Journal Abi

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Vascular Medicine

15(2) 91–97
Ankle–brachial index predicts level of, but not © The Author(s) 2010
Reprints and permission: http://www.
change in, cognitive function:The Edinburgh sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermission.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1358863X09356321

Artery Study at the 15-year follow-up http://vmj.sagepub.com

Wendy Johnson1,2, Jacqueline F Price3, Snorri B Rafnsson3,


Ian J Deary1 and F Gerald R Fowkes3

Abstract
The ankle–brachial index (ABI), a marker of generalized atherosclerosis, is related to cognitive impairment in older adults.
We investigated whether ABI is associated specifically with age-related cognitive decline. We measured ABI at recruit-
ment and 5 and 12 years later in a sample of individuals aged 55–74 years. Cognition was measured in 717 of these
participants 10 years after recruitment and 5 years later. It was found that ABI was associated with the level of cognitive
function, even after adjustment for estimated premorbid function and concurrently measured anxiety and depression
(standardized coefficient of 0.07), but this was attenuated by anxiety and depression. ABI was not associated with change
in cognitive function. In conclusion, over long time periods, low ABI may be associated with reduced cognitive function in
older adults, at least partly because the associated poor health creates anxiety and depression.

Keywords
ankle–brachial index; change in cognitive function; late-life cognitive function

Introduction
Cognitive function varies widely among individuals, yet The ankle–brachial index (ABI; the ratio of systolic
almost everyone experiences declines with age. Though peo- blood pressure in the ankle to that in the arm) has been pro-
ple can maintain an independent life with a wide range of posed as one such measure.4 Originally developed to aid in
cognitive function, there is some essentially threshold level the diagnosis of peripheral arterial disease, it is both a reli-
of cognitive function below which cognitive function limits able indicator of generalized atherosclerosis5 and a predic-
the ability to live independently and must be considered a tor of fatal cardiovascular event, myocardial infarction and
disability. In youth, cognitive function below this threshold stroke.6–7 It has been associated with cognitive function in
level is associated with mental retardation. Among people several cross-sectional studies.8–13 In the Edinburgh Artery
over perhaps the age of 55 years, because declines with age Study (EAS4), it was shown to predict performance in non-
are often observed, cognitive function below this threshold verbal reasoning, verbal fluency and processing speed 10
level is generally considered impairment from a prior higher
level and is commonly associated with dementia or an inter-
mediate state such as mild cognitive impairment (MCI). 1
 entre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, Department
C
Cognitive impairment has prevalence rates of about 8% of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
for frank dementia1 and can range to 20% for MCI.2 It 2
Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota – Twin Cities,
affects both care costs and quality of life for older peo- Minneapolis, MN, USA
ple.3 A major public health challenge is thus to identify
3
Centre for Population Health Sciences, Division of Community Health
Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
older people at risk of and suffering from cognitive
impairment who may benefit from preventive, supportive Corresponding author:
and/or therapeutic measures to minimize future decline. Wendy Johnson
Identification of simple and accurate measures that are Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology,
Department of Psychology
acceptable to patients and can serve as indicators of cur-
University of Edinburgh
rent cognitive impairment or risk of cognitive decline 7 George Square
could be helpful in developing long-term preventive and Edinburgh EH8 9JZ, UK
treatment programmes. Email: wendy.johnson@ed.ac.uk
92 Vascular Medicine 15(2)

years later. For processing speed, the association remained cuff position proximal to the malleoli. Because arterial disease
significant after adjustment for premorbid cognitive func- can occur unilaterally, we calculated the ABI by dividing the
tion, suggesting that ABI may also predict cognitive lower of the indices obtained for the two legs by the brachial
decline. pressure. All participants who were alive were invited to return
The purpose of this study was to explore this association for the same examinations in 1993 and 2000, at intervals of 5
further in the same study population, augmenting the origi- and 12 years since baseline. Cognitive testing, which took
nal data by including new cognitive data collected 15 years about 50 minutes, was carried out by a specially trained nurse
after the study’s inception, 5 years after the initial report.4 and a research psychologist. The National Adult Reading Test
This made it possible to estimate associations with the (NART),15 Logical Memory,16 Raven’s Progressive Matrices,17
directly measured late life level of, and change in, cognitive Verbal Fluency,18 and Digit Symbol19 were administered in this
function. order. Scores on the NART show little decline with age, so it is
often used to estimate peak lifetime or premorbid cognitive
function.
Methods As part of each cognitive assessment, participants
responded to the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale
Participants (HADS),20 a well-validated measure of these mood states.
The EAS began in 1987. Its purpose was to study the inci- Because anxiety and depression are associated with both
dence, prevalence and natural history of peripheral arterial hypertension21 and cognitive function,22 we carried out
disease. Participants aged 55–74 years were randomly analyses both with and without adjustment for these scale
recruited from the registers of 10 general medical practices scores at the first cognitive assessment and for change in
located in diverse geographic and sociodemographic areas scores from first to second assessment. We used standard-
throughout the city of Edinburgh. At study inception, the ized residuals created by regressing scores at the second
sample included 809 men and 783 women, a 65% response assessment on those at the first assessment to estimate
rate to the initial invitations. Full details of study recruit- change in anxiety and depression over the 5-year period
ment and procedures have been described previously.14 between cognitive assessments.
The study was approved by the Lothian Health Board eth- We adjusted our analyses for smoking history. In the
ics committee. Each participant provided informed con- first clinical assessment, participants reported current
sent. The EAS is longitudinal, with clinical assessments at smoking status, average numbers of cigarettes smoked per
5 and 12 years after inception. day and numbers of years smoked. We converted this to
In 1998, 10 years after study inception, the EAS obtained pack-years of smoking, entering zeros for life-long non-
consent to recruit study participants for cognitive testing.4 smokers. At each follow-up, participants again reported
Invitations were issued by mail, and participants could elect current smoking status and average numbers of cigarettes
to be tested at home or at the University of Edinburgh. As smoked, as well as time since quitting if applicable. We
previously reported,4 at that time, 1103 of the original par- adjusted pack-years of smoking from the first assessment
ticipants were available; 461 were no longer participating. by adding average pack-years since the first clinical assess-
Of these 1103, 740 initially agreed to be tested and 717 actu- ment and subtracting half of pack-years of smoking since
ally completed cognitive testing (65% response). Cognitive quitting when relevant. We adjusted our analyses for the
testing was repeated in 2003, 15 years after study inception. resulting 5-year smoking variable and change in smoking
At that time, 601 (84%) of the 717 were still alive, but 23 from 5 to 12 years. We used the standardized residuals cre-
were excluded by their general practitioners and four could ated by regressing scores at the 12-year assessment on
not be located. Of the remaining 574 invited, 460 agreed: those at the 5-year assessment to estimate change in smok-
101 refused and 13 did not reply. Of those who agreed, 452 ing over the 7-year period between clinical assessments.
actually completed testing (79% response): seven withdrew We did not adjust for risk factors for atherosclerotic dis-
from the study and one could not be contacted. All partici- ease such as hypertension, presence of diabetes, body mass
pants who completed cognitive testing were included in this index, or levels of triglycerides and cholesterol. Because
study. we hypothesized that ABI could be a marker of future cog-
nitive decline through development of atherosclerotic dis-
ease, to do so would have been to remove variance related
Measures to atherosclerosis and thus possibly inappropriately fail to
Study participants attended initial clinical examinations at uni- observe the association in which we were interested.
versity or hospital clinics in 1988. They completed a written Similarly, we did not adjust for demographic factors such
questionnaire providing data on age, sex, lifetime smoking as education and income that are generally associated with
history and history of cardiovascular disease. Participants also lifetime peak cognitive function as well as level of
underwent clinical examination by specially trained nurses. cognitive function at any particular point in later life.
Systolic blood pressure was measured in the right arm after 10
minutes of rest using a random zero sphygmomanometer.
Ankle systolic pressure was measured in the posterior tibial Statistical analysis
artery of the right leg and then the left leg using a Doppler We used a latent variable growth curve model (ref. 23 for
ultrasound and a random zero sphygmomanometer with the methodology) to estimate level of and change in cognitive
Johnson W et al. 93

Table 1.  Descriptive statistics for the total cohort and the cognitively tested subgroup at baseline

Characteristic Total cohort Cognitively tested p-value of


difference
Mean SD Mean SD

Age   64.9   5.7   63.6   5.4 < 0.001


Symptomatic cardiovascular disease (%)   16.0 N/A   12.4 N/A < 0.001
Ankle–brachial index    1.03   0.18    1.06   0.16 < 0.001
Systolic blood pressure 144.5 24.1 140.9 22.8 < 0.001
Serum cholesterol, mmol/l    7.03   1.33    7.07   1.27 0.35
Pack-years of cigarette smoking   16.81 21.87   14.19 18.66 < 0.001
Symptomatic heart disease was history of myocardial infarction, angina pectoris, stroke, or intermittent claudication.

function over the 5-year period. A key aspect of the growth Results
curve model was its simultaneous incorporation of assess-
ments of latent variable measures of level of, and change The participants who completed initial cognitive testing
in, cognitive function. This eliminated the potential for were slightly younger and had lower prevalence rates of
bias due to variance in individual tests. In addition, it was cardiovascular disease, cardiovascular risk factors and
possible to measure separately the effects of age, sex, higher mean ABI than the full EAS sample. Because all of
smoking, anxiety, depression and ABI on level of cognitive these factors also tend to be associated with cognitive abil-
ability at the first assessment and change in cognitive ity, the participants also likely had higher cognitive test
ability from the first to the second assessment. We could scores than those who were not tested would have received.
also measure the correlation between level of cognitive Comparison data are shown in Table 1. As we were inter-
ability and its change. Probably most importantly, we could ested in cognitive function as an outcome, we made use
include the participants who were present at the first cogni- only of participants who completed at least the initial cog-
tive assessment, but not at the second, using full informa- nitive testing. The resulting restriction of range acted to
tion maximum likelihood estimation. attenuate the associations of interest.
We fit several models of potential interest in explain- All cognitive test scores were positively correlated with
ing associations between ABI and level of, and change in, the ABI measures taken nearest to their time of completion,
cognitive function. The baseline model considered only though correlations were low (0.03–0.20). Only the corre-
age, sex and prior smoking (at year 5) as covariates in lations between ABI and Verbal Fluency were not signifi-
order to assess the total effects of pre-existing ABI cant, and the correlation between ABI at year 5 and Verbal
(reflected by baseline assessment) on level of cognitive Fluency at year 15 was negative (–0.01). Hospital Anxiety
function at year 10. It also considered the effects of the and Depression were similarly correlated with ABI at the
same variables on change in cognitive function between respective time periods. Individual differences in ABI itself
years 10 and 15, as well as the effects of change in smok- were not particularly stable: the correlation between assess-
ing (years 5–12) and change in ABI (years 5–12) on ments at years 5 and 12 was 0.39. The strongest correla-
change in cognitive function. These time points of assess- tions were between assessments of the same test measures.
ment of ABI did not tie optimally with the time points of The average correlation between assessments of the same
assessment of cognitive function, but our approach did cognitive tests was 0.79, and the average correlation
make possible a look at the effects of level of, and change between assessments of the HADS scales was 0.67. The
in, ABI over an extended time period. To evaluate the full correlation table for the sample that completed cogni-
extent to which the ABI’s association with cognitive tive testing is shown in the Appendix.
function might be mediated by anxiety and depression, The columns entitled ‘Excluding HADS and NART’ in
we also estimated a second model using the HADS (at Table 2 show the estimated model effects on cognitive func-
year 10) as covariates of level of cognitive function, and tion before consideration of Anxiety and Depression. The
change in these scales between year 10 and year 15 as model fit quite well by all fit statistics compiled (specific
covariates on change in cognitive function. Finally, to details on fit statistics are available from the first author upon
provide some idea of the extent to which lifetime peak request). There was a moderate effect of –0.46 of age on level
cognitive function might be associated with that decline, of function. This indicates that a younger age at baseline was
we estimated a third model including the NART as a associated with a later higher level of cognitive function.
covariate. To avoid possible distortion of results due to Smoking also had a small but significant effect (–0.12), and
the presence of a few individuals with very high values of there was a marginally significant (p = 0.09) tendency for
ABI possibly associated with cardiovascular disease, we men to have better cognitive function than women (coeffi-
ran our analyses both including all data at the values cient = –0.08). ABI also had a small but significant effect on
reported and limiting ABI values to 1.4. As results were level of function (coefficient = 0.15, p = 0.001). The mean
essentially identical, we present only those based on the cognitive change parameter was –0.04, which indicated that,
reported data. after adjustment for covariates, cognitive function declined
94

Table 2.  Estimated associations between variables of interest and level of cognitive function at year 10, and change in cognitive function between years 10 and 15, including and excluding
adjustment for anxiety, depression and pre-existing cognitive function

Excluding HADS and NART Excluding only NART Including all

Coefficient Standard error p-value Coefficient Standard error p-value Coefficient Standard error p-value

Effects on level of cognitive function


   Age -0.46 0.04 < 0.001 -0.44 0.04 < 0.001 -0.44 0.04 < 0.001
   Sex -0.08 0.05 0.09 -0.03 0.05 0.52 -0.05 0.04 0.27
   Smoking at year 5 -0.12 0.05 0.015 -0.10 0.05 0.046 -0.08 0.04 0.01
   ABI at baseline   0.15 0.05 0.001   0.14 0.05 0.002   0.07 0.04 0.03
   Hospital anxiety at year 10 N/A N/A N/A -0.14 0.05 0.002 -0.08 0.04 0.024
   Hospital depression at year 10 N/A N/A N/A -0.11 0.04 0.011 -0.06 0.05 0.07
   NART at year 10 N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A   0.57 0.03 < 0.001
Effects on change in cognitive function
   Sex   0.05 0.08 0.57   0.02 0.08 0.77   0.03 0.08 0.73
   Smoking at year 5 -0.04 0.31 0.91 -0.13 0.31 0.67 -0.15 0.31 0.62
   ABI at baseline -0.04 0.12 0.71 -0.09 0.12 0.45 -0.07 0.12 0.56
   Change in smoking, years 5–12 -0.08 0.34 0.81   0.06 0.34 0.87   0.07 0.34 0.83
   Change in ABI, years 5–12   0.13 0.10 0.20   0.14 0.10 0.14   0.14 0.10 0.15
   Hospital anxiety at year 10 N/A N/A N/A   0.11 0.09 0.21   0.11 0.09 0.24
   Hospital depression at year 10 N/A N/A N/A -0.08 0.10 0.41 -0.09 0.10 0.35
   Change in hospital anxiety, years 10–15 N/A N/A N/A -0.22 0.08 0.006 -0.22 0.08 0.099
   Change in hospital depression, years 10–15 N/A N/A N/A -0.19 0.09 0.033 -0.19 0.09 0.039
   NART at year 10 N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A -0.08 0.08 0.31
HADS, Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; NART, National Adult Reading Test (which represents pre-existing cognitive function);  ABI, ankle–brachial index.
Vascular Medicine 15(2)
Johnson W et al. 95

at 0.04 standard deviation per year over the period between be an issue in participants with subclinical subclavian ste-
cognitive assessments. Its standard deviation, however, was nosis. Moreover, our measures of smoking habits, anxiety
only 0.05, indicating that most participants experienced and depression were based on self reports and thus are sub-
about the same rate of cognitive decline, again after adjust- ject to the inaccuracies of long-term recall and bias, and we
ment for covariates. Consistent with this, there was no sub- made our adjustments for the effects of having smoked but
stantial correlation between level of and change in cognitive quit judgementally.
function, and none of the change covariates had any signifi-
cant effect on cognitive change.
The columns entitled ‘Excluding only NART’ in Table 2 How is ABI associated with cognitive function?
show results adjusting for HADS scores at first cognitive Our results were consistent with previous studies suggest-
assessment as contributors to level of cognitive function and ing an association between low ABI and poor cognitive
changes in these scores as contributors to change in cognitive function.4,8–13,24–26 Price et al.4 proposed that this association
function. The model fit indicators improved with addition of may indicate that ABI is a marker of coming cognitive
these variables (specific details on fit statistics are available decline. The current results, extending the initial findings in
from the first author upon request). The effects of year 10 the same sample, provide tentative support for this sugges-
Anxiety on level of cognitive function and change in Anxiety tion, in that baseline ABI was a significant predictor of later
on change in cognitive function were particularly significant cognitive function even after adjustments for the HADS
(–0.14, p = 0.002; –0.22, p = 0.006, respectively), but scales and, marginally, the NART were considered.
Depression’s effects were also significant. They did not However, they more strongly support the hypothesis that
account for the effect of ABI, however, as its coefficient low ABI and low cognitive function are associated over
remained significant (0.14, p = 0.002). The addition of the very long time spans primarily because low ABI is a good
NART, however, reduced this effect to marginal significance indicator of poor general vascular health and poor cognitive
(0.07, p = 0.052). The addition of the NART also reduced the function is a weak indicator of poor general health. All
effects of year 10 Anxiety and Depression on level of cogni- findings should be considered in the context of the 5-year
tive function to non-significance, though they did not sub- period between the relevant assessments of ABI and cogni-
stantially alter the effects of change in Anxiety and change in tive function, and the fact that anxiety and depression
Depression on change in cognitive function. explained part of the association we observed.
The involvement of anxiety and depression in the
association between ABI and cognitive function is
Discussion intriguing from two perspectives. First, changes in anxi-
In this study, we used a second cognitive assessment from ety and depression from years 10 to 15 were associated
EAS to refine and extend initial observations4 of the effects with changes in cognitive function during the same
of ABI on cognitive function. In doing so, we considered period. It is unusual to be able to pick up variables asso-
the effects of estimated lifetime peak cognitive ability as ciated with rates of change in cognitive function because
measured by the NART, ABI measured earlier in the study, in many studies participants appear to decline in function
and anxiety and depression as measured concurrently with at very similar rates. This is partly an issue of statistical
cognitive function by the HADS. Our results indicated power, but it is also an indication that individual differ-
modest but important effects of pre-existing ABI on initial ences in change in cognitive function are much smaller
level of assessed cognitive function. These effects were than individual differences in level of cognitive function
diminished but not completely eliminated by consideration at any age. Thus, simply our ability to detect associations
of anxiety and depression. There was also some suggestion with changes in anxiety and depression warrants atten-
that baseline ABI might be associated with change in cog- tion. Second, the involvement of changes in anxiety and
nitive function since peak cognitive function, but there was depression speaks to the psychological processes
no evidence for direct effects of change in ABI on mea- involved in aging. It is at least as likely that observed
sured change in cognitive function between years 10 and 15 decline in cognitive function brought on increased anxi-
of the study. ety and depression as increased anxiety and depression
reduced ABI which in turn impaired cognitive function.
Still, the association suggests the participants’ awareness
Study limitations of, and emotional responses to, the difficulties encoun-
The primary limitations of this study were that the clinical tered in aging.
and cognitive assessments were not optimally timed to pro- ABI is simple to collect. It provides reliable information
vide maximum power to measure the associations of inter- about atherosclerotic conditions that are associated with
est, and the sample that completed cognitive testing was in poor cognitive function. It thus provides a cheap and effec-
better health than the whole sample assessed by the EAS. In tive means of collecting a general indicator of overall cog-
addition, though the NART is generally considered a good nitive and physical health. Our results add to the growing
estimate of peak life cognitive function, measured later in evidence that measures of physical health also pick up
life it cannot completely substitute for a cognitive assess- information about cognitive health, and suggest that pre-
ment at some earlier age. ABI measurements did not include ventive and therapeutic interventions should be evaluated
blood pressure measurements in the left arm, which could for effects on cognitive as well as physical function.
96 Vascular Medicine 15(2)

Acknowledgements 11. Muller M, Grobbee DE, Aleman A, Bots M, van der Schouw
Wendy Johnson holds a RCUK Fellowship. The Edinburgh YT. Cardiovascular disease and cognitive function in middle-
Artery Study is supported by the British Heart Foundation. aged and elderly men. Atherosclerosis 2007; 190: 143–149.
Wendy Johnson, Jacqueline Price and Ian Deary are members 12. Waldstein SR, Tankard CF, Maier KJ, et al. Peripheral artery
of the University of Edinburgh Centre for Cognitive Ageing and disease and cognitive function. Psychosom Med 2003; 65:
Cognitive Epidemiology, which is supported by the BBSRC, 757–763.
EPSRC, ESRC and MRC as part of the Lifelong Health and Well- 13. Woo J, Lynn H, Wong SYS, et al. Correlates for a low ankle–
being Initiative. brachial index in elderly Chinese. Atherosclerosis 2006; 186:
References 360–366.
14. Fowkes FG, Housley E, Cawood EH, et al. Edinburgh Artery
  1. Graham JE, Rockwood K, Beattie BL, et al. Prevalence and Study: prevalence of asymptomatic and symptomatic periph-
severity of cognitive impairment with and without dementia eral arterial disease in the general population. Int J Epidemiol
in an elderly population. Lancet 1997; ii: 15–21. 1991; 20: 384–392.
  2. Geda YE, Roberts RO, Knopman DS, et al. Prevalence of 15. Nelson HE, Willison J. National Adult Reading Test (NART)
neuropsychiatric symptoms in mild cognitive impairment test manual. Windsor: NFER-Nelson, 1991.
and normal cognitive aging: population-based study. Arch 16. Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale – revised. San Antonio,
Gen Psychiatry 2008; 65: 1193–1198. TX: The Psychological Corporation, 1987.
  3. Stern PC, Carstensen LL. The aging mind: opportunities 17. Raven JC, Court JH, Raven J. Manual for Raven’s progres-
in cognitive research. Washington, DC: National Academy sive matrices and vocabulary scales. London: Lewis, 1977.
Press, 2000. 18. Lezak MD. Neuropsychological assessment, 3rd edition.
  4. Price JF, McDowell S, Whiteman MC, Deary IJ, Stewart MC, New York: Oxford University Press, 1995.
Fowkes FGR. Ankle brachial index as a predictor of cognitive 19. Wechsler D. Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence
impairment in the general population: ten-year follow-up of the Scale – revised. New York: The Psychological Corporation,
Edinburgh Artery Study. J Am Geriatr Soc 2006; 54: 763–769. 1981.
  5. Fowkes FGR. The measurement of atherosclerotic peripheral 20. Zigmond AS, Snaith AP. The Hospital Anxiety and
arterial disease in epidemiological surveys. Int J Epidemiol Depression Scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1983; 67: 361–370.
1988; 17: 248–254. 21. Muhson K, Lipsitz J, Garty-Sandalon N, Gross R, Green
  6. Heald CL, Fowkes FGR, Murray GD, Price JF. Risk of mor- MS. Correlates of generalized anxiety disorder: indepen-
tality and cardiovascular disease associated with the ankle– dent of comorbidity with depression. Findings from the first
brachial index: systematic review. Atherosclerosis 2006; Israeli National Health Interview Survey (2003–2004). Soc
189: 61–69. Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2008; 43: 898–904.
  7. Ankle Brachial Index Collaboration. Ankle Brachial Index 22. Terry AV, Buccafusco JJ, Wilson C. Cognitive dysfunction in
combined with Framingham Risk Score to predict cardio- neuropsychiatric disorders: selected serotonin receptor sub-
vascular events and mortality: a meta-analysis. JAMA 2008; types as therapeutic targets. Behav Brain Res 2008; 195: 30–38.
300: 197–208. 23. Muthen B. Latent variable modeling with longitudinal and
  8. Fujiwara Y, Chaves PHM, Takahashi R, et al. Arterial pulse multilevel data. In: Raftery A (ed). Sociological methodology
wave velocity as a marker of poor cognitive function in an #73. Boston: Blackwell Publishers, 1997, p. 453–480.
elderly community dwelling population. J Gerontol 2005; 24. Haan MN, Shemanski L, Jagust WJ, et al. The role of APOE
60A: 607–612. epsilon4 in moderating effects of other risk factors for cogni-
  9. Fukuhara M, Matsumura K, Toshihiro A, et al. Prediction tive decline in elderly persons. JAMA 1999; 282: 40–46.
of cognitive function by arterial stiffness in the very elderly. 25. Saxton J, Ratcliff G, Newman A, et al. Cognitive test per-
Circ J 2006; 70: 756–761. formance and presence of subclinical cardiovascular disease
10. Kim Y-S, Kim D-H, Choi BH, Sohn E-H, Lee AY. in the cardiovascular health study. Neuroepidemiology 2000;
Relationship between brachial–ankle pulse wave velocity 19: 312–319.
and cognitive function in an elderly community-dwelling 26. Breteler MMB, Claus JJ, Grobbee DE, et al. Cardiovascular
population with metabolic syndrome. Arch Gerontol Geriatr disease and distribution of cognitive function in elderly
2009; 49: 176–179. people: The Rotterdam Study. BMJ 1994;308:1604–1608.
Appendix.
Johnson W et al.

Correlations among study variables in cognitively tested subgroup

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19

  1. Age at baseline 1.00


  2. Sex -0.02 1.00
  3. Smoking, year 5 -0.02 -0.25 1.00
  4. ABI, year 5 -0.14 -0.17 -0.11 1.00
  5. NART,15 year 10 -0.03 -0.03 -0.05 0.08 1.00
  6. Anxiety, year 10 -0.06 0.24 -0.01 -0.08 -0.15 1.00
  7. Depression, year 10 0.12 0.08 0.10 -0.19 -0.13 0.46 1.00
  8. Logical Memory,16 -0.22 -0.04 -0.02 0.17 0.30 -0.12 -0.13 1.00
year 10
  9. Raven,17 year 10 -0.33 -0.15 -0.04 0.12 0.47 -0.16 -0.14 0.42 1.00
10. Verbal Fluency,18 -0.11 -0.02 -0.03 0.03 0.49 -0.10 -0.14 0.25 0.39 1.00
year 10
11. Digit Symbol,19 -0.40 0.04 -0.12 0.13 0.41 -0.10 -0.21 0.38 0.56 0.45 1.00
year 10
12. Smoking, year 12 0.00 -0.19 0.96 -0.13 -0.08 0.02 0.11 -0.04 -0.09 -0.06 -0.16 1.00
13. ABI, year 12 -0.21 -0.22 -0.13 0.39 0.16 -0.02 -0.10 0.13 0.19 0.08 0.20 -0.16 1.00
14. Anxiety, year 15 -0.04 0.19 0.09 -0.08 -0.21 0.68 0.41 -0.10 -0.21 -0.14 -0.15 0.14 -0.08 1.00
15. Depression, 0.19 0.09 0.07 -0.24 -0.12 0.35 0.65 -0.09 -0.11 -0.12 -0.26 0.10 -0.13 0.43 1.00
year 15
16. Logical Memory,16 -0.18 -0.04 -0.02 0.16 0.29 -0.11 -0.13 0.71 0.39 0.20 0.33 -0.05 0.13 -0.12 -0.14 1.00
year 15
17. Raven,17 year 15 -0.29 -0.17 0.00 0.20 0.46 -0.23 -0.15 0.37 0.77 0.34 0.56 -0.04 0.24 -0.26 -0.19 0.46 1.00
18. Verbal Fluency,18 -0.16 -0.03 0.01 -0.01 0.46 -0.11 -0.11 0.16 0.35 0.82 0.45 -0.02 0.02 -0.15 -0.17 0.22 0.38 1.00
year 15
19. Digit Symbol,19 -0.39 0.06 -0.12 0.15 0.33 -0.11 -0.20 0.30 0.54 0.39 0.85 -0.14 0.19 -0.20 -0.29 0.37 0.65 0.45 1.00
year 15
ABI, ankle–brachial index; NART, National Adult Reading Test.
Depending on the number of participants in each cell, correlations greater than about 0.08 were significant at p < 0.05, with no correction for multiple testing.
The ‘Anxiety’ and ‘Depression’ scores come from the HADS.
The column numbers refer to the variables numbered in the rows.
Correlations referred to in the text are shown in bold.
97
Copyright of Vascular Medicine is the property of Sage Publications, Ltd. and its content may not be copied or
emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission.
However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like