Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 3

Current Biology

Dispatches

Fear: It’s All in Your Line of Sight


Melis Yilmaz and Andrew D. Huberman*
Department of Neurobiology, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA 94305, USA
*Correspondence: adh1@stanford.edu
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2019.10.008

The brain circuits that create our sense of fear rely on ancient ‘hard-wired’ components of the limbic
system, but also use sensory processing to determine what we become afraid of. A new study shows
that, when viewing of simple oriented line stimuli is coupled with aversive experiences, neurons in
primary visual cortex rapidly alter their responses in a manner that indicates the line stimuli become a
source of fear.

Fear is an emotional state of anxiety those stimuli are paired with aversive the non-conditioned stimulus (NS)
that occurs in response to a perceived experiences. Given that V1 resides and were paired with a juice reward.
threat and that can trigger a variety of well outside the limbic system, Like humans, monkeys can be
defensive reactions [1–3]. Because fear but early in the hierarchical processing stubborn about performing tasks, so
can be attached to stimuli of various stream of objects and their locations, the juice reward was needed to motivate
kinds, it can serve either adaptive or these new findings have important the monkeys to stay focused on the
maladaptive roles. For example, it can implications for understanding how stimuli. Eye-blinking is a well-
promote avoidance and escape from generic fear states created in the established response that monkeys
genuinely threatening situations or, limbic pathways are attached to and humans show when anticipating
when attached to innocuous stimuli, different types of life events, and an aversive air-puff. The authors
can promote unnecessary stress. Given the stability of those associations measured the eye-blink responses
that maladaptive fear is a central over time. during the interval between the
component of many debilitating To assess how aversive experiences stimulus delivery and the air-puff or
psychiatric and mental conditions, such impact sensory processing of particular juice delivery as the main assessment
as post-traumatic stress disorder stimuli, Li et al. [5] focused on the visual of fear-associated learning. The
(PTSD) and major depression, system — vision being the sensory conditioning lasted for 4–6 days. At
understanding how the brain labels modality humans rely on most to that point the authors switched the
particular sensory events as ‘fearful’ navigate the world and survive. The CS and NS orientations and repeated
is of paramount importance. The authors recorded from V1 neurons in conditioning with the reversed
study of the brain circuits controlling adult macaque monkeys using orientations to control for any
fear has largely centered on the role of implanted microelectrode arrays. orientation-specific effects. They
the limbic system: a collection of Macaque monkeys are an Old-World recorded V1 responses to the gratings
ancient neural circuits that reside primate species that has a visual in the absence of the air puff before and
deep beneath the neocortex and system very similar to that of humans, after conditioning sessions every day to
that include the amygdala complex, including trichromatic color vision, high assess the effect of conditioning in
the stria terminalis and various acuity and goal-directed eye neural responses and fear extinction,
hypothalamic nuclei [4]. A hallmark movements. A hallmark of V1 neurons is respectively.
feature of these circuits is their ability their orientation selectivity: many V1 Using this approach, Li et al. [5] found
to activate the sympathetic branch neurons fire best when the animal is that V1 neurons had enhancements
of the autonomic nervous system, viewing particular angles of light or dark in their firing rate in response to the
which relies on neural circuits and ‘line stimuli’ [6]. After they obtained orientation that had been paired with
hormonal pathways such as adrenalin stable responses from a V1 cell and the air-puff (CS) during the conditioning
secretion to increase overall alertness recorded its ‘preferred orientation’, the sessions, compared to the pre-
and bias us for action. What has been authors introduced a fear-conditioning conditioning sessions. This was
less clear is how the brain decides paradigm in which oriented gratings specific to the CS and did not occur
which particular sensory stimuli to tilted to one side (rightward or leftward in trials with NS–juice pairings
become fearful of and when that from the vertical) were paired with an (Figure 1). In other words, the air puff/
process occurs during the fear- aversive air-puff delivered to the face of oriented line stimulus pairing
learning process. As they report in the monkey. In this configuration the potentiated the neuron’s response to
this issue of Current Biology, Li et al. [5] gratings served as the conditioned the CS orientation. This enhancement,
have discovered that the neurons in stimulus (CS) and the air puff served as referred to by the authors as the
primary visual cortex (V1) that fire the unconditioned stimulus (US). ‘fear-related’ signal, emerged only
action potentials to simple oriented Gratings tilted to the opposite side after a few trials of pairings
line stimuli alter their responses when of the conditioned stimulus served as during the conditioning session and

R1232 Current Biology 29, R1224–R1251, December 2, 2019 ª 2019 Elsevier Ltd.
Current Biology

Dispatches

remained for a full 20 presentations Conditioned Unconditioned V1 neuron


of the CS gratings when the stimulus stimulus response
air puff was removed after
conditioning. The signals diminished
quickly after that. A
Interestingly the responses to the
CS were potentiated regardless of
whether or not it was the neuron’s
preferred orientation. This also
+ (None)

impacted the neuron’s orientation-


tuning properties. For example, if the
CS orientation was the neuron’s
preferred orientation, the neuron B Reward
became even more responsive to it —
that is, made it more sharply tuned.
If the CS was the un-preferred
orientation, however, the neuron still
+
became more responsive to that
orientation compared to the pre-test
condition, and in doing so, made
the neuron relatively less well tuned to C Aversive
its preferred orientation. This is
remarkable as it means that fear-related
signals can be attached to any stimulus,
regardless of a neuron’s pre-fear tuning
+
properties.
The fear-related enhancement
observed by Li et al. [5] was Current Biology

surprisingly fast, appearing as early


as 30 milliseconds after the stimulus Figure 1. Preferential enhancement of V1 responses to oriented gratings when coupled with
aversive but not rewarding experiences.
onset. This raises important (A) Illustration showing typical V1 responses to an oriented grating stimulus. (B) V1 neuron response to the
questions about the possible sources same grating is unchanged when coupled with a reward. (C) V1 neuron responses are enhanced when
of V1 neuron modulation in this coupled with an aversive stimulus (illustrated with the skull).
context. The amygdala, a central
component of subcortical limbic
circuitry involved in threat detection, the CS and NS orientations were what they saw at the time the learning
projects to and synapses in V1 [7–9]. masked. They did that by adding occurred.
V1 also receives feedback projections horizontally oriented drifting gratings Neurons in V1 respond to stimuli
from higher visual cortical areas on top of them, making the CS presented within specific regions of
too [10]. However, the activation of appear as a coherently moving ‘plaid’ visual space and thereby exhibit a
these inputs to V1 occurs with rather than as a set of overlapping ‘visuotopic receptive field’. Li et al. [5]
latencies longer than the mere 30–70 gratings. The presentation of these asked whether the response
milliseconds it took to see V1 neuron plaids was randomly interspersed enhancement they observed was
enhancement [10,11]. Neither with the CS and NS trials during specific to the visuotopic location in
amygdala-feedforward nor cortical- conditioning and did not lead to which the CS (oriented grating) was
feedback signals, therefore, are likely to eye-blink responses. This stimulus presented. In other words, once a V1
be the source of the V1 enhancement did, however, preferentially enhance neuron adopts a threat response to
reported in this paper. Instead, the the neural responses to the a given stimulus — in this case an
authors hypothesized that the plaids containing the CS orientation. oriented line (CS) — does the brain
enhancement is likely due to ‘bottom-up This is noteworthy because it means care about where a threat is in the
processing’ carried out in V1 itself. If that a given stimulus is ‘tagged’ or world or merely that it is present
they are correct, that means that ‘learned’ as fearful even if the someplace? They trained the
primary sensory areas can acquire fear- observer (in this case, the monkey) is monkeys with a CS shown at a
associated responses independently of unaware of the presence of a stimulus different location than the receptive
the classical fear pathways of the limbic inside a masking stimulus. Put field of the neuron they were
system. differently, neurons in V1 change their recording from (that is, outside the
To test that hypothesis, Li et al. [5] responses to visual stimuli that are neurons so-called ‘classical receptive
examined whether the enhancement paired with a bad experience, even if the field’). After training was complete,
of signals in V1 occurred even when animals do not consciously perceive they recorded responses of the V1

Current Biology 29, R1224–R1251, December 2, 2019 R1233


Current Biology

Dispatches

neuron to the CS orientation that was the activities of pulvinar during 3. Anderson, D.J., and Adolphs, R. (2014). A
framework for studying emotions across
presented within their receptive fields. fear conditioning to test its role in species. Cell 157, 187–200.
The fear-related enhancement was the fear-related signals observed by Li
not observed and animals did not show et al. [5]. 4. Adolphs, R. (2013). The biology of fear. Curr.
Biol. 23, R79–R93.
eye-blink responses to the CS Our behavioral repertoire is
presented in a different location, dependent on integration of external 5. Li, Z., Yan, A., Guo, K., and Li, W. (2019). Fear-
indicating that fear learning is specific to sensory events with our internal related signals in the primary visual cortex.
Curr. Biol. 29, 4078–4083.
the location in which the CS is states. Where and when this
presented and reinforcing the idea that integration occurs in the brain 6. Hubel, D.H., and Wiesel, T.N. (1959).
Receptive fields of single neurones in
individual neurons, rather than entire remains largely unknown. The new the cat’s striate cortex. J. Physiol. 148,
maps of neurons or regions of V1, paper from Li et al. [5] suggests that 574–591.
change their firing properties in such integration occurs earlier than
7. Amaral, D.G., Behniea, H., and Kelly, J.L.
response to the CS (lines)–US (air previously thought along the (2003). Topographic organization of
puff) pairings. A stimulus is only tagged hierarchy of visual processing circuits, projections from the amygdala to the visual
in V1 if it appears in the same visual and that it can impact neural cortex in the macaque monkey. Neuroscience
118, 1099–1120.
location as the location where it had processing of features as simple as
previously been paired with an aversive oriented lines. In other words, sensory 8. Freese, J.L., and Amaral, D.G. (2005). The
organization of projections from the amygdala
outcome. This suggests that the events can arrive in our awareness to visual cortical areas TE and V1 in the
plasticity mechanisms underlying this already pre-labeled as ‘potential macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 486,
form of fear learning involve synaptic threats’ or ‘neutral’. Given that fast- 295–317.
specificity and/or intrinsic properties of fear-tagging of what we see, the 9. Silverstein, D.N., and Ingvar, M. (2015). A
neurons rather than, say, broad scale question arises: can we ever truly multi-pathway hypothesis for human visual
neuromodulation. separate our sensory perceptions fear signaling. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 9, 101.

Arguably one of the most surprising from emotions? Studies of emotional 10. Buffalo, E.A., Fries, P., Landman, R., Liang, H.,
findings of Li et al. [5] is the extremely and sensory processing have and Desimone, R. (2010). A backward
progression of attentional effects in the ventral
short latency with which fear- traditionally come from non-overlapping stream. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 107,
associated response modulation is fields of neuroscience. As a 361–365.
observed in V1 neurons. While consequence, we now understand a
nisxor, L., van der Togt, C., Pennartz,
11. Sta
these latencies argue against the great deal about how physical C.M.A., and Roelfsema, P.R. (2013). A unified
involvement of feedback signals stimulus features like direction and selection signal for attention and reward in
primary visual cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
from the cortex or the amygdala [12–14], orientation are encoded, but still
USA 110, 9136–9141.
the data presented were not sufficient know quite little about how our
to rule those influences out. More internal states shape those responses. 12. Paton, J.J., Belova, M.A., Morrison, S.E., and
Salzman, C.D. (2006). The primate amygdala
definitive conclusions about the The current study adds strong represents the positive and negative value
source of this fast-non-visual evidence to the idea that emotional of visual stimuli during learning. Nature 439,
modulation of V1 will likely be context impacts sensory perception 865.
available when the current paradigm very fast, even before that 13. Belova, M.A., Paton, J.J., and Salzman, C.D.
is combined with simultaneous information reaches our awareness. (2008). Moment-to-moment tracking of state
value in the amygdala. J. Neurosci. 28, 10023–
recordings from cells in V1, amygdala This has major implications for how
10030.
and other cortical areas. Another we approach the study of emotion
mechanism brought up by the and neural circuits involved in 14. Peck, C.J., Lau, B., and Salzman, C.D. (2013).
The primate amygdala combines information
authors as a potential source of this mental disorders involving fear and about space and value. Nat. Neurosci. 16,
fast-non-visual modulation is feed- anxiety. They also suggest it may 340–348.
forward signals from the pulvinar, a be time to shift our efforts away from
15. Zhou, N.A., Maire, P.S., Masterson, S.P., and
thalamic structure known to studying emotions and sensory Bickford, M.E. (2017). The mouse pulvinar
modulate cortical function and to perception as separate processes nucleus: Organization of the tectorecipient
zones. Vis. Neurosci. 34, E011.
incorporate contextual cues such as and instead establish more
the overall state of the animal, speed paradigms that resemble events 16. Zhou, N., Masterson, S.P., Damron, J.K.,
of locomotion and so on [15,16]. that trigger fear states in real life. Guido, W., and Bickford, M.E. (2018). The
mouse pulvinar nucleus links the lateral
Previous studies showed extrastriate cortex, striatum, and amygdala.
that excitation of pulvinar neurons J. Neurosci. 38, 347–362.
REFERENCES
causes a fast enhancement of V1
17. Purushothaman, G., Marion, R., Li, K., and
signals in the early phase of their 1. LeDoux, J.E., and Pine, D.S. (2016). Using Casagrande, V.A. (2012). Gating and control of
responses [17]. The pulvinar has neuroscience to help understand fear and primary visual cortex by pulvinar. Nat.
anxiety: a two-system framework. Am. J. Neurosci. 15, 905–912.
also been implicated in fast,
Psych. 173, 1083–1093.
innate defensive behaviors such as 18. Wei, P., Liu, N., Zhang, Z., Liu, X., Tang, Y., He,
freezing in response to a looming 2. Yilmaz, M., and Meister, M. (2013). Rapid X., Wu, B., Zhou, Z., Liu, Y., and Li, J. (2015).
innate defensive responses of mice to Processing of visually evoked innate fear by a
stimulus in rodents [18]. Future looming visual stimuli. Curr. Biol. 23, 2011– non-canonical thalamic pathway. Nat. Comm.
studies should focus on manipulating 2015. 6, 6756.

R1234 Current Biology 29, R1224–R1251, December 2, 2019

You might also like