Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/274709139

Organizational Justice

Chapter · December 2015


DOI: 10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.22033-3

CITATIONS READS
23 14,700

2 authors:

Russell Cropanzano Agustin Molina


University of Colorado Boulder Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile
148 PUBLICATIONS   30,505 CITATIONS    20 PUBLICATIONS   144 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

WORK-FAMILY CROSSOVER: A META-ANALYTIC REVIEW View project

The Bright Side of Work - Promoting resources and well-being at work View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Russell Cropanzano on 13 October 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Author's personal copy

Provided for non-commercial research and educational use only.


Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use.

This article was originally published in the International Encyclopedia of the Social
& Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edition, published by Elsevier, and the attached copy
is provided by Elsevier for the author’s benefit and for the benefit of the
author’s institution, for non-commercial research and educational use including
without limitation use in instruction at your institution, sending it to specific
colleagues who you know, and providing a copy to your institution’s administrator.

All other uses, reproduction and distribution, including


without limitation commercial reprints, selling or
licensing copies or access, or posting on open
internet sites, your personal or institution’s website or
repository, are prohibited. For exceptions, permission
may be sought for such use through Elsevier’s
permissions site at:
http://www.elsevier.com/locate/permissionusematerial

From Cropanzano, R., Molina, A., 2015. Organizational Justice. In: James D. Wright
(editor-in-chief), International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences,
2nd edition, Vol 17. Oxford: Elsevier. pp. 379–384.
ISBN: 9780080970868
Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Ltd. unless otherwise stated. All rights reserved.
Elsevier
Author's personal copy

Organizational Justice
Russell Cropanzano, Leeds School of Business, Boulder, CO, USA
Agustin Molina, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain
! 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Abstract

Research on organizational justice explores the psychological mechanisms by which people render judgments of fairness, as
well as their responses to these perceptions. When workers believe that they have been treated justly, they tend to show higher
job performance, better work attitudes, and lower levels of stress. With these benefits in mind, this article reviews the
literature on organizational justice, placing emphasis on the different types of fairness, as well as interactions among these
types. Individual-level and group-level approaches to justice are also contrasted, with special attention to recent theoretical
and empirical developments.

Within the organizational sciences, the term ‘organizational on procedural justice was taken from John Rawls, and work on
justice’ can be understood as having two meanings, one narrow distributive justice traces back (at least) to Aristotle. Regardless,
and one general. The narrow meaning refers to particular even when borrowing philosophical concepts, organizational
judgments concerning various types of events. When deciding justice researchers study them descriptively. For example, they
whether an occurrence is just or unjust, individuals (or groups) are unlikely to emphasize whether, say, a voice procedure is
ascertain whether or not it is morally appropriate. More truly fair, but are more likely to investigate whether people
generally, the term ‘organizational justice’ can also refer to evaluate it as fair. Organizational justice scholars also study the
scholarly literature. That is, it refers to a body of knowledge consequences of having made a fairness judgment.
about these fairness evaluations, including such topics as how
they are formulated, their dimensional structure, and their
individual- and group-level effects. Forming Perceptions of Justice
In general, workers respond more favorably – both for
themselves and for their employers – when they perceive that This descriptive perspective favored by organizational justice
they have been treated justly. They respond less well when they researchers places a great deal of emphasis on individual
perceive an injustice. Research has found that justice percep- perceptions of fairness. There are a number of different theories
tions are positively correlated with organizational commit- that attempt to account for how justice evaluations are
ment, job performance, leader–member relations, and helpful formulated. However, most of these conceptual models share
organizational citizenship behaviors (OCB). They are nega- certain common features (Cropanzano et al., 2011b), which
tively correlated with work stress, counterproductive work are reviewed here.
behaviors, and turnover intentions (Cohen-Charash and In most cases, individuals make fairness judgments when an
Spector, 2001; Rupp et al., 2014). event, and often one that is personally disappointing, occurs.
To understand whether or not the event was unfair, rather than
simply unfortunate or surprising, individuals make use of
Descriptive vs Normative Justice a comparative referent, which acts as a sort of moral yardstick.
These points of comparison go by a number of names, such as
Within the organizational sciences, justice is generally studied ‘norms’, ‘standards’, ‘rules’, or even ‘justice criteria’. When
descriptively. That is, it focuses on the perceptions or evaluations events are negative and inconsistent with these referent points,
of fairness that are formulated by people at work. Put more then people are apt to decide that an injustice has occurred. In
starkly, an event is treated as ‘just’ or ‘unjust’ because some short, individuals compare an actual occurrence to a normative
person or persons believe it to be. This approach is subjective standard and then evaluate the discrepancies (Hollensbe et al.,
and allows disagreement as to what is and is not appropriate. 2008). As described so far, the formation of justice evaluations
The descriptive paradigm taken by organizational justice may appear to be somewhat simple and logical. However, this
researchers can be distinguished from the more familiar omits a number of complicating factors.
normative perspective that is often employed by philosophers. First, before people begin to appraise the fairness of an
Speaking very generally, ethical philosophers use logic and event, it needs to be salient. If it does not catch sufficient
observation to determine what is ‘prescriptively’ or ‘normatively’ attention, then individuals will be less likely to do the cognitive
fair (Cugueró-Escofet and Fortin, 2014). That is, philosophers work involved in formulating a fairness judgment. This is one
seek to understand what the objective events or attributes are reason why negative events that are surprising and personally
that make something just (Cropanzano et al., 2011b). relevant are especially likely to be viewed as unfair.
In practice, organizational justice researchers tend to borrow Second, in real-world settings people do not always go
ideas from ethical philosophy. For example, seminal thinking through the careful steps outlined above. Often, employees

International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edition, Volume 17 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.22033-3 379

International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 379–384
Author's personal copy
380 Organizational Justice

simply model the behavior of their coworkers, thereby adopt- divided by their own inputs, while the second is a referent’s
ing the views of their peers rather than working out their own outcomes divided by the referent’s inputs. In order for equity
personal assessments. This is an important finding, which as to exist, these two ratios should match. A counterintuitive
will be seen is relevant to the formation of group-level justice prediction of equity theory is that people care about over-
perceptions. reward, as well as underreward. If an individual’s ratio is larger
Third, people differ as to who is and who is not included than that of a referent person, this means that the individual
within their scope of justice. When other people are within has greater outcomes given his or her inputs. In such
this scope, such as in-group members, evaluators are more a circumstance, Adams found that the individual will attempt
solicitous and scrupulous about applying fairness rules. Unfor- to restore equity by either raising performance (if pay is fixed)
tunately, when others are outside the scope of justice, such as or lowering performance (if pay is piece rate). In general, equity
out-group members, then evaluators becomes less circumspect theory has received support, including predictions for over-
and more disinterested in how potential victims are treated. reward. But it is not the whole story.
Findings for scope of justice may go far in explaining how An equality allocation provides everyone with the same
observers can be nonchalant about the mistreatment of others. amount, without regard to contributions. Equality allocations
Fortunately, everyone does not show this effect to the same are most likely to be used when the goal is to maximize group
degree. The scope of justice effect is stronger for those with weak harmony; equity allocations are most common when the goal
moral identities. Conversely, for individuals who see morality as is to maximize productivity. As such, equality is relatively
important to their identity, this effect is weaker (Reed and common among close, communal groups. There is some
Aquino, 2003). evidence that equality, as opposed to equity, is also preferred
Fourth, while people seem to care about fairness, they when decision making is allocating something negative, such
show considerable egocentric biases in their judgment. These as budget cuts. If we must share suffering, it seems that indi-
biases can be automatic and unconscious. For instance, when viduals often prefer to suffer by the same amount or
individuals are assigned a side in a legal case, either defendant percentage. Interestingly, equality may also have an informa-
or plaintiff, and then asked to read about the alleged events, tion processing advantage, as it is a very simple allocation rule
they show biased recall of facts and more extreme evaluative that requires less effortful thought than others. In this regard,
judgments. However, when individuals are told to read about researchers have documented the existence of an equality
the alleged events and only later assigned a role in the case, heuristic, whereby people simply divide things evenly unless
they show more accurate recall of acts and less extreme they have the cognitive resources to consider a more complex
judgments (Thompson and Loewenstein, 1992). This suggests norm (such as equity).
that personal relevance affects the processing of factual A third allocation rule is need, which provides outcomes on
information. the basis of a perceived deficit. Perhaps surprisingly, need
allocations have been less widely studied in the organizational
sciences, although even profit-making firms make at least some
Dimensions of Justice use of them. For instance, family leave policies are allocated to
those who need them – only people with particular needs (e.g.,
Thus far we have described the precipitating stimuli for justice child care or elder care) receive time off.
judgments broadly, using some terms as ‘events’ or ‘occur- In practice, when people are asked to make allocation
rences’. In fact, justice scholars have devoted considerable decision, they often mix the rules together, such that they are
attention to the types of things that people evaluate. Broadly not always used in a pure form (Cugueró-Escofet and Rosanas,
speaking, these can be broken into three classes of event – 2013). For example, one strategy might be to provide ‘neces-
outcomes (distributive justice), decision-making processes sities’ or a minimum income to all. This would be an equality
(procedural justice), and interpersonal treatment (interactional allocation. However, above this equal minimum the equity
justice). Each of these is reviewed each, introducing alternative rule could be employed. In this regard, one might use merit to
formulations where these are relevant. allocate additional payments. Another mixed approach might
be to allocate different goods in different ways. Among
American employers, for example, socioemotional goods
Distributive Justice
indicative of group standing are often allocated equally,
Distributive justice refers to the perceived fairness of an allo- whereas economic goods are often allocated through equity
cation or, more broadly, to how people judge what they (Chen, 1995). These and other sorts of mixed approaches
receive. It was probably the first type of justice to gain the seek to balance the strengths and weaknesses of the different
attention of organizational justice scholars and continues to allocation rules.
receive wide attention. In general, there are three rules that
people can use to decide whether or not their outcomes are fair
Procedural Justice
– equity, equality, and need (Deutsch, 1985).
Equity allocations treat a fair allocation as one that pays Outcomes need to arrive from somewhere. Procedural justice
individuals in accordance with their contributions. There are refers to the decision-making process or the set of policies that
different models of equity – Aristotle even had one – but by far are used to make allocation decisions. For example, trials are
the most influential is Adams’ (1965) equity theory. According one way that disputes are adjudicated in many societies.
to Adams, individuals calculate equity comparing two Research suggests that, to the extent that people perceive
outcome/input ratios. The first ratio is their own outcomes control within the legal process (e.g., they have ‘voice’, or the

International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 379–384
Author's personal copy
Organizational Justice 381

opportunity to present their case), they are more willing to Justice Rules in Real-World Settings
accept an unfavorable decision (Tyler, 2006). Interestingly,
In general, fairness researchers argue for three or four families
there seems to be marginal ‘fairness returns’ on at least some
of events – outcomes, processes, interpersonal transactions,
procedural safeguards. If a valued process is absent, then even
and information – that are appraised in accordance with a set of
small gains in its amount will boost perceptions of justice.
normative standards. While these standards certainly cover
However, at a certain point, some amount of the process
a broad class of situations, it is not clear that actual employees
appears to be satisfactory and additional gains will produce
apply them as neatly as some research might imply. Some
fewer benefits.
research suggests that decision makers supplement the better
Leventhal et al. (1980) provided a list of six well-known
researcher approaches to justice with so-called emergent rules
criteria that are used to decide whether a procedure is just. In
(Hollensbe et al., 2008). For instance, individuals may notice
general, the process should treat all parties consistently, be free
high turnover and work backwards to assume that this was
from bias, use accurate information in rendering decisions, take
caused by injustice. Alternatively, individuals might assess the
into account the views of all (something like voice), be
level of supportiveness and use this to infer that a supervisor is
correctable in the event of an error, and remain consistent with
fair. These emergent rules are important and should be the
prevailing ethical norms. These six criteria have remained
subject of future research.
influential, although some additional rules might also be
important in certain settings. For example, in some settings,
people prefer advance notice before something potentially
Interactions between and among the Types of Justice
negative occurs.
Research reviewed by Tyler (2006) suggests that procedural So far we have only considered the individual main effects of
justice place an important role in promoting citizen compli- the different types of justice. Research has long supported the
ance with the law. When people believe that the political idea that the various types of fairness interact. For instance,
system is essentially fair, such as when citizens are adequately individuals dislike receiving an unfair, or even unfavorable,
represented by their elected representatives, they are more outcome. However, the ill effects of an unjust allocation can be
likely to accept legal rules, even when these might not be partially addressed if the allocation process is viewed as fair. For
viewed as personally desirable. example, people who lose their court cases are unhappy with
the outcome, but they are less likely to derogate the legal
system if they perceive that it treated them fairly. Similar
Interactional Justice
effects have been found for workplace decisions (for a review,
Bies (2001) has argued that in addition to formal outcomes see Brockner, 2002).
and procedures, individuals evaluate fairness via social or In general, the outcome by process interaction suggests that
communication criteria. That is, they look to how they were ill effects of fairness are maximized when both types of justice
treated by others. Broadly defined, these social criteria have are low (e.g., low distributive justice þ low procedural justice).
been found to fall into at least two categories (although there Either type of fairness can buffer the effects of the other. Thus, it
may be more than two) – interpersonal justice and informa- could also be said that individuals prefer to be provided with,
tional justice. say, voice, which is a characteristic of fair procedures. However,
Interpersonal justice pertains to the dignity and respect with the absence of voice will not be problematic unless an outcome
which people are treated. An interpersonally fair transaction is unfair or at least unfavorable.
would avoid personal attaches, refrain from unnecessary Despite these supportive findings, there are other times
harshness, eschew bigotry, and so forth. Some scholars have when a fair process is unlikely to ally the anger from an unfa-
argued that this interpersonal variety of interactional justice has vorable outcome. A good example of this comes from research
an especially unambiguous relationship to judgments of on ‘moral mandates’ (Mullen and Skitka, 2006). A moral
immorality. mandate refers to a strongly held belief or opinion, which is
Informational justice, as its names applies, refers to the grounded in an ethical judgment. A moral mandate would exist
provision of relevant evidence and explanations, and this is for an issue like abortion, when the debate is framed as an issue
especially important when things go wrong. In particular, of human rights (e.g., the right to choose vs the right to life).
research has found that organizations should provide social For those possessing strong moral mandates about an issue, an
accounts, indicating to the employee why things happen as unfavorable outcome is more difficult to mitigate by means of
they do. Such accounts are important (and surprisingly effec- the procedure. Rather, the process itself is likely to be derogated
tive) in mitigating feelings of unfairness (Shaw et al., 2003). as a result of the moral mandate violation.
There is some debate over interactional justice’s structure. Interesting, the two-way interaction works the same for
Many scholars combine interpersonal and informational interactional justice as it does for procedural justice. That is,
justice together. This results in a three-factor model for fair- interactional justice has the same buffering effects on unfor-
ness – distributive, procedural, and interactional. Others tunate outcomes, and this has been observed in both the
separate out the interpersonal and informational compo- laboratory and the field (Brockner et al., 2003). This raises the
nents, producing a four-factor solution (Colquitt, 2001). possibility that any of the three types of fairness could allay
Both approaches seem to work psychometrically. Choosing the negative impact of the other two. By extension, this suggests
between the three- and four-factor models may partially that there should be a three-way interaction, such that the
have to do with the goals of the researcher and the under- effects of unfairness are disproportionately present when all
lying theory. three are low. There is some evidence for this. Studies have

International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 379–384
Author's personal copy
382 Organizational Justice

found that this three-way interaction predicts workplace retal- Group-Level Justice
iation and organizational attractiveness. At the present time,
research on the three-way interaction is limited but promising, The discussion so far has focused on individual-level percep-
as the effect has been replicated (Cropanzano et al., 2011). tions of justice. In the workplace, though, individuals are
However, less work has examined the four-factor model of constantly submerged within collective social structures. These
justice in this way, although presumably this could be done may take such forms as teams, branches, and overall organi-
as well. zations. Justice scholars are no strangers to these structures or to
their subjacent social dynamics. Over the last two decades,
organizational justice has extended the scope of interest to
Overall Justice
include the appraisal of justice events and occurrences at the
A recent trend within justice research, called overall justice, group level. Even though there are several arguments to
suggests that individuals form global judgments of how they describe the emergence of group-level justice phenomena,
are treated. Instead of focusing on the different sets of justice social information processing (SIP) theory and the attraction–
perceptions as three (or four) pieces of a puzzle, this approach selection–attrition (ASA) model represent the two approaches
allows researchers and practitioners to treat justice as a holistic most used.
assessment. The overall justice approach does not reduce the
importance of the specific types of fairness. On the contrary,
Social Information Processing Theory
evidence suggests that overall justice judgments can be made in
a ‘bottom-up’ fashion, as the specific justice perceptions are According to the SIP theory (Salancik and Pfeffer, 1978),
cognitively combined into an overall opinion. In other words, individuals do not work in isolation from one another. On the
the types of justice act as the antecedents of overall justice contrary, employees generally engage in social interactions to
judgments (Ambrose and Schminke, 2009). make sense of the events that take place during their daily
activities. As explained in a previous section, employees may
form justice assessments by simply modeling the behavior of
Multifoci Justice their coworkers (Hollensbe et al., 2008). Consequently, as time
goes by, the justice perceptions of each member of the group
In the research viewed thus far, justice judgments have been are said to converge. In other words, if group members receive
treated as if they were directed toward a class of events, such as similar outcomes, are subjected to similar procedures, and are
outcomes or processes. However, it is also important to treated in a similar fashion by a source of fairness (e.g.,
consider the entity, such as an organization or a supervisor, supervisor), over time, a shared judgment of justice emerges. In
who is the target of these appraisals. In this way, multifoci direct support of the SIP theory, Roberson (2006) demon-
justice theory adds to this body of literature by evoking the strated that the more time spent in sense-making activities, the
importance of the foci or sources of justice. In the workplace, higher the convergence of group members’ procedural and
employees generally engage in social exchange relationships distributive perceptions.
with multiple parties (Lavelle et al., 2007). Based on the nature
of those exchanges, employees can form different perceptions
Attraction–Selection–Attrition Model
for each of the sources of justice with whom they interact (e.g.,
customers, supervisor, coworkers, and the organization as The ASA model (Schneider et al., 1995) posits that individuals
a whole). Multifoci theory then highlights the importance of with similar characteristics are attracted to, selected into, and
holding others accountable for the just or unjust acts that retained by the same group. The first step, the attraction,
individuals experience in the workplace. suggests that individuals find organizations differentially
In short, research on this area has argued that employees attractive based on the congruence of the organizations’ goals
can distinguish among different sources (Rupp and Spencer, and their own personalities. The next step, the selection,
2006) and meta-analytic research has supported this concerns the recruitment and hiring of individuals who possess
contention (Rupp et al., 2014). For example, Rupp and the desired attributives looked for by the organization. The
Cropanzano (2002) found that employees form supervisory final step, the attrition, argues that the employees who do not fit
– procedural and interactional – justice perceptions. These the organization will leave their work, whereas employees who
authors further observed that those supervisory-focused do fit the organization will remain in their jobs.
perceptions could be differentiated from organizational- Colquitt et al. (2002) were among the first scholars to
focused perceptions – organizational procedural and interac- provide evidence for the ASA model. These scholars observed
tional justice. Another interesting finding from multifoci that a characteristic such as the demographic – age, ethnicity,
theory is that the relationship between a justice perception and gender – diversity within a team predicted the level of
and a variable of interest will be stronger when the source of convergence among team members’ perceptions on procedural
justice matches that of the outcome (Lavelle et al., 2007). If an justice.
organization that values its employees’ commitment as core
strength, for example, was worried about the impact of a new
set of policies that have been implemented, the best approach Justice Climate and Peer Justice
to examine the antecedents on organizational commitment
would be to measure its employees’ perceptions of organiza- Thanks to the advancements on multifoci theory, research on
tional procedural justice. group-level justice has been able to distinguish between two

International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 379–384
Author's personal copy
Organizational Justice 383

sources of justice from which shared perceptions can emerge. also examined holistically. Justice perceptions can be directed at
These are justice climate and peer justice. The former refers specific targets (multifoci justice) or formulated at the individual
to how the unit believes that it is treated by an authority or group level of analysis. In all these ways, justice has been
figure, while the latter refers to how teammates behave toward shown to be an important aspect of work life.
one another.
See also: Emotions and Work; Happiness and Work;
Justice Climate Organizational Citizenship Behavior; Organizational
Commitment; Supervision, Abusive; Work Motivation;
Naumann and Bennett (2000) were the first to use the term Workplace Deviance; Workplace Stress.
justice climate. They referred to this construct as the shared
perception of how fairly a group is treated by an authority
figure – such as the organization or the supervisor. In their
study, Naumann and Bennet measured the group’s shared
perceptions on the procedures enacted by the organization Bibliography
(i.e., procedural justice climate). Their results revealed that
procedural justice climate predicted helping behaviors above Adams, J.S., 1965. Inequity in social exchange. In: Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in
Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 2. Academic Press, New York, pp. 267–299.
and beyond individual-level perceptions of procedural justice.
Ambrose, M.L., Schminke, M., 2009. The role of overall justice judgments in orga-
That is, the perception of the group explained a part of the nizational justice research: a test of mediation. Journal of Applied Psychology 94
helping behavior performed by employees that could not be (2), 491–500.
explained by its individual counterpart (individual-level Bies, R.J., 2001. Interactional (in)justice: the sacred and the profane. In:
procedural justice). Several studies have been conducted since Greenberg, J., Cropanzano, R. (Eds.), Advances in Organizational Justice. Stanford
University Press, Stanford, pp. 89–118.
then attesting the importance of justice climate as a predictor of Brockner, J., 2002. Making sense of procedural fairness: how high procedural fairness
workplace attitudes (e.g., satisfaction, commitment), processes can reduce or heighten the influence of outcome favorability. Academy of
(e.g., citizenship behaviors, cooperation), withdrawal behav- Management Review 27 (1), 58–76.
iors (e.g., absenteeism, turnover intentions), and unit perfor- Brockner, J., Heuer, L.B., Magner, N., Folger, R., Umphress, E., Van den Bos, K.,
Vermunt, R., Magner, M., Siegel, P.A., 2003. High procedural fairness heightens
mance (e.g., customer satisfaction, financial performance;
the effect of outcome favorability on self-evaluations: an attributional analysis.
Whitman et al., 2012). Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 91 (1), 51–68.
Chen, C.C., 1995. New trends in rewards allocation preferences: a Sino-U.S.
comparison. Academy of Management Journal 56 (2), 408–428.
Peer Justice Cohen-Charash, Y., Spector, P.E., 2001. The role of justice in organizations: a meta-
analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 86 (2), 278–321.
This construct refers to the shared perception regarding how Colquitt, J.A., 2001. On the dimensionality of organizational justice: a construct
individuals judge the fairness with which they treat one another validation of a measure. Journal of Applied Psychology 86 (3), 386–400.
(Li and Cropanzano, 2009). Peer justice thus examines how Colquitt, J.A., Noe, R.A., Jackson, C.L., 2002. Justice in teams: antecedents
people who do not have formal authority over each other and consequences of procedural justice climate. Personnel Psychology 55 (1),
83–109.
behave, or believe that they behave, internally to the group.
Cropanzano, R., Li, A., Benson III, L., 2011a. Peer justice and teamwork processes.
Despite the novelty of this construct, peer justice has already Group and Organization Management 36 (5), 567–576.
demonstrated a great potential for the organizational behavior Cropanzano, R., Stein, J.H., Nadisic, T., 2011b. Social Justice and the Experience of
domain. Cropanzano et al. (2011a), for instance, showed that Emotion. Routledge, New York.
peer justice predicts task and interpersonal teamwork processes Cugueró-Escofet, N., Fortin, M., 2014. One justice or two? A model of reconciliation of
normative justice theories and empirical research on organizational justice. Journal
which, in turn, predict performance and citizenship behaviors. of Business Ethics 124, 435–451.
Similar to justice climate, the effects of peer justice were above Cugueró-Escofet, N., Rosanas, J.M., 2013. The justice design and use of management
and beyond the effects of its individual-level counterpart. control systems as requirements for goal congruence. Management Accounting
Justice climate and peer justice provide two key instruments Research 24 (1), 23–40.
Deutsch, M., 1985. Distributive Justice. Yale University Press, New Haven.
for industries and organizations that keep increasing the
Hollensbe, E.C., Khazanchi, S., Masterson, S.S., 2008. How do I assess if my
importance of team settings. By measuring both constructs, supervisor and organization are fair? Identifying the rules underlying the
researchers and practitioners can assess the full spectrum of entity-based justice perceptions. Academy of Management Journal 51 (6),
group-level justice phenomenology. Whereas justice climate 1099–1116.
reflects the treatment the group receives from an outside person Lavelle, J., Rupp, D.E., Brockner, J., 2007. Taking a multifoci approach to the study of
justice, social exchange, and citizenship behavior: the target similarity model.
or entity, peer justice provides a picture from inside the group, Journal of Management 33 (6), 841–866.
shedding light on the internal social dynamics. Leventhal, G.S., Karuza, J., Fry, W.R., 1980. Beyond fairness: a theory of allocation
preferences. In: Mikula, G. (Ed.), Justice and Social Interaction. Springer-Verlag,
New York, pp. 167–218.
Li, A., Cropanzano, R., 2009. Fairness at the group level: interunit and intraunit justice
Concluding Thoughts climate. Journal of Management 35 (3), 564–599.
Mullen, E., Skitka, L.J., 2006. Exploring the psychological underpinnings of the moral
Organizational justice is an area of study that investigates how mandate effect: motivated reasoning, identification, or affect? Journal of Person-
and why people come to form judgments of fairness in regard to ality and Social Psychology 90 (4), 629–643.
their work settings. Justice scholars also explore the consequen- Naumann, S.E., Bennett, N., 2000. A case for procedural justice climate: development
and test of a multilevel model. Academy of Management Journal 43 (5), 881–889.
tial behavior and attitudes that result from these evaluations. In Reed II, A., Aquino, K., 2003. Moral identity and the expanding circle of moral regard
general, justice perceptions can be organized into at least three toward out-groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 84 (6),
broad families – distributive, procedural, and interactional – and 1270–1286.

International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 379–384
Author's personal copy
384 Organizational Justice

Roberson, Q.M., 2006. Justice in teams: the activation and role of sensemaking in the Salancik, G.R., Pfeffer, J., 1978. A social information processing approach to job
emergence of justice climates. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision attitudes and task design. Administrative Science Quarterly 23 (2), 224–253.
Processes 100 (2), 177–192. Schneider, B., Goldstein, H., Smith, D.B., 1995. The ASA framework: an update.
Rupp, D.E., Cropanzano, C., 2002. The mediating effects of social exchange Personnel Psychology 48, 747–773.
relationships in predicting workplace outcomes from multifoci organizational Shaw, J.C., Wild, E., Colquitt, J.A., 2003. To justify or excuse?: a meta-analytic review
justice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 89 (1), of the effects of explanations. Journal of Applied Psychology 88 (2), 444–458.
925–946. Thompson, L., Loewenstein, G., 1992. Egocentric interpretations of fairness and
Rupp, D.E., Shao, R., Jones, K.S., Liao, H., 2014. The utility of a multifoci approach to interpersonal conflict. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 51
the study of organizational justice: a metaanalytic investigation into the consider- (2), 176–197.
ation of normative rules, moral accountability, bandwidth-fidelity, and social Tyler, T.R., 2006. Why People Obey the Law. Princeton University Press, Princeton.
exchange. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 123, 159–185. Whitman, D.S., Caleo, S., Carpenter, N.C., Horner, M.T., Bernerth, J.B., 2012. Fair-
Rupp, D.E., Spencer, S., 2006. When customers lash out: the effect of customer ness at the collective level: a meta-analytic examination of the consequences and
interactional injustice on emotional labor and the mediating role of discrete boundary conditions of organizational justice climate. Journal of Applied Psychology
emotions. Journal of Applied Psychology 91 (4), 971–978. 97 (4), 776–791.

International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 379–384
View publication stats

You might also like