Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

The Mourning After Brain Injury:

Understanding Loss and Grief

Jean Ann Ruddle


B. Rudi Coetzer

Traumatic brain injury (TBI) in the UK is a common occurrence. known empirical study of grief was conducted by Lindemann
It is estimated that 100-150 per 100,000 of the population (1944). His interviews identified ‘symptoms’ and changes in mental
experience a disability as the result of a TBI (British Society of state. This portrayed an acute grief response, where distress
Rehabilitation Medicine 1998). Taking into account ‘significant emerged in waves, and physical illness such as tightness in the
disablement’, Greenwood and McMillan (1993) state this equates throat and an empty stomach were common. Lindemann also
to 250-375 persons in the average health district, or one family in identified problems when people did not engage in ‘grief work’.
every 300. Despite methodological problems, Lindemann’s psychiatric
Grief is traditionally defined as the psychological process that approach to grief formed the basis of future studies in the area.
one goes through following the loss of a loved one. However, The 1960’s saw a number of influential articles on grief; Pollock
other losses can give rise to a similar reaction (Parkes 1972, 1973 (1961) and Averill (1968) in America, and Bowlby (1960 & 1961) in
& 1976). An individual exposed to a traumatic brain injury is Britain. Due to the relative lack of empirically sound research on
likely to experience multiple losses, including physical, social the subject of grief, Wortman and Silver (1987) and Stroebe (1994)
and emotional (Haynes 1994). Because of the chronic disability state that we need to critically examine many of the assumptions
following severe TBI, models of loss and grief have intuitive or ‘myths’ about the grief process. One of the most easily
appeal for helping clinicians understand some of the emotional identified ‘myths’ in grief work are the models based on ‘stages’
consequences following TBI. or ‘phases’. These theories state that the bereaved progress
through a set of distinct categories that can be identified and
Loss and grief described, based on their characteristics. Bowlby’s account is an
example of this. Originally based on children’s reactions to
Grief is a natural human response to loss, a universal separation (Bowlby 1960), but later extended to encompass adult
phenomenon that is evident in all cultures (Archer 1999). But grieving (Bowlby 1980), the four stages he identified were;
whilst its existence is unquestioned, research into grief has been numbness and disbelief; yearning and searching; disorganisation
relatively neglected by the scientific community (Parkes 1996). and despair and finally, reorganisation.
This is in part related to the fact that loss is such a fundamental Kübler-Ross’s (1969) name has become synonymous with the
part of human existence, and therefore grief is seen as a ‘normal’ stages of grief model. She identified five stages that dying people
response. This in turn has meant that grief has been overlooked were reported to go through; these being; denial, anger,
as an area of scientific research due to the almost ordinariness of bargaining, depression and acceptance. This was later extended
the phenomenon. to grieving in general. Whilst this model has been hugely
Shand’s (1914 & 1920) early psychological accounts of grief influential, it has also been heavily criticised not least for the lack
drew on poetry and literature, yet despite this lack of empirical of evidence to support its existence and whether people move
evidence, Shand described four types of grief reaction; one active, from stage one through to stage five (Corr, Nabe & Corr 1997).
which was directed externally; one depressive, which lacked Parkes (1970, 1972 & 1996) is generally viewed as providing
energy; one suppressed through self control and lastly, one the best accounts of the grief process, and despite adopting a
involving frenzied activity. Shand acknowledged individual ‘stage’ approach, initially; he later looked more specifically at the
variations and changes over time. He also identified other factors components that delineated grief into two categories (Parkes
such as the importance of social support. Freud (1917) went further 1985 & 1996). These were; episodic reactions such as anger and
to state that much psychiatric illness was the expression of searching and a general background disturbance which included
pathological grieving. His theory centred on the loss of the loved affective symptomology such as anxiety and depression.
‘object’ and the painful reality when this is difficult to accept. Worden (1991) moved from a ‘stage’ model to a ‘task’ based
Despite early psychoanalytical accounts of grief, the first well approach, stating that transition through grief involved the
completion of four tasks namely; to accept the reality of the loss;
Jean Anne Ruddle, D. Clin Psy., Adult Mental Health Service, Conwy
to work through the pain of grief; to adjust to an environment in
& Denbigshire NHS Turst, Aston House, Deeside Community Hospi-
tal, Plough Lane, Aston, Deeside, CH5 3LX, United Kingdom. B. Rudi which the deceased is missing and to emotionally relocate the
Coetzer, North Wales Brain Injury Service, Conwy & Denbigshire NHS deceased and move on with life. This approach was more flexible
Trust, Colwyn Bay Hospital, Hesketh Road, Colwyn Bay LL29 8AY, and less rigid than the ‘stage’ approach as the tasks were not
United Kingdom. seen as being completed in a set order.

Fall 2005 - The Journal of Cognitive Rehabilitation 13


More recent research has focused on the neural correlates of injury literature however, has focussed more heavily on the
grief using fMRI techniques (Gündel, O’Connor, Littrell, Fort & traditional psychiatric classification (E.g. Diagnostic and
Lane 2003). Grief was elicited by showing photographs of the Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders IV, APA 1994) of
deceased to bereaved women combined with words specific to observable signs and symptoms to describe the emotional
the death event versus neutral words. Many centres of the brain disturbance following TBI.
were seen to be activated, showing that the grief response was
mediated by a complex distributed network which subserved; Emotional and psychological difficulties following TBI
affect processing, mentalizing, episodic memory retrieval, pro-
cessing of familiar faces, visual imagery, autonomic regulation Parkes (1996) states that medical classification of disease is
and modulation/coordination of these functions. based upon the symptoms observed when aetiology is unknown.
Stroebe and Schut (1995) offered a ‘dual process approach’ to In relation to grief, Parkes describes how psychiatric labels have
loss. They recognised that there was no ‘grief process’ per se, been used to describe grief in terms of anxiety states, phobias,
but a complicated interaction between polarised states; namely a and depressive reactions for example. Labelling grief based on
‘loss orientation’ and a ‘restoration orientation’ that occurred symptoms however, reduces the experience to a state rather than
concurrently, the former is associated with traditional ‘grief’ re- a process that people move in and out of and ignores individual
actions and the latter by attempts to rebuild ones life and move circumstances.
on, as smoothly as possible. They moved away from a biological Although depression is a common condition in non-brain
‘natural’ theory of grief and acknowledged interactions on a psy- injured populations, Stonnington (2001) states that it is more
chological, social and cultural level and elevate ‘loss’ to a level of prevalent for individuals with a brain injury. In an evaluation of
complexity outside of normal human experience that avoids the the type and extent of psychiatric syndromes one year after a
tendency to oversimplify. This translates as no ‘healing’ proce- TBI, Deb, Lyons and Koutzoukis (1999) interviewed 120 patients
dure, or procession of stages, but an oscillation between the two aged 18-64 and compared psychiatric presentation in their group
orientations with one dominating at any given time. They state against a study of the general population. Depressive illness
that in the early stages of a period of grief the ‘loss orientation’ is was the most common diagnosis across both groups, the second
likely to dominate whilst ‘restoration orientation’ will gain domi- most common diagnosis post-injury was panic disorder occurring
nance over time. They avoid the notion of a simple translation of in nine percent of the sample compared to less than one percent
one stage to another, and enable the ‘loss orientation’ to emerge in the general population.
temporarily a long time after the loss event for example at anni- In a similar study, Deb, Lyons and Koutzoukis (1998) specifi-
versaries. This encompasses the tension aspect of grief, rather cally focusing on neuropsychiatric sequelae one year post minor
than a simplified linear pathway. head injury, interviewed 148 adults who had been admitted to
The work by Gündel et al. (2003) and Stroebe and Schut (1995) hospital with this diagnosis. They established that seventeen
enable us to make the shift from grief associated with the loss of percent were diagnosed with psychiatric problems and fifty-five
a loved one to other types of losses. These have included, divorce percent of participants showing at least one of the symptoms of
(Kitson 1982) unemployment (Fagin & Little 1984), forced post-concussional syndrome. The most common presentation
migration (Munoz 1980), childlessness (Houghton & Houghton after one year was neurobehavioral with thirty percent reporting
1977) and the losses involved in recovery from cancer (Maker irritability, twenty-nine percent describing sleep disturbance and
1982). Hainsorth (1998) and Read (2002) have looked at twenty-seven percent reporting impatience.
professions involving multiple losses i.e. nursing, as increasing Assessing a population based sample, Silver, Kramer,
the potential psychological risk that this exposes individuals to. Greenwald and Weissman (2001) found ‘hidden TBI’ to be a
Parkes has also extended his research to look at grief experienced common occurrence within the general population. That is,
due to loss of a limb and loss of a home (1972, 1973 & 1976). Like individuals with a previous head injury that do not associate
death, Parkes describes loss as often being a ‘post hoc’ their current problems with a previous TBI. Silver et al. found
attribution, that is, it is only after events that we realise the impact higher rates of psychiatric symptoms in the TBI group relating
of the loss. this to the effects of the accident and/or the effects of disabilities
Grief has therefore been used to describe a process that can arising from the TBI. They also assessed quality of life (QOL)
occur after any significant loss or perceived loss event. Brain variables including physical health, emotional health or being on

14 Fall 2005 - The Journal of Cognitive Rehabilitation


welfare or disability support and found that the TBI group scored To this end, the brain injured individual is faced with multiple
lower on these variables. losses. Haynes (1994) states that the brain injured individual
Anxiety disorders are common following TBI and Hiott and stands to lose life as it was known to them. Judd and Wilson
Labbate (2002) provide an overview of the topic. The relation- (1999) go on to say that brain injury can be one of the “most
ship between brain injury and Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder devastating, disempowering and seemingly incomprehensible
(PTSD), while perhaps controversial, is receiving more attention experiences” to affect an individual as well as their family. Tasker
in the literature. For example, Feinstein, Hershkop, Jardine and (2003) highlights not only the tangible losses, but also losses
Ouchterlony (2000) identified symptoms of PTSD of varying se- such as the “illusion of self control and self protection” as well
verity present in eighty-four percent of their sample two months as a sense of emptiness, reduced existence and the frustration of
post injury. They found evidence to support the hypothesis that not feeling understood.
pre-morbid personality and lifestyle factors were vulnerability Persinger (1993) states that a significant altering in self con-
factors. They also found evidence of psychiatric co-morbidity cept may occur following severe closed head injuries. This is due
with disorders such as depression and anxiety following TBI. to the diffuse synaptic modification within the temporofrontal
Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) has also been reported regions resulting in a loss of sense of self. This loss of sense of
following TBI (Berthier, Kulisevsky, Gironell & Lopez, 2001). self should provoke a grief like reaction. Persinger describes the
Again, there may be some overlap between symptoms common grief as in response to the alteration of the pre-morbid self con-
to both OCD and TBI and making a diagnosis can sometimes be cept and would constitute a kind of “identity crisis”. This fragile
difficult (Coetzer & Stein, 2003). self concept could easily be devastated by other losses enforced
One of the more serious implications of head injury is the upon them, activities which would have previously defined them
potential link between TBI and vulnerability to suicide (Teasdale as individuals. Loss of sense of self after TBI is discussed in
& Engberg 2001; Fleminger, Oliver, Williams & Evans, 2003). It greater depth by Nochi (1998) and also Myles (2004).
has been suggested (Fleminger et al. 2003) that some of the Loss of self-awareness after brain injury has received a great
depressive and emotional distress evident post TBI may be related deal of attention from researchers. Newman, Garmore, Beatty and
to an increase in awareness of disability, and that insight deficits Ziccardi (2000) report that self-awareness is a unique construct
may function as a protective defence mechanism. It is the in the early stages of rehabilitation and that it needs to be
recognition of losses that Fleminger et al. see as a trigger to monitored throughout recovery. Fleming, Strong and Ashton
psychological disorders such as depression. (1998) found that rehabilitation, timing and approach may need
A common theme in the literature is that depression and several to be tailored to match the individual’s levels of self awareness,
other disorders appear to be more prevalent following a TBI and motivation and emotional distress. Similarly, Trundel, Tyron and
that an individual’s presentation post TBI is dependent on a Purdman (1998) found that impaired awareness negatively impacts
complex interaction between biological, psychological, social, on vocational and residential placements and that it is a significant
environmental and pre-injury factors. Clearly the use of predictor of outcome variables. Hillier and Metzer (1997) state
psychiatric classification systems has some clinical utility. In that people with a TBI are more likely to be aware of residual
addition, models of grief can potentially enrich practitioners’ physical disabilities to the exclusion of social domains. They
understanding of the psychological consequences of TBI. describe a ‘hierarchy of needs’ ranging from lower physiological
skills that need to be accomplished before the individual can
Loss and grief following TBI move on to higher order self-actualizing skills.
Lezak (1987) makes the point that brain injured individuals
There are a number of similarities between the psychiatric are much more likely to be seriously handicapped by emotional
classification of emotional sequelae in TBI and the early scientific and personality disturbances than by their residual cognitive or
accounts of grief (Lindemann 1944), not least that they are both physical difficulties. Judd and Wilson (1999) describe the multiple
symptom based. Using grief as a model in TBI populations is losses as requiring careful re-negotiation and adaptation into the
therefore an intuitive link to make. It enables us to move away identity of the self, and that feelings of worthlessness,
from a structuralist approach to more functionalist based helplessness and a dislike of the ‘new’ self could be a
hypotheses. This focuses not on the classification of symptoms, manifestation of mourning for the pre-injury self.
but the importance of the meaning of the experiences, which in Tasker (2003) describes how brain injury “renders the parts of
many ways reflects the way research into bereavement as a whole the whole disrupted and shattered, exposed and torn apart” and
has changed. Indeed, from a clinical perspective there is likely to when “the body and brain are brilliantly yet objectively treated”
be a significant overlap between what is labelled as grief, there is cost to the individual and their family. One of the main
depression and/or PTSD and that it is not always possible to consequences of experiencing a TBI according to Tasker (2003)
disentangle the biological and psychological factors affecting is that the individual develops a conscious involvement with
an individual’s presentation (Coetzer and Corney, 2003). death and that in order to gain wholeness and meaning, one must
In a study of disability, Sapey (2002) states that a person’s integrate the past, present and future, and that ‘optimal resolution’
response to events will depend on their individual circumstances; involves a synthesis of apparently incompatible poles, e.g. hope
that is how they have construed the possibility of impairment versus hopelessness, acceptance verse denial, struggle verses
prior to disability. Similarly, it has been shown that finding surrender and depletion verses creativity.
meaning is central in adjusting to loss and trauma with regards to Meredith and Rassa (1999) found a positive correlation
one’s sense of self worth and one’s beliefs and assumptions between a TBI individual’s level of awareness and their stage of
about how the world works (Davis & Nolen-Hoekesma 2001). grief, and that a gradual progression through the stages of

Fall 2005 - The Journal of Cognitive Rehabilitation 15


grieving would coincide with the improvements of self awareness In assessing awareness, Fleming, Strong and Ashton (1996)
of deficits. They found that emotional reaction to changes were advocate an approach that looks at multiple measures from
described as similar to the Kübler-Ross (1969) model of grief after multiple sources. Whilst later stating that rehabilitation needs to
loss of a person, these being; denial, anger, bargaining, depression be timed and tailored to match an individual’s level of self
and acceptance. In a population of brain injured patients, Coetzer motivation and emotional distress (Fleming, Strong and Ashton
and Corney (2001) found that feedback of findings from self 1998). Gasquoine and Gibbens (1994) believe that assessment of
awareness assessments to participants resulted in a decrease of awareness can assist in rehabilitation enabling people with a TBI
subjective grief in those individuals. to participate in therapies, whilst at the same time, reducing staff
More common in the literature is a focus on the carers’ sense of failure. Coetzer and Corney (2003) state that failure to
experience of loss. Webster, Daisley and King (1999) state that all acknowledge emotional processes for the individual and the carers
family members need to make an adjustment to changed life roles, will severely compromise efforts at rehabilitation. Leading on
and that the losses experienced by the family are not final, and from this, Coetzer (2004) argues that psychotherapeutic work
difficulties might arise due to the injured person being a constant needs to take into account both levels of self awareness and the
reminder of what has been lost, making the grieving process grieving process, and how they interact. Coetzer (2004) goes on
more protracted. In focussing on grief after TBI, MacFarlane to suggest that understanding the long term effects of brain
(1999) states that not only does the individual have to deal with injury in terms of its relationship to grief and self-awareness may
a variety of losses, but the family members do also, one of the assist rehabilitation professionals in their psychotherapeutic
common reactions feeling that the person they know has ‘died’. interventions with this population.
This finding is supported by Zinner, Ball, Stutts and Philput (1997) Models of rehabilitation are based on a philosophy of facili-
who found that mothers of TBI individuals reported more intense tating maximum recovery by drawing on strengths and compen-
grief than parents who had experienced other significant non- sating for weaknesses. However, rehabilitation has often ne-
death losses. glected the emotional recovery that must inevitably take place.
An increased understanding of symptoms post brain injury As discussed in this paper, failure to take care of an individuals
and the expression of emotions through a person centred psychological needs can adversely affect their outcome. In this
approach and theory of loss framework may be prerequisites for context, is important to remember that according to Ben-Yishay
the longer term tasks of increasing self awareness and making (2000), a person had entered the stage of acceptance when he or
meaning from the losses endured (Coetzer, 2004). It is evident she ceases to mourn the losses sustained following TBI.
that there is a growing body of literature applying models of loss
and grief to explain some of the emotional effects associated with
a TBI. References
American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and Sta-
Loss as a consideration in neuro rehabilitation tistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Forth Edition. Wash-
ington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Several neuro-rehabilitation models now incorporate
psychotherapy as a component of the programme. Indeed, Judd Archer, J. (1999). The nature of Grief. The evolution and Psy-
and Wilson (1999) believe that psychotherapy should be offered chology of reactions to loss. Brunner-Routledge.
routinely for all neurologically damaged patients. They make the
point that re-assimilation of identity does not have an end point Averill, J.R. (1968). Grief: Its Nature and Significance. Psycho-
but is a life long adaptive process whether an individual is brain logical Bulletin, 70, 721-748.
injured or not. It stands to reason therefore that an individual
with limited cognitive abilities may need more guidance in making Ben-Yishay, Y. (2000). Postacute neuropsychological rehabilita-
sense of what has happened to them. As such, there is a growing tion: a holisticperspective. In A-L. Christensen & B. P. Uzzell
body of literature looking at adapting traditional forms of (Eds.), International Handbook of Neuropsychological
psychotherapy to meet the needs of a brain injured population Rehabilitation (pp. 127 – 135). London, Kluver Academic/
(e.g. Khan-Bourne & Brown 2003, and van den Broek 1999). Plenum Publishers.
However, on a more cautionary note, it should be pointed out
that not all persons with TBI will necessarily benefit from Berthier, M. L., Kulisevsky, J. J., Gironell, A. & Lopez, O. L. (2001).
psychotherapy (Prigatano, 1999). Obsessive-compulsive disorder and traumatic brain injury:
In terms of outcome, group affiliation and acceptance have behavioral, cognitive and neuroimaging findings. Neuropsy-
been identified as factors needed post injury in order for chiatry, Neuropsychology & Behavioral Neurology,14 (1):
individuals to experience a greater sense of control and 23 – 31.
empowerment (Vandiver & Christofero-Snider 2000). Snead and
Davis (2002) found that individuals with TBI who had a positive Bowlby, J. (1960). Separation Anxiety. International Journal of
attitude towards disability had a greater level of acceptance of Psychoanalysis, 41, 89-113.
their own, and others’ disability. In order to maximize outcome in
therapy, Meredith and Rassa (1999), state that one should utilize Bowlby, J. (1961). Processes of Mourning. International Jour-
techniques to facilitate the simultaneous progression of both nal of Psychoanalysis, 42, 317-340.
awareness of deficits and acceptance of losses, where a grief like
process will be evident.

16 Fall 2005 - The Journal of Cognitive Rehabilitation


Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and Loss, Volume 3. Loss: Sad- Fleminger, S., Oliver, D.L., Williams, W.H. & Evans, J. (2003). The
ness and Depression. London: The Hogarth Press and Insti- Neuropsychiatry of Depression after Brain Injury. Neuropsy-
tute of Psychiatry. chological Rehabilitation, 13 (1-2), 65-87.

British Society of Rehabilitation Medicine. (1998). Rehabilita- Freud, S. (1917). Mourning and Melancholia. Reprinted in: J.
tion after Traumatic Brain Injury. A Working Party Report of Strachey (trans. and ed.), Standard Edition of Complete Psy-
the British Society of Rehabilitation Medicine. London: Au- chological Works of Sigmund Freud, Vol. 14 (pp.239-260).
thor. London: Hogarth Press and Institute of Psychoanalysis
(1957).
Coetzer, B.R. (2004). Grief, Self Awareness, and Psychotherapy
Following Brain Injury. Illness, Crisis and Loss, 12, 2: 171- Gasquoine, P.G. & Gibbens, T.A. (1994). Lack of awareness of
186. impairment in institutionalized, severely and chronically dis-
abled survivors of traumatic brain injury: A preliminary inves-
Coetzer, B.R. & Corney, M.J.R. (2001). Grief and Self-Awareness tigation. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 9, 4: 16-24.
Following Brain Injury and the Effect of Feedback as an Inter-
vention. The Journal of Cognitive Rehabilitation, Winter: 8- Greenwood, R.J. & McMillan, T.M. (1993). Models of rehabilita-
14. tion programmes for the brain-injured adult. 1: Current provi-
sion, efficacy and good practice. Clinical Rehabilitation, 7:
Coetzer, B.R. & Corney, M.J. (2003). Grief Following Traumatic 248-255.
Brain Injury. Grief Matters, Winter.
Gündel, H., O’Connor, M.F., Littrell, L., Fort, C. & Lane R. (2003).
Coetzer, B. R. & Stein, D. J. (2003). Obsessive-compulsive disor- Functional Neuroanatomy of Grief: An fMRI Study. The
der following traumatic brain injury: clinical issues. The Jour- American Journal of Psychiatry, 160: 1946-1953.
nal of Cognitive Rehabilitation, 21 (4): 4 -8. Hainsorth, D. (1998). Reflections on Loss without Death: The
lived Experience of Acute Care Nurses Caring for Neurologi-
Corr, C.A., Nabe, C.M. & Corr, D.M. (1997). Death and Dying, cally Devastated Patients. Holistic Nursing Practice, 13, 1:
Life and Living. Pacific grove, CA, Brookes/Cole. 41-50.

Davis, C.G. & Nolen-Hoekesma, S. (2001). Loss and Meaning. Haynes, S.D. (1994). The Experience of Grief in the Head-Injured
How do people make sense of loss? American Behavioural Adult. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 9, 4: 323-336.
Scientist, 44, 5:726-741.
Hillier, S.L. & Metzer, J. (1997). Awareness and perceptions of
Deb, S., I. Lyons, & Koutzoukis, C. (1998). Neuropsychiatric se- outcomes after traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 11, 7: 525-
quelae one year after a minor head injury. Journal of Neurol- 536.
ogy, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 65: 899-902.
Hiott, D. W. & Labbate, L. (2002) Anxiety disorders associated
Deb, S., Lyons, I, & Koutzoukis, C. (1999). Neurobehavioral symp- with traumatic brain injuries. NeuroRehabilitation, 17 (4):
toms one year after a head injury. British Journal of Psychia-
try, 174: 360-365. 345 -355.

Fagin, L. & Little, M. (1984). The Forsaken Families. Houghton, P. & Houghton, D. (1977). Unfocused Grief: Responses
Harmondsworth: Penguin Books. to Childlessness. Birmingham: The Birmingham Settlement.

Feinstein, A., Hershkop, S., Jardine, A. & Ouchterlony, D. (2000). Judd, D.P. & Wilson, S.L. (1999). Brain Injury and Identity – the
The prevalence and neuropsychiatric correlates of posttrau- role of the counselling psychologists. Counselling Psychol-
matic stress symptoms following mild traumatic brain injury. ogy Review, 14: 4-16.
Brain and Cognition, 44, 78-82.
Khan-Bourne, N. & Brown, R.G. (2003). Cognitive behaviour
Fleming, J.M., Strong, J. & Ashton, R. (1998). Cluster Analysis of therapy for the treatment of depression in individuals with
self-awareness levels in adults with traumatic brain injury brain injury. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 13, 1 /2:
and relationship to outcome. Journal of Head Trauma Reha- 89-107.
bilitation, 13, 5: 39-51.
Kitson, G.C. (1982). Attachment to the Spouse in Divorce: A Scale
Fleming, J.M., Strong, J, & Ashton, R. (1996) Self-awareness of and its application. Journal of Marriage and the family, May,
deficits in adults with traumatic brain injury: how best to mea- 379-93.
sure? Brain Injury, 10, 1: 1-15.
Kübler-Ross, E. (1969). On Death and Dying. New York:
Macmillan.

Fall 2005 - The Journal of Cognitive Rehabilitation 17


Lezak, M. (1987). Relationships between personality disorders, jury as a grief response to the loss of sense of self: phenom-
social disturbances, and physical disability following trau- enological themes as indices of local lability and
matic brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, neurocognitive structuring as psychotherapy. Psychologi-
2, 1: 57-69. cal Reports, 72, 1059-1068.

Lindemann, E. (1944). Symptomology and Management of Acute Pollock, G.H. (1961). Mourning and Adaptation. International
Grief. American Journal of Psychiatry, 132, 159-163. Journal of Psychoanalysis, 42, 341-361.

MacFarlane, M.M. (1999). Treating Brain-Injured Clients and their Prigatano, G. P. (1999). Principles of neuropsychological
Families. Family Therapy, 26, 1:13-29. rehabilitation. Oxford University Press, New York.

Maker, E.L. (1982). Anomic Aspects of Recovery from Cancer. Read, S. (2002). Loss and Bereavement: A Nursing Response.
Social Science and Medicine, 16: 907. Nursing Standard, 16, 37: 47-53.

Meredith, K. & Rassa, G.M. (1999). Aligning the Levels of Aware- Sapey, B. (2002). Disability. Chapter 9 In, Loss and Grief: A guide
ness with the Stages of Grieving. The Journal of Cognitive for Human Services Practitioners. Neil Thompson (ed).
Rehabilitation, 10-12. Palgrave.

Munoz, L. (1980). Exile as Bereavement. Socio-psychological Shand, A.F. (1914). The Foundations of Character. London:
Manifestations of Chilean Exiles in Britain. British Journal of Macmillan.
Medical Psychology, 53: 227-32.
Shand, A.F. (1920). The Foundations of Character, 2nd edn. Lon-
Myles, S. M. (2004). Understanding and treating loss of sense of don: Macmillan.
self following brain injury: A behavior analytic approach.
International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Silver, J.M., Kramer, R., Greenwald, S. & Weissman, M.. (2001).
Therapy, 4 (3): 487 - 504. The association between head injuries and psychiatric disor-
ders: findings from the New Haven NIMH Epidemiologic
Newman, A.C., Garmore, W., Beatty, P. & Ziccardi, M. (2000). Self Catchment Area Study. Brain Injury, 15, 11: 935-945.
awareness of traumatically brain injured patients in the acute
inpatient rehabilitation setting. Brain Injury, 14, 4: 333-344. Snead, S.L. & Davis, J.R. (2002). Attitudes of individuals with
acquired brain injury towards disability. Brain Injury, 16, 11:
Nochi, M. (1998). “Loss of self” in the narratives of people with 947-953.
traumatic brain injuries: a qualitative analysis. Social Sci-
ences Medicine, 46 (7): 869 - 878. Stroebe, M.S. (1994). The Broken Heart Phenomenon: an Exami-
nation of the Mortality of Bereavement. Journal of Commu-
Parkes, C.M. (1970). The First Year of Bereavement: a Longitudi- nity and Applied Social Psychology, 4, 47-61.
nal Study of the Reaction of London Widows to the Death of
their Husbands. Psychiatry, 33, 444-467. Stroebe, M.S. & Schut, H.A.W. (1995). The Dual Process model
of coping with loss. Presented at the International Confer-
Parkes, C.M. (1972). Components of the Reaction to Loss of a ence on Grief and Bereavement in Contemporary Society,
Limb, Spouse or Home. Journal of Psychosomatic research, Stockholm, 12-16 June.
16: 343-9.
Stonnington, C. M. (2001). Editorial: Depression and Traumatic
Parkes, C.M. (1973). Factors Determining the persistence of Phan- Brain Injury. Brain Injury, 15, 7: 561-562.
tom Pain in the Amputee. Journal of Psychosomatic research,
17 (2) 97-108. Tasker, S. (2003). Acquired Brain Injury: Meaning-Making Out of
Lived Trauma. Illness, Crisis and Loss, 11, 4: 337-349.
Parkes, C.M. (1976). The Psychological Reaction to Loss of a
Limb: The First Year After Amputation. Ch 24 In, Modern Teasdale, W. & Engberg, A.W. (2001). Suicide after traumatic
Perspectives in the Psychiatric Aspects of Surgery (ed. J.G. brain injury: a population study. Journal of Neurology, Neu-
Howells). New York: Brunner Mazel. rosurgery and Psychiatry, 71: 436-440.

Parkes, C.M. (1985). Bereavement. British Journal of Psychiatry, Trundel, T.M., Tyron, W.W. & Purdman, C.M. (1998). Awareness
146, 11-17. of disability and long-term outcome after traumatic brain in-
jury. Rehabilitation psychology, 43, 4:267-281.
Parkes, C.M. (1996). Bereavement: Studies of Grief in Adult Life,
3rd edn. London Routledge. van den Broek, M.D. (1999). Cognitive rehabilitation and Trau-
matic Brain Injury. Reviews in Clinical Gerontology, 9: 257-
Persinger, M.A. (1993). Personality Changes following Brain In- 264.

18 Fall 2005 - The Journal of Cognitive Rehabilitation


Vandiver, V.L. & Christofero-Snider, C. (2000). TBI Club: A psy-
chosocial support group of adults with Traumatic brain in-
jury. The Journal of Cognitive Rehabilitation, July/August,
22-27.

Webster, G., Daisley, A. & King, N. (1999). Relationship and fam-


ily breakdown following acquired brain injury: the role of the
rehabilitation team. Brain Injury. 13, 8: 593-603.

Worden, W.J. (1991). Grief Counselling and Grief therapy: A


Handbook for the Mental Health Practitioner, 2nd edn, Lon-
don Routledge.

Wortman, C.B. & Silver, R.C. (1987). Coping with Irrevocable Loss.
In G.R. Vanden Bos and B.K. Bryant (eds), Cataclysms, Cri-
sis and Catastrophes: Psychology in Action (pp. 189-235).
Washington DC: American Psychological Association.

Zinner, E.S., Ball, J.D., Stutts, M.L. & Philput, C. (1997). Grief
reactions of mothers of adolescents and young adults with
traumatic Brain injury. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychol-
ogy, 12, 5: 435-447.

Fall 2005 - The Journal of Cognitive Rehabilitation 19

You might also like