Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 26

ArkSHA Convention

Little Rock, AR
October 17, 2013

What Works:
Speech Sound Disorders Update

Presented by

Gregory L. Lof, Ph.D. CCC-SLP


Chair/Professor ASHA Fellow
Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Boston, MA glof@mghihp.edu www.mghihp.edu

Page 1
Why Not to use Nonspeech Oral Motor Exercises
Nonspeech Oral Motor Exercises Defined NSOME (Hodge et al. 2005); 71.5% of SLPs
 Any technique that does not require the child al., 2009) and 46% in Minnesota (Louma &
to produce a speech sound but is used to Collins, 2012).
influence the development of speaking abilities  Most frequently used exercises (in rank
(Lof & Watson, 2008). order): Blowing; Tongue push-ups; Pucker-
 A collection of nonspeech methods and smile; Tongue wags; Big smile; Tongue-to-
procedures that claim to influence tongue, lip, nose-to-chin; Cheek puffing; Blowing kisses;
and jaw resting postures, increase strength, Tongue curling.
improve muscle tone, facilitate range of  Reported benefits (in rank order): Tongue
motion, and develop muscle control (Ruscello, elevation; Awareness of articulators; Tongue
2008). strength; Lip strength; Lateral tongue move-
 Oral-motor exercises (OMEs) are nonspeech ments; Jaw stabilization; Lip/tongue pro-
activities that involve sensory stimulation to or trusion; Drooling control; VP competence;
actions of the lips, jaw, tongue, soft palate, Sucking ability.
larynx, and respiratory muscles which are  These exercises are used for children with
intended to influence the physiologic under- (in rank order): Dysarthria; Apraxia of speech
pinnings of the oropharyngeal mechanism and (CAS); Structural anomalies; Down syndrome;
thus improve its functions. They include active Enrollment in early intervention; “Late talker”
muscle exercise, muscle stretching, passive diagnosis; Phonological impairment; Hearing
exercise, and sensory stimulation (McCauley, impairment; Functional misarticulations.
Strand, Lof, et al., 2009).
Do SLPs use NSOME? What Kind?
 85% of SLPs in the USA use NSOME to
change speech sound productions (Lof &
Watson, 2008); 85% of Canadian SLPs use
Logical Reasons to Question Using NSOME
 Some logical questions about NSOME… only in self-published materials and on
There is evidence that shows that NSOME do proprietary websites? Why do these websites
not work. Why is it being ignored? There is have a section for “testimonials” but not for
NO evidence that shows that NSOME do “research”? How could one procedure work
work. Why is this being ignored? Why are to remediate so many disparate types of
the materials and procedures used in NSOME problems? What is the monetary benefits to
not submitted for peer-review scrutiny? Why the promoters of NSOME?
are the materials and procedures promoted
Theoretical Reasons to Question Using NSOME
Part-Whole Training and Transfer parts of the movement alone; training on just
the parts of these well-organized behaviors can
• Basic questions: Does training on a smaller actually diminish learning. Highly organized
portion of the articulatory gesture transfer over tasks require learning the information
to the whole gesture? Is it more efficient and processing demands, as well as learning time-
better for learning by first training just part of sharing and other inter-component skills
the movement and not the whole movement? (Kleim & Jones, 2008; Wightman & Lintern,
 Tasks that comprise highly organized or 1985).
integrated movements (such as speaking) will
not be enhanced by learning the constituent

Page 2
 “Fractionating a behavior that is composed of NSOME do not follow this basic strength
interrelated parts is not likely to provide training paradigm so there are probably no
relevant information for the appropriate actual strength gains occurring due to these
development of neural substrates” (Forrest, exercises.
2002). • Articulators can be strengthened (e.g., the
• Some clinician-researchers believe that it can tongue for oral phase of swallowing or the VP
be more effective to “Train the Whole” complex) but these strengthened articulators
(Ingram & Ingram, 2001) and to use “Whole- will not help with the production of speech.
Word Phonology and Templates” (Velleman Clark et al. (2009) and Robbins et al. (2005)
& Vihman, 2002) rather than breaking up the have demonstrated ways to increase oral
gesture into small parts. strength.

Strengthening the Articulatory Structures • Measurements of strength are usually


highly subjective (e.g., feeling the force of the
• Basic questions:  Is strength necessary for tongue pushing against a tongue depressor or
speaking? If so, how much?  Are the against the cheek or just “observing”
articulators actually strengthened by using weakness), so clinicians cannot initially verify
NSOME?  How do SLPs objectively that strength is actually diminished and then
document weakness of articulators and they cannot report increased strength
objectively document supposed increases in following NSOME.
strength after NSOME?  Do children with
speech sound disorders have weak
• Only objective measures (e.g., tongue force
transducers, Iowa Oral Performance Instru-
articulators?
ment [IOPI]) can corroborate statements of
• Articulatory strength needs are VERY low strength needs and improvement. Without
for speech and the speaking strength needs do such objective measurements, testimonials of
not come anywhere close to maximum articulator strength gains must be considered
strength abilities of the articulators. For suspect.
example, lip muscle force for speaking is only
about 10-20% of the maximal capabilities for
• “To assess tongue strength, clinicians
commonly hold a tongue depressor beyond the
lip force, and the jaw uses only about 11-15%
lips and the patient pushes the tongue against
of the available amount of force that can be
the depressor. Strength is rated perceptually,
produced (see also Bunton & Weismer, 1994).
often with a 3-5 point equal-appearing
• “…only a fraction of maximum tongue force is interval scale or with binary judgments of
used in speech production, and such strength “normal” or “weak” (Solomon & Monson,
tasks are not representative of the tongue's 2004).
role during typical speaking. As a result,
caution should be taken when directly
• Preschool children with speech sound
disorders may actually have STRONGER
associating tongue strength to speech…” tongues than their typically developing peers
(Wenke et al., 2006).
(Sudbery et al., 2006).
• Agility and fine articulatory movements,
• Tone vs. Strength. Muscle tone refers to the
rather than strong articulators, are required for
resilience or elasticity of the muscle at rest.
the ballistic movements of speaking. NSOME
"Low tone” indicates less contraction of the
encourage gross and exaggerated ranges of
fibers than typical. Observing low tone does
motion, not small, coordinated movements that not automatically mean that the child has
are required for talking.
weakness. Working on strengthening probably
• NSOME may not actually increase will not influence tone (Clark, 2010).
articulator strength. To strengthen muscle, Relevancy of NSOME to Speech
the exercise must be done with multiple
repetitions, against resistance, until • Relevancy is the only way to get changes in
failure…and then done again and again. Most the neural system; the context in which a skill

Page 3
is learned is crucial. In order to obtain transfer produced by puffing out the cheeks; no sound
from one skill to another, the learned skills is produced in the same way as blowing is
must be relevant to the other skills. produced. Oral movements that are irrelevant
• “…muscle fibers are selectively recruited to to speech movements will not be effective as
perform specific tasks, so static non-speech speech therapy techniques.
tasks do not account for the precise and Task Specificity
coordinated activity needed during speech” • The same structures used for speaking and
(Hodge & Wellman, 1999). other “mouth tasks” (e.g., feeding, swal-
• For sensory motor stimulation to improve lowing, sucking, breathing, etc.) function in
articulation, the stimulation must be done different ways depending on the task and each
with relevant behaviors, with a defined end task is mediated by different parts of the brain.
goal, using integration of skills. “The The organization of movements within the
PURPOSE of a motor behavior has a nervous system is not the same for speech and
profound influence on the manner in which the nonspeech gestures. Although identical
relevant neural topography is marshaled and structures are used, these structures function
controlled” (Weismer, 2006). differently for speech and for nonspeech
• Most NSOME dis-integrate the highly activities.
integrated task of speaking (e.g., practicing • Weismer (2006): The control of motor
tongue elevation to the alveolar ridge with the behavior is task (speaking) specific, not
desire that this isolated task will improve effector (muscle or organ) specific. There is
production of the lingual-alveolar sound /s/). strong evidence against the “shared control”
For example, a motor task (e.g., shooting a for speech and nonspeech. “Motor control
free throw using a basketball) must be learned processes are tied to the unique goals, sources
in the context of the actual performance goal. of information (e.g., feedback), and
By analogy, no one would teach a ballplayer to characteristics of varying motor acts, even
pretend to hold a ball and then pretend to when those share the same effectors and some
throw it toward a non-existent hoop with the neural tissue.”
eventual hope of improving free throwing • Some examples of task specificity: Babbling
ability. Breaking down basketball shooting or and early nonspeech oral behaviors are not
the speaking task into smaller, unrelated related (e.g., Moore & Ruark, 1996); Patients
chunks that are irrelevant to the actual can have dysphagia with and without speech
performance is not effective. problems (i.e., “double dissociations;” Ziegler,
• Another non-speaking example would be the 2003); It is well documented that the VP
illogical finger pounding on a tabletop to mechanism can be strengthened, however,
simulate playing on a piano. Learning and reduction of speech nasality does not occur
improving piano playing must be practiced on (e.g., Kuehn & Moon, 1994); Breathing for
a piano, not on a tabletop. Likewise, learning speech is different than breathing at rest or
and improving speaking ability must be during other activities (e.g., Moore, Caulfield,
practiced in the context of speaking. To & Green, 2001). See Weismer (2006) for
improve speaking, children must practice summary of 11 studies that show that speech
speaking, rather than using tasks that only and nonspeech are different for a wide variety
superficially appear to be like speaking. of structures, including facial muscles, jaw
• Because isolated movements of the tongue, motion, jaw operating space, jaw coordination,
lips and other articulators are not the actual lingual movement, lip motions, levator veli
gestures used for the production of any sounds palatini, and mandibular control.
in English, their value for improving • Research has shown that non-speech
production of speech sounds is doubtful. That movements activate different parts of the brain
is, no speech sound requires the tongue tip to than does speech movements (Bonilha et al.,
be elevated toward the nose; no sound is 2006; Ludlow et al., 2008; Schulz et al., 1999;

Page 4
Yee et al., 2007). This shows that the neural  knowledge about the articulators’ movement
basis of motor control is different for speech and placement.
and non-speech oral movements.  Awareness and its role in therapy is always
• Bunton (2008) and Wilson, Green, questioned. It is well known that young
Yunusova, and Moore (2008) provide children have difficulty with various
excellent examples and concepts dealing with metaphonological awareness tasks (Kamhi &
the importance of task specificity. Catts, 2005). For articulation awareness,
Klein, Lederer and Cortese (1991) reported
Warm-Up/Awareness/Metamouth that children age 5 and 6 years had very little
 Warm-up has a physiological purpose consciousness of how speech sounds were
during muscle exercise: to increase blood made; 7 year olds were not very proficient
circulation so muscle viscosity drops, thus with this either. According to Koegel, Koegel,
allowing for smoother and more elastic muscle and Ingham (1986), some children older than 7
contractions (Safran, Seaber, & Garrett, 1989). years were successful during a metalinguistic
 Warm-up of muscles may be appropriate speech intervention program, but only when
(Pollock et al., 1998) when a person is about to they have the “…cognitive maturity required
initiate an exercise regimen that will to understand the concept of a sound…”
maximally tax the system (e.g., distance  It appears that young children cannot take
running or weight training). However, muscle advantage of the non-speech mouth cues
warm-up is not required for tasks that are provided during NSOME that can be
below the maximum (e.g., walking or lifting a transferred to speaking tasks. More research is
spoon-to-mouth). Because speaking does not needed to determine the minimum cognitive,
require anywhere near the oral muscular linguistic, and motor abilities of children that
maximum, warm-up is not necessary. are necessary for such “meta” skills.
 If clinicians are not using the term warm-up to
identify a physiological task to “wake up the
mouth,” then perhaps they believe that they
are providing some form of “metamouth”

Disorders that SLPs Often Use NSOME


Childhood Apraxia of Speech (CAS) Technical Report on Childhood Apraxia of
Speech, 2007).
• Children with CAS have adequate oral
structure movements for nonspeech Cleft Lip/Palate
activities but not for volitional speech (Caruso  The VP mechanism can be strengthened
& Strand, 1999), so this would preclude the through exercise (many studies have
use of NSOME because non-speech is not the demonstrated this since the 1960s), but added
problem. strength will not improve speech productions.
• There is no muscle weakness for children  “Blowing exercises, sucking, swallowing,
with CAS, so there is no need to do gagging, and cheek puffing have been
strengthening exercises. If there is weakness, suggested as useful in improving or
then the correct diagnosis is dysarthria, not strengthening velopharyngeal closure and
apraxia. speech. However, multi-view videofluoroscopy
• “The focus of intervention for the child has shown that velopharyngeal movements of
diagnosed with CAS is on improving the these nonspeech functions differ from
planning, sequencing, and coordination of velopharyngeal movements for speech in the
muscle movements for speech. Isolated same speaker. Improving velopharyngeal
exercises designed to "strengthen" the oral motion for these tasks does not result in
muscles will not help. CAS is a disorder of improved resonance or speech. These pro-
speech coordination, not strength.” (ASHA cedures simply do not work and the premises

Page 5
and rationales behind them are scientifically NSOME for Children with Dysarthria
unsound.” (Golding-Kushner, 2001).  NSOME are frequently used for acquired
 Ruscello (2008) evaluates the use of NSOME dysarthria, but their use is influenced by
and craniofacial anomalies in his article. “folklore” and not by evidence of
NSOME for Non-Motor Speech Disorders effectiveness (Mackenzie et al., 2010).
 Some may believe that motor exercises can  Following guidance from adults with acquired
help children with motor production speech dysarthria, “…strengthening exercises are
problems, such as functional misarticulators probably only appropriate for a small number
(phonetic/ articulatory problems) or children of patients” (Duffy, 2005).
with structural problems; however the  “…weakness is not directly related to
evidence does not support this. intelligibility...” for patients with ALS”
 It makes no sense that motor exercises could (Duffy, 2005).
help improve the speech of children who have  Based on the adult acquired dysarthria
non-motor problems such as language/ literature, it appears that NSOME are not
phonemic/phonological problems like children recommended as a technique that can improve
in Early Intervention diagnosed as late talkers. speech productions.
 It is puzzling why clinicians would use a
motor approach for non-motor speech
disorders; therapy must target the system that
is impacting the speech problem.

Evidence Against the Use of NSOME


Evidence-Based Systematic Review: Effects of which approach is providing therapeutic
Nonspeech Oral Motor Exercises on Speech benefit. Additionally, whenever intervention
(McCauley et al., 2009). Purpose was to approaches are combined, it is unknown if and
conduct evidence-based systematic review on how they actually work in conjunction with
NSOME. Only 8 peer-reviewed articles met each other to enhance performance.
rigorous criteria for inclusion. “Insufficient • There is much evidence that the NSOME
evidence to support or refute the used of OMEs portion of combined treatments is irrelevant to
to produce effects on speech was found…” speech improvements.
There are a few studies evaluating the
effectiveness of NSOME that are not in peer- • NSOME probably do not harm the child when
reviewed journals; most of these studies were used in combination with traditional
reported at ASHA Conventions. Of the 11 approaches (however, Hayes [2006] found that
studies available, 10 showed that NSOME were some children may be negatively affected by a
NOT effective as a treatment approach. See combination approach).
Lass and Pannbacker (2008) and Ruscello • It seems reasonable that if there is no speech
(2008) for a review of these and other studies. improvement using combined approaches,
then clinicians should eliminate the approach
Evidence Against the Horn Hierarchy that is not effective (i.e., NSOME) so as to not
 Research has shown that there is no systematic waste valuable therapy time with an
increase in pressure or resistance from one ineffectual technique.
horn to the next. There really is NO hierarchy
in the Horn Hierarchy (Jones, Hardin-Jones &
Brown, 2011; 2012).
Combining Treatment Approaches
• Most SLPs use a combination of treatment
approaches so it is difficult to “tease apart”

Page 6
In Conclusion
 Potential reasons why NSOME continue to  Phonetic placement cues that have been used
be used (Lof, 2009): The procedures can be in traditional speech therapy are NOT the
followed in a step-by-step “cookbook” same as NSOME.
fashion;  The exercises are tangible with the  NSOME are a procedure not a goal. The
appearance that something therapeutic is being goal of speech therapy is NOT to produce a
done; There is a lack of understanding the tongue wag, to have strong articulators, to puff
theoretical literature addressing the dis- out the cheeks, etc. Rather, the goal is to
similarities of speech-nonspeech movements; produce intelligible speech.
The techniques can be easily written out to  We have been burned before. In the 1990s
produce; There are a wide variety of many SLPs inappropriately embraced Facili-
techniques and tools available for purchase tated Communication (FC) as a treatment
that are attractively packaged; Many approach because they thought they observed
practicing clinicians do not read peer-reviewed that it worked. Once it was tested using
articles but instead rely on unscientific scientific methodology, it was found to not
writings; SLPs attend non-peer reviewed work. Pseudoscientific methodologies can
activities that encourage their use; Parents persuade clinicians to provide the wrong
and therapists on multidisciplinary teams treatment.
encourage using NSOME; Frequently, other  Following the guidelines of Evidence-Based
clinicians persuade their colleagues to use Practice, evidence needs to guide treatment
these techniques. decisions. Parents need to be informed that
 If clinicians want speech to improve, they NSOME have not been shown to be effective
must work on speech, and not on things that and their use must be considered experimental.
LOOK like they are working on speech.
References
Aberhamsen, E., & Flack, L. (2002, Nov.). Do sensory and motor techniques improve accurate
phoneme production? Poster presented at the annual meeting of the American Speech-Language-
Hearing Association, Atlanta, GA.
ASHA Technical Report on Childhood Apraxia of Speech (2007). http://www.asha.org/docs/
html/PS2007-00277.html
Bahr, D., & Rosenfeld-Johnson, S. (2010). Treatment of children with speech oral placement disorders
(OPDs): A paradigm emerges. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 31, 131-138
Bernstein Ratner, N. (2006). Evidence-based practice: An examination of its ramifications for the
practice of speech-language pathology. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in the Schools, 37,
257-267.
Bonilha, L, Moser, D., Rorden, C., Bylis, G., & Fridriksson, J. (2006). Speech apraxia without oral
apraxia: Can normal brain function explain the physiopathology? Brain Imaging, 17(10), 1027-1031.
Bowen, C. (2006). What is the evidence for oral motor therapy? Acquiring Knowledge in Speech,
Language, and Hearing, 7, 144-147.
Bunton, K. (2008). Speech versus nonspeech: Different tasks, different neural organization. Seminars in
Speech and Language, 29(4), 267-275.
Bunton, K., & Weismer, G. (1994). Evaluation of a reiterant force-impulse task in the tongue. Journal
of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 1020-1031.
Bush, C., Steger, M., Mann-Kahris, S, & Insalaco, D. (2004, Nov.). Equivocal results of oral motor
treatment on a child’s articulation. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the American Speech-
Language-Hearing Association, Philadelphia, PA.

Page 7
Caruso, A., & Strand, E. (1999). Clinical management of motor speech disorders in children. New
York: Thieme.
Christensen, M., & Hanson, M. (1981). An investigation of the efficacy of oral myofunctional therapy
as a precursor to articulation therapy for pre-first grade children. Journal of Speech and Hearing
Disorders, 46, 160-167.
Cima, C., Mahanna-Boden, S., Brown, K., & Cranfill, T. (2009, Nov.). Clinical decision making in the
use of non-speech oral motor exercises in the treatment of speech sound disorders. Poster presented at
the annual meeting of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, New Orleans, LA.
Clark, H. (2010). Nonspeech oral motor intervention (Ch. 24). In A. Williams, S. McLeod, & R.
McCauley, Interventions for Speech Sound Disorders in Children. Baltimore: Brookes Publishing.
Clark, H. (2008). The role of strength training in speech sound disorders. Seminars in Speech and
Language, 29(4), 276-283.
Clark, H. (2003). Neuromuscular treatments for speech and swallowing: A tutorial. American Journal of
Speech-Language Pathology, 12, 400-415.
Clark, H. (2005, June 14). Clinical decision making and oral motor treatments. The ASHA Leader, 8-9,
34-35
Clark, H., O’Brien, K., Calleja, A., & Corrie, S. (2009). Effects of directional exercise on lingual
strength, Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 52, 10.3-1047.
Clark, H., Henson, P., Barber, W., Stierwalt, J., & Sherrill, M. (2003). Relationships among
subjective and objective measures of tongue strength and oral phase swallowing impairments.
American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12, 40-50.
Colone, E., & Forrest, K. (2000, Nov.). Comparison of treatment efficacy for persistent speech
disorders. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Speech-Language-Hearing
Association, Washington, D.C.
Connaghan, K., Moore, C., & Higashakawa, M. (2004). Respiratory kinematics during vocalization
and nonspeech respiration in children from 9 to 48 months. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
Research, 47, 70-84.
Davis, B., & Velleman, S. (2008). Establishing a basic speech repertoire without using NSOME: Means,
motive, and opportunities. Seminars in Speech and Language, 29(4), 312-319.
DePaul, R., & Brooks, B. (1993). Multiple orofacial indices in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Journal of
Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 1158-1993
Dollaghan, C. (2004, April 13). Evidence-based practice: Myths and realities. The ASHA Leader, 12, 4-5.
Dollaghan, C. (2007). The handbook for evidence-based practice in communication disorders.
Baltimore: Brooks Publishing.
Duffy, J. (2005). Motor speech disorders: Substrates, differential diagnosis, and management (2nd ed.).
St. Louis: Mosby.
Dworkin, J., & Culatta, R., (1980). Tongue strength: Its relationship to tongue thrusting, open-bite, and
articulatory proficiency. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 45, 277-282.
Fields, D., & Polmanteer, K. (2002, Nov.). Effectiveness of oral motor techniques in articulation and
phonology therapy. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the American Speech-Language-
Hearing Association, Atlanta, GA.
Finn, P., Bothe, A., & Bramlett, R. (2005). Science and pseudoscience in communication disorders:
Criteria and application. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 14, 172-183.
Forrest, K. (2002). Are oral-motor exercises useful in the treatment of phonological/articulatory
disorders? Seminars in Speech and Language, 23, 15-25
Forrest, K., & Iuzzini, J. (2008). A comparison of oral motor and production training for children with
speech sound disorders. Seminars in Speech and Language, 29, 304-311.
Golding-Kushner, K. (2001). Therapy techniques for cleft palate speech and related disorders. Clifton
Park, NY: Thompson.
Gommerman, S., & Hodge, M. (1995). Effects of oral myofunctional therapy on swallowing and
sibilant production. International Journal of Orofacial Myology, 21, 9-22.

Page 8
Green, J., Moore, C., Higashikawa, M., & Steeve, R. (2000). The physiologic development of speech
motor control: Lip and jaw coordination. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43,
239-255.
Green, J., & Wang, Y. (2003). Tongue-surface movement patterns in speech and swallowing. The
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 113, 2820-2833.
Guisti Braislin, M., & Cascella, P. (2005). A preliminary investigation of the efficacy of oral motor
exercises for children with mild articulation disorders. International Journal of Rehabilitation
Research, 28, 263-266.
Hayes, S. (2006). Comparison of an oral motor treatment and traditional articulation treatment in
children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Nova Southeastern University, Ft. Lauderdale, FL.
Hodge, M. (2002). Nonspeech oral motor treatment approaches for dysarthria: Perspectives on a
controversial clinical practice. Perspectives on Neurophysiology and Neurogenetic Speech and
Language Disorders, 12, 22-28.
Hodge, M., Salonka, R., & Kollias, S. (2005, Nov.). Use of nonspeech oral-motor exercises in
children’s speech therapy. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the American Speech-Language-
Hearing Association, San Diego, CA.
Hodge, M., & Wellman, L. (1999). Management of children with dysarthria. In A. Caruso & E. Strand
(Eds.), Clinical management of motor speech disorders in children. New York: Thieme.
Hoit, J. (1995). Influence of body position on breathing and its implications for the evaluation and
treatment of speech and voice disorders. Journal of Voice, 9, 341-347.
Ingram, D., & Ingram, K. (2001). A whole word approach to phonological intervention. Language,
Speech & Hearing Services in the Schools, 32, 271-283.
Jensen, J., Marstrand, P., & Nielsen, J. (2005). Motor skill training and strength training are associated
with different plastic changes in the central nervous system. Journal of Applied Physiology, 99, 1558-
1568.
Joffe, B., & Pring, T. (2008). Children with phonological problems: A survey of clinical practice.
International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 43, 154-164.
Jones, D., Hardin-Jones, M., & Brown (2011, Nov.; 2012; Nov.). Posters presented at the annual
meeting of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, San Diego, CA and Atlanta, GA.
Kamhi, A. (2004). A meme’s-eye view of speech-language pathology. Language, Speech and Hearing
Services in the Schools, 35, 105-111.
Kamhi, A. (2008). A meme’s-eye view of nonspeech oral motor exercises. Seminars in Speech and
Language, 29, 331-339.
Kamhi, A., & Catts, H. (2005). Language and reading: Convergences and divergences. In H. Catts & A.
Kamhi (Eds.). Language and reading disabilities (2nd ed.), Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Kent, R. (2000). Research on speech motor control and its disorders: A review and prospective. Journal
of Communication Disorders, 33, 391-428.
Kent, R. (2004). The uniqueness of speech among motor systems. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 18,
495-505.
Kleim, J., & Jones, T. (2008). Principles of experience-dependent neural plasticity: Implications for
rehabilitation after brain damage. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 51, S225-
S239.
Kleim, J., Barbay, S., Cooper, N., Hogg, T., Reidel, C., Remple, M., & Nudo, R. (2002). Motor
learning-dependent synaptogenesis is localized to functionally reorganized motor cortex.
Neurobiology, Learning & Memory, 77, 63–77.
Klein, H., Lederer, S., & Cortese, E. (1991). Children’s knowledge of auditory/articulator
correspondences: Phonologic and metaphonologic. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 559-
564.
Koegel, L., Koegel, R., & Ingham, J. (1986). Programming rapid generalization of correct articulation
through self-monitoring procedures. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 51, 24-32.

Page 9
Koenig, M., & Gunter, C. (2005). Fads in speech-language pathology. In J. Jacobson, R. Foxx, & J.
Mulich (Eds.), Controversial therapies for developmental disabilities: Fad, fashion, and science in
professional practice. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Kuehn, D., & Moon, J. (1994). Levator veli palatini muscle activity in relation to intraoral air pressure
variation. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37,1260–1270.
Lass, N., & Pannbacker, M. (2008). The application of evidence-based practice to nonspeech oral motor
treatment. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in the Schools,39, 408-421.
Lemmon, R., Harrison, M., Woods-McKnight, R., Bonnette, A., & Jackson, K. (2010, Nov.). Speech-
language professionals’ perceptions of the efficacy of oral motor exercises. Poster presented at the
annual meeting of the American Speech-Language Hearing Association, Philadelphia, PA
Lof, G. (2002). Two comments on this assessment series. American Journal of Speech-Language
Pathology, 11, 255-257.
Lof, G. (2003). Oral motor exercises and treatment outcomes. Perspectives on Language, Learning and
Education, 10, 7-12.
Lof, G. (2004). What does the research report about non-speech oral motor exercises and the treatment of
speech sound disorders? http://www.apraxia-kids.org/site/c.chKMI0PIIsE/b.980831/apps/s/content
.asp ?ct=464461.
Lof, G. (2008). Introduction to controversies about the use of nonspeech oral motor exercises. Seminars
in Speech and Language, 29(4), 253-256.
Lof, G. (2009). The nonspeech-oeral motor exercise phenomenon in speech pathology practice. In C.
Bowen, Children’s speech sound disorders. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.
Lof, G. (2011). Science-base practice and the speech-language pathologist. International Journal of
Speech-Language Pathology. 13(3), 189-196.
Lof, G. (2012, Nov.). Science vs. pseudosicnec in CSD: A checklist for skeptical thinking. Poster
presented at the annual meeting of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, Atlanta, GA.
Lof, G., & Watson, M. (2008). A nationwide survey of non-speech oral motor exercise use: Implications
for evidence-based practice. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 39, 392-407.
Lof, G., & Watson, M. (2010). Five reasons why nonspeech oral-motor exercises do not work.
Perspectives on School-Based Issues, 11.109-117.
Ludlow, C., Hoit, J., Kent, R., Ramig, L., Shrivastav, R., Strand, E., Yorkston, K., & Sapienza, C.
(2008). Translating principles of neural plasticity into research on speech motor control recovery and
rehabilitation. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 51, S240-S258.
Mackenzie, C., Muir, M., & Allen, C. (2010). Non-speech oro-motor exercise use in acquired dysarthria
management: Regimes and rationales. International Journal of Language and Communication
Disorders, 45,617-629.
Martin, R. (1991). A comparison of lingual movement in swallowing and speech production. Ph.D.
dissertation, University of Wisconsin–Madison.
McAuliffe, M., Ward, E., Murdoch, B., & Farrell, A. (2005). A nonspeech investigation of tongue
function in Parkinson’s Disease. Journals of the Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and
Medical Sciences, 60, 667-674.
McCauley, R., & Strand, E. (2008). Treatment of childhood apraxia of speech: Clinical decision making
in the use of nonspeech oral motor exercise. Seminars in Speech and Language, 29(4), 284-293.
McCauley, R., Strand, E., Lof, G.L., Schooling, T., & Frymark, T. (2009). Evidence-based systematic
review: Effects of non-speech oral motor exercises on speech. American Journal of Speech-Language
Pathology. 18, 343-360.
Moore, C., Caulfield, T., & Green, J. (2001). Relative kinematics of the rib cage and abdomen during
speech and nonspeech behaviors of 15-month-old children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing
Research, 44, 80-94.
Moore, C., Smith, A., & Ringel, R. (1988). Task-specific organization of activity in human jaw muscles.
Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 31, 670-680.

Page 10
Moore, C., & Ruark, J. (1996). Does speech emerge from earlier appearing motor behaviors? Journal of
Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 1034-1047.
Muttiah, N., Georges, K., & Brackenbury, T. (2011). Clinical and research perspectives on nonspeech
oral motor treatments and evidence-based practice. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology,
20, 47-59.
Occhino, C., & McCann. J. (2001, Nov.). Do oral motor exercise affect articulation? Poster presented at
the annual meeting of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, New Orleans, LA.
Peter-Falzone, S., Trost-Cardamone, J., Karnell, M., & Hardin-Jones, M. (2006). The clinician’s
guide to treating cleft palate speech. St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
Pollock, M., Gaesser, G., Butcher, J., Despres, J., Dishman, R., Franklin, B., et al. (1998). American
College of Sports Medicine position stand: The recommended quantity and quality of exercise for
developing and maintaining cardiorespiratory and muscular fitness, and flexibility in healthy adults.
Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 30, 975-991.
Powell, T. (2008). Prologue: The use of Nonspeech oral motor treatments for developmental speech
sound production disorders: Interventions and interactions. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services
in the Schools, 39, 374-379.
Powell, T. (2008). Epilogue: An integrated evaluation of nonspeech oral motor treatments. Language,
Speech, and Hearing Services in the Schools, 39, 422-427.
Remple, M., Bruneau, R., VandenBerg, P., Goertzen, C., & Kleim, J. (2001). Sensitivity of cortical
movement representations to motor experience: Evidence that skill learning but not strength training
induces cortical reorganization. Behavioral and Brain Research, 123, 133–141.
Roehrig, S., Suiter, D., & Pierce, T. (2004, Nov). An examination of the effectiveness of passive oral-
motor exercises. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the American Speech-Language-Hearing
Association, Philadelphia, PA.
Robbins, J., Gangnon, R., Theis, S., Kays, S., Wewitt, A., & Hind, J. (2005). The effects of lingual
exercise on swallowing in older adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 53, 1483-1489.
Ruscello, D. (2008). Oral motor treatment issues related to children with developmental speech sound
disorders. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 39, 380-391.
Ruscello, D. (2008). An examination of nonspeech oral motor exercise for children with velopharyngeal
inadequacy. Seminars in Speech and Language, 29(4), 294-303.
Safran, M., Seaber, V., & Garrett, W. (1989). Warm-up and muscular injury prevention: An update.
Sports Medicine, 8, 239-249.
Sackett, D., Rosenberg, W., Gray, J., Haynes, R., & Richardson, W. (1996). Evidence based
medicine: What it is and what it isn’t. British Medical Journal, 312, 71-72.
Schulz, G., Dingwall, W., & Ludlow, C. (1999). Speech and oral motor learning in individuals with
cerebellar atrophy. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 42, 1157-1175.
Secord, W., Boyce, S., Donohue, J., Fox, R., & Shine, R. (2007). Eliciting sounds: Techniques and
strategies for clinicians. Clifton Park, NY: Thomson.
Sjögreena, L., Tuliniusb, M., Kiliaridisc, S., & Lohmanderd, A. (2010). The effect of lip
strengthening exercises in children and adolescents with myotonic dystrophy type 1. International
Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 74(10), 1126-1134.
Solomon, N., & Munson, B. (2004). The effect of jaw position on measures of tongue strength and
endurance, Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 584-594.
Sudbery, A., Wilson, E, Broaddus, T., & Potter, N. (2006, Nov.). Tongue strength in preschool
children: Measures, implications, and revelations. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, Miami Beach, FL.
Terumitsu, M., Fujii, Y., Suzuki, K., Kwee, I., & Nakada, T. (2006). Human primary motor cortex
shows hemispheric specialization for speech. Neuroreport, 17, 1091-1095.
Tyler, A. (2008). What works: Evidence-based intervention for children with speech sound disorders.
Seminars in Speech and Language, 29(4), 320-330.
Velleman, S. (2003). Childhood apraxia of speech: Resource guide. Clifton Park, NY: Thomson.

Page 11
Velleman, S., & Vihman, M. (2002). Whole-word phonology and templates: Trap, bootstrap, or some of
each? Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in the Schools, 33, 9-23.
Watson, M., & Lof, G. L. (2005, Nov.). Survey of universities’ teaching: Oral motor exercises and
other procedures. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the American Speech-Language-Hearing
Association, San Diego, CA.
Watson, M., & Lof, G.L. (2008). What we know about nonspeech oral motor exercises. Seminars in
Speech and Language, 29, 320-330.
Watson, M., & Lof, G.L. (2009). A survey of university professors teaching speech sound disorders:
Nonspeech oral motor exercises and other topics. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in the
Schools, 40, 256-270.
Watson, M., & Lof, G.L. (2011). Parent-friendly information about nonspeech oral motor exercises.
Poster presented at the annual meeting of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, San
Diego, CA.
Weismer, G. (1996). Assessment of non-speech gestures in speech-language pathology: A critical
review. Telerounds 35 (videotape). National Center for Neurologic Communication Disorders,
University of Arizona.
Weismer, G. (2006). Philosophy of research in motor speech disorders. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics,
20, 315-349.
Weijnen, F., Kuks, J., van der Bilt, A., van der Glas, H., Wassenberg, M., & Bosman, F. (2000).
Tongue force in patients with myasthenia gravis. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 102, 303-308.
Wenke, R., Goozee, J., Murdoch, B., & LaPointe, L.(2006). Dynamic assessment of articulation during
lingual fatigue in myasthenia gravis. Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology, 14, 13-32.
Wightman, D., & Lintern, G. (1985). Part-task training of tracking for manual control. Human Factors,
27, 267-283.
Wilson, E., Green, J., Yunusova, Y., & Moore, C. (2008). Task specificity in early oral motor
development. Seminars in Speech and Language, 29(4), 257-265.
Yee, M., Moore, C., Venkatesh, L., Vick, J., Campbell, T., Shriberg, L.D., Green, J., & Rusiewicz,
H. (2007, Nov.). Children’s mandibular movement patterns in two nonspeech tasks. Paper presented at
the annual meeting of the American Speech-Language Hearing Association, Boston, MA.
Yorkston, K., Beukelman, D., Strand, E., & Hakel, M. (2010). Management of motor speech disorders
in children and adults. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Ziegler, W. (2003). Speech motor control is task-specific: Evidence from dysarthria and apraxia of
speech. Aphasiology, 17, 3-36.

© 2013 Gregory L. Lof

Page 12
SOME THOUGHTS ON ASSESSMENT
Procedures that should be accomplished…
 Articulation test (52% of SLPs use Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation)
 Continuous speech analysis (citing vs. talking; Morrison & Shriberg, 1992)
 Hearing screening
 Stimulability assessment
o Stimulability as a Dynamic Assessment
o Teaching Stimulability:
 Enhancing stimulability to increase the phonetic inventory (Miccio & Elbert, 1996;
Miccio, 2005), using a multi-sensory approach: Concept, Movement, Icon
 Use with young children with a very limited phonetic inventory.
 Pair consonant sounds with alliterative characters and motions.
 http://www.speech-language-therapy.com/awm.pdf
 Seven Components of Treatment to Enhance Stimulability
1. Determine Stimulability
2. Directly Target Nonstimulable Sounds
3. Make Targets the Focus of Joint Attention (more likely to produce a sound when
they are attending to and interested in its corresponding referent. Speech sounds
may be easier to learn when they are associated with interesting objects that have
been verbally labeled for them.)
4. Associate Speech Sounds with Hand/Body Motions (multi-modal input increases
children’s ability to retain newly learned speech sounds. Hand motions serve as
retrieval cues for remembering).
5. Associate Speech Sounds with Alliterative Characters of Interest to the Child.
(interesting characters increase interest in the activity and encourage full
participation. Enhances the opportunity for a child to develop conscious
awareness of the newly learned sound segments. For example:
P Putt-putt pig b Baby bear t Talkie turkey
d Dirty dog k Coughing cow g Goofy goat
f Fussy fish v Viney violet ‘ Thinking thumb
s Silly snake z Zippy zebra c Shy sheepy
http://speech-language-therapy.com/pdf/miccio4s.pdf
6. Encourage Vocal Practice (do not use drill; instead, encourage vocal practice by
including sound elicitation activities that involve turn-taking and requesting).
7. Ensure Early Success (teach stimulable and nonstimulable sounds concurrently so
they can have success but also receive remediation for nonstimulable sounds).
o Summary of Stimulability
 Production of a sound during stimulability testing attests to the child’s ability to
perceive, to recognize as different, and to produce the sound in question.
 If the child is not stimulable for a sound, then one might question the child’s: Motoric
abilities, Perceptual abilities, Linguistic abilities, Attention (focus), Non-compliance.
 If a child is stimulable for a sound, then that sound is likely to be added to the child’s
phonetic inventory, even without direct treatment on that sound.
 If a child is NOT stimulable for a sound, then the likelihood of short term gains is
poor; normalization without therapy is much poorer than normalization for sounds
that the child is stimulable for.

Page 13
 Training on stimulable sounds is likely to improve regardless of what is taught.
 Sounds that are NOT stimulable are unlikely to change without direct treatment.
 In therapy, we need to encourage exploratory sound productions and provide phonetic
placement or other types of cues to effect stimulability skills.
 Once stimulability has been achieved, generalization is more likely to occur.
 To avoid frustration in training nonstimulable sounds, use less directive sound play
activities (e.g., those suggested by Miccio & Elbert, 1996) to provide a nurturing and
supportive therapy environment.
 Perception/discrimination assessment
o Is it still relevant? What assessment tools should be used?
o Locke (1980) Speech Production/Perception Test (SPPT)
 Oral-peripheral screening
o Is it still relevant? Why? Why not? (Lof, 2002)
 Case history—Some potential question to ask parents: Describe your child’s current speech
performance; How often can your child’s speech be understood by parents, siblings,
playmates, other relatives, strangers? How often does your child try to self-correct the
speech errors? How willing is your child to repeat words after you, to try to say them
correctly? How is your child’s speech in imitation compared to when s/he says the words by
him/herself? How would you describe your child’s willingness to repeat his/her idea if it is
not understood? How would you describe your child’s willingness to talk? What is your
estimation of severity of your child’s speech problem? Did your child ever have ear
infections or uninfected fluid in the ears? Would you consider your child’s physical
development as typical? Please comment.
 PCC: Percent of Consonants Correct
o An objective severity metric; The examiner makes correct/incorrect judgments of
individuals sounds produced.
Number of Correct Consonants
PCC = X 100
Number of Correct + Incorrect Consonants
o Problems with PCC: Does not take age into account; Only evaluates consonants;
Originally based only on conversational speech; Does not take into account type of
errors.
o Potential solution instead of conversational speech (Johnson, Weston & Bain, 2004).
 Research shows the conversational speech and this imitative approach provide
comparable results.
 Efficient and valid approach for children 4 to 6 years old (but can be used for other
children).
 Uses sentence imitation.
 Easily scored on the form and easy PCC calculation.
 Intelligibility
o A guideline for expected intelligibility can be calculated by dividing the child's age in
years by four and converting that number into a percentage: 2-year-old: 50%; 3-year-old:
75%; 4-year-old: 100% (Hodson, 2011)
 Intelligibility (see Ertmer, 2011)
o “Speech assessments that rely mainly on clinician impressions and word-based
articulation tests appear to be inadequate for monitoring the development of intelligible
connected speech.” “Recent research has shown that word-based articulation tests are
not dependable estimates of connected speech intelligibility.”

Page 14
o Words from the G-F Articulation Test had higher percentage of words identified than
words in short sentences (86.7% vs. 54.5%).
o Articulation tests were poor predictors of connected speech intelligibility. “Children may
correctly articulate a variety of consonants and vowels in single words but still not have
readily intelligible connected speech.”
o Intelligibility Assessment: Scaling Procedures
 Asking listeners to rate intelligibility along a continuum (10-point equal appearing
interval scale or use descriptors: not at all, seldom, sometimes, most of the time,
always).
 Disadvantages: Listeners have different internal criteria (pretty good for one may be
not very good to another); Numerical numbers do not have clear meaning; Scaling is
insensitive to the middle range of intelligibility (ratings may not well distinguish
between 30% and 60% intelligibility).
 Modifications: Use clear descriptors instead of numbers: No words were understood;
a few words were understood; approximately half of the words were understood;
most of the words were understood; almost all of the words were understood.
o Intelligibility Assessment: Item Identification Procedures
 Open set where listener writes down the words understood; Audio recordings of
unfamiliar sentences, listeners write words understood, place an X for words not
understood; Scored as a percentage of the number of times there was a match
o Intelligibility Assessment: Item Identification Procedures for preschool and early
elementary (4 lists, 10 sentences each)
 Presentation: Clinician says each sentence while using objects to act it out; Children
watch and then imitate the sentence; Presented 2x for listener to write down all
words; Percentage of words correctly written
o Intelligibility Assessment : Item Identification Procedures for children who can read
 Presentation: Clinician shows the card and says the sentence; Card is turned over and
child is asked to say the sentences; Presented 2x for listener to write down all words;
Percentage of words correctly written
 Intelligibility in Context Scale (McLeod, Harrison & McCormack, 2012)
o An easy to administer, valid, and reliable estimate of preschool children’s intelligibility
when speaking with people of varying levels of familiarity and authority.
o Developed as a parent-rated measure of intelligibility when speaking with people of
varying levels of familiarity and authority: Parents and 6 other types of communication
partners (e.g., “Do you understand your child?”  “Do strangers understand your
child?”)
o The rating is on a 5-point Likert scale: 1 = Never; 2 = Rarely; 3 = Sometimes; 4 =
Usually; 5 = Always
Procedures that are “always used” in assessment (Skahan, Watson, & Lof, 2007)
76% Case History 51% Phonological Processes
75% Intelligibility 36% Connected Speech Sample
74% Single Word Test 36% Phonological Inventory
71% Hearing Screening 31% Classroom Observation
68% Stimulability 13% Perception/Discrimination
61% Parent Interview 13% Phonological Awareness
58% Oral Motor-Non Speech 11% Contextual Testing
54% Oral Motor-Speech 11% Syllable/Word Shapes

Page 15
Helpful Assessment Forms
 Place/Manner/Voicing forms
 Phonetic/Phonemic Inventory Forms
o Phonetic Inventory: A listing of speech sounds that are produced regardless of the target
(independent analysis) (See Eisenberg & Hitchcock, 2010)
o Phonemic Inventory: A listing of the speech sounds produced in comparison to the target
sound (relational analysis)
 Cluster Analysis Forms : Powell (1995); McLeod, van Doorn, & Reed (2001); Smit (1993);
McLeod & Hand (1991)
Some questions to ask about potential targets (Tyler, 2005; 2008)
 How are word/syllable structures affected by error patterns?
o Frequency of different syllable structures; Match between target & structure productions;
Determine affected syllable structures
 Which sound classes are proportionally more affected by error patterns?
o Fricatives? Stops? Liquids? Place/Manner/Voicing analysis is helpful
 Are there positional constraints?
o Do the errors occur in all positions? Is it an inventory constraint or positional constraint?
 What sounds are present/absent in the phonetic inventory?
o Use a phonetic inventory form; do you expand the inventory or do you make the
inventory more useful?
 What is the stimulability status of the sounds in error?
o Do you select stimulable or nonstimulable sounds?
10 Factors to Consider for Selecting Targets (based on Powell, 1991)
(1) Age of Child/Age Appropriateness of Error(s): Use norms and other developmental
information (Shriberg, 1993)
Order Continuous Speech Articulation Test
Early 8 M b y n w d p h M b n w d p h g
Middle 8 T a k g f v . j T a k f v . j y
Late 8 C ‘ ; s z l r x C ‘ ; s z l r
(2) Effect on Intelligibility--Error type: Deletions  Substitutions  all others
(3) Effect on Intelligibility--Deviancy: Unusual, deviant, idiosyncratic
(4) Frequency of Sound Occurrence (Shriberg & Kent, 2013)
/ n,t,s,r/ account for 25% of all phoneme occurrences.
/1,n,t,8,s,r,I,l,d,2 / account for nearly half of all phoneme occurrences.
/n,d,t,r,z/ comprise more than 69% of the final consonant occurrences in words.
Order of sound occurrence:
1. n 5. l 9. m 13. p 17. g 21. j
2. t 6. d 10. w 14. v 18. y 22. c
3. s 7. ; 11. z 15. f 19. a 23. .
4. r 8. k 12. b 16. h 20. ‘ 24. x
(5) Homonymy: The production of one phonetic form for several adult target forms (e.g.,
[bat] for bad, bark, bent, bite; [bi] for beach, beat, beak, bike)
(6) Markedness (a part of complexity models): The aspects that have more features are
considered marked and more complex. Working on the most marked aspects can be
more effective in therapy (e.g., fricatives are more marked than stops; affricates are more

Page 16
marked than fricatives; clusters are more marked than singletons; voiced is more marked
than voiceless).
(7) Morphological Status: Evaluate the tense markers and language structures that mark
agreement. Those complex final clusters should then be targeted (especially if client has
speech AND language problems):
Morpheme Example
Present Progressive “ing” barking
Regular Plural “-s” dogs
Possessive “’s” baby’s
Regular Past “-ed jumped
Regular Third Person “-s” she eats
Contractable copula He’s the baby
Contractable auxiliary She’s going slow
(8) Phonetic Inventory: Determine completeness of the phonetic inventory, the repertoire
of speech sounds produced independent of the adult model.
(9) Relevance to Child: Select sounds/patterns important to the client.
(10) Stimulability: Reasons to and not to select nonstimulable sounds.

PHONOLOGICAL THERAPY APPROACHES


Baker (2004); Baker and McLeod (2011); Williams, McLeod & McCauley (2010)

UNPROVEN Therapy Technique: Nonspeech Oral Motor Exercises (Lof & Watson, 2008,
2010; Watson & Lof, 2008)
Minimal Pairs (a.k.a.: Minimal Opposition Contrast Therapy)
• Use pairs of words that differ by one phoneme only; for example: bow/boat
• Used to establish contrasts not present in the phonological system
• Usually words are selected with one word as the target, the other the replacement
• Child should be stimulable for correct sound
• Have child say both words in the pair
• Show a communicative confusion if both words are said the same
Maximal Pairs (a.k.a.: Maximal Oppositions Therapy)
• Word pairs have multiple feature contrasts (maximal oppositions)
• Features can differ on place, manner, and voicing
• The oppositions contrasts only two sounds
• A sound the child correctly produces is compared to a maximally different one
• An example: chop/mop
o Suppose a child produces t/c
o Maximal Pairs: /t/ is contrasted with maximally opposed sound from /c/ (perhaps /m/);
For example: me/she; Mack/shack, my/shy
• Follow the procedures for minimal pairs
• Who is best to use Maximal Pairs?
o Best used for moderate/severe children
o Meant to change the child’s entire phonological system because research shows it
promotes generalization
Multiple Oppositions

Page 17
• Much like minimal pairs, but pairs all or most errors simultaneously
• Good approach if child substitutes a single sound for multiple sounds
• Child confronts the rule from multiple contrasts
• For example: /t/ for /s,k,.,tr/
s sip Sue
k kip coo
t . tip chip two chew
tr trip true

Metaphonological
 A subcategory of metalinguistics.
 Involves child’s conscious awareness of sounds within the language.
 Phonological awareness is the awareness of sound/phonological structure of spoken words in
contrast to written words.
 Intervention to enhance early phoneme awareness and letter knowledge, combined with
intervention to improve speech intelligibility, may ensure that children with speech
impairment approach literacy instruction with age-appropriate phonological awareness
development and will help with speech sound productions.
 This combined approach has shown to work for both speech sound training and for literacy
development.
 Work on intelligibility, phoneme awareness, and letter-name/letter-sound knowledge.
o Phoneme blending (adult says: b—a—l, child says “ball”).
o Phoneme segmentation (adult says: “ball”, child says “b—a—l”).
o Phoneme manipulation: Say “boat” without the “t”; What word would you make if you
put “o” before “pen”?
Cycles Approach
 First developed by Hodson & Paden (1983).
 Typically a phoneme (or consonant cluster) is targeted one hour per week (i.e., one 60-
minute session, two 30-minute sessions, or three 20-minute sessions), with each pattern
typically being targeted from two to five hours per cycle
 Phonological patterns are divided into primary (those targeted first and then recycled as
needed until they began emerging in conversational speech) and secondary patterns (see
Hodson, 2011).
 Remediation Structure
o The child reviews production-practice picture cards from the previous session. These
cards are then set aside (and may be used again along with new, more complex words
during a later cycle, depending on whether the phonological pattern needs to be
recycled). The SLP then reads a list of approximately 20 words that contain the new
target pattern for the week. The child listens attentively, but must not repeat these words.
o The child participates in experiential-play production-practice motivational activities
(e.g., bowling, flashlight game), naming pictures and objects of four or five carefully
selected target words with the week's pattern before taking a turn in the activity. The SLP
changes activities every eight to 10 minutes. Many of these activities are repeated during
ensuing weeks.

Page 18
o A metaphonology activity is incorporated (e.g., rhyming, segmentation) to enhance the
child's phonological awareness skills.
o The SLP probes for the optimal phoneme target for the next week (within the
phonological pattern designated from phonological assessment results). For example, if
the current pattern is /s/ clusters, the clinician models words with various /s/ clusters (e.g.,
spot, store, snow). The cluster that the child produces most successfully becomes the
target for the ensuing week.
o Parents receive the listening list and the production-practice picture cards and are asked
to provide two minutes of home practice every day.
o Do a web search to find many resources and information about this approach.
Language-Based Intervention
 Many children with speech sound disorders also have difficulties with other aspects of
language.
 There is an intricate web of inter-dependencies between various aspects of language.
 Some studies have shown that moderately severe children may improve in phonology and
other language domains with a combined phonological/language approach.
 Children who are more severe may need focused attention directly on both domains.
 Cycles approach to language and phonological therapy has been shown to be effective.
 Work on language for one cycle, phonology for the next cycle.
 Work on morphology can also help with phonology (plural, possessives, past tense, etc.)
Other Approaches:
 PACT (Parents & Children Together); Psycholinguistic (using reading methods); Nonlinear
Phonology; Core Vocabulary

SOME PRINCIPLES OF PHONOLOGICAL


THERAPY
 The treatment is based on a phonological assessment, and the aims are defined by the
phonological assessment.
 Therapy is based on the principle that there are regularities in the child’s pronunciation
patterns (i.e., “order in disorder”).
 Therapy is based on the principle that the primary function of phonological organization is
communicative (i.e., differences in sounds and sequences signal meaning differences).
 Therapy aims to facilitate change in the child’s pronunciation patterns in order to build up a
more adequate system of sound contrasts and sound structures.
 Therapy is designed to make maximally effective use of the organization of phonological
patterning in the target system by introducing and establishing changes in the child’s patterns
through use of natural classes of contrastive phones and structures.
GENERAL COMMENTS AND SUGGESTIONS FOR
THERAPY
(Based on Hodson, 2011)
 Remediating a pattern, not individual sounds;
o Successive approximation; Not trained to 90% criteria
 Communication is the goal of training.
o Successful communication is its own best reward; Misarticulation is
miscommunication; Internal rewards, not external rewards

Page 19
 Use child-led interactive activities.
o Play-like therapy; Meaningful linguistic contexts
 Phonological acquisition is gradual.
 Work on phonetic problems along with phonological ones.
 Carefully select words and sounds for therapy.
 Children generalize new skills to other targets.

A H E L P F U L R E S O U R C E F O R A SS E S S M E N T &
TREATMENT
Caroline Bowen: http://speech-language-therapy.com/

REFERENCES
Baker, E. (2004). Phonological analysis summary and management plan. Acquiring Knowledge in
Speech, Language and Hearing, 6, 14-17.
Baker, E., & McLeod, S., (2011). Evidence-based practice for children with speech sound disorders: Part
1 and Part 2. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in the Schools, 42, 102-152.
Eisenberg, S. & Hitchcock, E. (2010). Using standardized tests to inventory consonant and vowel
production: A comparison of 11 tests of articulation and phonology. Language, Speech and Hearing
Services in the Schools, 41, 488-503.
Ertmer, D. (2011). Assessing speech intelligibility in children with hearing loss: Toward revitalizing a
valuable clinical tool. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in the Schools, 42, 52-58.
Hodson, B. (2011). Enhancing phonological patterns of young children with highly unintelligible speech.
ASHA Leader.http://www.asha.org/Publications/leader/2011/110405/Enhancing-Phonological-
Patterns-of-Young-Children-With-Highly-Unintelligible-Speech.htm
Hodson, B., & Paden, E. (1983). Targeting intelligible speech. San Diego: College Hill Press.
Johnson, Weston, Bain (2004). An objective and time efficient method for determining severity of
childhood speech delay. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 13, 55-65.
Locke, J. (1980). The inference of speech perception in the phonologically disordered child, Parts I and
II. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 45, 431-446.
Lof, G. (2002). Two comments on this assessment series. American Journal of Speech-Language
Pathology, 11, 255-256.
Lof, G., & Watson, M. (2008). A nationwide survey of non-speech oral motor exercise use:
Implications for evidence-based practice. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 39, 392-
407.
Lof, G., & Watson, M. (2010). Five reasons why nonspeech oral-motor exercises do not work.
Perspectives in Language and Learning, 11.109-117.
McLeod, S., & Hand, L. (1991). Single word test of consonant clusters. http://athene.riv.csu.edu.au/
~smcleod/Consonantclustertest.pdf
McLeod, S., Harrison, L., & McCormack, J. (2012). The intelligibility in context scale: Validity and
reliability of a subjective rating measure. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 55,
648-656.
McLeod, S., van Doorn, J., & Reed, V. (2001). Normal acquisition of consonant clusters. American
Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 10, 99-110.
Miccio, A. (2005). A treatment program for enhancing stimulability. In A. Kamhi & K. Pollock (Eds.),
Phonological disorders in children: Clinical decision making in assessment and intervention.
Baltimore: Brooks.
Miccio, A., & Elbert, M. (1996). Enhancing stimulability: A treatment program. Journal of
Communication Disorders, 29, 335-351.

Page 20
Morrison, J. & Shriberg, L. (1992). Articulation testing versus conversational speech sampling. Journal
of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 259–273.
Powell, T. (1991). Planning for phonological generalization: An approach to treatment target selection.
American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 1, 21-27.
Powell. T. (1995). A clinical screening procedure for assessing consonant cluster production. American
Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 4, 59-65.: http://ajslp.asha.org/cgi/reprint/4/1/59
Skahan, S., Watson, M., & Lof, G. (2007). Speech-language pathologists' assessment practices for
children with suspected speech sound disorders: Results of a national survey. American Journal of
Speech-Language Pathology, 16, 246-259.
Shriberg., L.D. (1993). Four new speech and prosody-voice measures for genetics research and other
studies in developmental phonological disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 105-
140.
Shriberg, L., & Kent, R (2013). Clinical Phonetics (4th ed). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Smit, A. B. (1993). Phonologic error distributions in the Iowa-Nebraska articulation norms project:
Word-initial consonant clusters. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 931–947.
Tyler, A. (2005). Promoting generalization: Selecting, scheduling, and integrating goals. In A. Kamhi &
K. Pollock (Eds.), Phonological disorders in children: Clinical decision making in assessment and
intervention. Baltimore: Brooks.
Tyler, A. (2008). What works: Evidence-based intervention for children with speech sound disorders.
Seminars in Speech and Language, 29, 320-330.
Watson, M., & Lof, G. (2008). What we know about nonspeech oral motor exercises. Seminars in
Speech and Language, 29, 320-330.
Williams, A., McLeod, S., & McCauley, R. (2010). Interventions for speech sound disorders in
children. Baltimore: Brooks

© 2013 Gregory L. Lof

Page 21
SCHEMATIC OF MODIFIED CYCLES APPROACH

PATTERN 1

Target Sound 1
WEEK 1
Target Sound 2

PATTERN 2

Target Sound 1

WEEK 2
Target Sound 2

PATTERN 3

Target Sound 1
WEEK 3
Target Sound 2

Page 22
Phonetic Inventory
Place-Manner-Voicing
Place a “9“ in the box if the sound is ever produced in the sample.
Place a “{“ in the box if the sound did not have an opportunity to be produced in the sample.

Place of Articulation
Manner Bilabial Labialdental Interdental Alveolar Palatal Velar Glottal

Voicing -V +V -V +V -V +V -V +V -V +V -V +V -V

p b t d k g
Stop

f v ‘ ; s z c x h
Fricative

. J
Affricate

m n A
Nasal

l R
Liquid

w y
Glide

Page 23
Client’s Name:
Consonant Cluster Date:

Pattern Analysis Clinician’s Name:

Segments: p b t d k g f v ‘ ; s c m n a l r w pr tr kr pl kw
Singleton C:
Word Initial
1 2 3
C + /w/

4 5 6 7 8 9
C + /y/

10 11 12 13 14 15
C + /l/

16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24
C + /r/

Page 24
25 26 27 28 29 15 3 30 31 32 33 34
/s/+ C

Word Final
35 36 37
/s/ + C

38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50
C + /s,z/

51 52 53 54
/l/ + C

55 56 57 58 59 60
/r/+ C

61 62 63 64
[+ nasal] + C

Powell, T.W. (1995). A clinical screening procedure for assessing consonant cluster production. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 4, 59-65.
Words for Assessing Consonant Cluster Production
1. twin tw 33. splash spl
2. queen kw 34. squirrel skw
3. sweep sw 35. wasp sp
4. pew py 36. west st
5. beauty by 37. ask sk
6. cue ky 38. caps ps
7. few fy 39. cabs bz
8. view vy 40. cats ts
9. music my 41. kids dz
10. play pl 42. box ks
11. blue bl 43. dogs gz
12. clay kl 44. caves vz
13. glue gl 45. bathes ;z
14. fly fl 46. combs mz
15. sleep sl 47. cans nz
16. pray pr 48. kings az
17. bread br 49. calls lz
18. tree tr 50. cars rz
19. drum dr 51. belt lt
20. cry kr 52. cold ld
21. grow gr 53. milk lk
22. fry fr 54. wolf lf
23. three ‘r 55. sharp rp
24. shrub cr 56. cart rt
25. spy sp 57. card rd
26. stay st 58. work rk
27. sky sk 59. warm rm
28. smell sm 60. barn rn
29. snw sn 61. camp mp
30. spray spr 62. tent nt
31. stray str 63. hand nd
32. screw skr 64. ink ak
LOF:Consonant ClusterAssessment Matrix

Page 25
Helpful Books Related to Speech-Sound Disorders

Velleman, S. (2003). Childhood apraxia of Hall, P., Jordan, L., & Robin, D. (2007). Hodson, B.W. (2007). Evaluating and
speech resource guide. Thomson Delmar Developmental apraxia of speech: Theory enhancing Children’s phonological systems:
Learning. and clinical practice (2nd ed.). Pro-Ed. Research and theory to practice. Thinking
Publications.

Williams, A. L. (2003). Speech disorders Smit, A.B. (2004). Speech sound disorders: Secord, W., Boyce, S., Donohue, J., Fox,
resource guide for preschool children. Resource guide for school-age children and R., & Shine, R. (2007). Eliciting sounds:
Thomson Delmar Learning. adults. Thomson Delmar Learning. Techniques and strategies for clinicians.
Thomson Delmar Learning.

Bliele, K. (2007). The late eight. Plural Paul, R., & Flipsen, P. (2010). Speech Kamhi, A. G., & Pollock, K. E. (2005).
Publishing. sound disorders in children. Plural Phonological disorders in children: Clinical
Publishing. decision making in assessment and
intervention. Brooks.

Ruscello, D. (2008). Treating articulation McLeod, S. (2007). The international guide Bowen, C. (2009). Children’s speech sound
and phonological disorders in children. to speech acquisition. Thomson Delmar disorders. Wiley-Blackwell
Mosby Elsevier. Learning.

Peterson-Falzone, S., Trost-Cardamone, Williams, A.L., McLeod, S., & McCauley, R. Caruso, A., & Strand, E. (1999). Clinical
J., Karnell, M., & Hardin-Jones, M. (2006). (2010). Interventions for speech sound management of motor speech disorders in
The clinician’s guide to treating cleft palate disorders in children. Brooks Publishing. children. Theime
speech. Mosby Elsevier.

Page 26

You might also like