The Mediational Role: Parenting Sensitivity, Parenta Depression and Child Health: Parental Self-Efficacy

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

Early Development and Parenting, Vol.

5 (4), 237-250 (1996)

Parenting Sensitivity, Parenta 1


Depression and Child Health:
The Mediational Role of Parental
Self-Efficacy
Douglas M. Teti*
University of Maryland Baltimore County, USA
Melissa A. O’Connell
University of Maryland Baltimore County, USA
Christine D. Reiner
University of Ma yland Baltimore County, USA

This paper discusses the theoretical role of parental self-efficacy, or


parents’ beliefs in their competence and effectiveness in the
parental role, as a mediator of relations between parent-child risk
and parental sensitivity. Evidence is marshalled fmm studies of
parenting in the contexts of maternal depression and child health
risk to support the premise that parent-child characteristics affect
parental sensitivity indirectly via their more direct impact on
parental feelings of efficacy, and that parenting efficacy represents
the ’final common pathway‘ in the prediction of parenting
sensitivity. Also considered in this working model are specific
social-contextual factors as independent contributors to parenting
efficacy and as possible moderators of relations between parent-
child characteristics and self-efficacy. Implications for intervention
are discussed. 01996 by John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Early Dev. Parent. 5: 237-250,1996.
No. of Figures: 0. No. of Tables: 0. No. of References: 123.

Key mrds: parental self-efficacy; parenting sensitivity; maternal


depression; special needs children

This contribution explores the theoretical role of then examine the role of parenting efficacy in the
parental cognitions, and in particular parental self- prediction of parental sensitivityin two contexts:(1)
efficacy, or parents’ beliefs about their own in parents sufferingfrom clinical depression, which
competence in the parenting role, in fostering has been linked empirically to problematic
sensitivity and competence in parenting. We begin parenting and child developmental risk (Downey
with a brief discussion of parental cognitions as and Coyne, 1990; Gelfand and Teti, 1990); and (2) in
central determinants of parental behaviour. We parents of young children with special needs by
virtue of medical fragility (e.g. low birthweight or
‘Correspondence to: Dr Douglas M. Teti, Department of prematurity) or identified developmental delay.
Psychology, University of Maryland Baltimore County, 1000 Interestingly, very little work has examined the
Hilltop Circle, Baltimore, MD 21228, USA. mediational role of parenting efficacy in the latter

CCC 1057-3593/96/040237-14$17.50 Received 16 December 1996


01996 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Accepted 22 January 1996
238 D. M . Teti, M . A. O‘Connell and C. D. Reiner

context, despite the considerable challenges that relationships, the content of which is shaped by
special needs children createfor their parents. These quality of care. These cognitive organizations are
two contextsare representativeof a broader array of seen as guiding the quality of ensuing behaviour
p a r e n t a l d risk factors that, we propose, should that young children exhibit towards caregivers
debilitate parental sensitivity indirectly via their and peers and influencing the manner in which
more direct impact on parental self-efficacy. In subsequent information from significant others is
addition to this working model, we consider some selected and interpreted (Main et al., 1985).
additional social-contextual predictors that, again, Systematic attempts to examine cognitive-
we propose will impact parental sensitivity behavioural interfaces in parenting have emerged
indirectly, either by directly affecting parental self- in earnest only in the last decade (eg. Dix and
efficacyor by moderating relations between parent- Grusec, 1983; Sigel, 1985), despite some early
child risks and self-efficacy. In either case, we attempts to identify cognitive mediators of
advance the premise that parental self-efficacy is parental functioning (e.g. Emmerich, 1969).
the ’final common pathway‘ mediating relations Cognitions and emotions are now placed at the
between parent-child characteristics, social- heart of conceptualizations of parenting, with
contextual factors and parental sensitivity. cognitiveaffective organizations parents bring to
the dyadic setting viewed as causal to parenting
behaviour and as potentially open to modifiability
COGNlTIONS AND PARENTING as parent-child relationships proceed. Attachment
theory and research, for example, has expanded to
Despite the ‘cognitive revolution’ that has incorporatethe effects of parental ’working models’
dominated psychology over the past three of relationships, believed to be rooted in (but not
decades, the interface of cognition and parenting necessarily veridical with) parents’ early
has been addressed only recently (Maccoby and relationship histories, in parents’ propensities to
Martin, 1983; Main et al., 1985; Miller, 1988; Sigel, perceive children’s cues without distortion and to
1985). Traditionally, parenting practices and respond to children sensitively and appropriately
family interaction patterns have been explained (Bakermans-Kranenburgh and Van Ijzendoorn,
more in terms of learning and personality 1993; Main et al., 1985; Van Ijzendoorn, 1992). Dix
processes, and although it is widely assumed and his colleagues (Dix and Grusec, 1983; Dix and
that cognition, emotion and behaviour are closely Lochman, 1990; Dix and Reinhold, 1991; Dix et al.,
intertwined, considerable debate exists over the 1986, 1990) conceptualize parenting as a process
primacy of each (Goodnow, 1988). Consistent with mediated by parental attributions about such
a transactional perspective on development factors as the degree to which children are to be
(Bandura, 1986; Goodnow, 1988; Sameroff, 1975), held responsible for their actions, children’s
there is evidence of complex interactive linkages intentions and children’s social competence, all of
between multiple systems. For example, in some which are age- and gender-dependent. Dix (1991)
circumstances, cognition is thought to drive has presented a model of parenting that places
behaviour (Bandura, 1986; Teti and Gelfand, affective organizations at the centre of the parenting
1991). In others, cognitions are viewed as arising process, conceptualized as a dynamic, reciprocal
from individual experiences with the environment interplay between emotional activation,
(Bandura, 1986; Goodnow, 1988). Finally, cognitive engagement and regulation. Although this model
appraisals of environmental events are seen as places greater weight on the role of emotion rather
intimately tied to action tendencies, which in turn than cognition in organizing parenting behaviour,
predispose emotion and behaviour (Frijda et al., cognitions (e.g. appraisal processes) are viewed as
1989). Curiously, social developmental theorists central determinants of emotions, and both are thus
have been more concerned with cognitive- closely intertwined. Whether it is cognition or
behavioural relations in children rather than in emotion, however, that immediately precedes
parents. Attachment theorists (Ainsworth et al., parental responding at a given moment is a
1978; Bowlby, 1973; Bretherton, 1985; Crittenden, theoretical issue that defies easy testing. We view
1992; Sroufe, 1983; Sroufe and Fleeson, 1986), for cognition, emotion and behaviour to be in constant
example, propose that children develop a reciprocal interplay, and attempts to establish
‘working model’ of their caregivers and primacy of any one over the others are fraught
themselves, conceptualized as a set of affectively with difficulty. In this paper, our discussion of
laden cognitions or ’rules‘ regarding interpersonal parental self-efficacy as a mediator of child

Early Dev. Parent. 5 237-250 (1996) 01996 by JohnWiley & Sons, Ltd.
Parental Self-Eficacy 239

outcomes operates from a level of analysis that and Wood (1989) define self-efficacy beliefs as
views cognitive appraisal processes as both judgments about one’s competency at a particular
predictive of and responsive to affective states and task or in a particular setting. Bandura‘s (1986)
behaviour. social-cognitive theory proposes that self-
efficacious individuals, who judge themselves as
competent and effective in a given task, are
PARENTING EFFICACY AND expected to persist in their endeavours to achieve
SENSITIVITY AMONG DEPRESSED success even in the face of rather formidable
MOTHERS obstacles, whereas self-inefficacious individuals
are expected to lack such tenacity and give up
Evidence is widespread that depressed mothers prematurely, even though success is potentially
hold decidedly negative cognitions about their achievable. Further, relations between self-efficacy
children and themselves. For example, depressed beliefs and performance attainments are viewed as
mothers have more negative perceptions of their bidirectional, in that how efficacious one feels at a
children’s social competence and psychiatric given task is also related to one‘s history of
adjustment than do nondepressed mothers successes and failures at that task. Although self-
(Fergusson et al., 1985; Forehand ef al., 1986; Fox efficacy is believed to be most strongly affected by
and Gelfand, 1994; Friedlander et al., 1986; Griest et performance attainments, it is also susceptible to
al., 1979; Pannaccione and Wahler, 1986; Rickard et other influences, such as affective state, vicarious
al., 1981; Rogers and Forehand, 1983; Schaughency experiences (e.g. modelling) and social persuasion
and Lahey, 1985; Webster-Stratton and Hammond, from support figures.
1988), which may predispose such mothers to Not surprisingly, depressed mothers of young
overestimate the extent and seriousness of their children report feeling less efficacious in the
children’s problems (Rickard ef al., 1981). parenting role than do nondepressed mothers
Depressed cognitions may figure importantly in (Fox and Gelfand, 1994; Teti and Gelfand, 1991;
accounting for the widespread documentation of Teti et al., 1990). Depressed mothers’ self-inefficacy
problematic parenting among depressed mothers. as parents may derive from the propensity of sad
The parenting of depressed mothers has affect to foster negative, pessimistic, ruminative
been variously characterized as insensitive, incom- thought processes about oneself and one’s
petent, apathetic and uninvolved, ineffective, environment (Clark and Teasdale, 1982; Isen, 1984;
emotionally flat, disengaged, intrusive and angry Teasdale and Fogarty, 1979) and to selectively
(Cummings and Davies, 1994; Gelfand and Teti, activate memories of failure experiences (Bower,
1990; Goodman, 1992). In turn, children of 1981). Inefficacious mothers’ self-doubts in turn
depressed parents are at risk for insecure should lead to insensitive parenting, marked by
attachment, emotional dysregulation and psycho- impatience, rigidity and withdrawal.
pathology (Cummings and Davies, 1994; Field, Using 38 nondepressed and 48 clinically
1984; Murray, 1992; Radke-Yarrow et aI., 1985; Teti depressed lower-middle to middle class mothers
ef al., 1995; Zahn-Waxler ef al., 1984). and their first-year infants, Teti and Gelfand (1991)
Interestingly, researchers have only recently specifically tested the hypotheses that (1)maternal
begun to examine maternal self-efficacy beliefs in self-efficacy would relate positively to mothers’
relation to maternal behaviour (e.g. Davis, 1990; behavioural competence (sensitivity) with their
Donovan and Leavitt, 1989; Gross and Conrad, infants, and (2) maternal self-efficacy would
1992; Teti and Gelfand, 1991), despite Bandura’s mediate relations between maternal behavioural
(1982, 1986) seminal formulations identifymg self- competence and the predictor variables of severity
efficacy as a central mediator of relations between of depressive symptoms, infant temperamental
knowledge and behaviour. This belated application difficulty and social-marital supports. All mothers
is likely because self-efficacy theory originally were in therapy for their depression at recruitment
emerged from laboratory work that assessed self- and had received DSM-III-R diagnoses of major
efficacy change as a function of deliberate depression, dysthymia or adjustment disorder
inductions of success or failure experience with depressed mood from their therapists. Non-
(Bandura and Wood, 1989), positive and negative depressed mothers were recruited from the same
moods (Kavanagh and Bower, 1985) or neighbourhds as the depressed mothers.
performance-contingent feedback (Newman and Severity of depressive symptoms was assessed
Goldfried, 1987). Bandura (1982,1986) and Bandura with the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck ef

01996 by JohnWiley & Sons, Ltd. Early Dev. Parent. 5: 237-250 (1996)
240 D.M. Teti, M. A. O‘Connell and C.D.Reiner

al., 1961). Social-marital supports were measured statistically controlled. Although these data are
with a standardized composite score derived from correlational, they nevertheless suggest that
the marital harmony scales of Locke and Wallace mothers‘ affective states and perceptions of their
(1959) and Spanier (1976) and social support social environments significantly influenced
subscales of the Interview Schedule for Social maternal self-efficacy beliefs, which in turn
Interaction (Henderson et al., 1981). The fussy- influenced parental sensitivity.
difficultness subscale of the Infant Characteristics
Questionnaire (Bates et al., 1979) served to obtain
mothers’ perceptions of infant temperamental ADJUSTMENT AMONG PARENTS OF
difficulty. Maternal self-efficacy was examined SPECIAL NEEDS CHILDREN
with a scale developed by the authors that
inquired about self-efficacy beliefs in nine The link between parental well-being and maternal
domains of parenting specific to mothers of first- self-efficacy (e.g. Cutrona and Troutman, 1986; Teti
year infants (e.g. soothing, understanding baby’s and Gelfand, 1991)among depressed mother-infant
wishes, maintaining baby‘s attention), with a final dyads suggests that parental self-efficacy, and in
question that inquired about the women’s global turn parenting sensitivity, may be especially
sense of self-efficacy as parents. A single score for compromised when parents are faced with the
maternal self-efficacy was obtained by summing challenges and uncertainties attendant on rearing a
the individual scores of the 10 items (alpha=0.86). child with special needs. Unfortunately, the relation
Finally, maternal behavioural competence was of maternal self-efficacy to parental sensitivity and
derived from observations of mothers’ behaviour competence has not been examined at length with
in contexts of feeding and free play. Individual parents of special needs children. Most of the
dimensions of mothers’ behaviour rated by ‘blind’ published literature on the parents of special
observers were adapted from scales developed by needs children has focused on overall parental
Zoll et al. (1984) and included sensitivity, warmth, adjustment and well-being.Such parents reportedly
flatness of affect, disengagement and anger. These experience high levels of anxiety (McGettiganet al.,
ratings were adjusted so that higher scores always 1994; Roman et al., 1995), depressive symptom-
reflected more competent behaviour and the 10 atology (Bennett and Slade, 1991; Bristol et al., 1988;
ratings (5 per context) were summed to create a McGettigan et al., 1994; Miller ef al., 1992; Roman et
composite maternal behavioural competence index. al., 1995), psychological distress (Goldberg et al.,
Interrater (Pearsonr=0.87 on 18dyads) and internal 1986), elevated discrete life stressors (Beckman,
reliability (alpha=0.86) on this composite was 1991; Friedrich and Friedrich, 1981; Hanson and
adequate. As expected, maternal self-efficacy Hanline, 1990; Innocenti et al., 1992) and marital
beliefs correlated negatively with mothers’ discord (Cmicet al., 1983a; Friedrich and Friedrich,
depressive symptomatology and perceptions of 1981).
infant temperament, indicating that mothers felt These adjustment difficultiestypically commence
less efficacious as parents when their depression at the moment parents are faced with the news that
levels were high and when they perceived their their baby is different, or at risk to become different,
infants as difficult. In addition, maternal self- from other children. Parents of mentally
efficacy beliefs correlated positively with mothers‘ handicapped children often initially feel ’stunned’,
social-marital supports and with independent ‘numb’, ‘sad’, ’shock’ and ’regret’ when first
observers’ judgements of their behavioural learning of their child’s diagnosis (Seideman and
competence with their infants. Subsequent Kleine, 1995).Further, mothers who give birth to a
multiple regression analyses indicated that child with serious medical complicationsmust deal
maternal self-efficacy beliefs continued to predict with the loss of the expected healthy infant (Solnit
maternal behavioural competence even after the and Stark, 1961)and adapt to the fact that the child
other variables were statistically controlled (i.e. may not develop normally. Parents of medically
maternal depressive symptomatology, social- fragile children who require neonatal intensive care
marital supports and perceptions of infant services are often anxious about their child‘s
temperament). In addition, relations of maternal condition and survival, and about their ability to
depressive symptoms, social-marital supports and care for the child after discharge from the hospital
perceptions of infant temperament to maternal (Pederson et al., 1987). Not surprisingly, the degree
behavioural competence were substantially of adjustment difficulties appears to relate
reduced when maternal self-efficacy beliefs were straightforwardly to the severity of neonatal risk

Early Dev. Parent. 5: 237-250 (1996) 01996 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Parental SeZj-Eficacy 241

status. For example, Bennett and Slade (1991) found among parents of special needs children, we
that when infants were at greater risk, mothers would expect that the adjustment difficulties
reported more difficulty in expressing affection parents experience in relation to the birth of a
towards their infants. Similarly, they found that the special needs child, and to the difficulties these
lower the infant’s birthweight, and the longer children create as a function of the severity of their
the infant‘s hospitalization, the longer it took for conditions, would impact directly upon parental
the mother to develop feelings of attachment to her feelings of efficacy, the quality of their parenting,
baby. Jeffcoateet al. (1979) reported that half of the and in turn the extent of individual differences in
mothers of preterm infants in their sample did not the developmental outcomes of these children.
feel emotionally attached to their infants until at Indeed, long-term, persistent difficultieshave been
least 2 months after the infants were born. found to pose problems for parenting efficacy and
There is also the unfortunate tendency for some the parent-child subsystem. In one of the few
parents of medically fragile or handicapped studies to assess parental self-efficacy among
children to experience guilt or self-blame about parents of developmentally delayed children,
their child’s condition. For example, some mothers Crnic et a2. (1983a) reported that mothers of such
of premature infants feel they have failed in their children reported greater depressive and
reproductive role by not carrying their babies to dysphoric effects, more preoccupation with the
term (Jeffcoate et al., 1979). Mothers of Down child, lowered feelings of parental efficacy (i.e.
syndrome children engage in self-blame more sense of competence), less enjoyment of the child
often that do mothers of developmentally normal and greater possessiveness.
children (Rodrigueef al., 1990),perhaps because the Interestingly, parents faced with uncertainty
genetic aetiology of Down syndrome predisposes about the developmental outcome of their
parents to feel responsiblefor their child’s condition children (e.g. being suspected of cerebral palsy,
(Goldberg et al., 1986). but not manifesting symptoms to a sufficient
Parents of medically at-risk children report degree to warrant a clear diagnosis) may
significant disruption in their parental roles if the experience greater adjustment difficulties, and
child must be hospitalized in the neonatal intensive parental inefficacy, than parents of children with
care unit (NICU) (Miles et al., 1992). Parents’ a clear-cut diagnosis. In a study by Goldberg (1983,
inability to bring their child home from the as cited in Goldberg et al., 1986), mothers of
hospital right away makes it difficult for parents children with Down syndrome were found to be
to feel as if the child is truly theirs (Jeffcoate et al., less stressed than mothers of children whose delay
1979). It may also be difficult for parents to really was related to unknown aetiologies. Goldberg et a2.
feel that they are the child’s parents when nurses (1986) suggested that parents of children with
and doctors are primarily responsible for seeing to Down syndrome had more time to adjust to child-
the child’s care. Despite the fact that NICU practices related difficulties than parents of children with
have changed within the last two decades such that developmental delay because they knew about the
parents are now encouraged to help care for their child’s diagnosis relatively early and could
infants on site, some problems make it more prepare accordingly. These parents would thus
difficult for the parents to become involved with be expected to experience less negative
their infant. For example, parents are often unable to adjustment, and greater feelings of parental
hold infants who are on respirators. Additionally, efficacy, than would parents whose children had
mothers may be unable to breast-feed infants who developmental delays that became manifest more
are in the NICU and to visit their children frequently gradually and for whom the prognosis for such
because they live significant distances from the developmental problems might be more uncertain.
hospital.
The Competent Parent-Child Dyad
PARENTING EFFICACY AMONG It is theoretically likely that feelings of efficacy in
PARENTS WITH SPECIAL NEEDS both parents and children may be adversely
CHILDREN affected in the context of child risk status or
handicap. In her seminal paper, Goldberg (1977)
Although surprisingly little research has targeted notes that parents of children who experience
relations between parenting efficacy and difficulty reading or responding to the other can
individual differences in parenting quality lead to ‘incompetent’ parent-infant dyads which

01996 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Early Dev. Parent. 5 237-250 (1996)
242 D. M. Tefi,M. A. O’Connell and C. D. R e i w

can foster feelings of inefficacy in both partners. arise because parents view their developmentally
Parental self-efficacy is predicated upon the delayed or at-risk child as vulnerable when, in fact,
parents’ ability to appropriately read, interpret the child may be developing normally. He reviews
and respond to the infant’s cues. Thus, infants studies in which infants who are randomly labelled
whose signals are clear and unambiguous should as premature are rated by mothers (Stem and
lead to parental feelings of efficacy, especially if Hildebrandt, 1986, as cited in Mitchell, 1987) and
parents in turn are able to witness the effectiveness by college students (Miller and Ottinger, 1986, as
of their efforts in responding to infant signals. cited in Mitchell, 1987) to be less alert and
Children who signal poorly, perhaps because of responsive social partners, less enjoyable to
immature and/or developmentally delayed or interact with and less attractive, and are treated
compromised nervous systems, would make it more delicately than infants who are randomly
more difficult for parents to read and respond labelled as full-term. Thus,expectancybiases about
appropriately, ultimately fostering parental the premature infant may influence parenting even
feelings of inefficacy. Children’s feelings of in the absence of identifiable developmental
efficacy, in turn, are predicated on their problems.
caregivers’ ability to respond appropriately to A mother’s belief about her effectiveness as a
their bids for food, comfort and attention, which, parent can influence how much and what she
to a great degree, is likely to be a function of the attends to, the effort expended in raising her child
child’s ability to provide the parent with clear, and her emotional reactions to her child’s
readable signals (and on the parent’s state of behaviour (Mash and Johnston, 1990). Further,
motivation to respond to such signals, which may parenting is influenced by what parents consider
be compromised by parental depression). A their child to need, which is a function of past
‘competent‘ parent-child dyad is thus one in experience with that child, the child’s clarity of
which the child’s social cues are readable and communication and social signalling and the
interpretable, and in turn are responded to child’s developmental status. When that child is
contingently and appropriately by the parent. medically compromised and/or developmentally
Parents of premature infants tend to be less delayed, communicative effectiveness may be
actively involved with their babies than parents of dramatically decreased. In turn, parents may
full-term infants. Parents of premies have been provide their child with less age-appropriate
found to make less body contact with their babies, stimulation and structure, with negative
spend less time face-to-face with their infants, consequences for development. On this note,
smile at their infants less, touch them less and talk parents of mentally retarded children who had
to them less (Goldberg, 1978). Given the difficulty doubts about their parenting abilities and of their
these parents may have understanding their capacity to adapt to their children’s condition
infant‘s cues, they may be more prone to develop reported more child-rearing problems than did
feelings that they are less competent parents. In a parents who expressed confidence in their
review of studies of parents’ interactions with parenting skills (Friedrich et al., 1985).
developmentally disabled and at-risk children,
Mitchell (1987) proposed explanations for why
some of these parent-child interactions tend to be PARENTAL SELF-EF’FICACY AND THE
dysfunctional. He notes that not only is it important SOCIAL CONTEXT
to consider the role of the plwnature infant in
interactions, but also the role of the ’premature Importantly, children of depressed mothers and
parent’ (Als ef al., 1979, as cited in Mitchell, 1987). children with a given special need experience
Parents may not be ready to deal with a special variable developmental outcomes (Conrad and
needs child, and may lack knowledge about how to Hammen, 1993; Goldberg et al., 1986; Radke-
read the child’s signals. Mitchell also suggests that Yarrow and Brown, 1993; Teti et al., 1990).Reasons
difficulties arise when parents must be separated for these individual differences are complex.
from the infant during the initial hospitalization Formulations addressing individual differences
period; hence, opportunitiesfor developingoptimal among children of depressed parents, which
interactionalstrategies may be quite limited. These usually target socioemotional developmental
interactional difficulties may foster feelings of outcomes more than cognitive outcomes, centre
parental inefficacy and helplessness. Mitchell around risk factors that increase children’s
furtherproposes that interactional difficulties may vulnerability to psychopathology (e.g.

Early Dev. Parent. 5: 237-250 (1996) 01996 by JohnWiley & Sons, Ltd.
Parental Self-Eficacy 243

'constitutional ' factors such as low intelligence) or as moderators of relations between parent-child
resilience factors that buffer children against the risks and parenting efficacy.
negative effects of inadequate caregiving
environments (e.g. social competence with peers) Infant Temperament
(Cicchetti and Aber, 1986; Conrad and Hammen,
1993; Garmezy, 1987; Rutter, 1987). Individual Although variously conceptualized, temperament
differences among special needs children have in infancy is generally believed to be genetically
(interestingly) targeted cognitive developmental based and can be operationally defined by such
outcomes more often than socioemotional dimensions as emotionality, activity, predictability
outcomes, with differences in socioeconomic of daily cycles and rhythms, adaptability to novel
status and severity of children's illness or environmental stimuli and sociability (Bates et al.,
condition most often cited as explanations (Blair 1979; Buss and Plomin, 1975; Carey and McDevitt,
and Ramey, 1997; Pearl, 1993). However, the direct 1978; Goldsmith et al., 1987; Rothbart, 1981;
link between parental self-efficacy and parental Thomas and Chess, 1977). A fussy, active, slow-
competence (Teti and Gelfand, 1991)suggests that to-adapt, unpredictable baby may be challenging
variation in the long-term outcomesof children who for any mother, who might then develop negative
are at risk, due to parental psychopathology, perceptions of the baby and of herself as a parent.
medical fragility or developmental delay, is an As discussed earlier, Goldberg (1977) has argued
important function of individual differences in that mothers' self-appraisalsof their effectivenessin
parental appraisals of themselves and their the caregivingrole (i.e. maternal feelings of efficacy)
children, in keeping with a model of cognitive should be fostered by infants who are easy to
mediation. manage and predictable. Conversely, self-
Thus far we have developed a working model of appraisals of ineffectiveness would be expected if
relations in which parental feelings of efficacy are mothers are faced with babies who are chronically
predictable from knowledge of risk factors fussy, difficult to read and unpredictable. That
inherent in the parent (e.g. parental infant temperamental difficulty can and should be
psychopathology, problematic developmental considered a 'main effect' determinant of parental
history) and in the child (e.g. prematurity/low self-efficacy is evidenced by findings from several
birthweight, developmental delay), and where sources emphasizing infant temperament's
parental self-efficacy in turn should be the 'final negative relation to maternal self-efficacy (Cutrona
common pathway' in the prediction of parental and Troutman, 1986; Davis, 1990; Deutsch e f aL,
sensitivity. Parenting efficacy, however, is 1988; Gross and Conrad, 1992; Teti and Gelfand,
complexly determined, and in addition to 1991). Temperamentally difficult babies, by
parent-child risks we now in particular consider definition, are those who are difficult to soothe,
the role of two (of potentially many) aspects of less 'readable' and signal poorly, and thus these
parents' social context in the prediction of findings lend support to Goldberg's (1977) and to
parenting efficacy. The first of these is infant Bandura's (1982, 1986) formulations, which view
temperamental difficulty, which has alternatively self-efficacy beliefs as based on performance
been conceptualized as a risk factor inherent in the attainments. Teti and Gelfand (1991) propose that
child (e.g. Belsky, 1984) but which we discuss as a maternal self-efficacyis crucial to an understanding
social-contextual risk because (a) it is not of the construct of 'goodness-of-fit' between parent
necessarily associated with child health status and child characteristics(Thomas and Chess, 1977).
(Gennaro e f al., 1992), and (b) as a construct that Indeed, relations between infant temperamental
is measured predominantly from parental report, difficulty and maternal responsivity are incon-
it functions as an important psychosocial stressor sistent, with some studies showing increases,
in the context of parent-child relations. The second decreases or no effect of infant temperament on
is the quality of social-marital supports, which maternal behaviour (Crockenberg, 1986). One
should affect parental self-efficacy via the explanation for these inconsistencies is that
influences of social persuasion and modelling difficult infant temperament may impact the
(Bandura, 1986), and via the provision of parenting behaviour of different mothers
encouragement, emotional support and respite differently, depending on mothers' initial levels
care. As we discuss, it is theoretically plausible to of selfefficacy. A woman with a difficult baby but
consider these two predictors alternatively as with high levels of maternal self-efficacy would be
'main effect' determinants of parental efficacy or expected to try harder, to make better use of her

81996 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Early Dev. Parent. 5: 237-250 (1996)
244 D. M . Teti, M . A. O‘Connell and C. D. Reiner

personal and social resources to achieve a of special needs children, in particular premature/
successful, harmonious relationship with her low birthweight children, are more difficult than
infant, than would a woman with an equally those of low-risk children (Goldberg, 1978;
difficult baby but with low maternal self-efficacy. MedoffCooper, 1986), which in turn can be
The first woman might view her baby as ‘feisty, exacerbated by medical complications at birth
vigorous and full of life’; the second might view and during the early postnatal period (Field et
her baby as ‘noisy, tiresome and difficult’. al., 1978). Premature infants are often less
In addition to conceptualizing infant responsive to social stimulation and less alert
temperamental difficulty as a ‘main effect‘ than full-term infants (Bakeman and Brown, 1980;
determinant of parenting efficacy, however, Teti Cmic et al., 1983c; Field, 1977; Honig, 1984), and
and Gelfand (1991) discuss the putative role of their cries have been found to be risk predictors for
infant temperament as a moderator of relations developmental disabilities (Honig, 1984).
between parental depression and self-efficacy. However, other studies comparing the
Depressed mothers report lower levels of self- temperaments of 4-to-8-month-old preterm vs
efficacy than do non-depressed mothers (Fox and full-term infants found no group differences
Gelfand, 1994; Gross and Rocissano, 1988; Teti and when using parent report (Oberklaid et al.,
Gelfand, 1991), and thus depressed mothers as a 1986b). Further, Gennaro et al. (1992) reported
group are more vulnerable to perceiving a that differences in temperamental difficulty
temperamentally difficult baby negatively and to among medically fragile and full-term infants
developing performance deficits in mothering. The could not be accounted for from knowledge of
combination of maternal depression and infant infants’ medical risk status.
difficulty may create problems for parent-hild The importance of taking stock of children‘s
relations and child development that exceed that temperaments, and their putative impact on
which would be predictable from a purely additive, parenting efficacy independently of children’s
‘main effect’ model of relations. In support of this, special needs status, is underscored by these
Teti and Gelfand (1991) reported in multiple data. Caring for a special needs child in and of
regression analyses that severity of maternal itself may create parental feelings of uncertainty
depressive symptoms (mothers’ BDI scores) and and helplessness. The social cues of the premature
perceptions of infant difficulty scores interacted in infant, for example, can be unpredictable, vague
predicting mothers‘ feelings of efficacy. Maternal and age-inappropriate, creating special challenges
self-efficacy was highest (predicted mean=29.38) for parents and negative consequences for self-
among mothers whose depressive symptoms were appraisals of parenting. When this child is also
relatively mild and who saw their infants as temperamentally difficult, the negative effects on
temperamentally easy. Intermediate self-efficacy parenting efficacy and sensitivity may be
scores were obtained from mothers who reported profound, and perhaps to a degree greater than
high levels of depressive symptoms but who that predictable from a purely additive model
perceived their infants as easy (predicted alone. Whether these relations conform to an
mean=20.63), and from mothers who reported additive model or a model in which
low levels of depressive symptoms but who saw temperament is conceived as a moderator of
their infants as difficult (predicted mean=26.28).By relations between children’s special needs status
contrast, maternal self-efficacy was lowest and parenting efficacy is an issue worthy of
(predicted mean=13.01), and lower than would exploration.
have been expected from a purely additive model
of relations among these variables, in mothers who
reported high levels of depressive symptomatology Social-Marital Supports
and who perceived their infants to be difficult.
Thus, the ‘double jeopardy’ combination of The quality of instrumental and emotional support
moderate-severe depression and a perceived experienced by parents represents a second social-
difficult baby may place mothers at substantially contextual influence on mother-child relations in
higher risk for parenting disturbances with their infancy and early childhood. Mothers with low
babies than the presence of either risk factor alone. social support and with unhappy marriages
The relation between temperament and children behave less optimally with their infants than do
with special needs is equivocal. On the one hand, mothers with adequate support networks and
there is evidence to suggest that the temperaments happy marriages (Cox et al., 1989; Crnic et al.,

Early Dev. Parent. 5 237-250 (1996) 01996 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Parenfal Se5f-Eficacy 245

1983b; Crockenberg and McCluskey, 1986; medical status, the parent’s emotional state,
Goldberg and Easterbrooks, 1984). potential problem areas in the parent-child
Like infant temperament, social-marital supports relationship and developmental outcomes.
should affect maternal functioning via their impact Psycho-educational interventions with the goal of
on maternal percepts and feelings. Self-efficacy fostering parental self-efficacy and parenting
theory (Bandura, 1982,1986) suggests that support sensitivity should make every effort to include
from intimates will directly affect mothers’ self- both parents, and not just mothers, when working
efficacy via modelling influences (e.g. learning with two-parent households, and/or to include
about successful child care routines by watching other intimate support figures who are significant
competent relatives and friends) and positive to mothers (e.g. the baby’s grandmother) if working
consequences in the form of encouragement and with single-parent households. In the context of
praise. This position receives support from several parental depression, intimate support figures‘
studies that have reported positive relations awareness of the debilitating nature of dep-
between quality of social-marital supports and ression, its association with marital discord and
mothers’ self-efficacy (Davis, 1990; Donovan and difficulties in child care, and its responsiveness to
Leavitt, 1989; Teti and Gelfand, 1991).However, we drug therapy, psychotherapy and social support
also view social-marital supports as a potentially from intimates should enable support figures to
important moderator of relations between parent- be more successful at ’objectifying’ parental
child risks and parenting efficacy. In an important depression and to support the depressed parent,
study, Crockenberg (1981) found that young both instrumentally and emotionally, in seeking
temperamentally difficult infants were sig- treatment for her/his illness and in daily activities,
nificantly more likely to become securely attached including child care (Beardslee et al., 1992; Gelfand
to their mothers when their mothers’ levels of social and Teti, 1990). Similarly, interventions targeting
support were high than when they were low. It is parents of special needs children should aim to
reasonable to expect that mothers prone to educate intimate support figures as well as the
depression but whose marriages and support primary caregiver about a child’s specific
networks are adequate would tend to have more condition, prognosis, difficulties to be expected
positive perceptions of themselvesand their infants, in child care and individual child strengths, which
and would be more behaviourally competent with in turn should assist support figures in their ability
their infants than would depression-pronemothers to provide parents with more concrete and useful
with inadequate supports. Similarly, it is plausible assistance. In both cases, the appropriate
that social-marital supports may moderate involvement of support figures is expected to
relations between children’s special needs status foster parents’ sense of well-being with positive
and parenting efficacy, reducing this association consequences being feelings of parental efficacy
when social-marital supports are high and/or and effective parenting.
increasing it when low. Again, we believe these Interestingly, the specific role of professional
moderator relations can and should be empirically supports in fostering parenting efficacy and
tested. parenting sensitivity has received comparatively
little systematic attention. A recently completed
home-based intervention study of clinically
IMPLICATIONS FOR INTERVENTION depressed mothers of infants, however,
emphasizes the role of professionals as both
Parenting cognitions, and in particular parental educators and support figures in the lives of
self-efficacy, should be important targets for depressed women and their children (Gelfand ef
intervention, and thus professionals should al., 1996). The intervention aimed to improve the
become aware of the parental self-efficacy beliefs quality of depressed mother-infant relationships
as they are shaped by parental well-being, and was delivered weekly over a 9-12-month
children’s special needs status and the social period by trained, registered public health nurses
context. Professionals such as paediatricians or beginning when infants were in their first year of
psychologists to whom parents have ready access life. This intervention was designed from a social-
during the initial transition to parenthood or during cognitive, psycho-educational theoretical frame-
a depressive episode are in a position to assist work, and it was not uncommon for intervention
parents and their intimate support figures through mothers to remark that they looked forward to the
the provision of information about the child’s nurses’ visits because of both the information and

61996 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Early Dev. Parent. 5: 237-250 (1996)
246 D.M. Teti, M. A. O’Connell and C.D.Reiner

the support that the nurses provided. Relative to efficacy) than did control parents, and their
depressed control mothers, intervention mothers children had higher intellectual development
showed a significant decrease in severity of scores than did control infants at 3, 4, 7 and 9
depressive symptoms and daily hassles and their years of age (no findings regarding mother-child
children were less likely to develop avoidant/ interaction were reported).
defended attachments. Reductions in depressive Direct evidence for improved parenting as a
symptoms in the full sample in turn were associated result of professional support comes from a study
with increases in mothers’ self-efficacy beliefs, by Affleck et al. (1989), who randomly assigned
mothers’ responsivity towards their children and mothers who had infants in the NICU to a
children’s interest in and involvement with their professional support intervention or to a control
mothers, and decreases in parenting stress. condition. The intervention was based on a
The role of professional supports in shaping consultation model in which nurses visited
parental perceptions of and behaviour towards mothers in the home to help these mothers make
their children may be especially relevant for the transition from hospital-to-home care after
parents of special needs children because of the infant discharge. The consultation model was
singularly important role that obstetricians, distinct from parent training models or infant
neonatologists, paediatricians, psychologists, cumculum models in that the mothers directed
nurses and social workers can play in providing which issues about their child or about parenting
crucial information to parents about children’s were to be emphasized. Results indicated that 6
medical condition, prognosis and potential impact months after infant discharge from the NICU,
on the parenting subsystem. It seems reasonable mothers who expressed a high need for support
to expect that professional supports may be prior to interventionexperienced improved feelings
especially important to parental feelings of of personal control over child health and
efficacy during the initial stages of adjustment to developmental outcomes, increased feelings of
a special needs child, and perhaps even more parenting competence, and were rated highly by
critical in the early stages of parenting than the nurse consultant as being sensitive and
quality of support provided by intimates. Cmic responsive to infant cues. However, the
et al. (1986) found that satisfaction with intervention appeared to worsen the adjustment
professional support was a better predictor of among mothers who expressed a low need for
satisfaction with parenting than support from support.
intimates, friends and the community 1 month Affleck et al. (1989) proposed that mothers who
after infant discharge from the NICU. Satisfaction expressed a high need for support were those who
with professional support also moderated the had difficulty obtaining support from family
relation between stress and satisfaction with members and friends, and thus were more likely
parenting and was related to more positive to benefit from professional support than were ’low
maternal affect during mother-infant interaction. need‘ mothers. ‘High need‘ mothers actively
The importance of professional supports is again requested information, advice, instruction and
highlighted in an important psycho-educational reassurance about their child and about
intervention study designed to foster development themselves as parents from the nurse consultants,
among low-birthweight infants (Achenbach et al., in contrast to ‘low need‘ mothers. ’Low need‘
1990, 1993; Rauh et al., 1988). The intervention mothers, by contrast, may have been given
involved 11 sessions, the first seven of which took information about their babies that the mothers
place during infants’ final week in the NICU and did not actively seek out or want. In turn,this may
the last four of which were delivered at home over have drawn ’low need‘ mothers’ attention to
the first 3 months of life. During these sessions, a problems about which they initially had less
trained paediatric nurse provided information to concern. This may have disrupted any favourable
parents (typically mothers, although fathers were attitudes the ‘low need’ mothers may have had
included if available) about children’s initially about their children’s condition and
developmental status and individual infant impaired their adjustment to parenting.
strengths in domains of autonomic and motor These studies attest to the power of ongoing,
functioning,state regulation and social interaction accurate, sensitive and appropriately tailored input
and play. Intervention parents reported higher from professionals. Such input can assist stressed,
scores on measures of parenting confidence and beleagured parents in developing a clearer and
satisfaction (constructs closely related to parenting more objective understanding of their infants (and

Early Dev. Parent. 5 237-250 (1996) 01996 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Paren fa1 Self Efficacy 247

of themselves, in terms of parent risks) and foster Beardslee, W. R., Hoke, L., Wheelock, I., Rothberg, P. C.,
feelings of parental efficacy,parental sensitivityand van de Velde, P. and Swatling, S. (1992). Initial
findings on preventive intervention for families with
children’s cognitive and social development over parental affective disorders. American Journal of
the long term. Psychiatry, 149, 1335-1340.
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J. and
Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring
depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561-571.
CONCLUSIONS Beckman, P. J. (1991). Comparison of mothers’ and
fathers’ perceptions of the effect of young children
In this paper we advanced the premise that the with and without disabilities. American Journal on
impact of parent-childand social-contextualfactors Mental Retardation, 95(5), 585-595.
on parental sensitivity is mediated by parental Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: a
feelings of efficacy, which serve as the ’final process model. Child Development, 55, 83-95.
common pathway’ in the prediction of parental Bennett, D. E. and Slade, P. (1991). Infants born at risk
sensitivity. Although the construct of self-efficacyin consequences for maternal post-partum adjustment.
British Journal of Medical Psychology, 64, 159-172.
clinical experimental research is well established Blair, C. and Ramey, C. T. (1997). Early intervention for
(Bandura, 1986), its importance as a determinant of low-birth-weight infants and the path to second-
parenting behaviour, and as a primary target of generation research. In M. J. Guralnick (Ed.), The
intervention in its own right, is still evolving.This is Effectiveness of Early Intervention. Baltimore: Paul H.
a positive trend and one that we hope continues. Brookes, pp. 77-97.
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American
Psychologist, 2, 129-148.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and Loss. Vol.2. Separation.
REFERENCES New York: Basic Books.
Bretherton, I. (1985). Attachment theory: retrospect and
Achenbach, T. M., Howell, C. T., Aoki, M. F. and Rauh, prospect. In I. Bretherton and E. Waters (Eds), Growing
V. A. (1993). Nine-year outcome of the Vermont Points of Attachment Theory and Research. Monographs of
Intervention Program for low birth weight infants. the Society for Research in Child Development, 50 (nos. 1-
Pediatrics, 91, 45-55. 2, Serial No. 209), 167-193.
Achenbach, T. M., Phares, V., Howell, C. T., Rauh, V. A. Bristol, M. M., Gallagher, J. J. and Schopler, E. (1988).
and Nurcombe, B. (1990). Seven-year outcome of the Mothers and fathers of young developmentally
Vermont Intervention Program for low-birthweight disabled and nondisabled boys: adaptation and
infants. Child Development, 61, 1672-1681. spousal support. Developmental Psychology 24(3),
Affleck, G., Tennen, H., Rowe, J., Roscher, B. and 441451.
Walker, L. (1989). Effects of formal support on Buss, A. H. and Plomin, R. (1975). A Temperament Theory
mothers’ adaptation to the hospital-to-home of Personality. New York Wiley.
transition of high-risk infants: the benefits and costs Carey, P. H. and McDevitt, S. C. (1978). Revision of the
of helping. Child Development, 60, 488-501. Infant Temperament Questionnaire. Pediatrics 61,
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E. and Wall, 735-739.
S. (1978). Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of Cicchetti, D. and Aber, L. (1986). Early precursors of
the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. later depression: an organizational perspective. In L.
Bakeman, R. and Brown, J. V. (1980). Early interaction: Lipsitt and C. Rovee-Collier (Eds), Advances in Infancy,
consequences for social and mental development at vol. 4. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Press, pp. 87-137.
three years. Child Development, 51, 437-447. Clark, D. M. and Teasdale, J. D. (1982). Diurnal variation
Bakermans-Kranenburgh,M. J. and Van Ijzendoorn,M. H. in clinical depression and accessibility of memories of
(1993). A psychometric study of the Adult Attachment positive and negative experiences. Iournal of Abnormal
Interview: reliability and discriminant validity. Psychology, 91, 87-95.
Developmental Psychology,29,870-879. Conrad, M. and Hammen, C. (1993). Protective and
Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanisms in human resource factors in high- and low-risk children: a
agency. American Psychologist, 37, 122-147. comparison of children with unipolar, bipolar,
Bandura, A. (1986). Social Foundations of Thought and medically ill, and normal mothers. Developmental and
Action: A Social-Cognitive Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Psychopathology, 5, 593-608.
Prentice-Hall. Cox, M. J., Owen, M. T., Lewis, J. M. and Henderson,
Bandura, A. and Wood, R. (1989). Effect of perceived V. K. (1989). Marriages, adult adjustment, and early
controllability and performance standards on self- parenting. Child Development, 60, 1015-1024.
regulation of complex decision making. Journal of Crittenden, P. (1992). Attachment in the preschool years.
Personality and Social Psychology, 56,805-814. Development and Psychopathology, 4, 209-241.
Bates, J. E., Freeland, C. A. and Lounsbury, M. L. (1979). Crnic, K. A., Friedrich, W. N. and Greenberg, M. T.
Measurement of infant difficultness. Child (1983a).Adaptation of families with mentally retarded
Development, SO, 794-803. children: a model of stress, coping, and family

01% by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Early Dev. Parent. 5: 237-250 (1996)
248 D. M. Teti, M. A. O’Connell and C. D. Reiner

ecology. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 88(2), reactions to children of three age levels. Child
125-138. Development, 57,879494.
Crnic, K. A., Greenberg, M. T., Ragozin, A. S., Robinson, Donovan, W. L. and Leavitt, L. A. (1989). Maternal self-
N. M. and Basham, R. (1983b). Effects of stress and efficacy and infant attachment: integrating physiology,
social support on mothers and premature and full- perceptions, and behavior. Child Development, 60,
term infants. Child Development, 54, 209-217. 460-472.
Cmic, K. A., Greenberg, M. T. and Slough, N. (1986). Downey, G. and Coyne, J. C. (1990). Children of
Early stress and social support influences on mothers’ depressed parents: an integrative review.
and high-risk infants’ functioning in late infancy. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 50-76.
Infant Mental Health Journal, 7(1), 19-33. Emmerich, W. (1969). The parental role: a functional-
Crnic, K. A., Ragozin, A. S., Greenberg, M. T., Robinson, cognitive approach. Monographs of the Society for
N. M. and Basham, R. B. (1983~).Social interaction and Research in Child Development, 34 (No.8, Serial
developmental competence of preterm and full-term NO.132), 1-71.
infants during the first year of life. Child Development, Fergusson, D. M., Honvood, L. J., Gretten, M. E. and
54,1199-1210. Shannon, F. T. (1985). Family life events, maternal
Crockenberg, S. B. (1981). Infant irritability, mother depression, and maternal and teacher descriptions of
responsiveness, and social support influences on the child behavior. Pediatrics, 75, 30-35.
security of infant-mother attachment. Child Field, T. M. (1977). Effects of early separation, interactive
Development, 52,857-865. deficits, and experimental manipulation of mother-
Crockenberg, S. B. (1986).Are temperamental differences infant face-to-face interaction. Child Development, 48,
in babies associated with predictable differences in 763-771.
caregiving? In J. V. Lerner and R. H. Lerner (Eds), Field, T. M. (1984). Early interactions between infants
Temperament and Social Interaction in Infants and Children. and their postpartum depressed mothers. Infant
San Francisco: Jossey-Bass,pp. 53-73. Behavior and Development, 7,517-522.
Crockenberg, S. and McCIuskey, K. (1986). Changes in Field, T., Hallock, N., Ting, G., Dempsey, J., Dabiri, C.
maternal behavior during the baby’s first year of life. and Shuman, H. H. (1978). A first-year follow-up of
Child Development, 57, 746-753. high-risk infants: formulating a cumulative risk index.
Cummings, E. M. and Davies, P. T. (1994). Maternal Child Development, 49, 119-131.
depression and child development. Journal of Child Forehand, R., Lautenschlager, G. J., Faust, J. and Graziano,
Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 73-112. W. G. (1986). Parent perceptions and parent-child
Cutrona, C. E. and Troutman, B. R. (1986). Social interactions in clinic-referred children: a preliminary
support, infant temperament, and parenting self- investigation of the effects of maternal depressive
efficacy: a mediational model of postpartum moods. Behavior Research and Therapy, 24,73-75.
depression. Child Development, 57, 1507-1518. Fox, C. R. and Gelfand, D. M. (1994). Maternal depressed
Davis, A. L. (1990). Clarifying the role of maternal self- mood and stress as related to vigilance, self-efficacy,
confidence in maternal interactional competence. and mother-child interactions. Early Development and
Paper presented at the 7th International Conference Parenting, 3, 233-243.
on Infant Studies, Montreal, CA, April. Friedlander, S., Weiss, D. S . and Traylor, J. (1986).
Deutsch, F. M., Brooks-Gunn, J., Fleming, A., Ruble, Assessing the influence of maternal depression on the
D. N. and Stangor, C. (1988). Information-seeking and validity of the Child Behavior Checklist. Journal of
maternal self-definition during the transition to Abnormal Child Psychology, 14, 123-133.
motherhood. Journal of Personality and Social Friedrich, W. N. and Friedrich, W. L. (1981).
Psychology, 55,420-431. Psychosocial assets of parents of handicapped and
Dix, T. (1991). The affective organization of parenting: nonhandicapped children. American Journal of Mental
adaptive and maladaptive processes. Psychological Deficiency, 85(5), 551-553.
Bulletin, 110, 3-25. Friedrich, W. N., Wilturner, L. T. and Cohen, D. S. (1985).
Dix, T. and Grusec, J. (1983). Parental influence Coping resources and parenting mentally retarded
techniques: an attributional analysis. Child children. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 90(2),
Development, 54,645-652. 130-139.
Dix, T. and Lodunan, J. (1990). Social cognition and Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P. and ter Schure, E. (1989).
negative reactions to children: a comparison of Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional
mothers of aggressive and nonaggressive boys. action readiness. Journal of Personality and Social
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9, 418438. Psychology, 57,212-228.
Dix, T. and Reinhold, D. P. (1991). Chronic and Garmezy, N. (1987). Stress, competence, and
temporary influences on mothers’ attributions development: continuities in the study of
for disobedience. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 37, schizophrenic adults, children vulnerable to
251-271. psychopathology, and the search for stress-resistant
Dix, T., Reinhold, D. P. and Zambarino, R. J. (1990). children. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 57,
Mothers’ judgment in moments of anger. Merrill 159-174.
Palmer Quarterly, 36, 465-486. Gelfand, D. M. and Teti, D. M. (1990). The effects of
Dix, T., Ruble, D. N., Grusec, J. E. and Nixon, S. (1986). maternal depression on children. Ctinical Psychology
Social cognition in parents: inferential and affective Review, 10,329-353.

Early Dev. Parent. 5: 237-250 (1996) 01996 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Parental Self-Eficacy 249

Gelfand, D. M., Teti, D. M., Seiner, S. A. and Jameson, Jeffcoate, J. A., Humphrey, M. E. and Lloyd, J. K. (1979).
P. B. (1996). Helping mothers fight depression: Role perception and response to stress in fathers and
evaluation of a home-based intervention program for mothers following pre-term delivery. Sociaf Science 6
depressed mothers and their infants. Journal of Clinical Medicine 13A, 139-145.
Child Psychology, 25, 406-422. Kavanagh, D. J. and Bower, G. H. (1985). Mood and seif-
Gennaro, S., Medoff-Cooper, B. and Lotas, M. (1992). efficacy: impact of joy and sadness on perceived
Perinatal factors and infant temperament: a capabilities. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 9,
collaborative approach. Nursing Research, 41, 375-377. 507-525.
Goldberg, S. (1977). Social competence in infancy: a Locke, H. and Wallace, K. (1959). Short marital-
model of parent-infant interaction. Merrill Palmer adjustment and prediction tests: their reliability and
Quarterly, 23(3), 163-177. validity. Marriage and Family Living, 21, 251-255.
Goldberg, S. (1978). Prematurity: effects on parent-infant Maccoby, E. E. and Martin, J. P. (1983). Socialization in
interaction. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 3(3), 137-144. the context of the family: parent-child interaction. In
Goldberg, W. and Easterbrooks, M. A. (1984). Role of E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), P. H. Mussen (Series Ed.),
marital quality in toddler development. Developmental Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol.4. Socialization,
Psychology, 10, 504-514. Personality, and Social Development. New York Wiley,
Goldberg, S., Marcovitch, S., MacGregor, D. and pp. 1-101.
Lojkasek, M. (1986). Family responses to Main, M., Kaplan, N. and Cassidy, J. C. (1985).
developmentally delayed preschoolers: etiology and Security in infancy, childhood and adulthood: a
the father’s role. American journal of Mental Deficiency, move to the level of representation. In I. Bretherton
90(6), 610-617. and E. Waters (Eds), Growing Points of Attachment
Goldsmith, H. H., Buss, A. H., Plomin, R., Rothbart, Theory and Research; Monographs of the Society for
M. K., Thomas, A., Chess, S., Hinde, R. A. and McCall, Research in Child Development, 50 (Nos. 1-2, Serial
R. B. (1987). Roundtable: what is temperament? Four No. 209), 66-104.
approaches. Child Development, 58, 505529. Mash, E. J. and Johnston, C. (1990). Determinants of
Goodman, S. H. (1992). Understanding the effects of parenting stress: illustrations from families of
depressed mothers on their children. In E. F. Walker, hyperactive children and families of physically
R. H. Dworkin and 8. A. Cornblatt (Eds), Progress in abused children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology,
Experimental Personality and Psychopathology Research. 19(4), 313-328.
New York: Springer, pp. 47-109. McGettigan, M. C., Greenspan, J. S., Antunes, M. J.,
Goodnow, J. J. (1988). Parents’ ideas, actions, and Greenspan, D. I. and Rubenstein, S. D. (1994).
feelings: models and methods from developmental Psychological aspects of parenting critically ill
and social psychology. Child Development, 59, 286-320. neonates. Clinical Pediatrics, 33(2), 77-82.
Griest, D. L., Wells, K. C. and Forehand, R. (1979). An Medoff-Cooper, B. (1986). Temperament in very low
examination of predictors of maternal perceptions of birth weight infants. Nursing Research, 35(3),
maladjustment in clinic-referred children. Journal of 139-143.
Abnormal Psychology, 88, 277-281. Miles, M. S, Funk, S. G. and Kasper, M. A. (1992).
Gross, D. and Conrad, B. (1992). A longitudinal model of The stress response of mothers and fathers of
maternal confidence and maternal mental health preterm infants. Research in Nursing 6 Health, 15,
during toddlerhood. Paper presented at the 8th 261-269.
International Conference on Infant Studies, Miami, Miller, A. C., Gordon, R. M., Daniele, R. J. and Diller, L.
FL, May. (1992). Stress, appraisal, and coping in mothers of
Gross, D. and Rocissano, L. (1988). Maternal confidence disabled and nondisabled children. Special Issue:
in toddlerhood: its measurement for clinical practice The0ry-Driven Research in Pediatric Psychology: 1.
and research. Nurse Practitioner, 13, 19-29. Journal of Pediatric Psychofogy, 17(5), 587-605.
Hanson, M. J. and Hanline, M. F. (1990). Parenting a Miller, S. A. (1988). Parents’ beliefs about children’s
child with a disability: a longitudinal study of parental cognitive development. Child Dmelopment, 59,259-285.
stress and adaptation. Journal of Early Intervention, Mitchell, D. R. (1987). Parents’ interactions with their
14(3), 234-248. developmentally disabled or at-risk infants: a focus for
Henderson, S., Byme, D. G. and Duncan-Jones, P. (1981). intervention. Australia and New Zealand Journal of
Neurosis and the Social Environment. Sydney: Academic Developmental Disabilities, 13(2), 73-81.
Press Australia. Murray, L. (1992). The impact of postnatal depression on
Honig, A. S. (1984). Risk factors in infants and young infant development. Iournal of Child Psychology and
children. Young Children, 39(4), 60-73. Psychiatry, 33, 543-561.
Innocenti, M. S., Huh, K. and Boyce, G. C. (1992). Newman, C. and Goldfried, M. R. (1987). Disabusing
Families of children with disabilities: normative data negative self-efficacy expectations via experiences,
and other considerations on parenting stress. Topics in feedback, and discrediting. Cognitive Therapy and
Early Childhood Special Education, 12(3), 403427. Research, 11, 401417.
Isen, A. M. (1984). Toward understanding the role of Oberklaid, F., Prior, M., Nolan, T., Smith, P. and Flavell,
affect in cognition. In R. Wyer and T. Srull (Eds), H. (1986a). Temperament in infants born prematurely.
Handbook of Social Cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, Annual Progress in Child Psychiatry and Child
pp. 179-236. Development , 386-396.

01996 by John Wiley & Sons,Ltd. Early Dev. Parent. 5: 237-250 (1996)
250 D. M. Teti, M. A. O’Connell and C. D. Reiner

Oberklaid, F., Prior, M. and Sanson, A. (1986b). developmental delay/mental retardation. Nursing
Temperament of preterm versus full-term infants. Research, 44(1), 38-44.
Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 7(3), 159-162. Sigel, I. E. (1985) (Ed.). Parental Belief Systems. Hillsdale,
Panaccione, V. F. and Wahler, R. G. (1986). Child NJ: Erlbaum.
behavior, maternal depression, and social coercion as Solnit, A. J. and Stark, M. H. (1961). Mourning and the
factors in the quality of child care. Journal of Abnormal birth of a defective child. Psychoanalytic Study of the
Child Psychology, 14, 263-278. Child, 16, 523-537.
Pearl, L. F. (1993). Developmental follow-up of at-risk Spanier, G. B. (1976).Measuring dyadic adjustment: new
infants. In W. Brown, S. Kenneth Thurman and L. F. scales for assessing the quality of marriage and similar
Pearl (Eds), Family-Centered Early Intervention with dyads. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 38,15-28.
Znfants and Toddlers. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes, Sroufe, L. A. (1983). Infant-caregiver attachment and
pp. 211-243. patterns of adaptation in preschool: the roots of
Pederson, D. R., Bento, S., Chance, G. W., Evans, B. and maladaptation and competence. In M. Perlmutter
Fox, M. (1987). Maternal Emotional Responses to (Ed.), Minnesota Symposium in Child Psychology,
Preterm Birth. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry Vol. 16. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 41-81.
57(1), 15-21. Sroufe, L. A. and Fleeson, J. (1986). Attachment and the
Radke-Yarrow, M. and Brown, E. (1993). Resilience and construction of relationships. In W. Hartup and Z.
vulnerability in children of multiple-risk families. Rubin (Eds), The Nature and Development of
Development and Psychopathology, 5,581-592. Relationships. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 51-71.
Radke-Yarrow, M., Cummings, E. M., Kuczynski, L. Teasdale, J. D. and Fogarty, S. J. (1979). Differential
and Chapman, M. (1985). Patterns of attachment in effects of induced mood on retrieval of pleasant and
two- and three-year-olds in normal families and unpleasant events from episodic memory. Journal of
families with parental depression. Child Development, Abnormal Psychology, 88,248-257.
56, 884-893. Teti, D. M. and Gelfand, D. M. (1991). Behavioral
Rauh, V. A., Achenbach, T. M., Nurcombe, B., Howell, competence among mothers of infants in the first
C. T. and Teti, D. M. (1988). Minimizing adverse year: the mediational role of maternal self-efficacy.
effects of low birthweight: four-year results of an early Child Development, 62, 918-929.
intervention program. Child Development, 59, 544-553. Teti, D. M., Gelfand, D. M., Messinger, D. S. and Isabella,
Rickard, K. M., Forehand, R., Wells, K. C., Griest, D. L. R. (1995).Maternal depression and the quality of early
and McMahon, R. J. (1981). Factors in the referral of attachment: an examination of infants, preschoolers,
children for behavioral treatment: a comparison of and their mothers. Developmental Psychology, 31,
mothers of clinic-referred deviant, clinic-referred non- 364-376.
deviant and non-clinic children. Behaviour Research and Teti, D. M., Gelfand, D. M. and Pompa, J. (1990).
Therapy, 19,201-205. Depressed mothers’ behavioral competence with
Rodrigue, J. R., Morgan, S. B. and Geffken, G. (1990). their infants: demographic and psychosocial
Families of autistic children: psychological functioning correlates. Development and Psychopathology, 2,259-270.
of mothers. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 19(4), Thomas, A. and Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and
371-379. Development. New York Brunner/Mazel.
Rogers, T. R. and Forehand, R. (1983). The role of parent Thomas, A., Chess, S. and Birch, H. (1970). The origin of
depression in interactions between mothers and their personality. Scientipc American, 223, 102-109.
clinic-referred children. Cognitive Therapy and Research, Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1992). Intergenerational
7,315324. transmission of parenting: other-child interactions:
Roman, L. A., Lindsay, J. K., Boger, R. P., DeWys, M., an interbehavioral model. Psychological Bulletin, 105,
Beaumont, E. J., Jones, A. S. and Haas, B. (1995). 116-130.
Parent-to-parent support initiated in the neonatal Webster-Stratton, C. and Hammond, M. (1988).Maternal
intensive care unit. Research in Nursing 6 Health, 18, depression and its relationship to life stress,
385-394. perceptions of child behavior problems, parenting
Rothbart, M. K. (1981).Measurement of temperament in behaviors, and child conduct problems. Journal of
infancy. Child Development, 52, 569-578. Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 299-315.
Rutter, M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience and protective Westen, D. (1991). Social cognition and object relations.
mechanisms. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 57, Psychological Bulletin, 109, 429455.
316-331. Zahn-Waxler, C., Cummings, E. M., Ianotti, R. J. and
Sameroff, A. J. (1975). Early influences on development: Radke-Yarrow, M. (1984). Altruism, aggression, and
fact or fancy? Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 21, 3-33. social interactions in young children with a manic-
Schaughency, E. A. and Lahey, B. B. (1985). Mothers’ and depressive parent. Child Development, 55,
fathers’ perceptions of child deviance: roles of child 112-122.
behavior, parental depression, and marital Zoll, D. A., Lyons-Ruth, K. and Connell, D. (1984).
satisfaction. journal of Consulting and Clinical Infants at psychiatric risk maternal behavior,
Psychology, 53,718-723. depression, and family history. Paper presented at
Seideman, R. Y. and Klein, P. F. (1995). A theory of the 92nd Annual Convention of the American
transformed parenting: parenting a child with Psychological Association, Toronto, August.

Early Dev. Parent. 5 237-250 (1996) 01996 by JohnWiley & Sons, Ltd.

You might also like