Public Response To The Tokai Nuclear Accident: Tsunoda Katsuya

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Risk Analysis, Vol. 21, No.

6, 2001

Public Response to the Tokai Nuclear Accident

Tsunoda Katsuya1*

This article discusses the influence of the September 30, 1999 nuclear accident in Tokai vil-
lage (Japan) on the public’s attitudes toward nuclear power in Japan. The data used in this re-
port were taken from the results of two surveys conducted mainly to measure the attitudes of
the Japanese public with regard to the use of nuclear power in Japan. The first survey was
done before the accident in District 23 in Tokyo and also in Osaka and Nagoya. The second
survey, which took place after the accident in District 23 in Tokyo and in Osaka and Nagoya,
also included residents in a number of other cities of various sizes throughout Japan. The re-
sults of the two surveys showed that (1) acceptability of and trust in nuclear power operation
had decreased, (2) perceived accident likelihood and public interest had significantly in-
creased, and (3) there had been neither significant nor even a small change in the public’s
self-rated knowledge about nuclear power or their distrust of the government. The results
also showed that the ratio of nuclear power generation opponents to total respondents had
considerably increased (7% to 23%) whereas nuclear power generation supporters had mod-
erately decreased (1% to 12%).

KEY WORDS: Attitude; nuclear power; Tokai accident; Japan; covariance structure analysis

1. INTRODUCTION ation and radioactive substances that had been re-


On September 30, 1999, a criticality accident oc- leased from the site did not have any significant im-
curred at a uranium processing plant operated by pact either on the health of local residents or on the
JCO Co., Ltd. in Tokai village, Ibaraki Prefecture, environment. In the JCO plant, however, three JCO
Japan. Gaseous substances (noble gas and iodine), plant workers were exposed to high levels of radia-
which were produced in the accident, were released tion in the accident, and one of them died in Decem-
into the environment, and the radiation dose rate ber of 1999 and another in April of 2000. This was the
(gamma radiation) rose at several monitoring sta- first time that a fatal accident had occurred at a nu-
tions in the area. Furthermore, decay products of clear facility in Japan.
iodine and noble gas with short half-lives, as well as The main question of this study was “How did
activation products generated by neutron activation, the Tokai accident affect public attitudes toward nu-
were detected in some environmental samples.(1) The clear power generation in Japan?” Various studies
nuclear safety commission(1) concluded that the radi- have shown that the Chernobyl and Three-Mile acci-
dents influenced public attitudes in Europe and the
U.S.(2–6) Moreover, a historically notable disaster on
an industrial facility not only influences attitudes to-
1
Institute of Social Research, Institute of Nuclear Safety System, ward the facility, but also other factors related to
Inc., 64 Sata. Mihama-cho, Fukui 919-1205, Japan.
*Address correspondence to Tsunoda Katsuya, 11-1-704, Saginuma
these attitudes, such as perception of risk and so
1-chome, Miyamae-ku, Kanagawa, 216-0004 Japan; PXN13000@ forth. Hence, using the data of the two surveys before
nifty.ne.jp. and immediately after the accident, the effects of the

1039 0272-4332/01/1200-1039$16.00/1 © 2001 Society for Risk Analysis


1040 Tsunoda

Tokai accident on public attitudes or acceptability, as 2. Survey 2 (after the accident)


well as other factors regarding nuclear power gener- Survey population: Adults aged over 20 who
ation were investigated. These other factors included lived in Japan.
perception of risk and necessity of nuclear power, Sampling method: Stratified two-stage ran-
trust in the government and electric power compa- dom sampling.
nies, and knowledge about and interest in the subject. Survey term: October 7 to October 17, 1999.
The hypothesis of this study was that the accident Sampling number: 4,000 participants.
affected not only attitudes, but also the other fac- Stratified method: Tokyo District 23 and 12
tors mentioned above. This hypothesis was derived ordinance-designated cities as large cities
from past research(6–11) that showed that the above- (over about 500,000 populations), mid-
mentioned factors were related to attitudes toward dle cities (over 100,000 populations with-
nuclear power. Sjöberg and Sjöberg,(6) Simooka,(7) out Tokyo District 23 and 12 ordinance-
and Tsunoda(8) showed a strong correlation between designated cities), small cities (under
attitudes and perceived risk, and Flynn et al.,(9) 100,000 populations), towns and villages in
showed a strong correlation between trust in nuclear each of the 10 districts of Japan. The 12
power operation and acceptance of a radioactive ordinance-designated cities included Osaka
waste repository. Flynn et al.(9) and Tsunoda(10) both and Nagoya cities. The districts surveyed
showed a correlation between perceived risk and were Hokkaido, Tohoku, Kanto, Hokuriku,
trust, Peters et al.,(5) showed a relation between trust Tozan (consisting of Yamanashi, Nagano,
in federal government and trust in the nuclear indus- and Gifu prefectures), Tokai, Kinki, Chu-
try in West Germany, and Kumagai(11) showed a cor- goku, Shikoku, and Kyushu.
relation between knowledge about nuclear power
and attitudes. In addition, it should be noted that the
number of opponents of nuclear power significantly
Table I. Demographic Data on the Respondents
increased, whereas that of its supporters decreased. in Survey 1 and Survey 2
Renn(3) showed that the number of opponents had
significantly increased immediately after the Cherno- Survey 1 Survey 2
byl accident in Europe.
Case 1 Case 2 Case 1 Case 2

Gender
2. METHOD Male 49.8 50.5 50.1 50.3
Female 50.2 49.5 49.9 49.7
Data for this study came from two surveys that Age
had been conducted mainly to analyze public atti- 18–19 1.1 1.5 — —
tudes toward nuclear power (March 1999(12) and Oc- 20–29 15.3 18.2 17.1 15.3
tober 1999(13)) in Japan. The questionnaires used in- 30–39 20.6 19.7 16.4 17.5
cluded the same 31 items to measure public attitudes 40–49 21.3 21.1 20.1 20.2
50–59 22.7 22.6 20.4 20.2
toward nuclear power, perception of risk and neces- 60–69 18.9 16.9 17.9 15.3
sity of nuclear power, trust in the government and 70–89 — — 8.1 11.5
electric power companies, and self-rated knowledge Educational attainment
about and interest in the subject. A 7-point scale (1 = Junior high 13.7 9.7 15.2 12.6
disagree, 7 = agree) was used. The details concerning High school 44.8 40.8 41.3 41.5
each survey were as follows: College 18.2 18.2 20.7 20.2
University or higher 22.3 30.3 22.3 25.1
1. Survey 1 (before the accident) Other 0.9 1.0 0.3 0.5
Survey population: Adults aged over 18 who Employment
Employee 13.1 11.9 14.5 16.9
lived in Tokyo District 23 and in Osaka city
Self-employed person 5.3 4.5 3.8 4.4
and Nagoya city. Family-operated
Sampling method: Two-stage random sam- employee 40.1 41.0 36.7 37.7
pling in each of the three areas. Housewife 28.9 29.1 29.0 25.1
Survey term: March 19 to March 28, 1999. Student 2.6 4.2 3.3 4.4
Unemployed 9.9 9.2 12.6 11.5
Sampling number: 560 participants in each of
the three areas. Note: All values are percentages.
Public Response to the Tokai Accident 1041

Table II. Comparisons of Before and After the Accident

Case 1: Large city Case 2: Tokyo

Survey 1 Survey 2 Survey 1 Survey 2


(before (after (before (after
accident) accident) Differences accident) accident) Differences
N = 1,218 N = 627 between M N = 402 N = 183 between M

Itema M SD M SD t M SD M SD t
b
1 4.06 1.28 4.18 1.35 0.12 1.83 4.07 1.37 4.21 1.37 0.14 1.16
2 3.86 1.42 3.63 1.48 20.23 23.18** 3.86 1.45 3.67 1.45 20.19 21.48
3 4.20 1.27 4.64 1.38 0.44 6.68** 4.21 1.32 4.73 1.32 0.52 4.42**
4 3.75 1.40 3.32 1.42 20.43 26.17** 3.78 1.41 3.25 1.41 20.54 24.26**
5 4.36 1.36 4.60 1.51 0.24 3.35** 4.22 1.42 4.54 1.42 0.32 2.43*
6 4.51 1.17 4.63 1.34 0.11 1.76 4.48 1.16 4.70 1.16 0.22 1.92
7 4.30 1.15 4.41 1.30 0.11 1.86 4.32 1.20 4.54 1.20 0.23 1.94
8 4.37 1.15 4.43 1.23 0.07 1.13 4.33 1.17 4.52 1.17 0.20 1.87
9 4.80 1.21 5.07 1.26 0.27 4.50** 4.89 1.15 5.03 1.15 0.14 1.31
10 3.22 1.39 3.26 1.27 0.04 0.70 3.12 1.40 3.27 1.40 0.15 1.23
11 3.65 1.31 3.90 1.29 0.25 3.95** 3.57 1.33 3.87 1.33 0.30 2.51*
12 4.69 1.42 4.64 1.35 20.05 20.73 4.76 1.44 4.58 1.44 20.17 21.36
13 4.95 1.26 5.40 1.27 0.45 7.27** 5.10 1.20 5.44 1.20 0.34 3.04**
14 4.59 1.23 5.13 1.35 0.54 8.41** 4.65 1.26 5.04 1.26 0.40 3.24**
15 4.83 1.33 5.49 1.28 0.65 10.10** 5.00 1.29 5.41 1.29 0.40 3.45**
16 4.22 1.20 4.54 1.36 0.32 5.03** 4.29 1.21 4.50 1.21 0.21 1.80
17 4.32 1.31 4.63 1.32 0.31 4.82** 4.55 1.27 4.69 1.27 0.14 1.22
18 4.50 1.29 4.91 1.14 0.41 6.91** 4.68 1.28 4.94 1.28 0.26 2.54*
19 3.94 1.35 4.60 1.33 0.65 9.89** 4.05 1.33 4.68 1.33 0.63 5.31**
20 4.16 1.47 4.66 1.42 0.50 7.00** 4.35 1.40 4.74 1.40 0.39 3.09**
21 2.87 1.36 2.65 1.35 20.22 23.29** 2.95 1.35 2.70 1.35 20.25 22.05*
22 2.63 1.35 2.78 1.31 0.14 2.20* 2.73 1.36 2.98 1.36 0.25 2.05*
23 2.99 1.34 2.86 1.39 20.14 22.03* 3.00 1.36 2.98 1.36 20.02 20.20
24 4.73 1.37 4.61 1.40 20.12 21.81 4.64 1.38 4.46 1.38 20.18 21.44
25 5.11 1.23 5.42 1.24 0.31 5.06** 5.08 1.21 5.45 1.21 0.36 3.40**
26 5.10 1.29 5.12 1.17 0.01 0.23 5.14 1.29 5.17 1.29 0.03 0.29
27 5.40 1.21 5.34 1.21 20.06 21.01 5.45 1.16 5.42 1.16 20.03 20.28
28 3.41 1.22 2.91 1.33 20.51 27.95** 3.26 1.23 2.87 1.23 20.38 23.44**
29 3.35 1.22 2.89 1.34 20.45 27.07** 3.27 1.25 2.85 1.25 20.42 23.68**
30 3.20 1.23 2.81 1.29 20.39 26.33** 3.07 1.28 2.77 1.28 20.31 22.69**
31 3.21 1.15 2.98 1.30 20.22 23.63* 3.07 1.12 2.91 1.12 20.17 21.60
a
All items are listed in the Appendix.
b
1 = disagree, 7 = agree.
* p , 0.05; ** p , 0.01.

3. RESULT the data of Survey 1 were compared with the data


of the respondents in Tokyo District 23 and the 12
In Survey 1, 402 questionnaires were collected in ordinance-designated cities of Survey 2 as Case 1.
Tokyo (response rate: 71.8%), 406 (72.5%) in Osaka, The data of the respondents in Tokyo for Survey 1
and 410 (73.2%) in Nagoya. In Survey 2, 2,985 ques- was also compared with the data of the respondents
tionnaires (response rate: 74.6%), including 183 in Tokyo for Survey 2 as Case 2. Demographic data in
(69.3%) in Tokyo, 60 (73.2%) in Osaka, and 53 all cases and surveys are shown in Table I. Items are
(79.1%) in Nagoya, were collected. The number of re- shown in the Appendix, and the means of responses
spondents in Tokyo District 23 and the 12 ordinance- to them are shown in Table II.
designated cities was 627 (71.7%) in Survey 2. Be- Maximum-likelihood factor analysis of the data
cause the number of respondents in Osaka and of Survey 2 was used in Case 1 with oblique promax
Nagoya in Survey 2 were too small to be analyzed, rotation (Table III). Factor I was interpreted as ac-
1042 Tsunoda

Table III. Rotated Factor Patterns Table IV. Factor Correlations in Factor Analysis

Factor patterns Factor


2a
Item I II III IV V VI VII VIII h I II III IV V VI VII

1 .86 .10 .03 .05 2.03 .03 .07 2.05 .71 I .42 2.24 2.45 2.08 .11 2.24 .58
2 .75 .03 .02 .02 .05 .01 2.04 .06 .67 II 2.23 2.07 .18 .13 .05 .26
3 2.66 2.03 2.03 .17 .08 .07 .05 .05 .56 III .05 .00 .14 .03 .08
4 .71 2.03 2.07 .02 2.02 .01 .00 .24 .64 IV .39 2.06 .45 2.47
5 2.53 .06 .06 .18 2.04 2.04 .08 .07 .53 V .37 .23 2.19
6 2.03 .90 .02 2.04 .00 2.02 .00 2.06 .62 VI 2.06 .17
7 2.01 .91 2.02 .03 2.06 .01 2.03 .02 .64 VII 2.29
8 .04 .68 .02 2.02 .04 .00 2.03 .09 .48
9 .14 .43 2.04 .04 .09 .08 .13 2.02 .41 Note: Factor descriptions are given in the text.
10 .02 2.03 .66 .05 .01 .04 .05 .10 .48
11 .01 .04 .74 .07 .07 2.05 .01 .02 .48
12 .06 .05 2.57 .16 .11 2.06 .09 .12 .25 ables were the factors in factor analysis, and observed
13 .00 .02 .03 .79 2.04 2.02 .01 .00 .67
variables in measurement equations were the items
14 2.10 2.01 2.05 .80 2.05 .00 2.08 2.07 .69
15 2.06 .03 2.01 .53 .11 2.13 2.02 2.11 .38 that had more than a 0.40 factor pattern in Table III.
16 2.05 2.09 .06 .41 .03 .09 .10 2.12 .33 For example, the observed variables of the latent
17 .05 2.01 .00 .02 .76 .06 2.03 .01 .74 variable, acceptability, were items 1 through 5. The
18 2.06 .03 2.02 2.10 .77 2.02 .01 .01 .66 same measurement equations were used in all cases
19 .05 2.01 .02 .09 .62 .14 2.01 2.03 .53 and surveys. Table V shows that higher levels of per-
20 2.06 .01 .00 .00 .55 2.06 .04 .02 .46
ceived efficiency, lower levels of perceived accident
21 .03 2.03 .02 .09 2.03 .87 .03 .07 .74
22 2.01 .02 .01 2.02 .01 .81 2.02 .02 .61 likelihood, and higher levels of trust in nuclear power
23 2.06 .04 2.04 2.02 .00 .95 .00 .04 .68 operation strongly correlated with acceptability of
24 2.05 .00 2.03 .12 2.07 2.51 .03 .19 .41 nuclear power. In contrast, perceived sufficiency,
25 2.06 .01 2.03 2.14 2.06 .01 .78 2.04 .71 interest, self-rated knowledge, and distrust of the
26 .01 .02 .10 .11 .07 2.05 .56 .00 .53 government in general were not strongly related to
27 2.01 2.01 2.07 .11 .03 .01 .50 2.06 .24
acceptability.
28 .20 .00 .02 .00 2.08 2.04 .06 .71 .55
29 .14 2.03 .00 2.15 .07 2.06 2.05 .55 .59 Moreover, covariance structure analysis with
30 2.10 .01 .00 2.06 .00 .00 .00 .81 .42 structure means was used.(14,15) In this analysis, all fac-
31 .00 .04 .00 2.03 .04 .03 2.11 .51 .45 tor means of the data of survey 1 were fixed at 0 in
each case. Therefore, the factor means of 2 in Table
Note: All items are listed in the Appendix. Factor descriptions are
given in the text. Boldface means )factor pattern) . 0.4. VI are the differences of factor means between the
a
Final Communality Estimate. data of Surveys 1 and 2, and are also the attitude
changes after the accident. For Case 1, the accident
appears to have caused acceptability and trust in nu-
clear power to decrease significantly; perception of
ceptability, that is the public acceptance of nuclear efficiency, perception of sufficiency of electric power,
power; Factor II as perception of efficiency; Factor III perceived accident likelihood, interest, and distrust
as perception of sufficiency of electric power; Factor of the government to increase significantly; and no
IV as perceived accident likelihood; Factor V as inter- significant change in self-rated knowledge about nu-
est, that is the interest in nuclear power; Factor VI as clear power. The only difference in the results for
self-rated knowledge of nuclear power; Factor VII Cases 1 and 2 is that in the latter case the change in
as distrust of the government; and Factor VIII as trust distrust of the government was not significant rather
in nuclear power operation, that is, trust in the gov- than significantly increased.
ernment and electric power companies concerning In all covariance structure analyses in this re-
nuclear power. Factor correlations are shown in Table search, RMSEA was about or less than 0.05. RMSEA
IV. The results of factor analysis of Survey 1 data had is the root mean square error of approximation, a
the same structure. measure of the discrepancy per degree of freedom.
Covariance structure analysis was used to con- Brown and Cudeck(16) concluded that a RMSEA
firm factor structure and calculate interfactor correla- value of about 0.05 or less would indicate a close fit of
tions (shown in Table V). In the analysis, latent vari- the model, whereas about 0.08 or less would indicate
Public Response to the Tokai Accident 1043

Table V. Factor Correlations

Factors (latent variables) I II III IV V VI VII


a
Case 1 (large cities), Survey 1 (before the accident)
I Acceptability .54** 2.18** 2.51** 2.05 .15** 2.18** .60**
II Perception of efficiency 2.14** 2.16** .14** .16** .02 .35**
III Perception of sufficiency of electric power .03 2.12** .03 2.03 .02
IV Perceived accident likelihood .40** 2.02 .40** 2.69**
V Interest .48** .21** 2.26**
VI Self-rated knowledge 2.05 .09*
VII Distrust of the government 2.44**
VIII Trust in nuclear power operation

Case 1 (large cities), Survey 2 (after the accident)b


I Acceptability .44** 2.24** 2.59** 2.09 .12** 2.34** .73**
II Perception of efficiency 2.24** 2.15** .17** .15** .01 .29**
III Perception of sufficiency of electric power .05 .01 .14** .05 .06
IV Perceived accident likelihood .35** 2.12** .52** 2.65**
V Interest .43** .25** 2.22**
VI Self-rated knowledge 2.09 .15**
VII Distrust of the government 2.40**
VIII Trust in nuclear power operation

Case 2 (Tokyo), Survey 1 (before the accident)c


I Acceptability .58** 2.29** 2.49** .00 .12* 2.18** .54**
II Perception of efficiency 2.23** 2.22** .12* .13* 2.03 .32**
III Perception of sufficiency of electric power .03 2.07 .01 .00 2.04
IV Perceived accident likelihood .35** 2.04 .44** 2.72**
V Interest .46** .22** 2.28**
VI Self-rated knowledge .02 .09
VII Distrust of the government 2.52**
VIII Trust in nuclear power operation

Case 2 (Tokyo), Survey 2 (after the accident)d


I Acceptability .32** 2.18 2.49** 2.10 .19* 2.27** .72**
II Perception of efficiency 2.13 .06 .25** .21* .21* .16
III Perception of sufficiency of electric power .01 2.01 .14 .01 .13
IV Perceived accident likelihood .37** 2.09 .59** 2.58**
V Interest .53** .42** 2.28**
VI Self-rated knowledge 2.02 .09
VII Distrust of the government 2.36**
VIII Trust in nuclear power operation

Note: Factor descriptions are given in the text. RMSEA 5 root mean square error of approximation.
a
N = 1,218, x2(406) = 1,684.995, p , 0.001, RMSEA = 0.051.
b
N = 627, x2(406) = 965.619, p , 0.001, RMSEA = 0.047.
c
N = 402, x2(406) = 909.250, p , 0.001, RMSEA = 0.056.
d
N = 183, x2(406) = 608.0317, p , 0.001, RMSEA = 0.052.
* p , 0.05; ** p , 0.01.

a reasonable error of approximation. They stated that 4. DISCUSSION


they would not want to employ a model with an
RMSEA greater than 0.1. Table V shows a strong correlation between (a)
Figure 1 shows that in all items other than Item acceptability of nuclear power and perceived acci-
1, in which there was no significant change of mean, dent likelihood, (b) acceptability and trust in nuclear
the ratio of the number of opponents of nuclear power operation, (c) perceived accident likelihood
power generation to the total number of respondents and trust, and (d) trust and distrust in the government.
considerably increased (7% to 23%), whereas sup- The first result agrees with Sjöberg and Sjöberg,(6)
porters moderately decreased (1% to 12%). Simooka,(7) and Tsunoda(8); the second, with Flynn et
1044 Tsunoda

Table VI. Factor Means: Survey 2—After the Accident

Case 1a Case 2b
(large cities) (Tokyo)

Factors (latent variables) M t M t

I Acceptability 20.26 24.21** 20.29 22.55*


II Perception of efficiency 0.12 2.26* 0.22 2.09*
III Perception of sufficiency of electric power 0.14 2.61** 0.21 2.27*
IV Perceived accident likelihood 0.52 9.62** 0.36 3.58**
V Interest 0.42 8.41** 0.32 3.50**
VI Self-rated knowledge 20.09 21.41 20.01 20.12
VII Distrust of the government 0.11 2.14* 0.14 1.54
VIII Trust in nuclear power operation 20.46 28.21** 20.36 2 3.70**

Note: The factor means of Survey 1 (before the accident) fixed to 0.


a
Case 1: x2(858) = 3,080.304, p , 0.001, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) =
0.037.
b
Case 2: x2(858) = 1,685.517, p , 0.001, RMSEA = 0.041.

al.(9); the third with Flynn et al.(9) and Tsunoda(10); and cantly in both cases. The government trustworthiness
the fourth with Peters et al.(5) In contrast, Table V in other domains was not implicated, so it is unsur-
shows that the correlations between self-rated knowl- prising that general trust in the government de-
edge and factors other than interest, and those be- creased only marginally. The same applies to the rela-
tween interest and acceptability were not large. The tion between self-rated knowledge and interest. The
former result does not agree with Kumagai,(9) but most likely explanation may be that the increase noted
Sjöberg and Sjöberg(17) and Biel and Dahlstrand(18) in ratings of interest was measuring an increase in
showed that knowledge had little correlation with risk peoples’ concern about their safety that was not large
perception and support the present study’s result. enough to produce a significance increase in informa-
Table VI shows that acceptability of nuclear tion seeking.
power and perceived accident likelihood significantly Figure 1 shows that in all items other than Item 1,
changed. In the U.S., however, Renn(3) found little the ratio of opponents to total respondents con-
change in the acceptability of nuclear power. Also, siderably increased (7% to 23%). This agrees with
Lindell and Perry(19) found no significant difference Renn,(3) who showed that opposition to nuclear power
between accident likelihood before the Chernobyl immediately after the Chernobyl accident had peaked
accident and the likelihood after the accident. This in Finland, Yugoslavia, and Greece (over 30%); con-
probably was due to the fact that their American siderable increase (over 20%) had been observed in
sample would not have been likely to think that an Austria, West Germany, and Italy; and moderate
accident at a Russian reactor had any relevance to the changes had taken place in the U.K., France, the Neth-
operation of an American nuclear power plant. In ad- erlands, Sweden, and Spain (12% to 18%). Figure 1
dition to this, Renn(3) pointed out that the countries shows that the level of the change that took place in
with the most dramatic attitude changes had been Japan after the Tokai accident is nearly equal to that in
those with high levels of radiation exposure. the U.K. and other areas after the Chernobyl accident.
Table VI also shows that distrust of the govern- On the other hand, Fig. 1 also shows that the
ment hardly changed. Table V, however, shows that ratio of supporters to total respondents moderately
distrust of the government correlated with trust in decreased (1% to 12%) in all items other than Item 1.
nuclear power operation, which significantly changed. In Items 2 and 5, the ratio of supporters to total
One explanation for these results may be as follows. respondents barely decreased (1% to 5%). One hy-
The respondents who distrusted nuclear power oper- pothesis is that opponents cited the news of the
ations also distrusted the government in general. This accident as support for their perception in the inevi-
would account for the moderate correlation between tability of a catastrophic accident, whereas support-
the two variables. However, the accident was directly ers might find confirmation for their perception that
related to negligence in nuclear power operations, so defense-in-depth had made nuclear power plants safe
trust in nuclear power operation declined signifi- enough for continuation, and consider the accidents
Public Response to the Tokai Accident 1045

Fig. 1. Ratios of opponents and supporters. In Items 1,


2, and 4, Categories 1–3 are opponents, 5–7 are support-
ers, and 4 is neutral. In Items 3 and 5, Categories 1–3 are
supporters, 5–7 are opponents, and 4 is neutral.

as unique, that is, not likely to happen again at nu- 3. We should slowly decrease the number of our
clear facilities.(4,19) In Items 3 and 4, however, the ratio nuclear power stations.
moderately decreased (5% to 12%). It follows from 4. In the future, we should also construct new nu-
what has been said that neutrals were more likely to clear power stations.
become opponents than supporters were likely to be- 5. New nuclear power stations should not be
come neutrals. constructed.
6. Nuclear power generator can produce a greater
amount of electricity and at a lower cost than
thermal power can.
APPENDIX: QUESTIONNAIRE ITEMS
7. A nuclear power generator is efficient.
1. In the future, we should also use nuclear power 8. We can use electricity at a low price with nuclear
generation. power.
2. If there is a vote for propulsion of nuclear power, 9. Nuclear power generation can provide a great
I will vote for it. amount of electricity with a small amount of fuel.
1046 Tsunoda

10. The amount of electricity used in Japan is too much. REFERENCES


11. Japan’s ability to generate electricity is sufficient.
12. In the future, it is likely that electricity will be 1. The Nuclear Safety Commission. (1999). A summary of the re-
port of the criticality accident investigation committee [provi-
insufficient. sional translation]. Tokyo, Japan.
13. Within my lifetime, a severe nuclear power plant 2. Nealy, S. M., Melber, B. D., & Rankin, W. L. (1983). Public
accident will occur somewhere again in the world. opinion and nuclear energy. Lexington, MA: Lexington.
3. Renn, O. (1990). Public responses to the Chernobyl accident.
14. A severe accident at a nuclear power plant will Journal of Environmental Psychology, 10, 151–167.
eventually occur in Japan as well. 4. Midden, C. J. H., & Verplanken, B. (1990). The stability of nu-
15. I am worried that an accident may occur in a clear attitudes after Chernobyl. Journal of Environmental
nuclear power station. Psychology, 10, 111–119.
5. Peters, H. P., Albrecht, G., Hennen, L., & Stegelmann, H. U.
16. Nuclear power plant accidents occur frequently. (1990). “Chernobyl” and the nuclear power issue in West Ger-
17. I am interested in the subject of nuclear power. man public opinion. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 10,
18. I am interested in the influences, contributions, 121–134.
6. Sjöberg, B. D., & Sjöberg, L. (1990). Risk perception and wor-
and problems that nuclear power presents to ries after the Chernobyl accident. Journal of Environmental
modern society. Psychology, 10, 135–149.
19. I often watch TV news and read news about 7. Shimooka, H. (1993). Process of public attitudes toward nu-
clear power generation. Journal of the Atomic Energy Society
nuclear power. of Japan, 35, 115–123.
20. I want to know the detailed mechanisms of nu- 8. Tsunoda K. (1998, September). The structure of the attitude to
clear power generation. nuclear power generation. Paper presented at the annual
meeting of the Atomic Energy Society of Japan, Fukui, Japan.
21. I am very familiar with nuclear power plants. 9. Flynn, J., Burns, W., Mertz, C. K., & Slovic, P. (1992). Trust as
22. I am very familiar with scientific technology. a determinant of opposition to a high-level radioactive waste
23. I know nuclear power better than the average repository: Analysis of a structural model. Risk Analysis, 12,
417–429.
person. 10. Tsunoda K. (1999). A study of determinants of risk perception
24. I have less knowledge about nuclear power than concerning nuclear power generation. Japanese Journal of
other persons. Risk Analysis, 11, 54–60.
11. Kumagai, A. (1995). Publicity work and survey on secondary
25. Current politics only emphasize the interests of education. Japanese Journal of Nuclear Power, 37, 379–383.
specific organizations. 12. Tsunoda K. (in press). Difference in the formation of attitude
26. The government intends to give priority to the toward nuclear power.
13. Tsunoda K. (2000). Public opinion about using nuclear power
benefit of big companies over political decision generation and its formation factors after JCO accident. Avail-
making. able from Institute of Nuclear Safety System, Inc., 64 Sata,
27. In truth, most of the important decisions in our Mihama-cho, Fukui 919-1205, Japan.
14. Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables.
society are kept from the general public. New York: Wiley.
28. We can trust electric power companies with nu- 15. Arbuckle, J. L., & Wothke, W. (1999). Amos 4.0 users’ guide.
clear power without anxiety. Chicago: SmallWaters.
16. Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1992). Alternative ways of
29. The nuclear policy of the government is trustworthy. assessing model fit. Sociological Methods and Research, 21,
30. Enterprises of nuclear power are faithful to the 230–258.
nation. 17. Sjöberg, B. D., & Sjöberg, L. (1991). Adolescents’ attitudes to
nuclear power and radioactive wastes. Journal of Applied So-
31. The government openly provides the public with cial Psychology, 21, 2007–2036.
information about nuclear power. 18. Biel, A., & Dahlstrand, U. (1995). Risk perception and the lo-
cation of a repository for spent nuclear fuel. Scandinavian
Journal of Psychology, 36, 25–36.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 19. Lindell, M. K., & Perry, R. W. (1990). Effects of the Chernobyl
accident on public perceptions of nuclear plant accident risks.
The author thanks Yutaka Kano, Naosuke Itoi- Risk Analysis, 10, 393–399.
gawa, Mohamed Hafsi, Sarah Yagi, and the reviewers
for helpful comments.

You might also like