in The testing measures examined language, letters, and phoneme
the early part of phoneme instruction. Phoneme blending and seg- and syllable awareness. The Test of Early Language Development
menting were analyzed separately because phoneme segmenting (TELD, Hresko, Reid, & Hammill, 1999) was administered for eli-
was expected to be more difficult to learn than blending, and syl- gibility and balancing conditions. The eligibility requirement was
lable manipulation activities might be particularly detrimental to a total TELD standard score of 84 (14 percentile). The TELD was
phoneme segmenting. The final hypothesis was that time spent on administered following the standardized procedures at pre-testing.
advanced tasks would not negatively affect first phoneme aware- The TELD is normed on 2217 children, including those with dis-
ness compared to first phoneme-only instruction. abilities, demographically stratified from 35 states. There is an
expressive and a receptive scale that sample vocabulary, concepts,
7. Method and sentence structure. The TELD manual reports high test–retest
and inter-rater reliability (r = .94, r = .99). For validity, the TELD
7.1. Design reports no differentiation of children by race/ethnicity, but differ-
entiation by age and by disability compared to normative groups.
This study employed a group experimental design with typically Letter name knowledge was evaluated at pre-testing with the
developing 4–5-year-olds in three conditions: syllable plus multi- Clay Letter Name Recognition Screening (Clay, 1979) for balancing
ple phoneme tasks (SP), multiple phoneme tasks without syllables conditions. Upper case letters were presented on a single sheet, not
(MP), and first phoneme tasks only (FP). The participant sample in alphabetical order. One line of letters was revealed at a time, with
was limited to typically achieving children who could be expected the tester indicating which letter to identify.
to make reasonable learning gains in a short instructional period. The primary outcome measure of phoneme awareness was
The outcome measures were immediate performance on phoneme raw score performance on the Phonological Awareness Test (PAT,
tasks. Robertson & Salter, 1997). Three subtests were used at pre-
The study was conducted in May and June of 2005 and 2007. and post-testing: first phoneme isolating, phoneme blending, and
Each data collection period was eight weeks. Children were pre- phoneme segmenting. Two PAT syllable awareness subtests were
tested in the week prior to SP commencing syllable instruction. also administered for descriptive purposes. Each subtest consisted
After two weeks of syllable instruction, SP participants were tested of 10 items. The PAT was administered and scored according to
on syllable performance and those who met criterion proceeded standardized procedures, except that the phoneme subtests were
into phoneme treatment. All conditions then commenced phoneme administered before the syllable subtests.
instruction. The SP and MP conditions received four weeks of The PAT is a norm-referenced test of phonological awareness
instruction. For the FP condition, after two weeks, the participants and letter-sound knowledge. The PAT is normed on demograph-
who met criterion on first phonemes received no further instruc- ically stratified 1235 children, with children with disabilities
tion in the remaining two weeks. Post-testing occurred in the week excluded. The PAT manual reports moderate to high test–retest reli-
immediately following SP and MP instruction. ability (r = .61–.95) for the relevant subtests and ages. For validity,
the PAT reports age differentiation for each item, and significant
7.2. Participants differences for at an alpha level of .05 or better between “randomly
selected subjects from the normative population” and a matched
Thirty-nine preschoolers (mean age = 5;1, range = 4;0–5;11) sample of those “at-risk for reading” (p. 52).
participated. The participants had typically developing language In addition to the standard scoring procedure, a partially correct
skills, as indicated by norm-referenced test scores and teacher scoring procedure was applied to the phoneme segmenting subtest.
report. They were eligible to start kindergarten the following Partial scoring is used in other assessment measures (e.g., Good et
August. No demographic data were collected, but the children were al., 2002). This version was developed for this study. One point was
observed to be almost all white with English as a first language. awarded for each phoneme the child correctly segmented, so long
The study took place in two childcare centers in a middle-class as at least two segments were present (e.g., fat as /f/-/at/ was 1 point,
neighborhood in the small college town of Laramie, Wyoming. The but /f/ was 0). One point was deducted for each phoneme insertion
childcare centers each had an enrollment of about 100 infants, or position reversal (e.g., liver: /l/-/I/-/l/-/ver/ was 1 point). The total
toddlers, and preschoolers, and provided after-school care. From possible was 36 points.
informal observation, literacy instruction was carried out infre- A secondary measure of outcome performance was the degree
quently and unsystematically. The teachers reported sometimes of confusion evidenced by children early in the phoneme instruc-
teaching what letters looked like and what sounds they make. Occa- tion. The first two phoneme instruction sessions were videotaped
sionally the teachers read a rhyming book to the class and asked for the MP and SP participants. These videotapes were examined
which words rhymed, blended the syllables of a child’s name, or for occasions in which a child showed task confusion between syl-
asked children what words started with certain sounds. lables and phonemes. One point was awarded each time a child: (a)
was asked for the first sound to a word and instead gave the first syl-
7.3. Procedure lable; (b) was asked to segment a word into phonemes and instead
segmented the word into syllables; and (c) was asked what had
7.3.1. Recruitment been learned that day and said syllables or the big sounds instead
The teachers handed out letters to parents explaining the study of the little sounds or just sounds in words.
purpose, procedures, and eligibility. The investigators were avail- To determine whether participants had mastered syllable
able to answer questions in the childcare centers at the beginning awareness in the four syllable instruction sessions prior to initiating
and end of several days. Of the 47 children with parental consent, 2 the phoneme phase of instruction, a criterion probe was adminis-
54 T.A. Ukrainetz et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 26 (2011) 50–60

Table 1
Participant characteristics for each condition and the total sample.

SP MP FP Total

Participants 15 12 12 39
Boys 8 8 3 19
Age in months 59.1 (6.2) 62.7 (6.5) 61.3 (4.5) 60.9 (5.9)
TELD standard score 101.9 (8.7) 96.8 (8.2) 100.8 (9.1) 100.0 (8.7)
Letter names 18.9 (10.1) 16.4 (10.1) 18.8 (10.4) 18.1 (9.6)
Obtained in 2005 8 7 0 15
Obtained in 2007 7 5 12 24

Note: SP = syllable plus phoneme, MP = multiple phoneme tasks, FP = first phoneme-only; no significant differences on balancing variables of age, language, or letters (p > .29).

tered during the second and the fourth syllable sessions. This probe and MP. One participant did not complete the study, resulting in 8
was a word list modeled on the items in the PAT syllable segmenting MP and 7 SP participants.
subtest. Any children who scored less than 5/10 at midpoint were In 2007, age was additionally considered in the balancing pro-
given additional instruction. Children who scored 7/10 or less at the cedure, along with the addition of the FP condition. A coded list
final session did not proceed into the phoneme phase of instruction. was used to devise three groups with similar means on age, lan-
FP instruction was planned for eight sessions, but was modified guage, and letters. The three groups were randomly assigned to
while in progress due to the high number of children mastering the three treatment conditions. Four participants were lost after the
first sound isolating within two sessions. A criterion test, mod- 2007 condition assignment. The resultant total sample was 39 with
eled on the PAT first sound isolating subtest was administered after 15 SP, 12 MP, and 12 FP participants (Table 1). A factorial ANOVA
the fourth treatment session. Participants who achieved 10/10 cor- showed no significant differences among the three conditions on
rect graduated from treatment after that session, then waited two the balancing variables of age, language, or letters (p > .29 for all
weeks for post-testing. comparisons).

7.3.3. Testing procedure 7.3.6. Treatment procedure


The testers were trained in two phases. First, the testers who Instruction was provided by three instructors (the second, third,
were also instructors (and graduate students in speech-language and fourth authors). The instructors were trained by the first author.
pathology) practiced with supervision from the first author (a The training consisted of observing demonstrations, viewing video-
speech-language pathologist and professor). The instructors then tapes, role-playing, and teaching pilot children from the university
trained the research assistants (undergraduate students in speech- preschool.
language pathology). All the testers practiced administration and Treatment conditions and instructors were crossed, so each
scoring on each other and on pilot children from the university condition was taught by two instructors, preventing an instruc-
preschool. tor effect. The children were taught mainly in pairs with some
Instructors and research assistants conducted the pre-testing. individual make-up sessions. The members of the pairs varied but
To avoid bias from knowing which children had received never crossed conditions (i.e., an SP child was not paired with an
which instruction, only the research assistants carried out the MP child). The number and type of activities used were consistent
post-testing. The 20–30 min testing sessions were administered across participants within a condition. Instruction sessions were
individually in quiet areas of the childcare centers. Scoring was 25–30 min long twice a week in isolated areas of the childcare
done on-line, but all the testing sessions were audiotaped as a centers.
backup and for reliability checks. Instruction involved the quality features of explicit attention to
skills, with repeated opportunities for learning and practice, and
7.3.4. Testing reliability systematic learning support (Schuele & Boudreau, 2008; Torgesen
To determine testing reliability in this study, a balanced selec- et al., 2005; Ukrainetz, 2006a). The focus was kept on speech
tion across the testers of 10 (26%) of the audiotaped pre- and sound awareness and tangential conversations were kept to a min-
post-testing sessions were independently re-scored. The point-to- imum. The children were asked to report the instructional target
point inter-rater agreement on the fully correct items from the PAT (e.g., What did you learn today? The small sounds in words) at the
was 96%. The word-to-word inter-rater reliability for the partial beginning and end of every session. Children were taught through
credit segmenting scores was 99%. For syllable-phoneme confu- scaffolding (McGee & Ukrainetz, 2009; Ukrainetz, 2006a). Interac-
sions during instruction, a random selection of 18 (20%) of the tive scaffolds included modeling, stressing a sound, repeating an
first two phoneme sessions was coded. There were approximately item, and simplifying to onset-rime segments for blending. Regu-
30–40 child responses per session. The two scorers agreed 100% latory scaffolds included obtaining eye contact, telling a child to
on phoneme responses. Of the 21 syllable responses, 83% were listen just before the question, and asking a child what he is learn-
identified by both scorers. ing in the sessions. Scaffolding was matched to task difficulty and
child need.
7.3.5. Condition assignment Syllable instruction. Participants in the SP condition were taught
After testing, participants were assigned to one of three condi- syllable blending and segmenting for two weeks (four sessions)
tions through a combination of balancing and group randomizing prior to commencing phoneme awareness instruction. The children
on language, letter knowledge, and age. For the initial balancing, a were taught the word “syllables,” which were described as the “big
standard score of 100 or above on the TELD received 1 point, and sounds in words.” Each session, a name activity (i.e., counting syl-
a score below that received 2 points. Children who knew 13 or lables in child and family names) and two to three other activities
more letters received 1 point, and those with fewer than 13 were were conducted. Activities included moving blocks to represent
given 2 points. The points were then averaged. Four children aver- syllables, clapping to syllables, and guessing words from syllable
aged 1 point, eight children averaged 1.5, and 4 children averaged segments, using materials like picture cards, plastic food, and the
2. Within each point level, children were randomly assigned to SP food words in The Very Hungry Caterpillar (Carle, 1969).
T.A. Ukrainetz et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 26 (2011) 50–60 55

The children mastered syllable segmenting within two sessions, data collection periods. All the sessions provided an explicit focus
with only one child scoring 5 or less. The instructor judged that this on phonemic awareness, not on letters or other topics. The instruc-
child understood syllable segmenting but was not good at clapping. tors interacted supportively and positively with the children. They
As a result, more clapping was used for all the children. All the provided clear behavioral expectations and support. Response and
children met the criterion and proceeded into phoneme instruction. linguistic scaffolds were provided to move children toward success.
Syllable-phoneme transition. The requirements of the syllable The variety of tasks and activities were tallied across the 30 ses-
and phoneme phases of instruction were maximally contrasted. No sions. For the seven syllable sessions, the instructors conducted
literature was located that informed on conducting this transition. syllable blending and segmenting tasks and no phoneme tasks. The
Transition features included: rare phoneme responses were corrected to syllables. Most of the
focus was on syllable segmenting. There was a skill preview (i.e.,
• Explicitly telling the children that expectations had changed from What are we learning?), a name activity, a book, and two card/object
syllables or big sounds to little sounds, and giving examples of activities in every session, but skill review (i.e., What did we learn
each. today?) occurred for only two of the seven sessions.
• Eliciting only syllable responses in the syllable phase and For the 19 multiple phoneme sessions, the instructors con-
phoneme responses in the phoneme phase. ducted only phoneme tasks and corrected every syllable (and letter)
• Immediately correcting responses in both phases (e.g., No, we are response. All three tasks of isolating, blending, and segmenting
doing small sounds now. Let’s do it again). occurred in every session reviewed, but phoneme segmenting was
• Providing different terms for the two speech units (“syllables” or a primary activity focus in four of the ten early sessions and either
“big sounds” versus “little sounds”). isolating or blending was not focused on in six of the nine late ses-
• Emphasizing different structural scaffolds (hand clapping in the sions. All the sessions started with a skill preview, then had a name
syllable phase versus finger extending in the phoneme phase). activity. The activity and book plan was followed in 16 sessions.
• Using different phonotactic shapes (primarily multisyllabic dur- Two sessions had an extra activity and one had only an extended
ing the syllable phase syllable versus primarily monosyllabic in book interaction with no card/object activity.
the phoneme phase). For the four first phoneme sessions, the instructors conducted
only first phoneme tasks of isolating, matching, and generating.
Multiple phoneme instruction. For the four weeks of SP and These sessions were all early sessions, due to a camera malfunc-
MP phoneme instruction, the three tasks of phoneme isolating, tion for the few late sessions that occurred (most participants had
blending, and segmenting were addressed in every session. Two graduated). The phoneme types (e.g., stops versus glides) were con-
activity sequences were alternated. Each session began with a trolled in the card/object activities, but a variety of phoneme types
name activity, in which the children’s names or other names were occurred in the name activity. One early session involved a verse
used for isolating, blending, or segmenting. Then in one sequence, book and the phoneme types were not controlled there. All the first
three card/object activities followed, with one focused on each of phoneme sessions showed a skill preview, a name activity, three
isolating, blending, or segmenting. The activities were simple enter- card/object activities, and a skill review.
taining activities from preschool manualized curricula, such as
matching games, word guessing games, and sound counting games
8. Results
using pictures and objects (Adams et al., 1998; Notari-Syverson et
al., 1998). The alternate sequence involved sharing a verse followed
The immediate effects of syllable and advanced phoneme
by one to two card/object activities. The sound talk embedded
awareness instruction were tested through three conditions: syl-
in rhyming books was based on McFadden (1998) and Ukrainetz
lable plus multiple phoneme tasks (SP), multiple phoneme tasks
(2006a). It consisted of identifying rhyming words, isolating the
(MP), and first phoneme tasks only (FP). The outcome measures
first phonemes in the word pairs, segmenting the phonemes in
were phoneme blending, phoneme segmenting (fully and partially
each word with a finger extended for each sound, and then com-
correct), syllable-phoneme confusions, and phoneme isolating.
paring word lengths. Then phonemes of another rhyming word
Effect sizes were calculated as Cohen’s standardized mean differ-
were presented for the children to blend and identify. This occurred
ence (d) for pairwise comparisons and eta-squared (2 ) for multiple
approximately every second page of a portion of the book.
comparisons. Values greater than 0.80 for d and 0.14 for 2 were
First phoneme instruction. The FP condition addressed only first
considered large (Cohen, 1977).
phoneme tasks, including generating, isolating, and matching. The
instructors carried out three to four activities in each session,
addressing phoneme isolating, pairs matching, phoneme gener- 8.1. Phoneme blending with and without prior syllable
ating, or matching from a multi-word array. For the card/object instruction
activities, the tasks and sounds were organized in order of increas-
ing instructional difficulty (e.g., /m/ is easier to show with the This first analysis determined both whether preschoolers could
mouth than /r/). The first two weeks involved generating and iso- learn the advanced task of phoneme blending, and whether prior
lating fricatives (e.g., /s/, /sh/), stops (e.g., /b/, /t/) and affricates (e.g., syllable instruction affected blending performance. Performance in
/dz/, /ch/). In the third and fourth weeks, clusters (e.g., /sp/, /kl/), liq- MP and FP was compared to performance associated with more
uids, glides, and vowels (e.g., /l/, /w/, /a/) were added. In the fourth conventional FP instruction (Table 2). Mean gains on phoneme
week, first sounds were identified for rhyming words in a verse blending were 4.0 and 3.2 words for SP and MP respectively, and 1.2
book in addition to the activities. This sequence was completed for words for FP. A repeated-measures ANOVA on two testing times and
only four participants because eight participants met the two-week three conditions for phoneme blending was conducted. The main
graduation criterion of 10/10 on first phoneme isolating. effect for condition was not significant, F(2, 36) = 2.35, p = .11. The
main effect for time and the time-by-condition interaction effect
7.3.7. Treatment fidelity were significant: F(1, 36) = 82.02, p < .01, 2 = 0.69; F(2, 36) = 7.44,
For treatment fidelity, 30 sessions were sampled from the video- p < .01, 2 = 0.29.
taped records of the syllable session 2 and sessions 1, 2, 7, and 8 of Related sample t-tests were used to examine which conditions
phoneme instruction. The sessions were randomly sampled across showed significant improvement from pre- to post-test (Fig. 1).
three conditions, three instructors, two childcare centers, and two With a Bonferroni correction to an alpha level of .02 for three
56 T.A. Ukrainetz et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 26 (2011) 50–60

Table 2
Phoneme awareness performance by condition.

Task Max Time SP, n = 15 MP, n = 12 FP, n = 12 Total, n = 39

Blend 10 Pre 0.7 (1.0) 2.5 (3.0) 1.7 (2.3) 1.6 (2.2)
Post 4.7 (2.1) 5.9 (3.2) 2.8 (3.2) 4.5 (3.0)
Gain 4.0 (1.9)* 3.4 (2.3)* 1.2 (1.7) 2.9 (2.3)

Segment 10 Pre 0.0 (0.0) 0.5 (1.4) 0.0 (0.0) 0.2 (0.8)
Post 1.0 (1.3) 2.4 (2.6) 0.3 (0.6) 1.2 (1.9)
Gain 1.0 (1.3)* 1.9 (2.0)* 0.3 (0.6) 1.1 (1.5)

Partial segment 36 Pre 0.0 (0) 3.8 (8.2) 0.5 (0.8) 1.3 (4.7)
Post 7.8 (6.8) 11.1 (10.8) 3.1 (4.7) 7.4 (8.2)
Gain 7.8 (6.8)* 7.3 (7.0)* 2.6 (4.0) 6.0 (6.5)

Isolate 10 Pre 2.9 (4.0) 3.6 (3.8) 4.8 (4.3) 3.7 (4.0)
Post 7.0 (3.1) 8.0 (2.1) 7.0 (3.1) 7.6 (3.1)
Gain 4.1 (3.6)* 4.4 (3.3)* 3.1 (3.0)* 3.9 (3.3)

Note: SP = syllable plus phoneme, MP = multiple phoneme tasks, FP = first phoneme-


only; means with standard deviations in ( ), Max = maximum points per task. Fig. 2. Mean performance on phoneme segmenting for three conditions: syllable
*
Significant at Bonferroni-corrected probability of p < .02. plus phoneme (SP), multiple phoneme (MP), first phoneme (FP).

comparisons, results showed significant pre- to post-test differ- on segmenting were not significant, t(25) = 1.45, p = .16, d = 0.59.
ences for SP and MP on phoneme blending, t(14) = 8.37, p < .01, For partial segmenting, mean gains of 7.8, 7.3, and 2.6 points
d = 2.1; t(11) = 5.11, p < .01, d = 1.47. FP gains were not significant, were demonstrated for SP, MP, and FP, respectively. A repeated-
t(11) = 2.38, p = .04, d = 0.71. To answer the question of whether measures ANOVA was conducted on testing time and condition for
SP provided an advantage over MP, SP and MP gains were com- partial segmenting. The main effect for condition was not signifi-
pared. Differences between SP and MP on blending gains were not cant, F(2, 36) = 3.19, p = .05, 2 = 0.15. The main effect for time was
significant, t(25) = 0.72, p = .47, d = 0.29. significant, F(1, 25) = 31.49, p < .001, d = 2.33. The time-by-condition
interaction effect was not significant, F(2, 25) = 0.04, p = .83, d = 0.08.
Related sample t-tests were used to examine which condi-
8.2. Phoneme segmenting with and without prior syllable
tions showed significant improvement from pre- to post-test on
instruction
partial phoneme segmenting. SP and MP gains were significant:
t(14) = 4.42, p < .01, d = 1.15; MP, t(11) = 3.57, p < .01, d = 1.04. FP seg-
This analysis determined both whether preschoolers could
menting gains were not significant at ˛ = .02, t(11) = 2.24, p = .05,
learn phoneme segmenting and whether prior syllable instruction
d = 0.65.
affected performance. Mean gains for phoneme segmenting were
1.0 words for SP, 1.9 words for MP, and 0.3 words for FP (see Table 2).
8.3. Learning confusion between syllables and phonemes
A repeated-measures ANOVA on two testing times and three con-
ditions for phoneme segmenting was conducted. The main effect
For the question concerning the effect of prior syllable instruc-
for condition was significant, F(2, 36) = 4.65, p = .02, 2 = 0.19. The
tion on confusion between syllables and phonemes early in
main effect for time and the time-by-condition interaction effect
phoneme instruction, the frequency of syllable responses in the first
were significant: F(1, 36) = 21.74, p < .01, 2 = 0.38; F(2, 36) = 4.23,
two phoneme instruction sessions was compared for SP and MP. FP
p = .02, 2 = 0.19.
was not involved in this comparison. Of the 15 SP participants, 14
Related sample t-tests were used to examine which condi-
made multiple confusions (4–16) and one participant made no con-
tions showed significant improvement from pre- to post-test on
fusions. Of the 12 MP participants, two participants made multiple
phoneme segmenting (Fig. 2). SP and MP gains were significant,
confusions (2 and 7), three made one confusion, and seven made no
t(14) = 2.96, p = .01, d = 0.77; t(11) = 3.36, p = .006, d = 0.95. FP seg-
confusions. MP showed a mean number of 1.0 (S.D. = 2) confusions.
menting gains were not significant, t(11) = 1.39, p = .191, d = 0.50.
SP showed a mean of 9.0 (22.1). A Mann–Whitney U-test showed
To specifically compare performance in SP and MP conditions, a t-
significant differences between SP and MP: U(25) = 12.50, p < .01,
test on gain scores was conducted. Differences between SP and MP
d = 2.21.

8.4. Effect of segmenting and blending instruction on phoneme


isolating

The next research question addressed the effects on first


phoneme awareness of using instructional time on phoneme blend-
ing and segmenting instead of teaching only first sound tasks.
For first phoneme isolating, mean performance increased from
less than 5 points at pre-testing to 7–8 points (see Table 2). The
main effect for condition was not significant, F(2, 36) = 0.71, p = .50,
2 = 0.04. The main effect for time was significant, F(1, 36) = 52.02,
p < .01, 2 = 0.59. The time-by-condition interaction effect was not
significant: F(2, 36) = 0.52, p = .60, 2 = 0.03.
To determine the source of the significant main effect, related
sample t-tests on change from pre- to post-test were run. Results
showed significant differences for SP, MP, and FP, t(14) = 4.38,
Fig. 1. Mean performance on phoneme blending for three conditions: syllable plus p < .01, d = 1.14; t(11) = 4.69, p < .01, d = 1.33; t(11) = 3.56, p < .01,
phoneme (SP), multiple phoneme (MP), first phoneme (FP). d = 1.03.
T.A. Ukrainetz et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 26 (2011) 50–60 57

9. Discussion 9.2. The benefits and costs of syllable instruction

9.1. Teaching advanced phonemic awareness to preschoolers The central question in this study concerned the effects of
instruction addressing the larger speech unit of the syllable. There
The first question in this study addressed the effect of teach- is an expectation that phonemic awareness is best introduced by
ing phoneme blending and segmenting to typically developing segmenting the larger, rhythmic units of syllables before engag-
preschoolers. These tasks have been considered developmentally ing in the awkward units of phonemes (Gillon, 2006; Phillips et al.,
advanced for young children, although recent recommendations 2008; Schuele & Boudreau, 2008). The results of this study did not
include them (e.g., Phillips et al., 2008). This study showed that support this idea.
preschoolers can make reliable gains in advanced phoneme tasks These results showed that similar improvements in phonemic
in a short period of time. awareness could be obtained from phoneme blending and seg-
Phoneme blending was well within the range of these menting instruction with and without prior syllable instruction.
preschoolers. Results showed participants taught to blend and seg- While the outcomes of the two conditions were not significantly
ment made significant mean gains in phoneme blending with a different, the effect sizes are suggestive that a larger sample size
large effect size. Of the 27 participants taught phoneme blending might reveal subtle differences. Interestingly, the differences are in
and segmenting, 93% blended two or more words correctly at post- opposing directions: the blending effect was slightly stronger for SP
testing, and 44% showed a fairly robust level of understanding with (d = 2.10) than MP (1.47), while the segmenting effect was slightly
six or more words blended correctly. stronger for MP (0.95) than SP (0.77).
For phoneme segmenting, the children taught phoneme blend- The design of the study does not allow determination of whether
ing and segmenting made significant mean gains from pre- to the possible SP advantage for blending was due to the syllable
post-testing, with moderate to large effect sizes. However, the component or to the additional two weeks of instruction. How-
individual results showed only two of the participants taught ever, it makes sense that syllable blending experience may aid with
to segment were able to correctly segment six or more words phoneme blending. First, the child’s response is the same, to say the
correctly. However, understanding was emerging, with nine chil- word, whether the instructor provides the component syllables or
dren segmenting two or more words correctly at post-testing. phonemes. Second, the two can be combined in a single teaching
This compared favorably to the children’s inability at pre-testing, episode without conflict. In this study, scaffolding occurred through
when 37 of the 39 children scored zero correct and the children decreasing the gap between phonemes then, if necessary, chunk-
declined to respond, repeated the words given, or gave only a first ing into onset-rime units (e.g., /d/-/awg/). For multisyllabic words,
sound. chunking the sounds into syllables as a scaffold toward phonemes
Following instruction, the items the children could segment cor- might be helpful and not confusing.
rectly were most often the three singleton single-syllable words: The small difference in effect size for segmenting against SP
off, me, and fat. Some partially correct responses to fairly difficult was consistent with the significant confusion evidenced by these
words were: brag as /br-a-g/, plop as /pl-aw-p/, plant as /pl/-/ae/- children after shifting from syllables to phonemes. Almost all the
/nt/, liver as /l␫-v-er/, and seashell as /si/-/ʃ/-/␧l/. To reveal this children (14/15) exposed to syllables first confused syllables and
emerging understanding, a partial scoring procedure was used. phonemes multiple times during the early sessions of phoneme
In this procedure, points were given if any phonemes were seg- instruction, in contrast to the few children (2/12) who did so with-
mented correctly from the word remainder, with points deducted out syllable instruction. The SP children tried to segment words
for position errors and missing segments (e.g., the brag and plop into syllables or imitate the instructor’s phonemes while clapping
responses above earned 2 points and the others earned 1 point). a syllable beat. Some of the children disputed the move to small
With this scoring system, subtle gains in knowledge were revealed. sounds or becoming silent and non-participatory, as in the follow-
Of the children taught segmenting (SP and MP conditions), 55% ing sample:
demonstrated some understanding with 6 or more segments cor- Instructor: Ok, now that we know the first sounds of our
rect across the 10 test words. names, should we count all the sounds?
Phoneme blending and segmenting were taught to these young Child 1: Yeah.
= Child 2 quietly watches the instructor and Child 1.
children in a simultaneous manner, along with more conven-
Instructor: Let’s see who has the longest name, okay? We’re
tional first phoneme activities. A sequential approach may work going to do hands up like this.
well too, but the simultaneous approach provided challenge for = Instructor puts a closed fist in the air.
children of differing levels of understanding within a group. Child 1: How about Jeff or Susan?
In addition, some children made progress that could not have Instructor: Ok, we’ll do that in a little bit. Let’s start with our
own names, okay. We’re going to start with Janae.
occurred in sequential instruction. For example, one study par-
Okay, ready, hands up like this.
ticipant, who could not do any of the tasks at pre-testing, was = Children put their elbows on the tables and hands in a fist to get ready.
still only able to score two correct on first phoneme isolat- Instructor: </dz/-/a/-/n/-/ai/>.
ing at post-testing. In a sequential approach, instruction would = Instructor raises one finger up for each sound. Participants speak chorally.
Child 1: </dza/-/nae/>
remain at first phoneme tasks, instead of presenting more diffi-
Child 2: </dza/-/nae/-/nae/-/nae/> {Repeats the final
cult tasks. However, at post-testing, this same participant blended syllable when she realizes the instructor is still
eight words correctly. Although he could not segment any words counting sounds}
fully correctly, he provided 15 partial phoneme segments. Engag- Instructor: Remember we’re doing the little sounds.
ing this child in phoneme isolating, blending, and segmenting = Instructor continues to hold up four fingers.
Instructor: How many sounds?
simultaneously allowed the child to learn what made sense to
Child 1: Four {answers faintly, unsure}. I thought you had
him. two {raises her voice accusingly}?
The current study shows that typically developing preschoolers Instructor: That was two syllables, but we’re doing the little
can benefit from even a brief period of phoneme blending and seg- sounds. These are the little sounds {shows four
fingers}. So I had four little
menting instruction. While accomplishments will vary, many can
sounds,/dz/-/a/-/n/-/ae/. Janae, four sounds.
achieve a robust understanding of phoneme blending and some will
be able to phonemically segment simple words or parts of harder Informal observation indicated that syllable confusion was per-
words. sistent for several of the children. For example, during the sixth
58 T.A. Ukrainetz et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 26 (2011) 50–60

phoneme session, the instructor shared a book that contained lating. The mean performance across conditions was 7.6, and 70%
many two-syllable words, such as, “Captain Bammer,” “Sergeant of the children scored eight or better.
Chowder,” and “General Border.” Even though syllable instruction The high competence in first phoneme isolating demonstrated
had been fully and explicitly discontinued three weeks previ- by these preschoolers indicates that very little first phoneme
ously, many of the SP children still attempted to segment the instruction may be needed for typically developing middle-class
names by syllables instead of phonemes. In addition, in the ear- preschoolers. These findings support the idea of moving quickly
lier syllable phase, one of the SP participants had to suppress his beyond first phoneme tasks to embedding them within instruc-
advanced knowledge when, given single-syllable words, he seg- tion directed at the more advanced tasks of phoneme blending and
mented phonemes and had to be corrected to syllables. segmenting.
It might be argued that the phoneme-syllable confusion in the
phoneme phase was due to inadequate syllable instruction or 9.4. Implications for preschool instruction
knowledge. However, evidence indicated syllable awareness was
robust. The children entered the study with considerable syllable The findings of this study have clear and direct implications for
blending awareness with mean pre-test scores of 6.7 (S.D. = 2.6) out preschool instruction in phonemic awareness. Other studies have
of 10 points. Incoming segmenting performance was lower, but still shown some success in teaching young children phoneme blending
showed emerging knowledge, with a mean pre-test score of 3.9 and segmenting (e.g., Hatcher, Hulme, & Snowling, 2004; Hesketh,
(2.6). The SP and MP incoming syllable blending and segmenting Dima, & Nelson, 2007; Yeh, 2003) but provide few details on the
were not significantly different (for both, p > .22). The SP children instruction. This study, along with Ukrainetz et al. (2000), pro-
were taught syllables through activities and procedures presented vides guidance on introducing young children to these advanced
in manualized curricula (e.g., Adams et al., 1998; Notari-Syverson tasks—with the caveat that both these studies involved small sam-
et al., 1998) and instructional articles (Gillon, 2006; Phillips et al., ples of typically developing preschoolers.
2008; Schuele & Boudreau, 2008). Treatment fidelity was good. Preschool teachers can start teaching phonemic awareness with
Every SP participant mastered syllable awareness after the four first sound tasks: isolating the first phoneme in words (e.g., What is
instructional sessions. the first sound in dog?), generating first phoneme words (e.g., What
It could also be argued that poor transition practices were pro- are words that begin with /d/?), and matching first phonemes (e.g.,
vided. There is no literature on how best to teach this transition Which of these words start with the same sound, dog, sun, down?).
to young children. Multiple instructional features were developed They can engage in the more difficult task of blending phonemes
that would minimize confusion, such as explicitly and consis- into words (e.g., I am going to say the sounds of the food I am holding
tently separating the two units of speech, immediately correcting behind me, you guess the word, /p/-/ea/-/ch/.). Preschool teachers
responses, and primarily using multisyllabic or monosyllabic words can even begin children on the advanced task of phoneme segment-
in the two instructional phases. These instructional features were ing so long as they initially provide maximal support (e.g., Let’s say
successful: the confusion was largely temporary, with no signif- the sounds in dog, get your fists ready to count the sounds, every-
icant differences between SP and MP at post-testing. Thus, the one say the sounds with me, /d/ [exaggerated mouth action, eye
evidence suggests that, if both speech units are taught, it is difficult contact with children, extend finger], /o/ [extend second finger],
to avoid confusion even with carefully planned instruction. /g/ [extend third finger]).
These results indicate no advantage for the development of In this study, these tasks were taught through simple activi-
phoneme awareness from prior syllable instruction. There were no ties using the objects, books, and writing materials common to any
significant differences between the two conditions and much more preschool. Preschool teachers can do the same, teaching through
initial learning confusion with syllable instruction. This study does matching picture cards and guessing the identity of food objects.
not examine possible later effects on phonemic awareness or read- Rhyming books provide excellent contexts for talking about sounds
ing, but other evidence suggests syllable awareness does not play in words (e.g., Let’s listen for the rhyming words. I will read it again,
a significant role in these either (Engen & Hoien, 2002; Hoien et al., There was a boy named Fred. He did not want to go to bed. Fred and bed
1995; McGuiness, 1997; Wood & Terrell, 1998). rhyme. What’s the first sound in Fred? What about bed? Let’s count
the sounds in Fred and bed and see which is longer). This study
9.3. Taking attention away from first phonemes did not employ writing activities, but prior studies have done so
successfully (e.g., Ukrainetz et al., 2000, 2009). As children explore
Instruction limited to first phoneme tasks, such as isolating, writing, teachers can talk about the sounds in words (e.g., Let’s
matching, and generating are conventional instructional targets for write a message about our dog Sam. Sam is hungry. We need to
preschoolers (McGee, 2005; Schuele & Boudreau, 2008). It would write Sam, what is the first sound in Sam? Let’s write a letter for
be of concern if teaching advanced skills compromised progress the sound/s/) (Ukrainetz, 2006a, 2006b). In addition, commercial
on these basic skills. The results of this study did not show any curricula (e.g., Adams et al., 1998; Notari-Syverson et al., 1998) can
detriment to first phoneme performance from attending to other be used as a source of activities. Teachers can move past the envi-
phoneme tasks. ronmental sounds, words, and syllable activities to go directly into
Part of the reason that first phoneme performance may have the phoneme activities presented.
fared so well across conditions may have been that the children As this study demonstrated, preschoolers can be introduced to
entered instruction with considerable awareness of first phonemes. the three tasks of phone isoloating, blending, and segmenting in
The mean pre-test isolating performance was 3.7, and 12 of the activities that combine the tasks (e.g., What is the first sound in
39 children scored eight or better before instruction commenced. man? Let’s say all the sounds in man), or in different activities within
In addition, first phoneme awareness appeared very teachable, a single session (e.g., a first sound matching card game followed by a
regardless of approach. The instruction in the FP condition was catching the fish segmenting game). It is not necessary to teach one
abbreviated for the 67% of the participants who mastered the tasks skill to mastery before introducing another of these inter-related
after only two weeks of instruction. The other two conditions skills.
addressed first phoneme awareness for the full four weeks, but only In teaching these skills, supportive instruction is important.
as one of three skills. As a result, all three conditions provided very Children need simple instructions, lots of modeling, and a consis-
little instruction in first phoneme tasks. Even so, at post-testing, all tent focus on sounds in words (not letters or meanings). Visually
three conditions showed reliable large gains in first phoneme iso- apparent and acoustically distinctive phonemes can be used, such
T.A. Ukrainetz et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 26 (2011) 50–60 59

as stops (e.g., /b/, /d/) and continuants (e.g., /m/, /s/), rather than instruction was provided in pairs and singles over a short period
vowels or glides (e.g., /r/, /w/), but ordering is not necessary beyond of time. Both the group size and the instructional period preclude
the introductory talk that focuses children on sounds rather than direct application to the regular preschool classroom. However, the
meanings or letters. Cueing techniques, such as emphasizing a tar- findings concerning what how to teach preschoolers and what they
get phoneme in a word (e.g., s-ad), repeating a phoneme (e.g., can learn contributes to understandings of best teaching practices.
b-b-bed), pointing to the ear to show listening for sounds, or rais- Finally, following the children into kindergarten would have
ing fingers to count sounds, help children learn. A lot of support is been informative. Kindergarten performance would reveal whether
needed initially, with a gradual reduction in cues as competence the early exposure to advanced phoneme tasks produced any
increases (see McGee & Ukrainetz, 2009; Ukrainetz, 2006a, 2006b). additional lasting benefit. It would be valuable to know whether
children who showed little immediate gain might demonstrate
9.5. Study limitations and future directions later benefits. For children at risk for reading difficulties, evidence
suggests that direct instruction is particularly important, because
This study provided useful findings on preschool phoneme these children are less likely than their typically achieving peers to
awareness instruction. However, there were a number of limita- learn phonemic awareness indirectly through reading and writing
tions and directions for future research. One limitation was the experiences (Byrne & Fielding-Barnsley, 1993; Hatcher et al., 2004).
small sample size. The sample size provided ample power to deter-
mine treatment versus no-treatment differences, but less power for 10. Conclusion
the more subtle differences expected among alternate treatments.
A concern was that the study was originally designed for a single This preliminary small-sample study showed that typically
year of data collection for two conditions, but was extended over 3 developing preschoolers can make sizeable gains in the advanced
years with a third condition added. Considerable effort was made tasks of phoneme blending and segmenting without negatively
to keep the participants and procedures consistent over that time affecting acquisition of first phoneme isolating. Results indicate
frame, including training the 2007 instructors from 2005 instruc- that prior syllable segmenting instruction may not benefit the
tion videos, balancing instructors across conditions, and balancing acquisition of phoneme awareness and may cause confusion dur-
the condition participants across the two time periods. However, ing instruction. It appears that preschool phoneme awareness can
there was likely more variation introduced by this post hoc exten- begin at the phoneme level without prior syllable instruction and
sion of the study. can include a supportive introduction to phoneme blending and
Related to the data collection period issue was that the FP condi- segmenting. Replication with a larger sample is recommended
tion was constituted solely of 2007 participants, in contrast to the before firm conclusions should be drawn. In addition, studies
SP and MP conditions, which drew from both time periods. In addi- should be done with children with various risks for literacy success,
tion, the preponderance of girls in the FP condition was a weakness such as low socioeconomic status, low language, and linguis-
that might have affected the FP results (although not the SP versus tic/cultural differences.
MP comparisons). Another limitation was in the study design, with
the SP condition receiving two extra weeks of instruction, which
References
conflated total duration of instruction with contents of instruc-
tion. However, this put the SP condition at an advantage, so the Adams, M. J., Foorman, B. R., Lundberg, I., & Beeler, T. (1998). Phonemic awareness in
lack of difference between SP and MP are perhaps more convinc- young children: A classroom curriculum. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
ing. Treatment fidelity was satisfactory, but there were divergences Anthony, J. L., Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., Driscoll, K., Phillips, B. M., & Cantor, B.
G. (2002). Structure of preschool phonological sensitivity: Overlapping sensi-
from the plan. The uneven focus on all three tasks of isolating, tivity to rhyme, words, syllables, and phonemes. Journal of Experimental Child
blending and segmenting went against the simultaneous instruc- Psychology, 82, 65–92.
tion approach plan, but the greater emphasis on the easier tasks Anthony, J. L., Lonigan, C. J., Driscoll, K., Phillips, B. M., & Burgess, S. R. (2003).
Phonological sensitivity: A quasi-parallel progression of word structure units
of isolating and blending initially and the harder task of segment-
and cognitive operations. Reading Research Quarterly, 38, 470–487.
ing later made sense instructionally. A treatment fidelity weakness Ball, E. W., & Blachman, B. A. (1988). Phoneme segmentation training: Effect on
for the FP instruction was that the phoneme types were not fully reading readiness. Annals of Dyslexia, 38, 208–225.
Brady, S., Fowler, A., Stone, B., & Winbury, N. (1994). Training phonological aware-
controlled in the first phoneme instruction. However, most of the
ness: A study with inner-city kindergarten children. Annals of Dyslexia, 44, 26–59.
children mastered phoneme isolating after just four sessions even Byrne, B., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1993). Evaluation of a program to teach phone-
with this mix of phoneme types. mic awareness to young children: A 1-year follow-up. Journal of Educational
The early graduation from the FP condition was a design weak- Psychology, 85, 104–111.
Carle, E. (1969). The very hungry caterpillar. Omaha, NE: World Publishing.
ness. The decision was made in the course of treatment as a Chaney, C. (1992). Language development, metalinguistic skills, and print awareness
response to the children’s early task mastery. Teaching children in 3-year-old children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 13, 485–514.
skills they already know is not appropriate pedagogy, but as long Clay, M. M. (1979). Reading: The patterning of complex behavior (2nd ed.). Portsmouth,
NH: Heinemann.
as the activities are entertaining, the children could have been kept Cary, L., & Verhaeghe, A. (1994). Promoting phonemic analysis ability among
reasonably engaged. The full duration of instruction should have kindergartners: Effects of different training programs. Reading and writing: An
occurred for all the FP participants. interdisciplinary journal, 6, 251–278.
Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (rev. ed.). New
A limitation was the homogeneity of this white middle-class York, NY: Academic Press.
sample, which limits generalization of these findings to children DeBaryshe, B. D., & Gorecki, D. M. (2007). An experimental validation of a preschool
of other cultural, linguistic, and socioeconomic backgrounds. Sim- emergent literacy curriculum. Early Education and Development, 18, 93–110.
Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Willows, D. M., Schuster, B. V., Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z., & Shana-
ilarly, this study involved only children with typically developing
han, T. (2001). Phonemic awareness instruction helps children learn to read:
language. Other children may take longer and need more scaffold- Evidence from the National Reading Panel’s meta-analysis. Reading Research
ing cues to achieve phoneme blending or segmenting. However, it Quarterly, 36, 250–287.
Engen, L., & Hoien, T. (2002). Phonological skills and reading comprehension. Reading
is not expected that the results would differ in kind: practices that
and writing: An interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 613–631.
confuse middle-class or typically achieving children are not likely Fox, B., & Routh, D. K. (1975). Analyzing spoken language into words, syllables,
to be beneficial for children of other backgrounds or lower language and phonemes: A developmental study. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 4,
levels (Johnston, 2006). 331–342.
Gillon, G. T. (2006). Phonological awareness intervention. In R. J. McCauley, & M. E.
The intent of this study was to determine under fairly controlled Fey (Eds.), Treatment of language disorders in children (pp. 279–308). Baltimore,
conditions the effect of certain instructional variables. As a result, MD: Brookes.
60 T.A. Ukrainetz et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 26 (2011) 50–60

Good, R. H., Simmons, D., Kame’enui, E., Kaminski, R. A., & Wallin, J. (2002). Summary Richgels, D. J., Poremba, K. J., & McGee, L. M. (1996). Kindergarteners talk about print:
of decision rules for intensive, strategic, and benchmark instructional recommen- Phonemic awareness in meaningful contexts. The Reading Teacher, 49, 632–642.
dations in kindergarten through third grade (technical report 11). Eugene, OR: Robertson, C., & Salter, W. (1997). The Phonological Awareness Test. East Moline, IL:
University of Oregon. Linguisystems.
Hatcher, P. J., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. (2004). Explicit phoneme training combined Schuele, C. M., & Boudreau, D. (2008). Phonological awareness intervention: Beyond
with phonic reading instruction helps young children at risk of reading failure. the basics. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 39, 3–20.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 338–358. Stanovich, K. E. (2000). Progress in understanding reading: Scientific foundations and
Hesketh, A., Dima, E., & Nelson, V. (2007). Teaching phoneme awareness to new frontiers. New York, NY: Guilford.
pre-literate children with speech disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Inter- Torgesen, J. K., Al Otaiba, S., & Grek, M. L. (2005). Assessment and instruction for
national Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 42, 251–271. phonemic awareness and word recognition skills. In H. W. Catts, & A. G. Kamhi
Hoien, T., Lundberg, I., Stanovich, K. E., & Bjaalid, I. (1995). Components of phono- (Eds.), Language and reading disabilities (2nd ed., pp. 127–156). Boston, MA: Allyn
logical awareness. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 171–188. & Bacon.
Hresko, W. P., Reid, K. R., & Hammill, D. D. (1999). Test of early language development Torgesen, J. K., Morgan, S. T., & Davis, C. (1992). Effects of two types of phonolog-
(3rd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. ical awareness training on word learning in kindergarten children. Journal of
Johnston, J. (2006). Thinking about language: Research to practice. Eau Claire, WI: Educational Psychology, 84, 364–370.
Thinking Publications. Ukrainetz, T. A. (2006a). Scaffolding young children into phonemic awareness. In
Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., Anthony, J. L., & Barker, T. A. (1998). Development T. A. Ukrainetz (Ed.), Contextualized language intervention: Scaffolding PreK-12
of phonological sensitivity in 2- to 5-year-old children. Journal of Educational literacy achievement (pp. 429–467). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Psychology, 90, 294–311. Ukrainetz, T. A. (2006b). Using emergent writing to develop phonemic awareness.
Lundberg, I., Frost, J., & Peterson, O. P. (1988). Effects of an extensive program In L. M. Justice (Ed.), Clinical approaches to emergent literacy intervention (pp.
for stimulating phonological awareness in preschool children. Reading Research 225–259). San Diego, CA: Plural Publishing.
Quarterly, 23, 263–284. Ukrainetz, T. A. (2008). Phonemic awareness instruction for preschoolers: The evi-
Maclean, M., Bryant, B., & Bradley, L. (1987). Rhymes, nursery rhymes, and reading dence for pre-phonemic versus phonemic tasks. EBP Briefs, 2, 47–58.
in early childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 255–281. Ukrainetz, T. A., Cooney, M. H., Dyer, S. K., Kysar, A. J., & Harris, T. J. (2000). An
McFadden, T. U. (1998). Sounds and stories: Teaching phonemic awareness in print investigation into teaching phonemic awareness through shared reading and
contexts. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 7, 5–13. writing. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 15, 331–355.
McGee, L. M. (2005, December). The role of wisdom in evidence-based preschool Ukrainetz, T. A., Ross, C. L., & Harm, H. M. (2009). An investigation of treatment
literacy curriculum. In Paper presented at the Presidential address to the National scheduling for phonemic awareness with kindergartners at risk for reading dif-
Reading Conference San Antonio, TX, ficulties. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 40, 86–100.
McGee, L. M., & Ukrainetz, T. A. (2009). Using scaffolding to teach phonemic aware- Vaughn, S., Wanzek, J., Woodruff, A. L., & Linan-Thompson, S. (2008). Prevention and
ness in preschool and kindergarten. Reading Teacher, 62, 599–603. early identification of students with reading disabilities. In D. Haager, J. Klingner,
McGuiness, D. (1997). Decoding strategies as predictors of reading skill: A follow-on & S. Vaughn (Eds.), Evidence-based reading practices for response to intervention
study. Annals of Dyslexia, 47, 117–150. (pp. 11–28). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Merriam Webster, Inc. (1993). Merriam Webster’s collegiate dictionary (10th ed.). Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Development of reading-
Springfield, MA: Author. related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bidirectional
Nancollis, A., Lawri, B. A., & Dodd, B. (2005). Phonological awareness intervention and causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology,
the acquisition of literacy skills in children from deprived social backgrounds. 30, 73–87.
Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 325–335. Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Comprehensive test of phono-
National Institute on Child Health and Human Development. (2000). National logical processing. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Reading Panel—Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the Wood, C., & Terrell, C. (1998). Poor readers’ ability to detect speech rhythm
scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruc- and perceive rapid speech. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16,
tion (NIH publication no. 00-4754). Washington, DC: U.S. Dept. of Health and 397–413.
Human Services. Retrieved November 30, 2003, from http:www.nichd.nih.gov/ Wyoming Department of Education. (2003). Early childhood readiness standards.
publications/nrp/findings.htm. Cheyenne, WY: Author. Retrieved from http://www.k12.wy.us/SA/standards.
Notari-Syverson, A., O-Connor, R. E., & Vadasy, P. F. (1998). Ladders to literacy: A asp.
preschool activity book. Baltimore, MD: Brookes. Yeh, S. S. (2003). An evaluation of two approaches for teaching phonemic awareness
Phillips, B. M., Clancy-Menchetti, J., & Lonigan, C. J. (2008). Successful phonological to children in Head Start. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 18, 513–529.
awareness instruction with preschool children. Topics in Early Childhood Special Yopp, H. K. (1988). The validity and reliability of phonemic awareness tests. Reading
Education, 28(1), 3–17. Research Quarterly, 23, 159–177.

You might also like