Rigid Vs Flexible Dieting

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

Appetite (2002) 38, 39±44

1
doi:10.1006/appe.2001.0445, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on

Original Article

Rigid vs. flexible dieting: association with


eating disorder symptoms in nonobese women
Tiffany M. Stewart, Donald A.Williamson and Marney A.White
Louisiana State University and Pennington Biomedical Research Center, Baton Rouge, LA, U.S.A.

(Received1August 2001, finalrevision16 November 2001, accepted in revised form 20 December 2001)

The correlates of rigid and flexible dieting were examined in a sample of 188 nonobese women recruited from the
community and from a university. The primary aim of the study was to test the hypothesis that women who utilize
rigid versus flexible dieting strategies to prevent weight gain report more eating disorder symptoms and higher body
mass index (BMI) in comparison to women who utilize flexible dieting strategies. The study sample included women
who were underweight (29%), normal weight (52%), and overweight (19%). None of the women were obese, as
defined by BMI > 30. Participants were administered a questionnaire that measures Rigid Control and Flexible
Control of eating. Body weight and height were measured and measures of eating disorder symptoms and mood
disturbances were administered. Our results indicated that BMI was significantly correlated with rigid dieting and
flexible dieting. BMI was controlled statistically in other analyses. The study found that individuals who engage in
rigid dieting strategies reported symptoms of an eating disorder, mood disturbances, and excessive concern with
body size/shape. In contrast, flexible dieting strategies were not highly associated with BMI, eating disorder
symptoms, mood disturbances, or concerns with body size. Since this was a cross sectional study, causality of eating
disorder symptoms could not be addressed. These findings replicate and extend the findings of earlier studies. These
findings suggest that rigid dieting strategies, but not flexible dieting strategies, are associated with eating disorder
symptoms and higher BMI in nonobese women.
# 2002 Elsevier Science Ltd

Introduction he called ``rigid control'' and ``flexible control'' of


eating behaviour. Rigid control was characterized by
The term dietary restraint refers to a person's intent to dichotomous ``all or nothing'' approach to eating,
restrict dietary intake to control body weight (Herman & weight, and dieting. Rigid control was associated with
Mack, 1975). Dietary restraint has been hypothesized a higher body mass index, more frequent and more
to play an important role in determining binge eating severe binge eating episodes, and higher scores of
associated with eating disorders (Polivy & Herman, disinhibition (overeating). In contrast, flexible control
1985). The Three Factor Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ), was characterized by a more graduated approach to
developed by Stunkard and Messick (1985), measures eating, weight, and dieting. Flexible control was
dietary restraint, disinhibition (overeating), and per- associated with lower body mass index, less frequent
ceived hunger. The dietary restraint scale of the TFEQ and less severe binge eating or overeating, and a higher
measures cognitive control of eating behaviour. In a probability of successful weight loss during a one-year
subsequent study of the TFEQ, Westenhoefer (1991) weight loss program. Subsequent research (McGuire et
found evidence that the construct of dietary restraint al., Shearin et al., 1994; Smith et al., 1998; Williamson
could be separated into two dieting strategies, that et al., 1995) provided partial support for the initial
findings of Westenhoefer (1991). A key difference in
the findings of these studies was that some investiga-
tions found that flexible control was associated with
Address correspondence to: Donald A. Williamson, Ph.D.,
Pennington Biomedical Research Center, 6400 Perkins Road, lower BMI and others did not.
Baton Rouge, LA 70808, U.S.A. Westenhoefer, Stunkard and Pudel (1999) added
E-mail: Williada@pbrc.edu items to the original scales and validated two revised

0195±6663/02/010039+06 $35.00/0 # 2002 Elsevier Science Ltd


40 T. M. Stewart et al.

scales called the Flexible and Rigid Control dimensions (27%), and women of other racial groups (27%). The
of dietary restraint. The end product was a 12- item scale mean age of the sample was 22.6 (SD ˆ 68). The mean
measuring Rigid Control and a 16-item scale measuring height (m) and weight (kg) of the overall sample was
Flexible Control. The validation studies for these scales 17 (SD ˆ 007) and 590 (SD ˆ 77), respectively. The
used correlations of the two scales with body mass index, mean BMI of the sample was 217 (SD ˆ 27).
the disinhibition scale of the TFEQ, and self-reported
eating disorder symptoms. The findings of the 1999
paper supported the conclusions of Westenhoefer (1991)
Assessment procedures
that rigid control was positively correlated with higher Rigid vs. Flexible Dieting Scale (Westenhoefer et al.,
BMI and greater eating disorder symptoms; whereas 1999). This self-report inventory has 12 questions that
flexible control was negatively correlated with BMI and measure Rigid Control of eating and 14 questions that
with eating disorder symptoms. Westenhoefer et al. measure Flexible Control of eating. The Rigid and
(1999) studied German men and women with a wide Flexible Control scales were an extension of earlier
range of BMI. research by Westenhoefer (1991) pertaining to the con-
In the study reported in this paper, adult women with struct of dietary restraint. The two scales were found
body weights ranging from underweight to overweight to have satisfactory reliability and validity in a series
(BMI < 30) were selected for study. This selection of of studies reported by Westenhoefer et al. (1999). In
nonobese women is important for the following rea- this study, the psychological and behavioural corre-
sons: (1) the adverse effects of dieting are of greatest lates of the Rigid and Flexible Control scales were
concern for nonobese women (National Task Force investigated. The psychological measures that were
on the Treatment and Prevention of Obesity, 2000); tested as correlates of Rigid and Flexible Control
(2) the relevance of the hypothesis that certain dieting are described below. Each measure was selected to
strategies are harmful is strongest in nonobese women, assess some aspect of the symptoms that are broadly
since these are the people most vulnerable for develop- associated with eating disorders (Williamson, 1990).
ing anorexia and bulimia nervosa; (3) most of the pre- Body Mass Index. Self-reported measurements of
vious tests of this hypothesis investigated samples of height (m) and weight (kg) were obtained from partici-
predominantly obese men and women that included pants. Self-reported weights have been shown to be
some obese participants. The primary aim of the study correlated with weight measured in the clinic (Bowman
was to test the hypothesis that Rigid Control of eating, & DeLucia, 1992). From these values, BMI was calcu-
but not Flexible Control of eating, is associated with lated using the formula: body weight (kg)/height (m2).
ED symptoms, mood disturbance, and higher BMI. Beck Depression Inventory II (BDI-II; Beck, Brown &
A secondary aim was to test the hypothesis that Flexible Steer, 1996). The BDI is a 21-item self-report ques-
Control of eating is negatively correlated with eating tionnaire measure that was utilized to assess the cogni-
disorder symptoms and BMI. tive, behavioural, affective, and somatic symptoms of
depression in the participants. The BDI has good
internal consistency and is correlated with other
measures of depression (Beck et al., 1996).
Method
Eating Attitudes Test (EAT; Garner & Garfinkel,
1979). The EAT is a 40-item self-report inventory that
Subjects
assesses behaviours and attitudes associated with
A sample of 188 female participants was recruited anorexia nervosa (Garner & Garfinkel, 1979). The reli-
from undergraduate courses at a large university and ability and validity of the EAT as a measure of eating
from the community residents. Inclusion criteria were: disorder symptoms has been supported by many studies
(1) female gender, (2) between the ages of 18 and 65 (Garner & Garfinkel, 1979; Williamson, 1991).
years, and (3) BMI < 30. Approximately one-half of Perceived Weight Cycling (PWC; Friedman Schwartz
the sample was recruited from undergraduate classes, & Brownell, 1998). The perception that a person has a
and the other half from the community. With regard to history of gaining and losing weight was measured
weight status, 303% of the participants had a Body using a 3 item scale. This scale was found to be corre-
Mass Index (BMI) under 20 (underweight); 543% of lated with psychological problems associated with
the participants had a BMI between 20 and 25 (normal weight cycling (Friedman et al., 1998).
weight), and 154% had a BMI between 25 and 30 Body Shape Questionnaire (BSQ; Cooper et al., 1987).
(overweight) (Bray, 1998). The sample contained The BSQ measures concerns with body shape, in par-
primarily Caucasian women (893%), with some ticular, the experience of ``feeling fat'' (Cooper et al.,
African American women (53%), Hispanic women 1987). The BSQ is correlated with measures of body
Rigid vs. flexible dieting 41

dissatisfaction and has been used to define subthreshold Table 1. Correlation analyses
eating disorders in nonobese women (Williamson et al.,
2000). Flexible control Rigid control
Three Factor Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ; Stunkard BMI 022* 032*
& Messick, 1985). The TFEQ (also called the Eating BDI-II 018 026*
Inventory) is a 51-item questionnaire that measures EAT 048* 066*
PWC 029* 055*
three constructs: dietary restraint, disinhibition, and BSQ 044* 070*
perceived hunger. The Dietary Restraint scale mea- TFEQ-D 018 049*
sures self-reported restrictive eating and dieting beha- TFEQ-PH ÿ005 019*
viour and many of the items of the Rigid and Flexible STAI-State 013 026*
Control scales are included in the Dietary Restraint STAI-Trait 015 030*
scale. Because of this item overlap, only scores from Abbreviations: BMI ˆ Body Mass Index, BDI-II ˆ Beck Depression
the disinhibition (TFEQ-D) and perceived hunger Inventory, EAT ˆ Eating Attitudes Test, PWC ˆ Perceived Weight
Cycling Measure, BSQ ˆ Body Shape Questionnaire, TFEQ ˆ The
(TFEQ-PH) scales were included in the statistical Three Factor Eating Questionnaire, D ˆ Disinhibition, PH ˆ Per-
analysis. ceived Hunger, STAI-State ˆ State-Trait Anxiety Inventory State
State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger anxiety scale, STAI-Trait ˆ State-Trait Anxiety Inventory Trait
anxiety scale.
et al., 1970). The STAI is a self-report measure desig- *Correlation is significant at the 001 level (2-tailed). Degrees of
ned to measure situational (STAI-State) and generali- freedom for all correlations were 186.
zed anxiety (STAI-Trait). The reliability and validity of
the STAI have been established as a measure of anxiety
in a wide variety of studies (Spielberger et al., 1970; Since the Rigid and Flexible Control scales were
Williamson, 1990). highly correlated, we were interested in the psycho-
logical and behavioural characteristics of groups of
Procedure participants that scored relatively high or low on the two
scales, while statistically controlling for variation
From an initial sample of 200 women, 12 were associated with BMI (since BMI was correlated with
excluded; 8 for BMI>30, and 4 for incomplete data, both Rigid and Flexible Control scales). To accomplish
leaving a study sample of 188 women. Women who this end, participants were categorized into dieting
were recruited from the student population were given strategy groups using a median split of scores on the
extra credit in undergraduate classes. All other Rigid (median ˆ 7) and Flexible (median ˆ 5) Control
participants were volunteers who did not receive any scales. Participants scoring below or at the median
incentive to participate in the study. All volunteers were classified as ``low'' while those scoring above the
signed consent to participate in the study. The median were classified as ``high''. Using this procedure,
participants completed the questionnaire packets, approximately 101% (N ˆ 19) of the sample could
which included questions about self-reported height be classified as ``Predominantly'' Rigid dieters and
and weight, age, race/ethnicity, and marital status. 133% (N ˆ 25) as ``Predominantly'' Flexible dieters.
For example, a Predominantly Rigid dieter scored above
the median on the Rigid Control scale, but below the
Results median on the Flexible Control scale. The remaining
portion of the sample was also classified according to
The Rigid and Flexible dieting scales were highly median splits on both scales: 420% (N ˆ 79) were
correlated (r(186) ˆ 070, p < 001). Table 1 sum- classified as Low Dieters on both scales, and 346%
marizes the correlations among the Rigid and Flexible (N ˆ 65) were classified as High Dieters on both scales.
Control scales with BMI and the psychological A 2 (flexible category)  2 (rigid category) analysis of
variables as assessed by the BDI-II, EAT, PWC, variance (ANOVA) was used to test whether BMI dif-
BSQ, TFEQ-D, TFEQ-PH, STAI-State, and STAI- fered across dieting strategy groups. There was no main
Trait scales. Flexible Control was positively correlated effect for Flexible control, F(1,184) ˆ 2406, p > 010.
with BMI, EAT, PWC, and BSQ scores. Rigid Control Also, there was no significant interaction of Rigid and
was positively correlated with BMI, BDI-II, EAT, Flexible control, F(1,184) ˆ 0000, p > 099. There was a
PWC, BSQ, TFEQ-D, TFEQ-PH, STAI-State anxiety, significant main effect for Rigid Control, F(1,184) ˆ
and STAI-Trait anxiety scores. Correlations between 4179, p < 005. The mean BMI for the Low Rigid
psychological variables and Rigid Control were con- Control group was 2114 (SD ˆ 273) and the mean
sistently higher than correlations between psychologi- BMI for the High Rigid Control group was 2246
cal variables and Flexible Control scores. (SD ˆ 255).
42 T. M. Stewart et al.

Table 2. Estimated marginal means (means adjusted for BMI as covariate)


Dependent variable Rigid median category Mean SE F(1, 182) p
BDI-II Low (7) 6616 0958 8842 0003
High (8) 10954 1089
EAT Low (7) 9987 1158 35336 0001
High (8) 20473 1317
PCW Low (7) 5398 0291 23003 0001
High (8) 7522 0331
BSQ Low (7) 80865 3318 48849 0001
High (8) 116192 3773
TFEQ-D Low (7) 5303 0422 23551 0001
High (8) 842 0479
TFEQ-PH Low (7) 5594 0379 6083 0015
High (8) 7017 0431
STAI-State Low (7) 48809 1213 638 0012
High (8) 53477 138
STAI-Trait Low (7) 51257 1334 4256 0041
High (8) 55451 1517
Abbreviations: BDI-II ˆ Beck Depression Inventory, EAT ˆ Eating Attitudes Test, PCW ˆ Perceived Weight Cycling
Measure, BSQ ˆ Body Shape Questionnaire, TFEQ ˆ Three Factor Eating Questionnaire, D ˆ Disinhibition,
PH ˆ Perceived Hunger, STAI-State ˆ State-Trait Anxiety Inventory State anxiety scale, STAI-Trait ˆ State-Trait
Anxiety Inventory Trait anxiety scale.

Ordinal regression analyses were conducted on the Control, and BMI as continuous variables, controlling
categorical demographic variables and revealed no for experiment-wise error. These analyses resulted in
differences in the proportion of predominantly rigid identical conclusions to the MANCOVA approach.
versus flexible dieting strategies across racial/ethnic
groups, 2(10) ˆ 504, p > 005, or marital status groups
2(4) ˆ 074, p > 005.
Group differences on each of the psychological Discussion
variables were first tested using MANCOVA, with a
2 (High/Low Rigid)  2(High/Low Flexible) data ana- This study supported the primary hypothesis that
lytic plan. BMI was used as a covariate since BMI was individuals engaging in rigid dieting strategies were
significantly correlated with both Flexible and Rigid more likely to report symptoms of an eating disorder,
Control scores. BMI was found to be a significant co- including mood disturbances and higher anxiety. The
variate F(8, 175) ˆ 5203, p < 0.01. There was a signifi- findings of the study did not support the secondary
cant main effect for Rigid Control, F(8, 175) ˆ 8396, hypothesis that higher Flexible Control scores were
p < 001, but the main effect for Flexible Control associated with lower BMI and fewer symptoms of
F(8, 175) ˆ 1662, p > 010, and the Flexible  Rigid eating disorders. It should be noted that the sample for
interaction F(8, 175) ˆ 0886, p > 05 were not statisti- this study was 100% women and that there were no
cally significant. Follow-up analyses using ANCOVAs obese participants in the study sample. A high
for each of the psychological variables are reported in correlation between the Rigid and Flexible Control
Table 2. High and Low Rigid Control groups differed scales was found. The only other study of the revised
on each of the psychological variables. The proportion Rigid and Flexible Control scales (Westenhoefer et al.,
of predominantly Rigid versus Flexible dieters did 1999) did not report the correlation between the two
not differ as a function of the BMI category (i.e., under scales. The finding of a strong correlation between the
weight, normal weight, and overweight), 2 (2, N ˆ two scales suggested that the women of this sample did
44) ˆ 397, p > 010. This multivariate data analytic not often rely upon a single dieting strategy to control
strategy was selected because the study hypothesis was body weight. To investigate this possibility, we divided
that rigid, but not flexible dieting, would be associated the sample into predominantly rigid versus flexible
with eating disorder symptoms, mood disturbances, and dieters using a median split of the scores on each scale.
higher BMI. This hypothesis called for the use of a Only 23% of the sample could be classified as
multivariate omnibus test (Huberty & Morris, 1989). Predominately Rigid or Flexible dieters. A larger pro-
We also tested the hypothesis using a series of multi- portion (35%) reported using both dieting strategies.
variate regression analyses using Rigid Control, Flexible A higher body mass index was associated with rigid
Rigid vs. flexible dieting 43

dieting strategies, but not flexible dieting strategies. Thus, the results of these studies have consistently
In the comparison of rigid versus flexible dieting groups supported the positive relationship between Rigid
on psychological variables, differences in BMI were Control and eating disorder symptoms and BMI, but the
controlled statistically. Group differences attributed negative correlation between Flexible Control and BMI
to Rigid Control were not diminished by covariance and eating disorder symptoms has not been consistently
of BMI. This pattern of results indicates a higher supported.
association of eating disorder symptoms and body
image disturbance with rigid dieting strategies
than with flexible dieting strategies, when covari- References
ance with BMI and other variables was controlled
statistically. Beck, A. T., Brown, G. K. & Steer, R. A. (1996). The Beck
Depression Inventory II. San Antonio, TX, Psychological
A limitation of the study is that the findings of Corporation.
the study were primarily based on self-report data. Bowman, R. L. & DeLucia, J. L. (1992). Accuracy of self-
In addition, the sample consisted of primarily healthy reported weight: A meta-analysis. Behaviour Therapy
young people. This could have limited the occurrence 23, 637±655.
of eating disorder symptoms in the sample, biasing Bray, G. A. (1998). Classification and evaluation of the over-
weight patient. In G. Bray, C. Bouchard, and W. James
the results. Another limitation is that the conclusions (Eds.) Handbook of Obesity. pp. 831±854. New York:
of the study were based upon correlational findings, Marcel Dekker, Inc.
which does not imply causation of the relationship Cooper, P. J., Taylor, B. S., Cooper, Z. & Fairburn, M. D.
between dieting strategies and behaviour. Thus, the (1987). The Development and Validation of the Body
directionality of the association between rigid dieting Shape Questionnaire. International Journal of Eating
Disorders 6(4), 485±494.
and eating pathologies is not known; it could be that Friedman, M. A., Schwartz, M. B. & Brownell, K. D. (1998).
individuals with greater concern with body size and Differential relation of psychosocial functioning with
shape and eating-disordered behaviours are more the history and experience of weight cycling. Journal of
likely to engage in rigid dieting strategies. Since this Consulting and Clinical Psychology 4, 646±650.
was a cross sectional study, causal associations Garner, D. M. & Garfinkel, P. E. (1979). The Eating Atti-
tudes Test: an index of symptoms of anorexia nervosa.
among the variables were not addressed. There is a Psychological Medicine 9, 273±279.
need for longitudinal studies to test for causal asso- Herman, C. P. & Mack, D. (1975). Restrained and unre-
ciations between Rigid versus Flexible Control of strained eating. Journal of Personality 43, 647±660.
eating and the development of eating disorder Huberty, C. J. & Morris, J. D. (1989). Multivariate analysis
symptoms and/or increased body weight. versus multiple univariate analyses. Psychological Bulletin
105, 302±308.
In summary, this is the first study to report a strong McGuire, M. T., Jeffery, R. W., French, S. A. & Hannan, P. J.
correlation between the Rigid and Flexible Control (2001). The relationship between restraint and weight and
scales. This is the second study that reports on the new weight-related behaviours among individuals in a com-
Rigid and Flexible Dieting scales. The findings of this munity weight gain prevention trial. International Journal
study partially replicated and extended the findings of of Obesity 25, 574±580.
National Task Force on the Prevention and Treatment of
Westenhoefer et al. (1999). Since Rigid and Flexible Obesity. (2000). Archives of Internal Medicine 160,
Control scores were highly correlated and since both 2581±2588.
scales were positively correlated with BMI, it was Polivy, J. & Herman, C. P. (1985). Dieting and binging:
imperative to control for covariation among the vari- A causal analysis. American Psychologist 40, 193±201.
ables, which was accomplished using a multivariate Shearin, E. N., Russ, M. J., Hull, J. W. & Clarkin, J. F.
(1994). Construct validity of the Three-Factor Eating
statistical approach. The finding that Rigid Control Questionnaire: Flexible and rigid control subscales. Inter-
scores were associated with eating disorder symptoms, national Journal of Eating Disorders 16(2), 187±198.
broadly defined, replicated and extended the basic Smith, C. F., Williamson, D. A., Bray, G. A. & Ryan, D. H.
conclusions of most other studies (e.g. Smith et al., 1999; (1999). Flexible vs. Rigid Dieting Strategies: Relation-
Westenhoefer, 1991; Westenhoefer et al., 1999) that has ship with Adverse Behavioural Outcomes. Appetite 32,
295±305.
implicated that rigid, but not flexible dieting strategies Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L. & Lushene, R. E. (1970).
are associated with eating disorder symptoms such as Manual for the State Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto,
binge eating. However, this study and the recently CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
published paper of McGuire et al. (2001) failed to find a Stunkard, A. J. & Messick, S. (1985). The three-factor eating
negative correlation between Flexible Control and BMI. questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition,
and hunger. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 29, 71±83.
Other studies, however, have reported a negative cor- Westenhoefer, J. (1991). Dietary restraint and disinhibition:
relation between Flexible Control and BMI (Smith et al., Is restraint a homogeneous construct? Appetite 16,
1999; Westenhoefer et al., 1999; Williamson et al. 1995). 45±55.
44 T. M. Stewart et al.

Westenhoefer, J., Stunkard, A. & Pudel, V. (1999). Valida- Williamson, D. A., Perrin, L., Blouin, D. C. & Barbin, J. M.
tion of the flexible and rigid control dimensions (2000). Cognitive bias in eating disorders: Interpretation
of dietary restraint. International Journal of Eating of ambiguous body-related information. Eating and
Disorders 26, 53±64. Weight Disorders 5, 143±151.
Williamson, D. A. (1990). Assessment of eating disorders:
Obesity, anorexia, and bulimia nervosa. New York:
Pergamon Press.

You might also like