Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 40

Cognitive Neuropsychology

ISSN: 0264-3294 (Print) 1464-0627 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pcgn20

Treatment of irregular word spelling in


developmental surface dysgraphia

Ruth Brunsdon , Max Coltheart & Lyndsey Nickels

To cite this article: Ruth Brunsdon , Max Coltheart & Lyndsey Nickels (2005) Treatment of
irregular word spelling in developmental surface dysgraphia, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22:2,
213-251, DOI: 10.1080/02643290442000077

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290442000077

Published online: 03 Feb 2007.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 908

View related articles

Citing articles: 47 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=pcgn20
COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2), 213–251

TREATMENT OF IRREGULAR WORD SPELLING IN


DEVELOPMENTAL SURFACE DYSGRAPHIA

Ruth Brunsdon
Macquarie University and Children’s Hospital at Westmead, Sydney, Australia

Max Coltheart and Lyndsey Nickels


Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia

An increasing number of cognitive neuropsychological treatment studies of acquired dysgraphia have


been published in recent years, but to our knowledge there are no corresponding studies of develop-
mental dysgraphia. This paper reports a cognitive neuropsychological treatment programme
designed for a child with developmental surface dysgraphia. The treatment aim was to improve func-
tioning of the orthographic output lexicon, and so treatment methods targeted irregular word
spelling. Treatment methods were based on previous successful treatments employed in cases of adult
acquired surface dysgraphia (Behrmann, 1987; De Partz, Seron, & Van der Linden, 1992; Weekes &
Coltheart, 1996). Results showed a significant treatment effect for both spelling and reading of irreg-
ular words that was largely stable over time and that generalised partially to spelling of untreated
irregular words. Homophone words were not treated but some aspects of homophone reading and
spelling also improved, though homophone confusion errors remained. Comparison of treatment
effectiveness with and without mnemonics suggested that the mnemonic cue itself was not necessary
to achieve treatment success for irregular word spelling. Analyses revealed that untreated irregular
words whose spellings became correct as a result of treatment generalisation were those whose origi-
nal misspellings were closest to being correct prior to treatment. Results also provided preliminary
evidence that the mechanism underlying treatment generalisation involved improved access to
orthographic representations, resulting in an increased tendency to employ orthography for spelling
attempts and reduced reliance on phoneme to grapheme conversion.

INTRODUCTION has neglected investigation of treatment options


(e.g., Bub & Kertesz, 1982; Ellis, 1988; Friedman
In the field of cognitive neuropsychology, dys- & Alexander, 1990; Katz & Deser, 1991; Miceli &
graphia has received far less research attention Silveri, 1985; Patterson, 1988; Rapcsak, 1997;
than dyslexia. In addition, most research has Robinson & Weekes, 1995; Roeltgen & Heilman,
focused on theoretical aspects of dysgraphia and 1985; Roeltgen & Tucker, 1988; Romani, Ward,

Correspondence should be addressed to Ruth Brunsdon, Rehabilitation Department, Children’s Hospital at Westmead, Locked
Bag 4001, Westmead, NSW 2145, Sydney, Australia (Email: ruthb2@chw.edu.au).
The authors are grateful to Alan Taylor for statistical advice. We are indebted to our client MC and his family for their enthusi-
astic participation throughout the project. Lyndsey Nickels was supported by an Australian Research Council QEII Fellowship and
Max Coltheart by an Australian Research Council Federation Fellowship during the preparation of this paper.
The authors also wish to thank Assistant Professor Argye Hillis and two anonymous reviewers for their very helpful comments
on a previous version of this paper.

© 2005 Psychology Press Ltd 213


http://www.tandf.co.uk/journals/pp/02643294.html DOI:10.1080/02643290442000077
BRUNDSON, COLTHEART, NICKELS

& Olson, 1999; Temple, 1988, 1990; Weekes, and is made available for the process of writing via
1994). However, a small but growing number of the graphemic output buffer (see Figure 1, route
treatment studies for acquired dysgraphia have been shown by arrows 1, 2, 3, 4). The graphemic output
published (e.g., Beeson, 1999; Beeson, Hirsch, & buffer is proposed to store ‘whole-word’ represen-
Rewega, 2002; Behrmann & Byng, 1992; tations temporarily while the physical process of
Carlomagno & Parlato, 1989; Conway et al., 1998; writing (or oral spelling) is completed. In contrast,
De Partz et al., 1992; Hatfield, 1983; Hillis & the sublexical spelling procedure (see Figure 1,
Caramazza, 1986; Luzzatti, Colombo, Frustaci, & route shown by arrows 5, 6, 7, 8) allows spelling of
Vitolo, 2000; Pound, 1996; Rapp & Kane, 2002; unfamiliar words and nonwords, and regularly
Seron, Deloche, Moulard, & Rousselle, 1980), but spelled familiar words, by applying rule-based
we are unaware of any published single case treat- phoneme to grapheme conversion. Some authors
ment studies of developmental dysgraphia. The have also proposed a third ‘lexical nonsemantic’
current paper reports on the treatment of spelling in spelling route that links the phonological input
a 12-year-old child with developmental surface lexicon to the orthographic output lexicon via the
dysgraphia and developmental surface dyslexia, and phonological output lexicon bypassing the seman-
is theoretically based on cognitive neuropsychologi- tic system (see Figure 1, route shown by arrows 1,
cal dual-route models of reading and spelling 9, 10, 4) (Ellis & Young, 1988; Patterson, 1986).1
(Coltheart, 1987; Ellis, 1982; Ellis & Young, 1988; Post-graphemic processing includes conversion of
Patterson & Shewell, 1987). abstract graphemic representations to visual letter
shapes or letter stroke features (often referred to as
allographic conversion) (Goodman & Caramazza,
Cognitive neuropsychological dual-route
1986a, 1986b; Rapp & Caramazza, 1997), and
models of reading and spelling
more peripheral writing includes coordination of
Cognitive neuropsychological dual-route models motor processes allowing sequences of strokes to
of reading and spelling (see Figure 1) propose two be formed into each written letter (Goodman &
main processing routes for spelling to dictation Caramazza, 1986a, 1986b).
and reading aloud: the lexical and sublexical pro-
cessing routes.
Reading
Most theories of reading also make the distinction
Spelling
between lexical and sublexical processing routes
For spelling to dictation (i.e., converting a spoken (Coltheart, 1985, 1987; Coltheart, Curtis, Atkins, &
stimulus into its written form), the lexical proce- Haller, 1993; Coltheart, Rastle, Perry, Langdon, &
dure relies on the retrieval of whole-word ortho- Ziegler, 2001; Ellis, 1982; Ellis & Young, 1988; but
graphic information from the orthographic output also see Seidenberg & McClelland, 1989; Plaut,
lexicon and allows efficient spelling of familiar Seidenberg, McClelland, & Patterson, 1996, for
words (both regular and irregular). Skilled readers discussion of alternative theoretical viewpoints).
first recognise the heard word through activation The lexical reading route relies on visual word recog-
of its entry in the phonological input lexicon, and nition through access to an internal store of learned
then access its associated meaning from the familiar words (see Figure 1, route 13, 14, 12, 11)
semantic system. Finally, the word’s written form and the sublexical reading procedure involves rule-
is retrieved from the orthographic output lexicon based grapheme to phoneme conversion, which

1
There are also several other possible routes that could be employed for spelling to dictation, including: direct route through
lexical phonology and then phoneme-to-grapheme conversion (shown by arrows 1, 9, 11, 7, 8); lexical phonological route via the
semantic system and then grapheme-to-phoneme conversion (shown by arrows 1, 2, 12, 11, 7, 8); lexical route via the semantic
system and then output orthography (shown by arrows 1, 2, 12, 10, 4).

214 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2)


TREATMENT OF SURFACE DYSGRAPHIA

Figure 1. Cognitive neuropsychological dual-route model of reading and spelling (adapted from Ellis & Young, 1988).

allows the skilled reader to ‘sound-out’ unfamiliar Rapcsak & Beeson, 2000). In brief, cases of both
words and nonwords (see Figure 1, route 16, 17). phonological dysgraphia (specific difficulties with
As with spelling, some authors also argue for a lex- nonword spelling, in the context of relatively
ical nonsemantic processing route in reading (e.g., unimpaired word spelling; e.g., Campbell &
Ellis & Young, 1988), which allows for processing Butterworth, 1985; Iribarren, Jarema, & Lecours,
of irregular words without access to their meaning 2001; Marien, Pickut, Engelborghs, Martin, & De
(see Figure 1, route shown by arrows 13, 15, 11). Deyn, 2001; Ogden, 1996; Shallice, 1981;
Snowling, Stackhouse, & Rack, 1986) and surface
dysgraphia (difficulties with irregular word spelling
Patterns of impairment in dysgraphia
in the context of relatively intact nonword spelling;
Reported cases of dysgraphia vary enormously in e.g., Behrmann & Bub, 1992; Hanley, Hastie, &
terms of the nature of the impairment and the Kay, 1992; Roeltgen & Blaskey, 1992; Temple, 1985,
resultant functional spelling difficulties (for a con- 1986) have been reported. Other cases of dysgraphia
cise summary of the nature of dysgraphia, see have been reported with specific impairments

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2) 215


BRUNDSON, COLTHEART, NICKELS

(single or multiple) involving other components of in order to support lexical processing, such as
the spelling system such as the graphemic output improving the use of phoneme to grapheme con-
buffer (Bonazzi, 1996; Caramazza, Miceli, Villa, version in order to cue whole-word lexical retrieval
& Romani, 1987; Piccirilli, Petrillo, & Poli, 1992; (Hillis & Caramazza, 1994; Hillis Trupe, 1986). A
Posteraro, Zinelli, & Mazzucchi, 1988; Rapp & more recent study capitalised on residual oral
Kane, 2002; Schiller, Greenhall, Shelton, & spelling skills and employed a compensatory treat-
Caramazza, 2001), post-graphemic processes ment method involving a letter-by-letter oral to
(Goodman & Caramazza, 1986a; Hanley & written spelling relay technique (Mortley, Enderby,
Peters, 2001; Zesiger, Martory, & Mayer, 1997), & Petheram, 2001). The current treatment study
or more peripheral aspects of writing such as selec- has a direct remediation methodology so only those
tion of motor programmes, motor planning, or studies reporting direct remediation of lexical
handwriting (Cubelli, Guiducci, & Consolmagno, impairments will be reviewed, with a focus on three
2000; Destreri, Farina, Alberoni, Pomati, Nichelli, studies particularly relevant to the current study
& Mariani, 2000; Ellis, Young, & Flude, 1987; (Behrmann, 1987; De Partz et al., 1992; Weekes &
Silveri, Misciagna, Leggio, & Molinari, 1997; Coltheart, 1996). Other studies that have employed
Venneri, Cubelli, & Caffarra, 1994). subjects with very severe dysgraphia affecting mul-
tiple components of the writing system and/or
groups of aphasia patients with different underlying
Remediation of lexical spelling impairment
impairments (e.g., Aliminosa et al., 1993; Beeson,
This paper will hereafter focus on the treatment 1999; Beeson et al., 2002; Carlomagno et al., 1994)
of dysgraphia and specifically treatment pro- will not be discussed further here.
grammes designed to treat lexical spelling impair- Behrmann (1987) reports a homophone
ments. Interested readers are also referred to spelling treatment study for CCM, a case of
Rapcsak and Beeson (2000) and/or Beeson and acquired surface dysgraphia without dyslexia.
Hillis (2001) for a recent overview of published The treatment aim was to help CCM link homo-
spelling treatment studies, including those with a phone words with their meanings and to improve
treatment focus on other aspects of spelling. As homophone spelling. During each weekly train-
there are no known published cognitive neuro- ing session CCM was required to examine the
psychological treatment studies of developmental spelling of both words in each homophone pair
dysgraphia, the following review refers solely to and to memorise the difference between them.
studies of the treatment of acquired lexical dys- Each word was presented with a pictorial repre-
graphia in adults. sentation of its meaning. Other training activities
Some treatment studies of lexical spelling included: matching each homophone word to
impairments have focused on direct remediation of its corresponding picture; writing each homo-
impaired functions, while others have focused on phone word when given a pictorial cue; and writ-
the strengthening of compensatory skills. Direct ing each homophone word to dictation.
remediation methods have predominantly aimed to Homework practice included a number of forced-
strengthen representations in the orthographic choice activities such as written word to picture
output lexicon (or improve access to them) and also matching, written homophone naming in response
facilitate lexical-semantic connections (Aliminosa, to picture cues, and sentence completion tasks
McCloskey, Goodman-Schulman, & Sokol, 1993; (e.g., “After Christmas the shops have a big . . .
Beeson, 1999; Beeson et al., 2002; Behrmann, [sale/sail]”). Behrmann conducted four assess-
1987; Carlomagno, Lavarone, & Colombo, 1994; ments prior to treatment to establish stable
De Partz et al., 1992; Rapp & Kane, 2002; Weekes pretherapy performance; however, each stimulus
& Coltheart, 1996). Compensatory techniques are homophone word was only given once, so base-
often targeted at sublexical or phonological skills line stability was not established for individual

216 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2)


TREATMENT OF SURFACE DYSGRAPHIA

treated words.2 Treatment resulted in an im- treatment studies target items are most often
provement in overall homophone spelling (from selected on the basis of incorrect responses at
a total of 49% correct responses at pretest to 67% baseline (i.e., the lower extreme of performance),
at post-test); however, there was not a reduction and thus regression to the mean must be consid-
in the number of homophone confusions.3 In ered as a possible factor in any increase in perfor-
contrast, there was a significant decrease—from mance on subsequent assessments. It is therefore
36% (35/70) to 18% (8/45)—in the number of important for researchers to statistically separate
nonword errors. In sum, it seems that treatment those improvements related directly to treatment
resulted in an improvement in spelling of homo- from those resulting from regression to the mean.
phone words but not in assigning each homo- Weekes and Coltheart (1996) employed a sim-
phone to the correct semantic context. There was ilar technique to Behrmann (1987) for treatment
no generalisation to untreated homophone pairs of NW’s difficulties with homophone spelling.
(though only 20 untreated homophone words NW had acquired his surface dysgraphia following
were evaluated) but generalisation to a list of a traumatic brain injury. During treatment, words
untreated irregular words was apparent. were always presented in their homophone pairs
Behrmann (1987) postulates that untreated hom- and with a pictorial mnemonic. NW was asked to
ophone spelling failed to improve because homo- examine the spelling of each word in the homo-
phones require word-specific training so, unlike phone pair, to notice the differences in their
irregular words, do not benefit as a result of gen- spelling, and to associate the mnemonic cue with
eral improvements in lexical processing. Finally, a the appropriate spelling. Treatment results indi-
group of 68 untreated homophone words that cated overall improvement in homophone spelling
were written correctly pretreatment were evalu- of target words (29% → 74%) but no generalisa-
ated post-treatment and the total number spelled tion to spelling of untreated homophone words,
correct actually decreased from 100% to 85%. replicating the findings of Behrmann (1987).
Given that each homophone word was only Weekes and Coltheart do not discuss the nature of
administered once pretreatment, this may simply spelling errors at baseline or change in error type
represent regression to the mean. The statistical with treatment.
phenomenon of regression to the mean refers to De Partz et al. (1992) targeted high-frequency
the tendency for lower pretest scores to be higher word spelling in their treatment of LP, a French-
on post-test and higher pretest scores to be lower speaking patient with acquired surface dysgraphia.
on the post-test. That is, the tendency for scores For lexical treatment LP was first familiarised
at the extremes to revert toward mean levels upon with a general visual imagery technique, then an
repeated testing (Heiman, 2001; Kazdin, 1982; imagery strategy was used to reteach the writing of
Shaughnessy & Zechmeister, 1990). Regression words. The target set of words consisted predomi-
to the mean effects are not often evaluated in nantly of words spelled incorrectly by LP from a
treatment studies, but in fact should be carefully list of high-frequency written words. For each tar-
considered when evaluating treatment outcome, get word a mnemonic was devised that was both
particularly when selection of items is not ran- semantically related to the word and also linked
dom and is based on extremes of performance. In specifically to misspelt letters. For example, the

2
Berhmann (1987) assessed a total of 138 homophone words (69 pairs) prior to treatment. However, only 34 words were given
at pretest one, 35 at pretest two, 34 at pretest 3, and 35 at pretest 4—with 50%, 46%, 59%, and 43% correct responses respectively.
Behrmann argues that because performance was stable over the four assessments a stable baseline was established. However, each
individual homophone word was only assessed once prior to treatment, so word-specific effects were not measured at baseline.
3
The overall proportion of homophone confusion errors increased from 57% to 82% but absolute number barely changed (i.e., a
change from 40/70 errors at pretest to 37/45 at post-test).

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2) 217


BRUNDSON, COLTHEART, NICKELS

mnemonic devised for a misspelling of pathologie Byng (1989) and Weekes and Coltheart (1996),
as patologie was a drawing of a hospital bed (i.e., a whose treatments focused on irregular word read-
semantically related cue) and was incorporated into ing, found generalisation to the reading of
the letter h (i.e., the defective letter) on the training untreated irregular words. Scott and Byng (1989)
card. In each therapy session LP was required to focused on the treatment of homophone reading,
copy the word with the drawing, reproduce the and results indicated mixed generalisation results
written word and drawing from memory after a with improvement of untreated items on one task, a
delay of 10 seconds, and finally reproduce the writ- forced-choice sentence discrimination task, but not
ten word and drawing in response to the spoken on the more difficult homophone definition task.
word. Treatment resulted in a dramatic improve- In sum, results of lexical treatment studies pro-
ment in trained words (0% → 91%) and also a vide evidence that treatment generalisation can
smaller but significant improvement in untrained occur for untreated irregular words in both reading
words (0% → 30%). De Partz et al. also used the and spelling. In contrast, results suggest that
Beauvois and Derouesne (1981) list of irregular, untreated homophone words do not consistently
regular, and ambiguous words as a control list that benefit from treatment generalisation.
was not treated and there was no generalisation of The reason for treatment generalisation has not
treatment to this control list.4 It is important to been unequivocally established. Weekes and
note that these generalisation results cannot be Coltheart (1996), while discussing generalisation
directly compared with those of Behrmann (1987) in surface dyslexia treatment, propose two effects
or Weekes and Coltheart (1996) as the target from item specific training. They propose that:
words were not restricted to homophones but were
one must be word-specific (since treated words
simply a mixed list of high-frequency written
benefit more than untreated words) and the other
words. De Partz et al. (1992) conducted a second
must be more general (since untreated words
treatment study that contrasted the effectiveness of
benefit). The specific effect we take to be . . .
the mnemonic cue with a purely didactic verbal
restoration of the ability to use local word-repre-
relearning technique (simple copying of the word
sentations in the visual word recognition system;
with no mnemonic). Results indicated a significant
restoration that occurs only for words that are
therapy advantage for the imagery technique.
part of the treatment programme. We speculate
that the general effect has to do with the general
Generalisation of treatment effects procedure by which entries in the visual word
to untreated stimuli recognition system are accessed; if this procedure
is damaged in these patients, and if treatment
The finding of treatment generalisation to un-
improves how well the procedure works, all
treated items in cognitive neuropsychological treat-
words, not just treated words will benefit (p. 305).
ment studies is now well established. Lexical
treatment studies of acquired dysgraphia that have The same could be argued to account for findings
employed treatment methods focused on homo- of generalisation in treatment of surface dysgraphia.
phone spelling have demonstrated treatment gener- That is, improvements in spelling of untreated
alisation to spelling of untreated irregular words irregular words could simply represent treatment
(Behrmann, 1987), but no treatment generalisation effects on the general process by which entries in the
to spelling of untreated homophones (Behrmann, orthographic output lexicon are retrieved. So, in
1987; Weekes & Coltheart, 1996). There is a similar addition to individual word restoration, general
trend in results for surface dyslexia. Coltheart and retrieval from the orthographic output lexicon is

4
This discrepancy may have related to differences in frequency between the two lists as LP’s spelling was significantly affected by
word frequency in pretreatment testing (it is not clearly indicated whether the untrained word list and the control lists were matched).

218 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2)


TREATMENT OF SURFACE DYSGRAPHIA

improved, so both treated words and untreated treatment effects over time, generalisation of treat-
words benefit, but treated words more so. With ment to spelling of untreated stimuli (irregular and
regard to homophones, Behrmann (1987) speculates homophone words), and also generalisation of
that they require word-specific training so do not treatment to reading performance.
benefit from this more general effect on the ortho-
graphic output lexicon.
Weekes and Coltheart (1996) have proposed CASE HISTORY
that, in treatment of acquired reading impairments,
some untreated irregular words may benefit more MC, a 12-year-old boy, was in his first year of high
than others, depending on their level of pretreat- school at the time of treatment. He was referred by
ment activation and whether the words were avail- his mother for assessment and intervention. There
able to the patient premorbidly or not.5 More had been concerns about his progress in reading
specifically, they have proposed that generalisation and spelling since early primary school, and as a
occurs for those words with premorbidly available result MC received extra assistance (both through
orthographic representation whose entries (or school and privately organised tuition) in Years 3,
access to them) have been degraded. In contrast, 4, and 5 of primary school. Detailed developmen-
generalisation will not occur for lower-frequency tal history revealed no prenatal or postnatal risk
words that were not premorbidly available to the factors and early developmental milestones were
patient. Therefore, in simple terms, one might the- within normal limits (if not a little advanced).
orise that those words with orthographic entries There was no history of neurological damage or
that are relatively inaccessible or degraded may dysfunction. Intellectual assessment conducted at
be more susceptible to generalisation than words age 7 years indicated superior intellectual skills.
whose orthographic entries are completely absent. There were also no concerns regarding general
Although treatment generalisation effects have cognitive, language, or behavioural development
been clearly documented in previous studies, other than MC’s specific difficulties with reading
researchers have failed to investigate possible and spelling. The following excerpt from a letter
underlying mechanisms in any detail. In contrast, written by MC’s school teacher illustrates this well.
the current treatment study was carefully designed
I have found [MC] to be a perceptive, intelligent
to enable detailed exploration of the reasons why
student who possesses excellent oral skills. He
treatment generalisation occurs. That is, the cur-
organises his thoughts and opinions effectively
rent study investigated which factors or variables
and expresses himself in a clear and confident
predict treatment generalisation through analyses
manner. He is able to listen to presentations
of differences between those untreated words that
and summarise main points [and] he also pos-
improved and those that did not.
sesses an excellent general knowledge of current
This study evaluated a lexical treatment pro-
affairs issues. [He] is, however . . . currently
gramme designed for MC, a child with develop-
reading at a level below his chronological age.
mental surface dyslexia and dysgraphia, and was
Furthermore, he finds it extremely difficult
theoretically based on cognitive neuropsychologi-
expressing himself on paper. Despite his best
cal dual-route theories of spelling. The aim of
efforts, [MC] cannot spell most common words
treatment was to improve the functioning of the
correctly.
orthographic output lexicon and targeted irregular
word spelling. Treatment design enabled an objec- Recent intellectual assessment using the
tive assessment of treatment efficacy, stability of WISC-III (Wechsler, 1991) revealed skills in the

5
For their case study of NW, premorbidly available words were irregular words that NW could define when presented in spoken
form (and were therefore presumed to have been readable by him prior to his acquired brain injury) and premorbidly unavailable
words were low-frequency irregular words that he could not define when presented aurally.

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2) 219


BRUNDSON, COLTHEART, NICKELS

Table 1. Intellectual assessment resultsa


Scaled score Scaled Score Standard score

Information 12 Picture Completion 12 VIQ 106


Similarities 15 Coding 4 PIQ 116
Arithmetic 5 Picture Arrangement 17 FSIQ 112
Vocabulary 11 Block Design 16
Comprehension 12 Object Assembly 13
Digit Span 7 Symbol Search 4
a
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–3rd revision (Wechsler, 1991).

above-average range, though MC’s pattern of sub- Single letter processing


test weaknesses suggested specific difficulties with
MC was able to name (26/26) and sound (24/26)
working memory (see Table 1). On an initial
single letters (errors: e → /i:/, u → /jə/). He was
screening of academic skills (Wide Range
also able to write letters when provided with their
Achievement Test-Revision 3) (Wilkinson, 1993)
names (26/26) or their sounds (26/26).
MC achieved standard scores of 90 for word read-
ing, 75 for word spelling, and 90 for arithmetic.
Of most interest, however, was the striking nature Visual lexical decision
of MC’s errors on the spelling subtest (e.g.,
circle → circel, reasonable → resanerball, surprise → Visual lexical decision was assessed using the
serpries, explain → exsplaen). Nearly all of his errors visual lexical decision task from PALPA (subtest
were phonologically plausible nonword responses, number 27, Kay, Lesser, & Coltheart, 1992). This
which immediately suggested the possibility of task contains an equal number of exception words,
surface dysgraphia with or without dyslexia (read- regular words, pseudohomophones (e.g., wich)
ing errors on this word set were less conclusive).
These preliminary test results and review of MC’s
prose writing prompted further investigation of
reading and spelling skills. Two samples of prose
writing taken from MC’s school exercise books are
shown in Figures 2 and 3 and illustrate other
interesting aspects of MC’s written output includ-
ing incorrect use and/or absence of punctuation
and capitalisation.

FURTHER ASSESSMENT AND


FUNCTIONAL LOCALISATION OF
IMPAIRMENT

Pretreatment testing included assessment of read-


ing, spelling, and phonological skills and also
included a screen of picture naming and semantics.
Assessment results (described below) revealed a
pattern confirming a diagnosis of developmental
surface dyslexia and dysgraphia. Figure 2. Sample of MC’s pretreatment prose writing.

220 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2)


TREATMENT OF SURFACE DYSGRAPHIA

Phonological processing
General phonology skills were assessed using
phonological segmentation of initial and final
sounds (PALPA subtests 16 and 17), word rhyme
judgements-auditory version (PALPA subtest
15), the Children’s Test of Nonword Repetition
(CNRep; Gathercole & Baddeley, 1996) and
Phonological Processing (a phonological
segmentation task) from the NEPSY (Korkman,
Kirk, & Kemp, 1997). MC’s performance on the
PALPA segmentation of sounds tasks was error free.
He also performed well on the other phonological
processing tasks, with scores of 58/60 on PALPA
word rhyme judgements, an age-scaled score of 10
on NEPSY phonological processing, and 36/40 on
nonword repetition (CNRep).

Naming and semantics


Semantic knowledge and naming were screened
using spoken word–picture matching, written
word–picture matching, and spoken picture
Figure 3. Sample of MC’s pretreatment prose writing. naming (PALPA subtests 47, 48, and 53). MC’s
performance on all three tasks was virtually error-
free with scores of 40/40, 40/40, and 39/40
respectively.

and nonhomophonic nonwords (e.g., plit).


Although no normal control data exists for 12- Reading and spelling of regular and
year-olds, comparison with data for a small group irregular words
of normal 9–10-year-olds certainly suggests that MC was first asked to read aloud and write 30 irreg-
MC’s lexical decision skills were below age expec- ular and 30 regular words from the Coltheart and
tations (see Table 2) and that he made more irreg- Leahy (1996) word/nonword test (see Appendix A).
ular (exception word) and pseudohomophonic He was also required to read and spell a list of 308
word errors than normal. monosyllabic irregular words (all with a written

Table 2. Visual lexical decision (PALPA, subtest number 27)


MC age 12 Normal controls age 9 and 10 (n ⫽ 6)
Test Raw scores Mean (SD) Range

Total (n ⫽ 60) 43 55.5 (3.02) 53–59


Regular words (n ⫽ 15) 11 14.7 (0.82) 13–15
Exception words (n ⫽ 15) 9 13.2 (0.75) 12–14
Pseudohomophones (n ⫽ 15) 10 13.7 (0.82) 13–15
Nonhomophonic nonwords (n ⫽ 15) 13 14.0 (1.26) 12–15

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2) 221


BRUNDSON, COLTHEART, NICKELS

frequency greater than 100). The list was obtained For the longer list of 308 irregular words, MC
by removing all homophones, plurals, and past-tense read a total of 210 correctly. A regression analysis
word variations and words with a written frequency including the variables spoken frequency and,
of ⬍ 100 from an original list of 1396 monosyllabic written frequency,6 word length (number of let-
irregular words. This original list consisted of all ters), and number of orthographic neighbours7
the monosyllabic words in the CELEX lexical was conducted to determine which had a signifi-
database (Baayen, Piepenbrock, & van Rijn, 1993) cant impact on reading accuracy. The analysis
that are irregular according to the set of grapheme revealed that the only stimulus variables which
to phoneme correspondences listed by Rastle and made independent contributions to irregular word
Coltheart (1999). reading accuracy were written frequency (Wald
␹2 ⫽ 12.056, p ⫽ .001), and number of ortho-
graphic neighbours (Wald ␹2 ⫽ 5.362, p ⫽ .021).
Reading
Irregular words read correctly (when compared to
Using the Coltheart and Leahy (1996) stimuli, those read incorrectly) were significantly higher in
reading performance on regular words (25/30, written frequency and had more orthographic
z ⫽ ⫺1.67) was better than with irregular words neighbours (see Table 4). The predominant error
(15/30 correct, z ⫽ ⫺2.41) (see Table 3), though type involved the attempted application of
both fell below an expected level based on MC’s grapheme to phoneme correspondence rules,
level of intellectual functioning. Note that the though a number of visual errors were also evident.
z-scores were calculated using developmental nor-
mative data provided by Edwards and Hogben
(1999), and indicate MC’s level of performance Spelling
relative to other children of his age. Predominant
error type for irregular word reading reflected the Using the Coltheart and Leahy (1996) stimuli, reg-
attempted (mostly but not invariably correct) ular word spelling (20/30) was better than irregular
application of grapheme to phoneme correspon- word spelling (12/30) (refer to Table 3; z-scores
dence rules (e.g., bouquet → /bəυkεt/, soul → are not provided as developmental normative data
/səυl/, also see Appendix A). Regular word read- was not available). As with reading, most errors
ing errors, however, were more visual in nature were phonologically plausible nonword responses
(e.g., peril → “pearl”, also see Appendix A). (e.g., choir → quwiyer, colonel → cernal, also see
Appendix A).
For the longer list of irregular words, MC
spelled 120/308 correctly. A regression analysis
Table 3. Total number of correct responses on the Coltheart including the variables spoken frequency, written
and Leahy (1996) word and nonword lists (n ⫽ 30) frequency, word length, and number of ortho-
graphic neighbours was again conducted to
Task Total correct z-score
determine which variables had a significant
Reading impact on spelling accuracy. The only stimulus
Regular words 25 ⫺1.67 variables that made independent contributions to
Irregular words 15 ⫺2.41
Nonwords 21 ⫺1.26
spelling accuracy were number of orthographic
Spelling neighbours (Wald ␹2 ⫽ 23.937, p ⫽ .000), written
Regular words 20 frequency (Wald ␹2 ⫽ 23.536, p ⫽ .000), and
Irregular words 12 spoken frequency (Wald ␹2 ⫽ 7.862, p ⫽ .005)
Nonwords 27 (see Table 4). The predominant error type again

6
Taken from the CELEX (Centre for lexical information) database (Baayen et al., 1993). Log frequency was used for all analyses.
7
Number of orthographic neighbours determined using the definition of Coltheart, Davelaar, Jonasson, and Besner (1977).

222 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2)


TREATMENT OF SURFACE DYSGRAPHIA

Table 4. Variables affecting reading and spelling of irregular words at baseline


Number of
orthographic
Length neighboursa,b Spoken frequencyb Written frequencya,b

Reading
Incorrect responses
Mean 4.95 4.41 72.01 1573.52
SD 0.94 3.86 298.13 7041.40
Correct responses
Mean 4.50 6.72 1237.43 16,540.68
SD 0.95 5.24 5287.40 81,696.54
Spelling
Incorrect responses
Mean 4.88 4.65 153.15 2184.37
SD 0.89 4.32 377.46 6097.94
Correct responses
Mean 4.22 8.37 2135.46 28,812.69
SD 0.96 5.16 7145.85 110,888.10
a
Variables that significantly affected reading accuracy.
b
Variables that significantly affected spelling accuracy.

involved the attempted application of phoneme Reading and spelling of homophones


to grapheme rules.
MC was required to read and write 96 homo-
phone words (48 homophone pairs) (see Appendix
Reading and spelling of nonwords B). Forty-two of the homophone words had an
irregular spelling (e.g., queue) and the remainder
Reading
had regular spellings (e.g., brake).
MC was asked to read aloud and write 30 non-
words from the Coltheart and Leahy (1996)
Reading of homophones
word/nonword test. In reading, 21/30 nonwords
were read aloud correctly (z ⫽ ⫺1.26, see Table 3) For assessment of homophone reading, MC was
(Edwards & Hogben, 1999), with 5 errors reflect- asked to read the homophone word aloud and then
ing visually similar real-word responses (e.g., form a sentence using the target word. He read
boril → “boil”, also see Appendix A). Nonword aloud a total of 77/96 homophone words
reading was also assessed using the Graded correctly (i.e., pronounced the word correctly),
Nonword Reading Test (Snowling, Stothard, & but for 13 of these correct responses he made a
McLean, 1996). On this test MC read 14/20 homophone confusion error when forming the
nonwords correctly, which placed him at the 25th sentence (e.g., pear → “pairs of them,” tale → “there
percentile for his age. is a tail on a dog”) (also see Appendix C).

Spelling Spelling of homophones


In contrast, nonword spelling was almost perfect. For assessment of homophone spelling, MC was
When required to write the 30 nonwords from required to listen to the target word both in isola-
Coltheart and Leahy (1996), MC spelled 27/30 tion and in context (in a sentence) prior to writing
correctly (see Appendix A and Table 3). it. Sixty-six per cent (63/96) of homophonic words

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2) 223


BRUNDSON, COLTHEART, NICKELS

were misspelled. Nineteen per cent of errors First, intact processes will be discussed. Relatively
(12/63) were homophone confusions (i.e., in good nonword reading suggests a relatively intact
response to “maid, the maid cleaned the house” (though not perfect) sublexical reading route (visual
MC wrote made). Sixty per cent (38/63) of errors analysis, grapheme to phoneme conversion, phono-
were phonologically plausible nonword errors (i.e., logical output buffer, linked by arrows 16 and 17).
in response to “prays, he prays everyday before Good nonword spelling to dictation suggests intact
going to sleep” MC wrote praes), and 21% of errors sublexical spelling processes (including auditory
(13/63) consisted of other error responses that analysis, acoustic to phonological conversion,
were not phonologically plausible (i.e., eight → grapheme to phoneme conversion, and the
etaght, their → thir) (also see Appendix D). graphemic output buffer, linked by arrows 5, 6, 7,
and 8). Intact expressive language and comprehen-
sion of spoken language suggests intact language
Summary of pretreatment assessment
processing routes and semantics (including the
In summary, MC’s reading and spelling were char- phonological input and output lexicons, and
acteristic of surface dyslexia and dysgraphia. That is, semantic system, linked by arrows 1, 2, 12, 11, and
sublexical processing was relatively preserved arrow 9).
(although not perfect), as shown by his better per-
formance on regular words and nonwords for both
Reading
reading and spelling. In contrast, MC displayed sig-
nificant difficulties with irregular word reading and MC’s lexical reading impairment can therefore be
spelling, homophone reading comprehension, and explained by specific difficulties with lexical
homophone spelling (with frequent homophone orthographic processes. MC had difficulty with
confusion errors), and overall his error pattern was visual lexical decision (see Table 2), which sug-
characterised by a large number of phonologically gested difficulties with access to and/or entries in
acceptable attempts, or regularisation errors. We the orthographic input lexicon. So when unable to
acknowledge that MC’s reading impairment did access the entry for an irregular word in input
not represent ‘pure’ surface dyslexia. It is not argued orthography MC was forced to rely on grapheme
that sublexical reading processes (i.e., nonword to phoneme correspondences (shown by arrows 16
reading) were entirely normal, only that lexical and 17, Figure 1), leading to regularisation errors.
reading skills were clearly impaired.8 This was apparent with irregular word reading
tasks (especially the longer list of irregular words),
on which MC made a large number of phono-
Functional localisation of impairment
logically plausible nonword responses. Can we be
With respect to models of reading and spelling, more specific regarding MC’s theoretical level of
MC’s dyslexia and dysgraphia can primarily be impairment in reading and, in particular, can we
explained by specific difficulties with lexical determine whether MC also had additional
orthographic processing. We can determine the deficits affecting connections from orthographic
level of impairment more specifically using pre- lexicon to semantic system (as shown by arrow
treatment assessment results and the theoretical 14)? We will argue that MC’s connections
framework of the dual-route model of reading and between orthography and semantics were not fully
spelling shown in Figure 1.9 developed.

8
The terms intact and impaired (when used in reference to MC’s abilities) refer to MC’s performance relative to normal age
expectations.
9
We note that MC’s pattern of impairment can be explained adequately by either the two lexicon or the single lexicon theoreti-
cal viewpoints. Data from the current case was unable to definitively adjudicate between the two theoretical viewpoints, and there-
fore this theoretical debate and relevant analyses are not discussed in the current paper.

224 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2)


TREATMENT OF SURFACE DYSGRAPHIA

MC was able to read aloud correctly a small input lexicon or access to them, as well as in their
number of irregular homophone words, which connections to semantics.
suggested that he was able to access their entries in
the orthographic input lexicon and was not relying
Spelling
on sublexical grapheme to phoneme conversion
for pronunciation. However, even with these irreg- MC’s spelling to dictation performance can also
ular homophone words he still made homophone be explained by difficulties with orthographic pro-
confusion errors (e.g., MC was able to read the cesses. However, it is more difficult to determine
word queue correctly but provided the wrong his exact level of impairment in this area than it is
semantic context “the actor needed a cue,” see for reading.
Table 5 for other examples). This suggested that MC’s homophone spelling, akin to reading,
for some words MC was able to access their ortho- revealed an interesting pattern of responses. For
graphic entries but that connections from these to some homophones, MC produced a homophone
semantics were faulty, forcing MC to use the confusion response, which had an irregular spelling
direct route to the phonological output lexicon but and which he spelled correctly (e.g., when provided
preventing word-specific semantic activation with the context “they bred the cattle carefully” he
(route shown by arrows 13, 15, 11, Figure 1). produced the homophone confusion bread, but
Homophone confusion errors with irregularly spelled it correctly despite it being an irregular
spelled homophones are rarely investigated or word; see Table 5 for more examples). This sug-
reported in developmental cases. One exception, gested that he was able to successfully access the
however, is a developmental case (CD) reported by response word’s representation in the orthographic
Coltheart, Masterson, Byng, Prior, and Riddoch output lexicon and write the word, but was unable
(1983) who also made a small number of such to access the stimulus word’s orthographic output
errors in reading. So, in summary for MC’s read- representation correctly from semantics. So, refer-
ing, it seems that impairments were evident both ring to Figure 1, we assume, based on his intact
at the level of representations in the orthographic auditory comprehension skills, that when asked to

Table 5. Reading and spelling of irregular homophones (examples from baseline


1 and 2)
(a) Reading

Homophone confusion:
Target Read correctly Definition provided by MC

Queue Yes “the actor needed a cue”


Pear Yes “socks . . . pairs of them”
Whole Yes “there is a hole in the ground”
Their Yes “there is a ball”
Steak Yes “I stuck a stake into the ground”
(b) Spelling

Homophone confusion:
Target Context provided MC’s written response

Bred ‘They bred the cattle carefully . . . ’ bread


Bare ‘The desert landscape was bare’ bear
Pair ‘He wore a pair of black shoes’ pear
Lock ‘The gate had a lock’ loch
Quay ‘The boat sailed into the quay’ key
Their ‘They were washing their clothes’ there

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2) 225


BRUNDSON, COLTHEART, NICKELS

spell a homophone (e.g., “bred”) after hearing the connections (shown by arrow 3) and deficits either
semantic context in a sentence (e.g., “they bred the at the level of entries in the orthographic output
cattle carefully”), MC was able to access the seman- lexicon or access to those entries from the phono-
tic system normally (route shown by arrows 1 and logical output lexicon (shown by arrow 10).
2).10 We also know he was able to retrieve the cor- It is important to note that we do not propose
rect spelling of the homophone confusion response that affected components of the reading and
from the orthographic output lexicon because he spelling system are abolished. For MC, a develop-
correctly wrote bread, which has an irregular mental case, we would propose that entries in the
spelling.11 The homophone confusion must there- orthographic lexicons and/or access to them were
fore arise from MC’s inability to access output poorly formed, immature, or unreliable. It is
orthography directly from semantics. He is instead important to note, however, that MC could read
forced to access the orthographic representation of and spell some irregular words correctly. He could
the word via the phonological output lexicon also read and spell some homophones correctly
(shown by arrows 12 and 10), which gives rise to and access their correct semantic representation.
the homophone confusion. This suggests impair- So, he did have some complete entries in his
ment in connections between the semantic system orthographic lexicons and he could access them
and orthographic output lexicon (shown by arrow 3 and retrieve correct semantic information consis-
in Figure 1). tently for some words; however, he certainly had
MC’s irregular word spelling performance (i.e., not reached a normal level of efficiency in func-
his spelling of irregular words that are not homo- tioning for his age.
phones) suggests additional deficits. MC made a
large number of phonologically plausible nonword
errors when spelling irregular words to dictation.
TREATMENT OF THE SPELLING
His phonologically plausible nonword spelling
IMPAIRMENT
errors most probably resulted from him relying on
the processing route through the phonological lex-
General treatment aims
icons and semantic system and then sublexical
phoneme to grapheme correspondences (shown by Treatment was focused on spelling, as this was
arrows 1, 2, 12, 11, 7, 8). He would be forced to perceived by MC and his family to be the major
employ this compensatory strategy if entries within functional difficulty. His family were very aware
the output lexicon were poorly formed and/or if of MC’s poor irregular word spelling as it was evi-
unable to access the orthographic output lexicon dent in his written work, even with very high-
from semantics (arrow 3) and the phonological frequency words. Assessment results suggested
output lexicon (arrow 10). In summary, using evi- that MC’s spelling difficulties resulted from
dence from both irregular and homophone words, impairments in access to output orthography and
it is proposed that MC’s spelling impairments most probably additional deficits in actual word
resulted from deficits in semantic to orthography representations in the orthographic output

10
Although semantic knowledge was not assessed for target words at an individual word level, the authors feel confident that
words were semantically represented and that access to these from spoken language was intact, especially given the target words’ high
written and spoken frequency. MC had normal language and semantics (as tested using PALPA 47, 48, and 53 and standard VIQ
assessment). He had no history of developmental language or semantic difficulties and no reported current difficulties in these areas.
In fact, he was described by his school teacher as highly articulate and intelligent, with good listening skills, good spoken expression
and good general knowledge (as quoted on p. 219).
11
It is interesting to note that MC’s homophone confusion responses that were correctly spelled irregular words did not simply
reflect a tendency for him to produce the higher-frequency homophone of the homophone pair. Some of these responses were the
lower-frequency member of the homophone pair.

226 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2)


TREATMENT OF SURFACE DYSGRAPHIA

lexicon. Therefore, treatment focused on irregular neighbours12 (␹2 ⫽ 7.71, p ⫽ .021), all further
word spelling and aimed to add new ‘whole irreg- analyses comparing word sets 1, 2, and 3 con-
ular word’ entries in his output lexicon. Because trolled for this difference. The words sets were
MC’s semantic representations of target irregular also matched according to the total number of
words were presumed intact, it was also hoped words read and spelled incorrectly on both base-
that treatment would facilitate his ability to access lines and the total number that were incorrect on
output orthography directly from semantics. It just one baseline (also matched for the number
was anticipated that a focus on irregular word incorrect on only the first baseline and only the
spelling would result in the best functional out- second baseline). Word set 4 consisted of 86
come for MC (when compared to a focus on words that were spelled consistently correctly
homophone spelling). over two baselines.
The treatment study was also carefully designed
to enable detailed post hoc investigation of the
mechanisms underlying treatment generalisation Baseline assessments
to untreated stimuli, which to our knowledge has
MC’s performance on the 222 target irregular
not been investigated in such detail previously. In
words did not vary across pretest assessments for
addition, treatment design enabled evaluation of
spelling (overall, McNemar ␹2 ⫽ 1.82, p ⫽ .178;
the efficacy of employing mnemonics in lexical
Set 1, p ⫽ .481; Set 2, p ⫽ .648; Set 3, p ⫽ .481)
treatment studies.
(see B1 vs. B2 in Figure 5) or reading (overall,
In summary, the treatment study aimed to:
McNemar ␹2 ⫽ 2.327, p ⫽ .127; Set 1, p ⫽ .804;
1. Improve MC’s irregular word spelling. Set 2, p ⫽ .481; Set 3, p ⫽ .238) (see B1 vs. B2 in
2. Determine factors underlying treatment Figure 7). There was no change in total correct for
generalisation to untreated items. Set 4 spelling (see B1 vs. B2 in Figure 6).
3. Determine the contribution of the specific Two baseline assessments were also conducted
mnemonic cue to the effectiveness of treatment. for reading and spelling of the 48 homophone
pairs (as described in assessment section above).
Spelling performance was recorded in terms of
Target stimuli
total errors, total homophone confusion errors
Two baseline assessments of reading and spelling (total HCE), total phonologically plausible non-
were conducted using 308 irregular words (the word errors (total PPNE), and total other errors
same list that was used for assessment of irregular (total OE). Other errors were those spelling
word reading and spelling above). The 222 target attempts that were nonwords but not phonologi-
stimuli were then selected. They included all cally plausible (see Appendix D for error exam-
those words for which MC’s spelling response ples). Homophone spelling was consistent across
was incorrect on at least one of two baselines. baselines for total errors (McNemar, p ⫽ 1.0),
The 222 target irregular words were then divided total PPNE (p ⫽ .359), total OE (p ⫽ .388) but
into three sets of 74 words (Sets 1, 2 and 3). The not for total HC errors (p ⫽ .039) (see B1 vs. B2
sets were matched as closely as possible on writ- in Figure 8). MC made significantly more HCE
ten frequency (Kruskal Wallis ␹2 ⫽ 0.69, p ⫽ .71), errors on the second baseline.
spoken frequency (␹2 ⫽ 2.05, p ⫽ .36), length Homophone reading performance (see Figure 9)
(␹2 ⫽ 3.93, p ⫽ .14), and number of orthographic was recorded in terms of total errors in reading
neighbours. Although the word sets did differ aloud and total homophone confusion errors (total
significantly in terms of number of orthographic HCE). MC’s homophone reading performance

12
Word set 2 (mean 5.74, SD 4.23), had a higher mean number of orthographic neighbours than Sets 1 (mean 4.05, SD 4.23)
and 3 (mean 4.9, SD 4.3).

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2) 227


BRUNDSON, COLTHEART, NICKELS

was consistent across baseline assessments (total 1 were Set 1 words, for training session 2 were Set
correct, McNemar, p ⫽ 1.0; total HCE, p ⫽ .424). 2 words, and for training session 3 were Set 3
words.
Treatment design
Overall treatment design is shown in Table 6. Stimuli
Treatment was conducted over a period of 1
All stimuli consisted of flash cards containing
month and focused on each irregular word set in
the target word written in lower case (size 48
turn. There were three training sessions, one for
arial font). Set 2 words were also assigned a
each word set (see italics, Table 6). There were
mnemonic cue (a semantically related picture
seven assessments—two baseline assessments,
cue; see Figure 4). Mnemonic cues were devised
three post-treatment assessments (post-treatment
by the first author and drawn beside the target
phase 1, 2, and 3), and two follow-up assessments
word on each flash card. Only Set 2 words were
(2 months and 4 months post-treatment). Each
assigned a mnemonic and, aside from this, treat-
included assessment of both reading and spelling
ment method and training sessions were identi-
of all homophone and irregular words (with the
cal for all word sets.
exception of the post-treatment phase 1 and the
second follow-up assessment, when only spelling
was assessed). Assessments were conducted over
Training sessions
two testing sessions to avoid repetition of stimuli
(that is, assessment of reading and spelling of the Training sessions were conducted by the first
irregular word set was conducted on separate days, author. Only one training session was conducted
as was assessment of reading and spelling of the for each word set. In the training sessions each tar-
homophone word set). get word was trained in turn and was trained only
once. For each target word MC was shown a flash
card displaying the correct spelling. He was first
Treatment method
asked to copy the word (the word was also read
As mentioned above, there were three treatment aloud by the first author). The word was then
phases with each phase targeting a different set of removed from view and MC was asked to write
irregular words. Target words for training session the word again after a 10-second delay. If his

Table 6. Treatment design


Testing session Date Testing Tasks

1 1/3/01 Baseline 1 Irregular word spelling and homophone reading


2 10/4/01 Baseline 1 cont Irregular word reading and homophone spelling
3 13/4/01 Baseline 2 Irregular word spelling and homophone reading
4 18/4/01 Baseline 2 cont Irregular word reading and homophone spelling
5 30/4/01 Word Set 1 training Session Irregular word spelling
6 10/5/01 Post-treatment 1 testing Irregular word spelling and homophone spelling
7 14/5/01 Word Set 2 training session Irregular word spelling
8 26/5/01 Post-treatment 2 testing Irregular word spelling and homophone reading
9 27/5/01 Post-treatment 2 testing cont Irregular word reading and homophone spelling
10 27/5/01 Word Set 3 training session Irregular word spelling
11 12/6/01 Post-treatment 3 testing Irregular word spelling and homophone reading
12 13/6/01 Post-treatment 3 testing cont Irregular word reading and homophone spelling
13 20/8/01 Follow-up 1 (2 mths post-treatment) Irregular word spelling and homophone reading
14 21/8/01 Follow-up 1 cont Irregular word reading and homophone spelling
15 23/10/01 Follow-up 2 (4 mths post-treatment) Irregular word spelling and homophone spelling

228 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2)


TREATMENT OF SURFACE DYSGRAPHIA

in a different order) MC’s parents were instructed


to:
1. Read aloud the word and ask MC to write
the word down.
2. Tell MC whether his response was correct
or incorrect.
3. If incorrect, show MC the corresponding
flash card with the correct spelling. Show him for
5 seconds and then ask him to rewrite the word
from memory. Provide no further help if his second
attempt was also incorrect.
4. Move on to the next word.

RESULTS

All comparisons between baseline and post-


treatment results employ the highest baseline
result for establishing improvements in total cor-
rect or increase in error responses and the lowest
baseline for establishing a reduction in errors.13

Irregular word spelling


Figure 4. Examples of mnemonic cues employed for
irregular words. General treatment results
Results for irregular word spelling are shown in
response was incorrect he was shown the flash card Figure 5. There was a significant increase in the
again for a few seconds. Finally, he was required to total number of irregular words spelled correctly
write the word to dictation. Again, he was shown from pretreatment baseline (33/222 at B1)
the flash card containing the correct spelling if his to post-treatment phase 3 (149/222) (overall,
response was still incorrect. McNemar ␹2 ⫽ 148.30, p ⫽ .000; Set 1, p ⫽ .000;
Set 2, p ⫽ .000; Set 3, p ⫽ .000). As expected all
individual word sets improved dramatically after
Home practice
they were specifically targeted for spelling treat-
In between training sessions MC was required to ment. That is, Set 1 words improved after treat-
conduct home practice. Detailed instructions were ment phase 1 when compared to baseline
provided for his parents, who supervised all home (McNemar ␹2 ⫽ 57.02, p ⫽ .000), Set 2 improved
practice sessions. Home practice was discontinued after treatment phase 2 when compared to post-
when MC spelled at least 90% of the words treatment phase 1 (McNemar ␹2 ⫽ 40.20,
correctly for 2 consecutive days. Duration of p ⫽ .000), and Set 3 words improved after treat-
treatment was 8 days for Set 1, 6 days for Set 2, ment phase 3 when compared to post-treatment
and 7 days for Set 3. For each word (once a day phase 2 (McNemar ␹2 ⫽ 39.02, p ⫽ .000).

13
For example, for irregular word spelling (see Figure 5) baseline one is employed for all comparisons and for homophone
reading (see Figure 9) baseline two is used for total error comparisons and baseline one is used for total homophone confusion error
comparisons.

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2) 229


BRUNDSON, COLTHEART, NICKELS

post-treatment to assessment at follow-up 1


(Generalised Estimating Equation [GEE] analysis,
␹2 ⫽ 3.15, p ⫽ .21).14 No further decline was evi-
dent at 4 months post-treatment (follow-up 2)
(overall, McNemar ␹2 ⫽ .078, p ⫽ .78; Set 1,
p ⫽ 1.0; Set 2, p ⫽ .424; Set 3, p ⫽ 1.0).
Post hoc analyses were conducted to investigate
possible reasons for the decline in performance
between post-treatment phase 3 and the first
follow-up assessment. A logistic regression analy-
sis was conducted to determine which variables
were significant in differentiating between words
that changed from correct to incorrect and words
that remained correct. Target words for the analy-
sis were responses for word sets 1–3 at the post-
treatment 3 assessment. Variables investigated
included word frequency, spoken frequency, length
(number of letters), and number of orthographic
neighbours. The only variable that made a signifi-
cant independent contribution was number of let-
ters, with longer words more likely to drop off
with time (Wald ␹2 ⫽ 7.162, p ⫽ .007), though
number of orthographic neighbours did also
approach significance (Wald ␹2 ⫽ 3.03, p ⫽ .082)
(those words with fewer orthographic neighbours
Figure 5. Percentage correct for irregular word spelling at
baseline, during treatment and at post-tests. were more likely to drop off with time).
Total number of words spelled correctly in Set
4 (i.e., words that were consistently correct pre-
At 2 months post-treatment (follow-up 1) a treatment) was significantly lower post-treatment,
total of 134/222 words were spelled correctly indi- as measured at post-treatment phase 3 and follow-
cating a significant decline from post-treatment up 1 assessments, when compared to baseline two
phase 3 (overall, McNemar ␹2 ⫽ 41.07, p ⫽ .000; (PT3: McNemar, p ⫽ .000; follow-up 1: p ⫽ .031)
Set 1, p ⫽ .004; Set 2, p ⫽ .004; Set 3, p ⫽ .000), (see Figure 6). Accuracy declined from 100% at
though this still represented a significant improve- baseline to 86% at post-treatment phase 3 and
ment when compared to pretreatment baseline 93% at follow-up. However, this degree of change
performance (overall, McNemar ␹2 ⫽ 93.458, (i.e., 14% and 7% respectively) was no greater than
p ⫽ .000; Set 1, p ⫽ .000; Set 2, p ⫽ .000; Set 3, a regression to the mean effect as measured over
p ⫽ .000). There was no difference between baseline (i.e., a regression to the mean effect of
the three word sets in degree of decline from 28%)15 and thus will not be discussed further.

14
GEE is an approach to the analysis of correlated response data, which is particularly useful when the responses are binary. It
takes account of the correlations between observations and uses them when calculating both parameter estimates and their standard
errors. Interested readers can refer to Hanley, Negassa, Edwardes, and Forrester (2003) for more information.
15
This regression to the mean effect was calculated by determining the percentage of correct responses at baseline 1 (from all
word sets) that were incorrect at baseline 2.

230 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2)


TREATMENT OF SURFACE DYSGRAPHIA

to the commencement of treatment). In contrast,


after treatment phase 1, untreated words from sets 2
and 3 that were spelled incorrectly on the second
baseline improved, with 21.1% becoming correct.
This represented a significantly larger proportion of
words than the 11.7% regression to the mean effect
(␹2 ⫽ 4.58, p ⫽ .032). Therefore, we argue that at
least part of the improvement in untreated words
was a true treatment generalisation effect. Similarly,
for Set 3, 27.6% of words that were wrong after
treatment phase 1 became correct after treatment
phase 2 (again significantly more than the 11.7
regression to the mean effect, ␹2 ⫽ 7.487, p ⫽ .006),
even though they had not yet received any treat-
ment. Again, this indicates that improvements in
spelling of untreated words cannot be explained
entirely by regression to the mean but in fact reflects
Figure 6. Percentage correct for irregular word Set 4 (at least in part) a treatment generalisation effect.16
(irregular words that were spelled correctly on both baselines
Post hoc analyses were conducted to investigate
and were never treated) at baseline and post-treatment.
possible predictors of generalisation. The analyses
conducted compared characteristics of those
untreated words that improved prior to their treat-
Generalisation to untreated words
ment (i.e., those affected by treatment generalisa-
Significant generalisation (of treatment effects) to tion) and those that didn’t improve. Target words
untreated irregular words was also apparent. After for the analysis included error responses for word
treatment phase 1 (which targeted Set 1 words sets 2 and 3, taken from the second baseline assess-
only) the number of words spelled correctly for ment. A logistic regression analysis was used to
the untreated Set 2 improved significantly when determine which variables significantly differenti-
compared to baseline 1 (McNemar, p ⫽ .023). ated between untreated words that improved with
Although Set 3 words had not significantly generalisation and those that didn’t. The dependent
improved following treatment phase 1 (p ⫽ .383), variable ‘generalisation’ was a binary outcome vari-
generalisation was evident after treatment phase 2 able. Predictor variables included were written word
(i.e., improvement occurred before Set 3 words frequency, spoken word frequency, length (number
were trained) (p ⫽ .000). of letters), number of orthographic neighbours, and
It was important to demonstrate that this effect a binary variable ‘error vs. stimulus length,’ which
was not a result of regression to the mean but a true represented whether the error response was the
treatment generalisation effect. We examined the same length as the stimulus. It was also considered
extent of variability across baseline testing in order that generalisation could possibly depend on how
to predict the size of the likely effects of regression close the error response was to being correct prior to
to the mean: Of all those words incorrect at the first treatment. It was hypothesised that those words
baseline, 11.7% were correct at the second baseline whose error responses were closer or more similar to
(i.e., a regression to the mean effect of 11.7% prior the correct response prior to treatment may respond

16
Given that the improvements in spelling of untreated irregular words was significantly greater than regression to the mean
effects, we can conclude that the larger improvement evident in spelling of treated irregular words following treatment was also
significantly greater than any regression to the mean effect.

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2) 231


BRUNDSON, COLTHEART, NICKELS

to treatment generalisation more readily. To investi- vowel slot with the remaining consonants assigned
gate this possibility, five variables representing a from left to right. For match 3, surrounding con-
series of ‘match’ values were also included in the sonants are assigned positions from right to left
analyses. These match variables essentially repre- surrounding the vowel.
sent a measure of how similar an error response is to
the correct stimulus (i.e., how closely the error Match 4. For match 4, the position of each letter
matches the correct spelling) in five different ways is coded relative to the first and last letters and the
according to different coding schemes. The five middle letters are then compared for similarity (so,
different coding schemes and their match values in a way, the first half of the word is coded accord-
will be referred to as match 1, match 2, match 3, ing to absolute letter position, while the second
match 4, and match 5, where the match value is half is coded relative to the final letter).
based on the measure of overlap between the stimu-
lus and error response divided by the number of let- Match 5. This is a form of spatial coding devised
ters in the stimulus (resulting in a match value by Davis (1999), in which the comparison between
between 0 and 1). An in-depth review of coding stimulus and error response is based on the spatial
schemes is beyond the scope of this paper; however, pattern of activity across the stimuli within the
a brief description of each match variable employed SOLAR computational model of reading rather
in the current study will follow (for match values than direct comparison of letters according to letter
1–4 also refer to Table 7).17 position. Briefly, in this scheme words are not
anchored according to letter position; letter order is
Match 1. The variable match 1 is based on a coded by relative activation of letters and resultant
simple comparison of letters according to absolute spatial patterns of activation are position-invariant.
letter position, in which the stimulus and error This enables recognition of similarity between a
response are aligned according to the leftmost letter. stimulus and response when letters are transposed
(or when the correct letters are employed but in the
Match 2 and 3. Match 2 and 3 are ‘vowel- wrong order (e.g., “weight”–wiehgt). For the current
centred’ schemes in which the stimulus and error analyses, the default parameters employed for
response are aligned using the first vowel as an match 5 placed slightly more weight on exterior let-
anchor. Match 2 and 3 use slightly different meth- ters. For the above examples of target and error
ods for aligning the surrounding consonants. For response (i.e., “strap”–spat) this coding scheme
match 2, surrounding consonants are assigned results in a match value of 0.41.
positions from left to right. Consonants preceding For a comparison of match values 1–5 for a
the vowel are assigned to slots until a vowel is sample of MC’s actual spelling error responses,
reached; this is then assigned to a designated refer to Table 8.

Table 7. Match variable 1–4 for stimulus “STRAP” and error response SPAT
Matching scheme S T R A P (Stimulus) Overlaps * Match value

1 S* P A T 1/5 .2
2 S* P A* T 2/5 .4
3 S P A* T 1/5 .2
4 S* P A* T 2/5 .4

17
For a discussion of the various orthographic coding schemes, see Davis (1999, pp. 77–81). We thank Colin Davis for making
available to us his program for calculating these various kinds of match values.

232 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2)


TREATMENT OF SURFACE DYSGRAPHIA

Table 8. Match values for a selection of MC’s spelling Because of the significant correlation between
errors the match variables, additional regression analyses
Error were conducted for each match variable in turn
Stimulus response 1 2 3 4 5 (with word characteristics and ‘error vs. stimulus’
Yield Yiald .80 .80 .80 .80 .84 length also included in the analysis) to examine
Phase Faes .20 .40 .40 .00 .44 the difference in their predictive powers when
Clerk Clarc .60 .40 .40 .60 .58 included alone. In this analysis, match 1, 3, 4, and
Pearl Perl .40 .60 .60 .80 .76 5 were all significant predictors of generalisation
Ward Word .75 .25 .50 .75 .80
(match 1, p ⫽ .038; match 3, p ⫽ .048; match 4,
Heart Hart .20 .20 .40 .60 .77
Built Biult .60 .60 .60 .60 .89 p ⫽ .016; match 5, p ⫽ .022). Finally, regression
Path Parth .50 .50 1.0 1.0 .90 analyses were conducted for pairs of match vari-
ables to determine whether any individual match
variable could predict generalisation independent
of the other (word characteristic variables were also
Analyses investigating the nature of generalisation
included as predictor variables). Match 4 was a sig-
First, point-biserial correlation analyses were con- nificant predictor of generalisation independent of
ducted to investigate the intercorrelation between match 2 (p ⫽ .043), with a trend for prediction
the match variables, the ‘error vs. stimulus length’ independent of match 1 (p ⫽ .089). Match 5 also
variable, the word characteristic variables (spoken approached significance for prediction of generali-
frequency, written frequency, number of ortho- sation independent of match 2 (p ⫽ .055). No
graphic neighbours, word length), and also the other comparisons between pairs of match vari-
binary outcome variable ‘generalisation.’ All of the ables were significant.
match variables were highly correlated with each Finally, a backwards logistic regression was con-
other (correlations ranged from .508 to .832). ducted to determine the best model of prediction for
Match variables 3, 4, and 5 were significantly cor- generalisation. All of the match variables and word
related with number of orthographic neighbours characteristic variables were included as predictors.
and match 4 was also significantly correlated with The best model for predicting generalisation con-
word length. None of the match variables were sisted of match 4 (Wald ␹2 ⫽ 7.235, p ⫽ .007), and
significantly correlated with word frequency. The written frequency (Wald ␹2 ⫽ 4.678, p ⫽ .031). As
variable ‘error vs. stimulus’ length was significantly the degree of match between the error responses and
correlated with match 1, written frequency, and stimuli increased (where the position of each letter
word length. All of the word characteristic vari- was coded relative to the first and last letters) and
ables were highly correlated with each other. The written frequency increased, the likelihood of
variable of interest, ‘generalisation,’ was signifi- improvement with generalisation also increased.
cantly correlated with the match variables 1, 3, 4, In summary, results suggest that match variables
and 5 (correlations ranged from .189 to .242) as 1, 3, 4, and 5 are all strong predictors of generalisa-
well as spoken frequency, written frequency, and tion. However, match 4 is consistently the strongest
number of orthographic neighbours. or best predictor of generalisation, and the best
Then, a logistic regression analysis including model for prediction includes both match 4 and
word frequency, spoken frequency, length and written frequency. Mean match values (at baseline
number of orthographic neighbours, ‘error vs. 2) for those words that improved prior to treatment
stimulus’ length, and the match variables was con- and those that didn’t are shown in Table 9.
ducted. Results indicated that no stimulus variable
made a significant contribution that was indepen-
Analyses of how errors changed during treatment
dent of the other variables. This was not a surpris-
ing result given the high degree of correlation Analyses were also conducted to investigate the
between predictor variables. nature of changes in error characteristics for

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2) 233


BRUNDSON, COLTHEART, NICKELS

Table 9. Match values for irregular words (spelling) that set of analyses included all error responses (from
improve prior to treatment and those that do not Set 3) made at baseline 2 and post-treatment
Match phase 1, for those words that were correct prior to
treatment (i.e., were correct after treatment phase
1 2 3 4 5
2). We were interested in determining whether
Words that improve their misspellings were closer to being correct after
Mean 0.62 0.67 0.75 0.69 0.77 treatment phase 1 when compared to baseline.
SD 0.19 0.19 0.18 0.22 0.15
Words that do not improve
Results indicated that there was a significant
Mean 0.53 0.59 0.64 0.56 0.68 increase in match 2 (Mann Whitney, p ⫽ .016),
SD 0.21 0.23 0.25 0.26 0.19 match 3 (p ⫽ .009), and match 5 (p ⫽ .015) values
between baseline 2 and post-treatment phase 1.
This suggests that the nature of the error
responses was changing and that misspellings of
untreated words during treatment. This interest words were improving with treatment generalisa-
arose from an observation that error responses for tion prior to becoming totally correct. Interestingly,
some words appeared to gradually change and it was the two vowel-centred match schemes
improve in orthographic accuracy as a result of (match 2 and 3) that improved significantly,
treatment generalisation but prior to becoming possibly suggesting a gradual improvement in
totally correct. For example, the stimulus word interior vowel patterns during treatment. The
“tough” (from treatment set 3) was an untreated analyses below further examine the role of match
word that benefited from treatment generalisa- schemes 2 and 3 in error change.
tion. However, there appeared to be a gradual
increase in the use of orthography from baseline 2 Examination of error characteristics at four testing
(taf ), to post-treatment phase 1 (taugh), prior to points during treatment. The second analyses
the correct response at post-treatment phase 2 examined a smaller subset of words in more detail
(tough) (also see Appendix E). This raised the pos- to allow specific examination of gradual changes in
sibility that treatment generalisation might actu- error responses for untreated words during treat-
ally result in a gradual improvement in spelling of ment, with a focus on match schemes 2 and 3. The
untreated words, rather than a sudden all-or-none stimuli for these analyses were those Set 3 words
change from incorrect to totally correct. This pos- that were consistently incorrect at baseline 1, base-
sibility was formally investigated in more detail line 2, and post-treatment phase 1 (B1, B2, PT1).
and is discussed below. These target stimuli were divided into two groups
The analyses focused only on words from Set 3, for the analyses: words that benefited from gener-
those words treated last. Limiting the focus to Set alisation (i.e., incorrect at B1, B2, and PT1 but
3 words enabled an examination of error charac- correct at PT2, N ⫽ 10; see Appendix E) and
teristics at four points prior to their treatment (i.e., words that failed to benefit from generalisation
at baseline 1, at baseline 2, after treatment phase 1 (incorrect at B1, B2, PT1, and PT2, N ⫽ 37).
and after treatment phase 2: B1, B2, PT1, PT2). Mean match values for coding schemes 2 and 3
This provided an opportunity to analyse how the were calculated for both groups for words at each
misspellings of words changed during treatment testing point.
rather than simply focusing on the identification Analyses revealed a very strong trend for an
of which words benefited most from treatment increase in match 2 (Wilcoxon, p ⫽ .066) and 3
generalisation. The analyses were conducted in (p ⫽ .066) values between baseline 2 and post-
two stages (described below). treatment phase 1 for words that benefited from
treatment generalisation (i.e., were correct at
Comparison of error characteristics at baseline 2 and post treatment phase 2). In contrast, there was no
post treatment phase 1. The data used for the first significant increase in match 2 (p ⫽ .688) or match

234 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2)


TREATMENT OF SURFACE DYSGRAPHIA

3 (p ⫽ .478) values from B2 and PT1 for errors had received spelling treatment, improved signifi-
that failed to benefit from treatment generalisa- cantly when compared to pretreatment (baseline
tion (i.e., remained incorrect at PT2). This sup- 2) (Set 1, McNemar ␹2 ⫽ 12.90, p ⫽ .000; Set 2,
ports the observation that errors gradually increase p ⫽ .000). In contrast, reading of irregular words
in degree of correctness (e.g., “tough” → taf → for Set 3 (untreated at this stage) did not signifi-
taugh example described above) as a result of treat- cantly improve, suggesting no treatment generali-
ment generalisation and that this improvement sation to reading of untreated words (McNemar,
predominantly involves interior letters.18 p ⫽ .302). Reading of Set 3 words, however, did
Finally, analyses were conducted to determine improve after treatment phase 3 when compared
whether the misspellings of words that do not ben- to baseline and post-treatment phase 2 (baseline,
efit from generalisation (those misspelled at post- McNemar p ⫽ .000; PT2, p ⫽ .000).
treatment phase 2) ever increase in accuracy. The
match values for this group of words did actually
Homophone spelling
eventually improve between post-treatment phase
1 and 2 (but still prior to being targeted for treat- Results for homophone spelling are shown in
ment) (match 2, p ⫽ .025; match 3, p ⫽ .04). So, Figure 8.
even those words that did not become correct as
a result of treatment generalisation did actually
show a treatment generalisation effect. Again it
was interior letters that appeared to improve most.

Irregular word reading


Results for irregular word reading are shown in
Figure 7. Irregular word reading was assessed at
baseline 1 and 2, after treatment phase 2 and 3,
and at 2 months post-treatment (follow-up 1).
There was a significant increase from baseline
(baseline 2) to post-treatment phase 3 in the total
number of irregular words read correctly (overall,
McNemar ␹2 ⫽ 58.53, p ⫽ .000; Set 1, p ⫽ .000;
Set 2, p ⫽ .000; Set 3, p ⫽ .000). At 2 months
post-treatment (follow-up 1) this improvement
was maintained, with no significant change in
total correct from post-treatment phase 3 to the
first follow-up assessment (overall, McNemar
p ⫽ .210; Set 1, p ⫽ 1.0; Set 2, p ⫽ .250; Set 3,
p ⫽ .727). At assessment post-treatment phase Figure 7. Percentage correct for irregular word reading at
two, reading of both Set 1 and Set 2 words, which baseline, during treatment, and at post tests.

18
It is important to note that neither group of words significantly changed in match values from baseline 1 to baseline 2 (match
2, p ⫽ .688; match 3, p ⫽ .478). So, there were no changes in error characteristics during baseline and therefore any changes that
occurred during treatment were due to treatment generalisation effects.

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2) 235


BRUNDSON, COLTHEART, NICKELS

Total errors initial baselines. No further significant changes


were evident during treatment or at follow-up,
The total number of homophone spelling errors
when compared to baseline 2 (PT1, McNemar
was reduced during treatment, with significantly
p ⫽ .804; PT2, p ⫽ .607; PT3, p ⫽ .804; follow-up
fewer errors evident after treatment phase 3 than
1, p ⫽ .629; follow-up 2, p ⫽ 1.0). When com-
at baseline 2 (McNemar p ⫽ .031), suggesting
pared to baseline 1, the total homophone confu-
treatment generalisation to homophone spelling.
sion errors post-treatment 3 was significantly
This treatment effect was maintained at 2 and 4
higher (p ⫽ .041) (NB. no other comparisons were
months post-treatment, when compared to Post-
significant), though this result is difficult to
treatment 3 (follow-up 1, McNemar ␹2 ⫽ 0.30,
interpret in the context of unstable baselines.
p ⫽ .584; follow-up 2, p ⫽ 1.0). Interestingly, the
predominant change occurred after treatment phase
two and the use of mnemonics, with the difference
Phonologically plausible nonword errors
between post-treatment phase 1 and 2 for total
correct approaching significance (p ⫽ .053). The total number of phonologically plausible non-
word errors (PPNE) was reduced during treat-
ment, with significantly fewer errors after
Homophone confusion errors
treatment phase 3 than at baseline 2 (McNemar
As mentioned above, total homophone confusion ␹2 ⫽ 16.7, p ⫽ .000), suggesting a change in the
errors (HCE) for spelling increased between nature of errors made as a result of treatment.
There was a significant decrease in PPNE
between baseline 2 and post-treatment phase 1
(p ⫽ .023), between post-treatment phases 1 and 3
(p ⫽ .019), but not between treatment phases 1
and 2 (p ⫽ .541). This treatment effect was main-
tained over time with no change between the end
of treatment and the two follow-up assessments
(follow-up 1, p ⫽ 1.0; follow-up 2, p ⫽ 1.0).

Other errors
The total number of other errors (OE) increased
significantly after treatment phase 1 when com-
pared to baseline 1 (McNemar, p ⫽ .012) but there
were no further significant changes during treat-
ment or at follow-up. These errors were responses
that were incorrect and were not phonologically
plausible (see Appendix D for examples).
Post hoc analyses were conducted to compare
the nature of homophone spelling errors before
and after treatment. This analysis included all
homophone error responses (both nonword errors
and homophone confusion word errors) and was
conducted to investigate whether error responses
Figure 8. Percentage of responses that were errors for moved closer to the target with treatment. In other
homophone spelling at baseline, during treatment, words, even though the responses were still incor-
and at post-tests. rect after treatment, were they closer to being

236 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2)


TREATMENT OF SURFACE DYSGRAPHIA

correct as a result of the irregular word spelling whether the error response was the same length as
treatment. The degree of match (using the match the stimulus. The binary outcome variable repre-
variables described previously) between error sented time of testing (baseline vs. post-treatment)
responses and the correct spellings were calculated for all error responses. Results indicated that error
for errors at baseline 2 and the first follow-up type was the only variable that differentiated sig-
assessment and then compared. nificantly between errors made before treatment
First, at baseline (baseline 2), there was not a and those made post-treatment. Errors at follow-
significant interaction between error type and up were less likely to be phonologically plausible
match values for coding schemes overall (match 1, (Wald ␹2 ⫽ 7.4, p ⫽ .006), than at baseline. There
Kruskal Wallis ␹2 ⫽ 4.719, p ⫽ .094; match 2, was also a trend for errors at follow-up to be more
p ⫽ .339; match 3, p ⫽ .411; match 4, p ⫽ .259; likely to be ‘other’ errors (Wald ␹2 ⫽ 3.246,
match 5, p ⫽ .060). This indicated that the match p ⫽ .072), consistent with results reported above
variables did not distinguish significantly between (again see Table 10).
the three error response classifications (e.g., HCE, Finally, a regression analysis was conducted to
PPNE, and OE) in homophone spelling. In other investigate the difference between errors at base-
words, HCE, PPNE, and OE were similar in how line that were correct at follow-up (N ⫽ 17) and
closely they matched the stimulus word (or correct those that were not (N ⫽ 45) (i.e., to determine
response). differences between those untreated homophone
A simple overall comparison of the errors made words that improved and those that didn’t). The
at baseline with those made at follow-up assess- same predictor variables were included. Results
ment indicated that there were significantly fewer indicated that no variable was able to predict
phonologically plausible (p ⫽ .000) and signifi- improvement in homophone words over and
cantly more other (p ⫽ .001) errors at follow-up above the other variables included; however the
(see Table 10). There was no difference according power of the analyses is limited due to the small
to stimulus regularity (p ⫽ .096), number of HCE number of homophone errors that improved
(p ⫽ .568), or any match values (match 1: p ⫽ .202, (N ⫽ 17).
match 2: p ⫽ .447; match 3 p ⫽ .418; match 4,
p ⫽ .154; match 5, p ⫽ .407).
Homophone reading
A regression analysis was then conducted to
further investigate differences between the errors Results for homophone reading are shown in
at baseline and those at follow-up (i.e., to deter- Figure 9.
mine what was different about the errors after
treatment). Predictor variables included match
Total words read correctly
values 1–5, error type (i.e., PPNE, HCE, or OE),
whether the stimulus was irregular or regular, and The total number of homophone reading errors
decreased during treatment, with significantly
fewer read incorrectly at post-treatment phase 3
and follow-up 1 assessments when compared to
Table 10. Total number of homophone errors at baseline 2
and follow-up assessment
baseline 2 (PT3, p ⫽ .035; post follow-up 1,
p ⫽ .002), suggesting some treatment generalisa-
Errors Baseline Follow-up tion to homophone reading. It is interesting to
Total HCE 20 18 note that, of those words that improved (i.e.,
Total PPNE 33 9 changed from incorrect to correct), 77% (10/13)
Total OE 9 21 were irregular homophonic words. Results indi-
Total errors 62 48
cated a significant increase in the total number of
HCE: homophone confusion errors; PPNE: phonologically homophone words read correctly during treatment
plausible nonword errors; OE: other errors. but, as with spelling, there was no consistent

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2) 237


BRUNDSON, COLTHEART, NICKELS

Set 2 irregular words but not for treatment of


words in Set 1 or Set 3.
There was no significant difference between
the word sets in terms of duration of treatment
required to reach ceiling (i.e., efficiency of treat-
ment). Duration of treatment for Set 1 was 8 days,
for Set 2 it was 6 days, and for Set 3 it was 7 days.
There was also no difference between the three
word sets in degree of decline from post-treatment
to assessment at follow-up 1 (Generalised
Estimating Equation [GEE] analysis, ␹2 ⫽ 3.15,
p ⫽ .21). The only relevant change was a reduction
of errors in homophone spelling, which predomi-
nantly occurred after treatment of Set 2 (and the
use of mnemonics), but this change only
approached significance (p ⫽ .053). Thus there was
no consistent evidence that the use of mnemonics
contributed to the efficacy of treatment.
Figure 9. Percentage of responses that were errors for
homophone reading performance at baseline, during
treatment, and at post-tests. Overview of results
1. The treatment method used in this case of
developmental surface dysgraphia was highly effi-
change in the number of homophone confusion cacious, since spelling of treated words improved
errors. significantly as a direct result of treatment.
2. Although there was some reduction in total
words spelled correctly at 2 months post-
Homophone confusion errors
treatment (indicating some loss of accuracy with
The total number of homophone confusion errors time), spelling of treated words remained signifi-
(made with those words read correctly) decreased cantly improved at 4 months post cessation of
significantly following treatment phase 3 when treatment when compared to baseline.
compared to baseline 2 (binomial p ⫽ .035) but 3. The treatment method employed resulted in
not baseline 1 (p ⫽ .118), but this decline was a significant generalisation effect, since spelling of
transitory and errors had increased by follow-up 1 untreated words also improved with treatment,
(p ⫽ .000) back to baseline level (B2, p ⫽ .210; B1, though not as dramatically as spelling of treated
p ⫽ .063). words.
4. Untreated words whose spellings became
correct as a result of treatment generalisation (as
Mnemonics
compared to untreated words whose spellings
The efficacy of employing mnemonics in lexical remained incorrect) were those whose errors were
spelling treatment was evaluated according to effi- closer to being correct prior to treatment and
ciency of treatment (speed at which target word those with a higher word frequency. The degree of
spelling improved) and stability of treatment (how correctness (or degree of match to the correct
resistant the words were to decline following treat- spelling) was best characterised by a coding
ment). Note: As mentioned in the Method scheme that placed relatively more weight on exte-
section, mnemonics were used for treatment of rior letters than interior letters.

238 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2)


TREATMENT OF SURFACE DYSGRAPHIA

5. A close examination of error characteristics 1. Weekes and Coltheart’s (1996) proposal


during treatment revealed that untreated words that two treatment effects can occur as a result of
improved gradually as a result of treatment gener- item-specific training: a word-specific treatment
alisation and that this improvement predominantly effect and a more general treatment effect impact-
involved interior letters. Even those untreated ing on access to orthography.
words that did not become correct prior to their 2. Behrmann’s (1987) speculation that homo-
treatment showed some improvements in spelling. phone comprehension difficulties require item-
6. There was no evidence that the use of specific training and that homophone
mnemonics contributed to the efficacy of treatment. comprehension does not improve as a result of
7. Although reading was not specifically treated, general improvements in orthographic processing.
those words whose spelling had been treated also 3. Weekes and Coltheart’s (1996) prediction
showed an improvement in reading accuracy. that some untreated irregular words may benefit
8. The total number of error responses made in more than others depending on their level of pre-
homophone reading and spelling was also signifi- treatment representation.
cantly reduced, but the number of homophone 4. The empirical proposal that exterior letters are
confusions did not change. However, there was a particularly important for orthographic processing
significant reduction in phonologically plausible (Humphreys, Evett, & Quinlan, 1990; Jordan, 1990;
spelling errors and an increase in other errors (i.e., Perea, 1998).
orthographically based spelling attempts) during 5. The theoretical proposal that a direct nonse-
treatment. mantic route is required in models of normal read-
9. In addition, a close examination of error ing and spelling.
characteristics (from both irregular word and hom-
ophone word spelling) provided preliminary evi- Each of these proposals will be discussed in
dence that the mechanism underlying treatment turn. Orthographic processing difficulties can
generalisation involved a general improvement result from two different levels of impairment;
(that was not word-specific) in the ability to access from word-specific impairment or from a general
and use output orthography for spelling attempts. impairment or inefficiency in access to stored lexi-
cal representations. The current study adds weight
to previous research findings that treatment
focused on improving orthography (in this case
DISCUSSION irregular word spelling) can benefit both forms of
impairment. This is consistent with Weekes and
The current study adds to the small number of Coltheart’s (1996) proposal that two treatment
cognitive neuropsychological case studies report- mechanisms arise from item-specific training: a
ing successful treatment of surface dysgraphia, and word-specific effect (which results in improve-
is the first study we are aware of to focus on the ment for target words) and a more general effect
cognitive neuropsychological treatment of devel- on access to orthography (which results in gener-
opmental surface dysgraphia. The results have alisation to untreated irregular words). This expla-
implications for current theoretical proposals nation makes sense for cases of acquired
about lexical processes in reading and spelling, as dysgraphia, but can it be applied to MC, a devel-
well as clinical methods used for treatment of opmental case? Adults with acquired dysgraphia
surface dysgraphia. will have had a normal store of lexical representa-
tions prior to their injury (assuming they were lit-
erate) and thus can benefit from treatments that
Theoretical implications
improve general access to those orthographic
The current results provide support for a number representations that remain relatively intact. But
of theoretical proposals, including the following. can a child with long-standing poor or unreliable

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2) 239


BRUNDSON, COLTHEART, NICKELS

access to orthography actually form adequate depending on their level of pretreatment represen-
word-specific representations that would then tation. More specifically, words with partial repre-
enable them to benefit from generalised improve- sentation prior to treatment will benefit more than
ments in access to orthography? We would argue those with little or no orthographic representation
in the affirmative, for the following reason. It is (Weekes & Coltheart, 1996). In order to investi-
conceivable that a child with poor access to gate this proposal the current study analysed the
orthography could gradually build up correct (or characteristics of untreated words that improved
partially correct) representations of individual as a result of treatment generalisation and exam-
words via multiple exposures to the correct ined how they differed from those untreated words
spelling, though presumably the formation of that didn’t improve. Pretreatment representation
orthographic representations would be delayed. was measured using the degree of match (or simi-
The child could then continue to have difficulty larity) between the error response and correct
accessing (or retrieving) these representations spelling prior to treatment. In addition, an attempt
(resulting in ongoing severe reading and spelling was made to investigate the actual mechanism
difficulties) but would show significant improve- underlying treatment generalisation through analy-
ments as a result of treatment generalisation, as ses of changes in error characteristics as a result of
seen in the current case MC. For MC, significant treatment. Overall and in combination, the results
treatment generalisation did occur, which provides of these investigations provide some support for
additional evidence that MC had a developmental the proposals of Weekes and Coltheart (1996).
impairment in general access to orthography and First, the current results indicate that, in general,
not just impairment at the level of orthographic untreated irregular words whose errors are closer to
representation.19 being correct prior to treatment (i.e., have a closer
The current results also provide strong support match to the correct spelling) are more likely to
for Behrmann’s (1987) speculation that homo- improve as a result of treatment generalisation.
phones require word-specific training (presumably However, the coding schemes and match values
in orthography-semantic association) so, unlike employed in the analyses did not distinguish
irregular words, do not benefit from a general between error type prior to treatment; that is, both
orthographic treatment effect. In the current phonologically plausible nonword spelling errors
study, the benefits of treatment generalisation did and nonphonological spelling errors (which often
not extend to orthography-semantic connections contained partial orthographic information) had
for untreated homophones. Although homophone similar match values. This is because both phono-
spelling improved, the improvement was only evi- logically and orthographically based errors can be
dent in the total number of homophone stimuli highly similar to the correct response (e.g., phono-
spelled correctly. There was no reduction in the logical attempts: soul → sowl, won → wun; non-
number of homophone confusion errors. phonological orthographic attempts: eight →
Of most interest, in the current study, were the etaght, their → thir). In addition, phonologically
results of the novel investigation into mechanisms plausible nonword errors are also often ortho-
underlying treatment generalisation, analyses that graphically similar to the correct response, so there
have been lacking in previous research. Weekes may have also been some overlap between error
and Coltheart (1996) propose that some untreated classifications. Therefore, all we can conclude
irregular words may benefit more than others, from this finding is that untreated irregular words

19
This is based on the assumption that the generalisation effects in the current study were due to improvements in orthographic
processing. An anonymous reviewer made the valid point that generalisation effects can occur following improvement in other
aspects of the spelling system, such as the graphemic buffer. However, in the current case, this would be unlikely as no such other
impairments were detected in assessment.

240 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2)


TREATMENT OF SURFACE DYSGRAPHIA

that improve with treatment generalisation have a reasonable, however, given the consistent findings
closer match to the correct spelling prior to treat- of various analyses, to suggest that some matching
ment. This does not provide definitive support for methods (or coding schemes) may be better than
a proposal that these words have partial ortho- others for predicting which untreated words will
graphic representations prior to treatment. improve with treatment. In the current study,
However, investigation into changes in error char- matching methods for coding schemes 4 and 5
acteristics during treatment provided some evi- (which both emphasised the exterior letters)
dence for an underlying treatment mechanism of seemed consistently superior for predicting which
an improved ability to access and use orthographic untreated irregular words would benefit from
representations for spelling attempts. Observation treatment generalisation. A post hoc examination
of untreated irregular words that improved over of the nature of MC’s actual error responses for
multiple testing points during treatment revealed untreated words was also highly consistent with
that many showed gradual improvement in degree this finding, particularly for the relative impor-
of similarity to target orthography. In addition, for tance of exterior letter positions. For those irregular
homophone spelling (all untreated) there was a words that improved with treatment generalisa-
reduction in phonologically plausible nonword tion, a large proportion of the errors at baseline
errors, which is consistent with incomplete but (75%) contained correct letters in the first and last
improved access to representations in orthography letter position. In contrast, for those untreated
resulting in less reliance on sublexical processing. words that failed to improve, less than half of the
There was also a co-occurring increase in the num- error responses (46%) contained correct first and
ber of ‘other’ errors, which essentially reflected an last letters. For interior letters, this effect was not
increase in attempts that were not phonologically present: Interior letters were correct in 15% of
plausible but more often represented incomplete untreated words that improved and 9% of untreated
orthographically mediated attempts. words that did not improve. This finding makes
In the current study, extensive analyses were theoretical sense as previous research has also indi-
conducted to determine the best predictors of cated the importance of exterior letters (over inter-
treatment generalisation. The main focus of the nal letter positions) in orthographic processing
analyses were the coding schemes (and match val- (Humphreys et al., 1990; Jordan, 1990; Perea,
ues) to determine not only whether degree of pre- 1998). Most previous studies have investigated
treatment ‘correctness’ could predict generalisation letter position effects in visual word recognition, but
(as discussed above), but also whether one method the current results provide some preliminary
of coding ‘correctness’ was superior to another. evidence that exterior letters may also be important
Overall, the results suggest that the best way to in spelling.
determine how correct an error response is prior to It is interesting to consider how the results for
treatment, in order to predict treatment generali- predicting which untreated words will benefit most
sation, is to use a coding method where the posi- from generalisation (as just discussed) relate to
tion of each letter is coded relative to the first and results describing the nature of error change during
last letters—that is, a coding method that places treatment. That is, can we speculate not only
relatively more weight on exterior letters. The best about which words will improve but also about
statistical model for predicting generalisation for how they will improve? As discussed above, error
untreated irregular words included this particular responses with a closer match (particularly involv-
method of coding combined with written fre- ing exterior letter positions) to the correct response
quency. However, given the very high correlation will benefit most from treatment generalisation. So
between all of the coding methods employed at baseline, those words with correct initial and final
(match variables), the authors would hesitate to letter positions are more likely to become correct
argue definitively that one matching method was as a result of treatment generalisation (many of
significantly superior to the others. It would seem these words would therefore have errors affecting

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2) 241


BRUNDSON, COLTHEART, NICKELS

interior letter positions). Interestingly, an examina- developmental dysgraphia. Results suggest the
tion of how error responses change during treat- following.
ment (prior to becoming correct) indicated a
1. Carefully designed theoretically based treat-
highly significant change in vowel-centred match
ment methods for developmental dysgraphia can
schemes (match 2 and 3), possibly indicating a
be highly efficacious.
gradual improvement in interior vowel patterns
2. Lexical treatment of developmental surface
during treatment. This was apparent for both
dysgraphia can result in dramatic improvements in
words that became correct as a result of treatment
target word spelling but also significant improve-
generalisation and those that didn’t, though the
ments in spelling of other untreated words.
latter took longer to respond to treatment generali-
3. Further improvements in efficiency of treat-
sation effects.
ment may be achieved by targeting particular sub-
Finally, in regard to theoretical implications,
sets of words, specifically those that are less likely
homophone confusions with irregularly spelled
to benefit from treatment generalisation.
homophones are rarely investigated or reported in
4. Lexical spelling treatment methods can
cases of surface dyslexia and more particularly in
result in substantial improvements in reading of
developmental surface dyslexia. As Coltheart et al.
target words.
(1983) discuss, these errors are of theoretical
5. It must not be assumed that mnemonics
importance as they provide additional evidence
always increase efficacy in treatment of irregular
for the existence of the direct nonsemantic route
word spelling in developmental dysgraphia.
in reading connecting the orthographic input
lexicon to the phonological output lexicon. When Each of these clinical implications will be dis-
an irregular homophone is read aloud correctly but cussed briefly. The theoretically based lexical treat-
a homophone confusion is made in comprehen- ment method used in the current study was highly
sion (e.g., reading the word ‘queue’ correctly but successful in improving irregular word spelling in a
providing the wrong definition “the actor needed a child with long-standing delays in the development
cue,” see Table 5 for other examples) the theoreti- of orthographic word reading and spelling. The
cal explanation must be as follows. The correct treatment improvements were dramatic, occurred
word entry is successfully accessed in the ortho- rapidly, and were also maintained over time. This
graphic input lexicon but word-specific communi- study shows that treatments previously used success-
cation with the semantic system fails. As a result, fully in cases of adult-acquired surface dysgraphia
output phonology is obtained via the direct route can be very effective for treatment of developmental
linking the orthographic input lexicon to the surface dysgraphia. It is hoped that this case study
phonological output lexicon, which prevents will encourage further research into treatment of
word-specific semantic activation (route shown by childhood reading and spelling disorders (a research
arrows 13, 15, 11, Figure 1), giving rise to the area that remains extremely limited), especially given
homophone confusion error. Documentation of the potentially dramatic benefits for the child.
multiple examples of such errors in the current The lexical treatment methods used in the cur-
developmental case, in addition to the case of rent study not only resulted in dramatic improve-
Coltheart et al. (1983), requires current models of ments in the specific words chosen for treatment
reading and spelling to include not only the but also resulted in better spelling of other
lexical-semantic and sublexical reading routes, but untreated words. Essentially, clinically, this implies
also the third nonsemantic reading route. that the benefits of treatment can reach well beyond
the small set of target words and that treatment
focused on single words can also benefit other
Clinical implications
words in the system that are not fully developed.
The current results also have clinical implications In addition, the current results suggest that it
for management of dysgraphia, particularly may be possible to predict, prior to treatment,

242 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2)


TREATMENT OF SURFACE DYSGRAPHIA

which words will benefit most from treatment significant therapy advantage for the mnemonic
generalisation by examining the nature of errors at technique. It is important to note that LP report-
baseline. This may therefore allow increased effi- edly had semantic deficits. In contrast, a recent
ciency in treatment by allowing the separation of treatment study focused on improving visual word
target words into two groups prior to treatment. recognition in developmental dyslexia (also
The first group would contain those words least focused on high-frequency words) questions the
likely to benefit from treatment generalisation, efficacy of mnemonics when compared to ‘word-
that is, those words with a low written frequency only’ treatment methods (Brunsdon, Hannan,
and with spelling errors that are not close to being Coltheart, & Nickels, 2002). In the current study,
correct prior to treatment (i.e., spelling errors that mnemonics were used for treatment stage two
do not have a high degree of match with the cor- (word Set 2 only) and, in general, results suggested
rect spelling). These errors would be targeted for no additional benefit from use of mnemonics.
word-specific treatment. The second group would Neither of these developmental cases benefited
contain those words most likely to benefit from from mnemonics and neither presented with
treatment generalisation, those words with a high semantic deficits. Overall, it would be reasonable
written frequency and with spelling errors that are to suspect that the use of mnemonics may be
close to being correct prior to treatment. These beneficial when the treatment aim is specifically to
words could be deliberately excluded from the train orthography-semantic connections, as in
treatment group as we might predict that they specific homophone reading and spelling treat-
would improve during treatment as a result of ment programmes, where the individual demon-
treatment generalisation. This proposal, that treat- strates impaired access to semantic information for
ment planning could potentially maximise effi- target words (though, even in this instance, a ver-
ciency of treatment, requires further investigation bally mediated orthography-semantic association
and research, however. treatment may be equally effective). In contrast,
Treatment of irregular word spelling in the cur- mnemonics may not offer any treatment advan-
rent case also resulted in significant improvement tage when treatment simply targets irregular word
in reading of treated words. The improvement in reading or spelling, when the individual already
reading of treated words is not an unexpected knows the meaning of target words.
finding as, although treatment focused on output Finally, with regard to clinical implications,
orthography and spelling, the treatment method surface dyslexia and dysgraphia are still commonly
also involved constant visual exposure to target overlooked or misdiagnosed in children. MC’s
words. In clinical terms this result implies that in case provides a typical example of this. His reading
developmental dysgraphia and dyslexia treatment and spelling difficulties were apparent from early
targeted at spelling is also likely to benefit reading. in primary school. He had undergone numerous
Mnemonics are commonly used as a treatment reading assessments and as a result received extra
tool in treatment of surface dyslexia and dys- reading tuition during primary school. Despite
graphia and are also employed for teaching begin- this, the exact nature of his reading and spelling
ner readers early in primary school. However, the disorder was not precisely diagnosed until the cur-
efficacy of mnemonics is not often empirically rent investigation, when MC was 12 years old and
evaluated (Behrmann, 1987; Byng & Coltheart, in high school. Although it is certainly never too
1986; Coltheart & Byng, 1989; Weekes & late to begin treatment, appropriate treatment ear-
Coltheart, 1996). De Partz et al. (1992) evaluated lier in primary school would obviously be better. It
the efficacy of mnemonics (compared to writing is hoped that the current case will provide a timely
to dictation and simple copying of the word with reminder of the importance of theoretically based
no mnemonic) in their treatment of spelling in assessment early in primary school for identifica-
an adult, LP, with acquired dysgraphia, which tion and then treatment of lexical spelling (and
targeted high-frequency words. They found a reading) disorders.

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (0) 243


BRUNDSON, COLTHEART, NICKELS

Theoretically based assessment and diagnosis is REFERENCES


fundamental to treatment planning and design. To
Aliminosa, D., McCloskey, M., Goodman-Schulman,
devise a successful treatment programme targeted R., & Sokol, S. (1993). Remediation of acquired dys-
at a particular impairment, the fundamental graphia as a technique for testing interpretations of
requirement is to first determine the nature of that deficits. Aphasiology, 7, 55–69.
impairment. Although the treatment design Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & Van Rijn, H.
(including the intensive nature of treatment and (1993). The CELEX lexical database (CD-ROM).
repeated exposure to target stimuli) is important, if Philadelphia: Linguistic Data Consortium, University
treatment is not targeted specifically at the impair- of Pennsylvania.
ment, success is likely to be limited. The current Beauvois, M., & Derouesne, J. (1981). Lexical or ortho-
case MC had undergone numerous special educa- graphic agraphia. Brain, 104, 21–50.
Beeson, P. M. (1999). Treating acquired writing impair-
tion programmes, including some periods of inten-
ment: Strengthening graphemic representations.
sive daily training. However, given that his surface Aphasiology, 13, 767–785.
dyslexia and dysgraphia had not previously been Beeson, P. M., & Hillis, A. E. (2001). Comprehension
diagnosed it must be suspected that treatment was and production of written words. In R. Chapey
not targeted appropriately at irregular words. (Ed.), Language intervention strategies in aphasia and
related neurogenic communication disorders (4th ed.).
Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
Final comments Beeson, P., Hirsch, F., & Rewega, M. (2002). Successful
In summary, the current study demonstrates suc- single-word writing treatment: Experimental analy-
cessful application of cognitive neuropsychological ses of four cases. Aphasiology, 16, 473–491.
Behrmann, M. (1987). The rites of righting writing:
theories and methods to the assessment and reme-
Homophone remediation in acquired dysgraphia.
diation of developmental spelling difficulties. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 4, 365–384.
Treatment was highly successful and resulted in a Behrmann, M., & Bub, B. (1992). Surface dyslexia and
dramatic improvement in spelling and reading of dysgraphia: Dual routes, single lexicon. Cognitive
irregular words that was stable over time. Neuropsychology, 9, 209–252.
Treatment also generalised to spelling of untreated Behrmann, M., & Byng, S. (1992). A cognitive
irregular words. Novel analyses indicate that approach to the neurorehabilitation of acquired lan-
untreated words that improved were closer to being guage disorders. In D. Margolin (Ed.), Cognitive
correct prior to treatment than untreated words neuropsychology in clinical practice. New York: Oxford
that failed to improve. Results also suggest that, University Press.
Bonazzi, C. A. (1996). Acquired dysgraphia with selec-
prior to treatment, it may actually be possible to
tive damage to the graphemic buffer: A single case
predict which words will benefit most from treat- report. Italian Journal of Neurological Science, 17,
ment generalisation. There is also preliminary evi- 249–254.
dence that the actual mechanism underlying Brunsdon, R. K., Hannan, T. J., Coltheart, M., &
treatment generalisation involves improved access Nickels, L. (2002). Treatment of lexical processing in
to orthographic representations, resulting in an mixed dyslexia: A case study. Neuropsychological
increased tendency to attempt to employ ortho- Rehabilitation, 12, 385–418.
graphic knowledge and a reduced tendency to rely Bub, B., & Kertesz, A. (1982). Evidence for lexico-
entirely on phoneme to grapheme conversion. graphic processing in a subject with preserved writ-
ten over oral single word naming. Brain, 105,
Results have both theoretical and clinical implica-
679–717.
tions and raise a number of interesting proposals
Byng, S., & Coltheart, M (1986). Aphasia therapy
worthy of future research. research: Methodological requirements and illustra-
Manuscript received 8 April 2003 tive results. In I. G. Nilsson & E. Hjelmquist (Eds.),
Revised manuscript received 3 October 2003 Communication and handicap: Aspects of psychological
Revised manuscript accepted 16 February 2004 compensation and technical aids. Amsterdam: North-
PrEview proof published online 15 December 2004 Holland.

244 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2)


TREATMENT OF SURFACE DYSGRAPHIA

Campbell, R., & Butterworth, B. (1985). Phonological Conway, T. W., Heilman, P., Rothi, L. J. G., Alexander,
dyslexia and dysgraphia in a highly literate subject: A A. W., Adair, J., Crosson, B. A., & Heilman, K. M.
developmental case with associated deficits of (1998). Treatment of a case of phonological alexia
phonemic processing and awareness. Quarterly with agraphia using the Auditory Discrimination in
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37, 435–475. Depth (ADD) program. Journal of the International
Caramazza, A., Miceli, G., Villa, G., & Romani, C. Neuropsychological Society, 4, 608–620.
(1987). The role of the graphemic buffer in spelling: Cubelli, R., Guiducci, A., & Consolmagno, P. (2000).
Evidence from a case of acquired dysgraphia. Afferent dysgraphia after right cerebral stroke: An
Cognition, 26, 59–85. autonomous syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 44,
Carlomagno, S., Lavarone, A., & Colombo, A. (1994). 629–644.
Cognitive approaches to writing rehabilitation: Davis, C. (1999). The self-organizing lexical acquisition
From single case to group studies. In G. W. and recognition (SOLAR) model of visual word recogni-
Humphreys & M. J. Riddoch (Eds.), Cognitive tion. Unpublished PhD thesis, University of New
neuropsychology and cognitive rehabilitation. Hove, South Wales, Australia.
UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd. De Partz, M., Seron, X., & Van der Linden, M. (1992).
Carlomagno, S., & Parlato, V. (1989). Writing rehabili- Re-education of a surface dysgraphia with a visual
tation in brain damaged adult patients: A cognitive imagery strategy. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 9,
approach. In X. Seron (Ed.), Cognitive approaches 369–401.
in neuropsychological rehabilitation. Hove, UK: Destreri, N., Farina, E., Alberoni, M., Pomati, S.,
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd. Nichelli, P., & Mariani, C. (2000). Selective upper-
Coltheart, M. (1985). Cognitive neuropsychology and case dysgraphia with loss of visual imagery of letter
the study of reading. In M. I. Posner & O. S. M. forms: A window on the organisation of graphomo-
Marin (Eds.), Attention and performance XI. tor patterns. Brain and Language, 71, 353–372.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.
Edwards, V., & Hogben, J. (1999). New norms for com-
Coltheart, M. (1987). Functional architecture of the
paring children’s lexical and nonlexical reading: A
language-processing system. In M. Coltheart,
further look at subtyping dyslexia. Australian Journal
G. Sartori, & R. Job (Eds.), The cognitive neuropsy-
of Psychology, 5, 37–49.
chology of language. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum
Ellis, A. W. (1982). Spelling and writing (and reading
Associates Ltd.
and speaking). In A. Ellis (Ed.), Normality and
Coltheart, M., & Byng, S. (1989). A treatment for sur-
pathology in cognitive functions. London: Academic
face dyslexia. In X. Seron (Ed.), Cognitive approaches
Press.
in neuropsychological rehabilitation. Hove, UK:
Ellis, A. W. (1988). Modelling the writing process.
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd.
Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). In G. Denes, C. Semenza, P. Bisiacchi, &
Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel- E. Andreewsky (Eds.), Perspectives in cognitive
distributed-processing approaches. Psychological neuropsychology. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum
Review, 100, 589–608. Associates Ltd.
Coltheart, M., Davelaar, E., Jonasson, J. T., & Besner, D. Ellis, A., & Young, A. (1988). Human cognitive neu-
(1977). Access to the internal lexicon. In S. Dornic ropsychology. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum
(Ed.), Attention and performance VI. Hillsdale, NJ: Associates Ltd.
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc. Ellis, A., Young, A., & Flude, B. (1987). “Afferent dys-
Coltheart, M., & Leahy, J. (1996). Assessment of lexical graphia” in a patient and in normal subjects.
and nonlexical reading abilities in children: Some Cognitive Neuropsychology, 4, 465–496.
normative data. Australian Journal of Psychology, 48, Friedman, R., & Alexander, M. (1990). Written spelling
136–140. agraphia. Brain and Language, 36, 503–517
Coltheart, M., Masterson, J., Byng, S., Prior, M., & Gathercole, S., & Baddeley, A. (1996). The Children’s
Riddoch, M. J. (1983). Surface dyslexia. Quarterly Test of Nonword Repetition. London: Psychological
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35, 469–495. Corporation.
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Goodman, R. A., & Caramazza, A. (1986a).
Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded Dissociation of spelling errors in written and oral
model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. spelling: The role of allographic conversion in writ-
Psychological Review, 108, 204–256. ing. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 179–206.

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2) 245


BRUNDSON, COLTHEART, NICKELS

Goodman, R. A., & Caramazza, A. (1986b). Aspects of Kay, J., Lesser, R., & Coltheart, M. (1992). Psycho-
the spelling process: Evidence from a case of linguistic Assessments of Language Processing in
acquired dysgraphia. Language and Cognitive Pro- Aphasia (PALPA). Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum
cesses, 1, 263–296. Associates Ltd.
Hanley, J., Hastie, K., & Kay, J. (1992). Developmental Kazdin, A. (1982). Single-case research designs: Methods
surface dyslexia and dysgraphia: An orthographic for clinical and applied settings. New York: Oxford
processing impairment. Quarterly Journal of University Press.
Experimental Psychology, 44A, 285–319. Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S. L. (1997). NEPSY: A
Hanley, J., Negassa, A., Edwardes, M., & Forrester, J. developmental neuropsychological assessment (manual).
(2003). Statistical analysis of correlated data using San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation/
generalized estimating equations: An orientation. Harcourt Brace.
American Journal of Epidemiology, 157, 364–375. Luzzatti, C., Colombo, C., Frustaci, M., & Vitolo, F.
Hanley, J. R., & Peters, S. (2001). Allograph errors and (2000). Rehabilitation of spelling along the sub-
impaired access to graphic motor codes in a case of word-level routine. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation,
unilateral agraphia of the dominant left hand. 10, 249–278
Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18, 307–321. Marien, P., Pickut, B. A., Engelborghs, S., Martin, J., &
Hatfield, F. M. (1983). Aspects of acquired dysgraphia De Deyn, P. P. (2001). Phonological agraphia fol-
and implications for re-education. In C. Code & D. lowing a focal anterior insulo-opercular infarction.
Muller (Eds.), Aphasia therapy. London: Edward Neuropsychologia, 39, 845–855.
Arnold. Miceli, G., & Silveri, M. C. (1985). Cognitive analysis
Heiman, G. (2001). Understanding research methods and of a case of pure dysgraphia. Brain and Language, 25,
statistics: An integrated introduction for psychology. 187–212.
New York: Houghton Mifflin Company. Mortley, J., Enderby, P., & Petheram, B. (2001). Using
Hillis, A. E., & Caramazza, A. (1986). Model-driven a computer to improve functional writing in a
treatment of dysgraphia. In R. H. Brookshire (Ed.), patient with severe dysgraphia. Aphasiology, 15,
Clinical aphasiology. Minneapolis: BRK Publishers 443–461.
Hillis, A. E., & Caramazza, A. (1994). Theories of lexi- Ogden, J. A. (1996). Phonological dyslexia and phono-
cal processing and rehabilitation of lexical deficits. In logical dysgraphia following left and right hemi-
G. W. Humphreys & M. J. Riddoch (Eds.), spherectomy. Neuropsychologia, 34, 905–918.
Cognitive neuropsychology and cognitive rehabilitation. Patterson, K. (1986). Lexical but nonsemantic spelling?
Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 341–367.
Hillis Trupe, A. E. (1986). Effectiveness of training Patterson, K. (1988). Acquired disorders of spelling. In
phoneme to grapheme conversion. In R. H. G. Denes, C. Semenza, P. Bisiacchi, & E. Andreewsky
Brookshire (Ed.), Clinical aphasiology. Minneapolis: (Eds.), Perspectives in cognitive neuropsychology. Hove,
BRK Publishers. UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd.
Humphreys, G. W., Evett, L. J., & Quinlan, P. T. Patterson, K., & Shewell, C. (1987). Speak and spell:
(1990). Orthographic processing in visual word Dissociations and word-class effects. In M. Coltheart,
identification. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 517–560. G. Sartori, & R. Job (Eds.), Cognitive neuropsychology
Iribarren, C., Jarema, G., & Lecours, A. (2001). of language. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Two different dysgraphic syndromes in regular Ltd.
orthography, Spanish. Brain and Language, 77, Perea, M. (1998). Orthographic neighbours are not all
166–175. equal: Evidence using an identification technique.
Jordan, T. R. (1990). Presenting words without interior Language and Cognitive Processes, 13, 77–90.
letters: Superiority over single letters and influence Piccirilli, M., Petrillo, S., & Poli, R. (1992). Dysgraphia
of postmask boundaries. Journal of Experimental and selective impairment of the graphemic
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, buffer. Italian Journal of Neurological Sciences, 13,
893–909. 113–117.
Katz, R., & Deser, T. (1991). Distinguishing representa- Plaut, D., Seidenberg, M., McClelland, J., & Patterson,
tion deficits and processing deficits in a case of K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired
acquired dysgraphia. Quarterly Journal of Experimental word reading: Computational principles in quasi-
Psychology, 43A, 249–266. regular domains. Psychological Review, 103, 56–115.

246 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2)


TREATMENT OF SURFACE DYSGRAPHIA

Posteraro, L., Zinelli, P., & Mazzucchi, A. (1988). Seidenberg, M., & McClelland, J. (1989). A distributed,
Selective impairment of the graphemic buffer in developmental model of word recognition and nam-
acquired dysgraphia: A case study. Brain and ing. Psychological Review, 96, 523–568.
Language, 35, 274–286. Seron, X., Deloche, G., Moulard, G., & Rousselle, M.
Pound, C. (1996). Writing rehabilitation using pre- (1980). A computer based therapy for the treatment
served oral spelling: A case for separate output of aphasic subjects with writing disorders. Journal of
buffers. Aphasiology, 10, 283–296. Speech and Hearing Disorders, 45, 45–58.
Rapcsak, S. Z. (1997). Disorders of writing. In L. J. Shallice, T. (1981). Phonological agraphia and the lexi-
Gonzalez Rothi & K. M. Heilman (Eds.), Apraxia: cal route in writing. Brain, 104, 413–429.
The neuropsychology of action. Hove, UK: Psychology Shaughnessy, J., & Zechmeister, E. (1990). Research
Press. methods in psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Rapcsak, S. Z., & Beeson, P. M. (2000). Agraphia. In Silveri, M., Misciagna, S., Leggio, M., & Molinari, M.
S. E. Nadeau & L. J. Gonzalez Rothi (Eds.), Aphasia (1997). Spatial dysgraphia and cerebellar lesion: A
and language: Theory to practice. New York: Guilford case report. Neurology, 48, 1529–1532.
Press. Snowling, M., Stackhouse, J., & Rack, J. (1986).
Rapp, B., & Caramazza, A. (1997). From graphemes Phonological dyslexia and dysgraphia: A developmen-
to abstract letter shapes: Levels of representation in tal analysis. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 309–339.
written spelling. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Snowling, M., Stothard, S., & McLean, J. (1996).
Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1130–1152. Graded Nonword Reading Test. Bury St Edmunds,
Rapp, B., & Kane, A. (2002). Remediation of deficits UK: Thames Valley Test Company.
affecting different components of the spelling pro- Temple, C. M. (1985). Developmental surface dysgraphia:
cess. Aphasiology, 16, 439–454. A case report. Applied Psycholinguistics, 6, 391–405.
Rastle, K., & Coltheart, M. (1999). Lexical and non- Temple, C. M. (1986). Developmental dysgraphias.
lexical phonological priming in reading aloud.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38A,
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception
77–110.
and Performance, 25, 461–481.
Temple, C. M. (1988). Developmental dyslexia and dys-
Robinson, G., & Weekes, B. (1995). Subtypes of devel-
graphia persistence in middle age. Journal of
opmental dysgraphia. In J. Fourez & N. Page (Eds.),
Communication Disorders, 21, 189–207.
Treatment issues and long-term outcomes. Brisbane,
Temple, C. M. (1990). Foop is still Floop: A six year
Australia: Academic Press.
follow-up of phonological dyslexia and dysgraphia.
Roeltgen, D. P., & Blaskey, P. (1992). Processes, break-
Reading and Writing, 2, 209–221.
downs and remediation in developmental disorders
Venneri, A., Cubelli, R., & Caffarra, P. (1994).
of reading and spelling. In D. Margolin (Ed.),
Cognitive neuropsychology in clinical practice. New Perseverative dysgraphia: A selective disorder in writ-
York: Oxford University Press. ing double letters. Neuropsychologia, 32, 923–931.
Roeltgen, D. P., & Heilman, K. M. (1985). Review of Wechsler, D. (1991). Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence
agraphia and a proposal for an anatomically-based Scale for Children—Third Edition (WISC-III). San
neuropsychological model of writing. Applied Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Psycholinguistics, 6, 205–229. Weekes, B. (1994). A cognitive neuropsychological
Roeltgen, D. P., & Tucker, D. M. (1988). Developmental analysis of allograph errors from a patient with
phonological and lexical agraphia in adults. Brain and acquired dysgraphia. Aphasiology, 8, 409–425.
Language, 35, 287–300. Weekes, B., & Coltheart, M. (1996). Surface dyslexia
Romani, C., Ward, J., & Olsen, A. (1999). Developmental and surface dysgraphia: Treatment studies and their
surface dysgraphia: What is the underlying cognitive theoretical implications. Cognitive Neuropsychology,
impairment. Quarterly Journal of Experimental 13, 277–315.
Psychology, 52A, 97–128. Wilkinson, G. (1993). Wide Range Achievement Test
Schiller, N., Greenhall, J., Shelton, J., & Caramazza, A. (WRAT-3) Administration Manual. Wilmington,
(2001). Serial order effect in spelling errors: Evidence DE: Jastak Associates.
from two dysgraphic patients. Neurocase, 7, 1–14. Zesiger, P., Martory, M., & Mayer, E. (1997). Writing
Scott, C., & Byng, S. (1989). Computer assisted reme- without graphic motor patterns: A case of dys-
diation of a homophone comprehension disorder in graphia for letters and digits sparing shorthand writ-
surface dyslexia. Aphasiology, 3, 301–320. ing. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 743–763.

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2) 247


BRUNDSON, COLTHEART, NICKELS

APPENDIX A

MC’s errors for irregular, regular, and nonword reading and spelling (using the Coltheart &
Leahy, 1996, word and nonword lists)
Regular words Reading response Spelling response Irregular words Reading response Spelling response

1. bed ✓ ✓ 16. head ✓ haed


2. brandy ✓ ✓ 17. iron ✓ iorne
3. chance “change” carns 18. island ✓ ✓
4. check ✓ ✓ 19. lose /ləυz/ lowes
5. chicken ✓ ✓ 20. meringue “merging” merrag
6. context ✓ contexs 21. pint /pnt/ pient
7. cord ✓ corwd 22. pretty /prεti:/ pritte
8. curb ✓ cerb 23. routine /raυtn/ rooten
9. drop ✓ ✓ 24. shoe ✓ ✓
10. flannel /flntεl/ ✓ 25. soul /səυl/ ✓
11. free ✓ ✓ 26. sure /ʃu::/ shore
12. hand ✓ ✓ 27. tomb /tɒmb/ ✓
13. life ✓ ✓ 28. wolf ✓ ✓
14. long ✓ ✓ 29. work ✓ werk
15. luck ✓ ✓ 30. yacht /jtʃt/ yote
16. market ✓ ✓
17. marsh ✓ ✓ Nonwords Reading response Spelling response
18. middle ✓ ✓
19. mist ✓ ✓ 1. aspy /sfiə/ ✓
20. navy ✓ navey 2. baft ✓ ✓
21. need ✓ ✓ 3. bick “bike” ✓
22. nerve /n:vi:/ nerv 4. bleaner /bεnə/ ✓ (blener)
23. peril “pearl” peral 5. boril “boil” ✓ (boral)
24. plant ✓ ✓ 6. borp ✓ ✓
25. pump ✓ pamp 7. brennet /brεnt/ ✓ (brenet)
26. stench ✓ staench 8. brinth ✓ ✓
27. tail ✓ ✓ 9. crat ✓ ✓
28. take ✓ ✓ 10. delk ✓ ✓ (delck)
29. weasel /wɒzli:/ ✓ 11. doash ✓ ✓(dowsh)
30. wedding ✓ weding 12. drick ✓ ✓
13. farl ✓ ✓ ( farll )
14. framp ✓ ✓
Irregular words Reading response Spelling response
15. ganten ✓ ✓
1. blood /blυd/ ✓ 16. gop ✓ ✓
2. bouquet /bəυkεt/ bowckay 17. grenty “gentle” granty
3. bowl ✓ ✓ 18. gurve “gravy” ✓ ( girv)
4. break ✓ braek 19. hest ✓ ✓
5. brooch ✓ browtch 20. norf ✓ ✓
6. ceiling ✓ cilling 21. peef /pəf/ ✓
7. choir “chore” quwiyer 22. peng “peg” pag
8. colonel /kɒləυnel/ cernal 23. pite ✓ ✓(piet)
9. come ✓ ✓ 24. pofe ✓ ✓(powf )
10. cough “caught” cofe 25. rint ✓ ✓
11. eye ✓ ✓ 26. seldent ✓ ✓(selldent)
12. friend ✓ ✓ 27. spatch ✓ ✓
13. gauge /g:rd/ gaeg 28. stendle ✓ ✓ (stendell)
14. give ✓ ✓ 29. tapple ✓ ✓ (tapell )
15. good ✓ ✓ 30. trope ✓ trop

248 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2)


TREATMENT OF SURFACE DYSGRAPHIA

APPENDIX B

Homophone stimuli
Irregular Regular Irregular Regular Irregular Regular

key ring sure shore one plane


quay wring bear bare won plain
loch lock heard herd aisle seen
stalk stork none nun isle scene
pear pair bread bred where missed
break brake waste waist wear mist
bury berry raise rays whole hole
seize sees soul sole knead
suite sweet queue cue need
son sun earn urn tax
prays praise two male tacks
build billed to mail sail
know no their fair sale
some sum there fare tail
steak stake would ate tale
heir air wood eight maid
great grate watt hire made
what higher cruise

APPENDIX C

Homophone confusion errors in reading at baseline 1


Target word Read aloud correctly? Spoken definition provided by MC

wring yes “the telephone rings”


pear yes “socks. . . . pairs of them”
mist yes “I missed the ball”
brake yes “I break the vase”
tale yes “there is a tail on a dog”
prays yes “a cat preys on a bird”
steak yes “I stuck a stake in the ground”
mail yes “the male toilet”
male yes “the mail came. . . . was delivered”
their yes “there is the ball”
whole yes “there is a hole in the ground”
queue yes “the actor needed a cue”
crews yes “a boat cruise. . . . a cruise ship”

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2) 249


BRUNDSON, COLTHEART, NICKELS

APPENDIX D

Homophone errors in spelling at baseline 1


Target Homophone Phonologically plausible Other
homophone MC’s written response confusion errors nonword errors errors

quay key 1
loch lock 1
stalk stork 1
wring ring 1
whole hole 1
knead need 1
scene seen 1
hire higher 1
maid made 1
fare fair 1
sum some 1
heard herd 1
pear pare 1
missed mistd 1
tax taxs 1
brake braek 1
break braek 1
sail sayal 1
tale taile 1
tail taile 1
bury berrey 1
berry berrey 1
prays praes 1
Praise praes 1
steak staek 1
stake staek 1
mail maile 1
male maeal 1
heir airr 1
there thair 1
watt whot 1
grate graet 1
great graet 1
sure shor 1
shore shor 1
bear bair 1
bare bair 1
build biuld 1
won wun 1
waist waest 1
raise raes 1
rays raes 1
soul sowl 1
sole sowl 1
cruise croos 1
crews croos 1
aisle ieal 1
earn ern 1

250 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2)


TREATMENT OF SURFACE DYSGRAPHIA

APPENDIX D (continued)

Homophone errors in spelling at baseline 1


Target Homophone Phonologically plausible Other
homophone MC’s written response confusion errors nonword errors errors

where whair 1
wear whair 1
tacks taxes 1
seize sesy 1
sweet swety 1
suite swets 1
plain plan 1
higher highter 1
eight etaght 1
their thir 1
none nune 1
queue qu 1
cue qu 1
isle lias 1
bred breaed 1

Phonologically plausible nonword errors refer to those errors for which MC employed plausible sound to letter conversion in his
spelling response (this classification was not restricted to the most common phoneme to grapheme rules).

APPENDIX E

Irregular word responses (from Set 3): Those responses that are incorrect at baseline 1,
baseline 2, and after treatment of Set 1 words but are correct after treatment of Set 2 words
Target irregular word At baseline 1 At baseline 2 Post-treatment 1

want wont wont wont


kind ciend cind cind
move more moov morve*
range rang rang raeng
staff starf starf starf
rise ries riess ries
clothes cloths clows clouthes*
tough taph taf taugh*
draft drarft drarft drarft
soup soop soop soupe*

*Untreated words with gradual increase in orthographic knowledge with treatment generalisation (Note: All of these untreated
words were spelled correctly at the next assessment).

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 22 (2) 251

You might also like