Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Soc. Sci. Med. Vol. 43, No. 10, pp.

1453-1460, 1996
Pergamon Copyright © 1996ElsevierScienceLtd
Printed in Great Britain.All rights reserved
S0277-9536(96)00045-7 0277-9536/96 $15.00 + 0.00

SEX DIFFERENCES IN COPING AND DEPRESSION


AMONG YOUNG ADULTS
VILMA H,~NNINEN ~ and HILLEVI ARO 2
~University of Tampere, Department of Sociology and Social Psychology, PO Box 607, 33101 Tampere,
Finland and 2National Public Health Institute, Helsinki and Tampere School of Public Health, Tampere,
Finland

Abstract--The main purpose of the study was to evaluate the notion that the association between sex and
depression is partly mediated by differences in coping styles. The study is based on questionnaire data
from 890 female and 766 male Finnish 22-year-old young adults. Of the 12 ways of coping presented,
self-blame, venting anger on others, seeking comfort in sweets and drinking beer were positively, and the
others negatively, associated with depression; these were called dysfunctional and functional ways of
coping, respectively. Women resorted much more often than men to dysfunctional ways of coping, except
drinking. They also scored lower on personal resilience and showed more depressive symptoms. The sex
difference in depressiveness was eliminated when dysfunctional coping was taken into account. Copyright
© 1996 Elsevier Science Ltd

Key words---depression, coping, sex differences

INTRODUCTION more often to maladaptive ways of coping than


men?
There is abundant research evidence (see e.g. [1, 2]) Studies on the effectiveness of different types of
that women suffer from both depressive affect coping have found that, on average, those
and clinical depression more often than men. This ways of coping which aim at problem-solving
sex difference emerges during adolescence [3-5]. are more effective than those which focus on
Several explanations have been offered, mainly emotions [8, 9]. This does not, however, mean that
focusing on either characteristics of women them- all emotion-focused ways of coping are ineffective
selves (biological or psychological factors) or or dysfunctional; rather, the category of emotion-
the social situation of women (social roles, amount focused coping seems to be heterogeneous
of life stress) (see [1]). A theory that now enjoys with respect to functionality. It has been noted
increasing popularity says that women on average that some emotion-focused ways of coping (e.g.
cope less effectively with adversities than men, positive reappraisal) pave the way to problem-
and therefore are more likely to be depressed [1, 6]. solving, while others (e.g. disengagement) exclude
For example, Petersen et al. [4] suggest on the problem-solving efforts [11]. A distinction has
basis of their results that negative life events induce been made between "salutary effort", which is a
boys, but not girls, to develop effective coping mature coping strategy including both problem-
strategies which protect them from depression later in focused and certain emotion-focused ways of
life. coping, and "stress palliation", which is a less
It is generally held that coping has a moderating mature coping strategy involving such emotion-
effect on the outcome of stressful situations. Studies focused ways of coping as blaming oneself, letting
dealing with coping in specific situations have shown bad feelings out, smoking, drinking or eating [12].
that the way of coping adopted affects both the A similar category of dysfunctional coping has
situation itself and its emotional consequences [7-9]. been termed by Billings and Moos [10] as "emotional
Coping style as a personal disposition has also been discharge".
found to be associated with levels of emotional There is accumulating support for the hypothesis
well-being (e.g. [10]). that the greater depressiveness of women might be
The question as to whether women are more due partly to their less effective coping. In general,
susceptible to depression than men because of women seem to be more inclined to resort to
their different coping style entails two subquestions: emotion-focused ways of coping and/or less to
which kinds of way of coping are functional problem-focused ways of coping than men. In the
and which are dysfunctional as regards depression; studies of Folkman and Lazarus [13], Vingerhoets
and do women resort less often to adaptive or and van Heck [6] and Stone and Neale [14], women

1453
1454 Vilma H~inninen and Hillevi Aro

appeared as less inclined than men to use problem- METHOD


focused ways of coping. On the other hand, in some
studies women have been found to be more inclined The data came from the second stage of a
to use emotion-focused ways of coping, including prospective study concerning stress, development and
dysfunctional ones [6, 8, 9, 14]. For example, Pearlin psychological well-being in adolescents [21-24]. The
and Schooler [8] and Menaghan [9] found that first stage of the study was carried out in 1983 and
women more often than men used "selective ignor- the second stage in 1989. Since the questionnaire of
ing", which was considered dysfunctional. In their the first stage did not include scales of depression and
study of depressed patients, Billings and Moos [10] coping, this study is based on the second stage only.
found that women made more frequent use of
emotional discharge, which was associated with more Sample
severe impairment. Nolen-Hoeksema [1, 15] argues The original sample included all the ninth-grade
that women's greater risk of depression stems from pupils from all secondary schools in one of the
their greater tendency to ruminate on their depressive biggest towns in Finland. The sample included 1071
symptoms, which exacerbates and prolongs their girls and 1123 boys aged about 16 years. Since the
depressive episodes. questionnaires were filled in during school hours, the
Discussions on the reasons underlying sex differ- original response rate was as high as 97%.
ences in coping have provided three main lines of At the second stage of the study, on which this
explanation, The first of these refers to personal study is based, the data were gathered by postal
differences; women are thought to cope less success- questionnaires which were sent to all participants of
fully because, as persons, they are less active, less the first stage. The response rate was 83% (n = 890)
self-confident and less internal in their control for women and 68% (n = 766) for men. The differ-
expectations than men. This is supported by the ence between men's and women's response rate was
research evidence which suggests that women, on significant (X2(1) = 65.8, P < 0.001). Participants and
average, have lower self-esteem than men [5, 16], non-participants of the later study had not been
which might contribute to their less effective coping. different in the earlier stage of the study as regards
Several studies indicate that a person's coping their amount of psychosomatic symptoms or level
dispositions are indeed associated with such personal of self-esteem. Among men, however, the non-
characteristics as self-esteem, locus of control, hard- participants had reported having been heavily drunk
iness, etc. [12, 17-19]. Unfortunately, sex differences by alcohol in the preceding year somewhat more
have received little attention in these studies. The often than the participants (t = 2.94, P < 0.01). It is
second type of explanation refers to the fact that the possible, then, that heavily drinking men are slightly
choice of coping strategy depends to a great extent underrepresented in the sample.
on situational factors [20]. In this vein it can be The mean age of the respondents was 21.9
hypothesized that the life situation of women allows (SD = 0.3) years. Thirty-five per cent of women and
for less active coping strategies than that of men (e.g. 19% of men were married or cohabiting (X2(1)=
[8]). A third line of explanation refers to the cultural 52.4, P < 0.001); 9% of women and 4% of men
norms, expectations and habits that encourage men had children (;(2(1)= 16.6, P < 0.001). Twenty-one
to adopt active or distracting coping strategies and per cent of women and 7% of men had at least
women to express and ruminate on their emotions a secondary vocational degree (X2(1)= 63.0,
[15] (pp. 171-2). P < 0.001). Forty per cent of women and 33% of
The main purpose of the study is to evaluate men were full-time students (~2(1) = 10.56, P < 0.05);
the notion that the association between sex and and 48% of women and 58% of men held a full-time
depression is mediated by differences in coping job (X2(1) = 14.0, P < 0.001).
styles.
Variables
(1) Can ways of coping be divided into functional Depression. The number of depressive symptoms
and dysfunctional ones as regards depression? was measured by a Finnish modification (RS-BDI)
Are the associations between coping and for population studies of the 13-item version of
depression different among women and Beck's Depression Inventory (BDI). BDI [25] is a
men? widely used self-rating scale with good reliability and
(2) Do women and men differ in their use of validity [26]. The shortened BDI has been found to
functional or dysfunctional ways of coping? have a high correlation (r = 0.96) with the original
(3) Can differences in coping be explained on the version and has been recommended for research use
grounds of women's lower resilience, higher by Beck and his colleagues [27, 28]. The suggested
amount of life stress, or by differences in life cutoff point for mild depression for the shortened
situation? scale is 5 [27]. The modified scale was otherwise
(4) Does the difference in functional or dysfunc- identical with Beck's original 13-item inventory
tional coping contribute to the sex difference in except that it also included introductory questions
depression? and an additional positive choice of answer for each
Sex differences in coping and depression among young adults 1455

item [29]. The additional positive choices did not (meaningfulness). The items reflecting low resilience
affect the scoring of depressive symptoms. In the were reversed, so that a high score represents high
sample, 56% scored 0, 33% scored 1-4 and 11% resilience. Cronbach's ~ for the whole scale was 0.89
scored 5 or more. For the logistic regression analyses, (M = 3.8, SD = 0.59).
the scale was dichotomized ( 0 - 4 = 0 , 5 or m o r e = l).
Coping. Ways of coping were determined by a FINDINGS
checklist (12 items) resembling the "Ways of coping
checklist" developed by Folkman and Lazarus [13]. Basic sex differences
The respondents were asked to think about recent The mean score of depressive symptoms was higher
adversities (not specified) they had encountered and among women than among men (t = 3.52,
to say how often (five-point scale: "never" to "very P < 0.001). (Table 1). The percentage of depressives
often") they used to act or think in certain ways (i.e. those scoring 5 or more on the RS-BDI) was 13.1
in those situations. The list was designed to fit the among women and 9.1 among men (X'(1)= 6.67,
life of young Finnish adults and to cover certain P < 0.01).
theoretically relevant dimensions (active problem- Women scored significantly lower than men on the
solving, distancing, positive reappraisal, seeking resilience score (t = - 7,96, P < 0.001). On the other
social support, inward and outward aggression and hand, comparison of the life stress measures of
palliation). women and men showed that there was no difference
The means and standard deviations of the coping in the number of negative life events (t = 0.84, ns) or
items are presented in Table 1. in subjective life stress (t = 1.18, ns) (Table 1).
Life stress. Life stress was assessed by two
measures. First, the number of life events was The associations of coping items with depressiveness
investigated by a 32-item life-events checklist In order to be able to compare the effectiveness
designed to cover the changes that are common and of women's and men's ways of coping, the ways
relevant in adolescence and young adulthood. The of coping were first evaluated in terms of their
time period concerned was 12 months. As depression effectiveness as regards depression (Table 1).
has been found to be associated mainly with negative All the coping items correlated significantly with
life events (e.g. [30] (pp. 100-106)), an index was depressive symptoms. As regards the direction of the
constructed for the purposes of this study, taking into correlation, the ways of coping fell into two opposite
account only those events that are objectively clusters.
definable and that are generally held to be negative Eight coping items correlated negatively with
(e.g. death of a family member, loss of job, divorce) depressive symptoms, i.e. the more one used these
0 5 items). ways of coping, the less often he or she reported
In addition, a subjective measure of life stress was depressive symptoms. The strongest negative corre-
obtained by asking the subjects to rate whether lations were found in "tackling the problem more
different spheres of life (e.g. work, financial situation, persistently" (r = - 0.24, P < 0.001), "thinking
relations with parents, sex life) induced in them "joy that there is no reason to get upset" (r = - 0 . 2 1 ,
and satisfaction" or rather "worry and resentment" P < 0.001), "reassuring oneself that everything will
(five-point scale; 10 items). The measure of subjective be alright" (r = - 0 . 1 6 , P < 0.001), "thinking the
life stress was only used to assess whether there are problem over with a friend" (r = - 0.16, P < 0.001),
such subtle differences between women and men in "trying to resolve the situation" ( r = - 0 . 1 5 ,
the stressfulness of life which would not be detected P < 0.001) and "trying to find something relaxing to
by the life-events checklist. A sum index of subjective do" (r = - 0.14, P < 0.001).
life stress was computed by counting the number of Four of the coping items, on the other hand, were
spheres that were evaluated negatively (values 4 or 5) positively associated with high scores of depressive
(M = 1.2, SD = 1.3). symptoms, namely "blaming oneself for what has
Resilience. The concept of personal resilience refers happened" (r = 0.26, P < 0.001), "seeking comfort in
to the individual's personal style that is supposed sweets" (r = 0.18, P < 0.001), "venting anger on
to encourage effective coping with difficulties. This others" (r = 0.14, P < 0.001) and "going out for a
variable overlaps theoretically with the dimension few beers" (r = 0.14, P < 0.001).
that Kobasa [31] has termed "hardiness" (which The coping items correlated with depressive
includes commitment, challenge and control). The symptoms in much the same way among women and
resilience scale is the mean of items of three inter- men.
correlated scales: beliefs of control (5 items), self- In general, functional ways of coping were more
esteem (7 items) and experiencing life as meaningful commonly used than dysfunctional ones (Table 1).
(5 items). These were all measured by a five-point
scale, indicating agreement with statements such as Indices of functional and dysfunctional coping
"How my life turns out depends on no one else but Since the ways of coping clearly fell into two
me" (control), "I think I have many good properties" clusters as regards their relation to depressive symp-
(self-esteem) and "I feel my life is lacking meaning" toms (and also as regards their intercorrelations),
1456 Vilma Hiinninen and Hillevi Aro

Table 1. Correlations with depressive symptoms, means and standard deviations of the main
variables in the total sample and among women and men
Total Women Men
fN = 1656) (n = 890) fn = 766)
Depressive symptoms M 1.63 1.88 1.35”’
SD 3.05 3.21 2.82
Resilience rL - 3.85
0.56*** - 0.57*** - 0.55’”
3.69 3.92***
SD 0.59 0.57 0.58
Negative life events 0.27**’ 0.25”’ 0.31***
L 1.12 1.14 1.09
SD I .27 1.33 1.19
Subjective life stress 0.46’** 0.45”’ 0.48”’
Ll 1.21 1.24 1.16
SD 1.34 I .32 1.34
Coping items
I tackle the problem more
persistently than before A - 3.36
0.24*** - 0.17*** - 0.32”’
3.28 3.479**
SD 1.oo 1.01 1.oo
I tell myself that there
is no reason to get upset LI - 3.27
0.21*** - 0.21*** - 0.20’”
3.21 3.34’
SD 1.12 1.12 1.11
I think the problem over
with a friend L - 3.60
0.16’” - 0.21*** - 0.19”’
3.92 3.22***
SD 1.20 1.06 1.24
1 do everything 1 can to
resolve the situation or
to prevent it from recurring IG - 3.96
0.16**’ - 0.13’” - 0.18***
3.92 4.00’
SD 0.84 0.82 0.86
I reassure myself that
everything will be ahight rli - 3.71
0.15*** - 0.21”. - 0.09*
3.79 3.75
SD 0.92 0.92 0.93
I try to find something
relaxing to do Ll - 3.55
0.14*** - 0.17*** - 0.09*
3.45 3.67’”
SD 1.08 1.11 1.04
I tell myself that I will
just have to adjust to the
situation IL - 3.63
0.10*** - 0.10** - 0.09’
3.58 3.69*
SD 1.03 I .05 1.00
1 ask for outside help rli - 2.40
0.06* - 0.05 - 0.09’
2.44 2.35
SD 1.12 1.16 1.07
I go out for a few beers 0.14** 0.16*** 0.14.”
IL 2.15 I .99 2.33*‘*
SD 1.28 1.24 1.30
I vent my anger
on other people 0.14*** 0.15*** 0.07
L 2.83 3.23 2.37”’
SD 1.25 1.19 1.16
I seek comfort in sweets 0.18*** 0.20’99 0.09*
Ll 2.49 2.91 2.01***
SD I .34 I .37 1.13
I blame myself for what
has happened r 0.27”’ 0.25*** 0.27”*
3.13 2.71***
S”D 2.93
1.14 1.10 1.14
Coping indices
Functional coping r - 0.30*** - 0.30*** - 0.30***
SE 0.53
3.44 3.45 3.44
0.54 0.53
Dysfunctional coping 0.29*** 0.31*** 0.24***
FCI 2.60 2.82 2.36***
SD 0.77 0.75 0.72
Nom: The coping items are ordered according to the direction and strength of their association
with depression.
Sex differences of the means are tested by two-tailed r-test.
*P < .05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.
Sex differences in coping and depression among young adults 1457

two indices of coping were computed. The index sweets" (t = 14.69, P < 0.001). Men, on the other
of functional coping was the mean of all the coping hand, more often than women resorted to "going
items that were negatively associated with depressive out for a few beers" (t = - 5.39, P < 0.001).
symptoms (M = 3.4, SD = 0.53), and the index of Comparison of the means of the sum indices of
dysfunctional coping was the mean of the coping coping shows that there was no difference between
items that were positively associated with depressive- women and men as regards functional coping, but
ness (M = 2.6, SD = 0.77). The ct coefficient was 0.52 women used altogether more dysfunctional ways o f
for functional coping and 0.45 for dysfunctional coping than men (Table I).
coping. The sex difference o f dysfunctional coping was
The indices of functional and dysfunctional coping similar among depressives and non-depressives,
were uncorrelated (r = 0.01, ns). Functional coping among those scoring below and those scoring above
correlated positively (r = 0.33, P < 0.001) and dys- the median of the resilience scale, among single and
functional coping negatively (r = - 0.40, P < 0.001) married, and among those having at least secondary
with resilience. Negative life events correlated posi- vocational degree and among the less educated. In
tively with dysfunctional coping (r = 0.13, P < 0.001), these subgroups, the size of the sex difference of the
but not with functional coping (r = 0.01, ns). mean of dysfunctional coping varied from 0.4 to 0.6
points.
Sex and coping
The sex differences in coping are presented in Sex, coping and depression
Table 1. As the last step in the analyses, a series of logistic
O f the eight functional coping items (i.e. those that regression equations were computed (by SPSS) with
were negatively correlated with depressive symp- depression as the dependent variable.
toms), one was more c o m m o n among women, In the first model (Model 1 in Table 2), sex and
namely "thinking the problem over with a friend" the coping variables were entered as independent
(t = 12.13, P < 0.001). On the other hand, men more variables. Functional and dysfunctional coping had
often than women used such functional ways of opposite but equally strong associations with
coping as "trying to find something relaxing to d o " depression (Wald = 72.6, P < 0.001 and 71.2,
(t = - 4 . 1 5 , P < 0.001) and "tackling the problem P < 0 . 0 0 1 , respectively). Sex had no effect on
more persistently than before" (t = - 3 . 8 2 , depression when the coping variables were taken into
P < 0.001). account.
O f the four dysfunctional ways of coping (those In the second model (Model 2 in Table 2), the
which correlated positively with depressive symp- negative life-events variable was added. It had a
toms), three were more c o m m o n among women: significant positive effect on the probability of
"venting anger on other people" (t = 14.8, depression (Wald = 39.7, P < 0.001). The co-
P < 0.001), "blaming oneself for what happened" efficients of the other variables remained almost
( t = 7 . 6 3 , P < 0 . 0 0 1 ) and "seeking comfort in unchanged.

Table 2. Logistic regression with depression"as the dependent variable and sexb, coping
variables, negative life events and resilienceas independent variables
Wald
(df= I) R exp(B) Model X2
Model 1
Sex 0.00 0.00 0.99
Functional coping 72.56*** - 0.25 0.27
Dyfunctional coping 71.23"** 0.25 2.77
161.0"**
(dr= 3)
Model 2
Sex 0.03 0.00 1.03
Functional coping 75.27*** - 0.26 0.25
Dysfunctional coping 56.19*** 0.22 2.49
Negative life events 39.66*** 0.18 1.44
200.25***
(df = 4)
Model 3
Sex 0.01 0.00 0.98
Functional coping 8.03** - 0.08 0.58
Dyfunctional coping 5.90* 0.06 1.43
Negative life events 29.17"** 0.16 1.44
Resilience 128.43"** - 0.34 0.08
363.7***
(df= 5~
,0 = RS-BDI < 5, I = RS-BDI > = 5.
t'0 = male, 1 = female.
*P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001.
1458 Vilma H/inninen and Hillevi Aro

Inclusion of resilience (Model 3 in Table 2), on the which also seemed to be a dysfunctional coping
other hand, brought about changes in the model. The tactic.
effect of resilience on the probability of depression In functional coping, on the other hand, no sex
was negative and very strong ( W a l d = 128.4, difference was found, since the men's stronger
P < 0.001). The size of the coefficients of the coping tendency to use ways of coping aimed at problem-
variables and life events in the model dropped, but solving or distancing was compensated by the
remained statistically significant. It seems, then, that women's stronger resort to seeking social support.
resilience partly mediates the association between The sex differences in coping found in this study
coping (and life events) and depression (or vice versa). are partly similar to those found in earlier studies.
The finding that women more often than men seek
DISCUSSION social support in stressful situations is consistent with
earlier findings (e.g. [33]). Similarly, there is some
The observed associations between the variables evidence that women are more inclined to reduce
under study are presented in Fig. 1. distress by eating [34]. Also, the finding that women
In the following, the results are summarized and more readily than men blame themselves for their
commented upon in the order of the research problems could be expected on the basis of earlier
questions presented in the Introduction. studies [35, 36]. On the other hand, the observation of
1. Ways of coping fell into two independent, and, women's greater inclination to vent their feelings on
as to their effect, opposite, clusters: functional and other people was somewhat surprising, because men
dysfunctional ways of coping. Dysfunctional ways of are usually thought to be more outwardly aggressive
coping included such items as self-blame, venting than women. In fact, however, there is no consistent
anger on other people, seeking comfort in sweets and empirical support to the notion of men's generally
drinking. Functional ways of coping included both higher outward aggressiveness; in some other studies
problem-focused and emotion-focused ones, such as too, women are found to be more prone to express
reassuring oneself that there is no reason to get upset their anger than men (see [15] (p. 136)).
or tackling the problem more persistently than A problem that warrants notice is that the
before. participation rate was significantly different for
The correlations between coping items and women and men. Those men who did not participate
depressive symptoms were highly similar among at this stage had, at the previous stage, reported being
women and men. Thus no qualitative differences were heavier drinkers than those who participated in the
found between the sexes in the functionality or follow-up. It is probable, then, that the most
dysfunctionality of different ways of coping. This dysfunctionally coping men have dropped out from
finding differs from that of the study of Stanton et al. the later stage of the study, which leads to
[32], in which certain emotion-focused ways of coping overestimation of the sex difference in dysfunctional
were found to be more beneficial for women than for coping. However, if all the men who did not respond
men. (360) had participated and if all of them fell above
2. Women resorted clearly more often than men the mean of the "dysfunctional coping" variable, the
to such dysfunctional ways of coping as blaming proportion of dysfunctional copers among men
themselves, venting anger on others and seeking would be 51%, which still does not quite reach the
comfort in sweets. The difference was partly balanced proportion of dysfunctional copers among the female
by men's stronger tendency to cope by drinking beer, participants (53%). Since it is not plausible that all

- I

• DYS~IC'I~NALCOI~IG ¢r DI~I~ ISlON

a 0-male 1 = f:nm~l~ lmll~m~ticm


- - - mlpnivem~iEiou
--

Fig. 1. Observed associations between sex, coping variables, life events, resilience and depression.
Sex differences in coping and depression among young adults 1459

the non-participating men were dysfunctional copers, tional coping correlated with depression, but they did
and since it is probable that also among female not correlate with each other.
non-participants the proportion of dysfunctional Low resilience was strongly associated with
copers is higher than among participants, we believe depression, so strongly that it seems to be something
that the difference in the response rate does not alone like a precursor of depression. It was also associated
explain the observed sex difference in dysfunctional with the same variables as depression--sex, coping
coping. variables and life events. Judging on the basis of the
3. Women scored lower than men on the resilience strength of the associations, the path from sex to
scale, and low resilience was associated with dys- depression would look like this: sex-dysfunctional
functional coping. However, the sex difference in coping-low resilience-depression. This interpretation
dysfunctional coping was not eliminated when of the nature of resilience as a close relative of
resilience was controlled for. Neither was the sex depression suggests that it is an altering state, subject
difference in coping attributable to situational to many influences, rather than an enduring
factors, such as negative life events, subjective life personality trait. Saying that women, on average,
stress, or familial or employment status. We find it show lower resilience than men would thus amount
probable, therefore, that the sex difference in coping to saying that they are, on average, closer to a
at least partly stems from cultural factors, i.e. depressive mood state.
different social habits, expectations and norms
regarding men's and women's coping. CONCLUDING REMARKS
4. The sex difference in depression was eliminated
when (dysfunctional) coping was controlled for. Coping research has traditionally started with the
Besides coping indices, depression was associated assumption that coping is an adaptive process which
with low resilience and negative life events. buffers the effects of life stress. However, several
No differences in life stress were found between studies, including this one, have revealed ways of
women and men by our methods and in our coping that not only fail to buffer but in fact tend to
sample. It is not, however, warranted to generalize exacerbate stress (e.g. 9, 10, 18, 37]). Moreover, these
this finding. The sample might well be exceptional in dysfunctional ways of coping seem to hang together.
this respect, since it consists of young adults only. The combination of seemingly different items of
Compared to studies using full adult samples (see [1]), dysfunctional coping in our study resembles the
the sex difference in depression observed in our study factors found in factor-analytical studies of coping,
was relatively small. It is reasonable to assume that where it has been referred to as "emotional
sex differences in life stress may increase later in life, discharge" [10], "stress palliation" [12] and "focusing
adding to the sex difference in depression. on and venting of emotions" [17]. Both theoretical
and empirical work is needed to gain more
Discussion on the relations between the variables understanding of the nature and background of such
Since this study was cross-sectional in nature, no dysfunctional coping style.
definitive statements about the causal paths between Why are some ways of coping functional and
the factors under study can be made--and, indeed, others dysfunctional? A simple characterization
we believe that there are no mechanical unidirectional could be that coping is functional when it helps to
causal relations in matters like this. It is, however, transcend the problem either practically or mentally,
possible to defend the idea that dysfunctional coping whereas dysfunctional coping "glues" the person
increases the risk of depression (and not solely vice to the situation by focusing his/her attention to its
versa). negative aspects. In their study of concrete stressful
As regards the path between sex, dysfunctional encounters, Folkman et al. [7] found that planned
coping and depression, sex is of course the starting problem-solving and positive reappraisal led to
point. The relation between coping and depression is, positive outcomes, while confrontive coping (which
however, less clear. Could the causal chain run from overlaps with our concept of dysfunctional coping)
sex to depression to poor coping? The notion that and distancing led to negative outcomes regarding
coping is causally prior to depression can, however, the stressful situation. In addition to failing to solve
be defended by referring to the finding that also the problem, dysfunctional ways of coping may lead
non-depressed women used more dysfunctional ways to adverse side-effects (obesity, alcohol problems,
of coping than men. problems in social relations, etc.) which have a
Second, could dysfunctional coping be just part of negative impact on well-being.
the depressive syndrome itself?. It has been noted [37] The coping style indices used in our study mainly
that many items used to measure emotion-focused measure variation in habitual responses to relatively
coping actually measure psychological impairment. If everyday adversities. It might be that in connection
dysfunctional coping were just a part of depression, with more severe crises different ways of coping
however, it would be associated with the same would appear as functional or dysfunctional--what is
explanatory factors as depression itself. This was not functional coping as regards a minor quarrel with the
the case in our study: both dysfunctional and func- spouse, for instance, might well be dysfunctional
SSM 43/10~-B
1460 Vilma H~inninen and Hillevi Aro

as regards divorce. Moreover, the functionality or 17. Carver C. F., Scheier M. F. and Weintraub J. K.
dysfunctionality of a coping style is defined in Assessing coping strategies: a theoretically based
approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 56, 267, 1989.
relation to depression only in this study. Further
18. Bolger N. Coping as a personality process: a prospective
research is warranted as regards the functionality/ study. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 59, 525, 1990.
dysfunctionality of coping strategies in different 19. Holahan C. H. and Moos R. H. Personal and
contexts. contextual determinants of coping strategies. J. Pets.
The question as to why women, more often than Soc. Psychol. 52, 946, 1987.
20. Mattlin J. A., Wethington E. and Kessler R. C.
men, cope in a dysfunctional way remains open. Situational determinants of coping and coping effective-
Understanding of the processes leading to differences ness. J. Hlth Soc. Behav. 31, 103, 1990.
in coping would, however, be very important. It 21. Aro H. Life stress and psychosomatic symptoms among
could be sought by conducting research on the 14 to 16-year-old Finnish adolescents. Psychol. Med.
17, 191, 1987.
development of coping styles in relation to life
22. Aro H., H~inninen V. and Paronen O. Social support,
experiences in adulthood, on differences in child- life events and psychosomatic symptoms among
rearing practices as regards coping, as well as on the 14-16-year-old adolescents. Soc. Sci. Med, 29, 1051,
cultural models, norms and habits which may induce 1989.
women to adopt dysfunctional coping strategies. 23. Aro H. and Taipale V. The impact of timing of puberty
on psycosomatic symptoms among fourteen- to
sixteen-year-old Finnish girls. Child Develop. 58, 261,
REFERENCES 1987.
24. Aro H. and Palosaari U. Parental divorce, adolescence,
1. Nolen-Hoeksema S. Sex differences in unipolar and transition to young adulthood: a follow-up study.
depression: evidence and theory. Psychol. Bull. 101,259, Am. J. Orthopsychiat. 62, 421, 1992.
1987. 25. Beck A. T., Ward C. H., Mendelson M., Mock J. E. and
2. Weissman M. M. and Klerman G. L. Sex differences in Erlbaugh J. K. An inventory for measuring depression.
the epidemiology of depression. Arch. Gen. Psychol. 34, Arch. Gen. Psychiat. 4, 561, 1961.
98, 1977. 26. Rabkin J. G. and Klein D. F. The clinical measurement
3. Graham P. and Rutter M. Adolescent disorders. In of depressive disorders. In The Measurement of
ChiM and Adolescent Psychiatry: Modern Approaches Depression (Edited by Marsella A. J., Hirschfield R. M.
(Edited by Rutter M. and Hersov L.), pp. 351-367. and Katz M. M.), pp. 30-83. John Wiley, Chichester,
Blackwell Scientific, Oxford, 1985. 1987.
4. Petersen A. C., Sarigiani P. A. and Kennedy R. E. 27. Beck A. T. and Beck R. W. Screening depressed patients
Adolescent depression: why more girls? J. Youth in family practice. Postgrad. Med. 81, December 1972.
Adolesc. 20, 247, 1991, 28. Beck A. T., Rial W. Y. and Rickels K. Short form
5. Avison W. R. and McAlpine D. D. Gender differences of depression inventory: cross-validation. Psychol. Rep.
in symptoms of depression among adolescents. J. Hlth 34, 1184, 1974.
Soc. Behav. 33, 77, 1992. 29. Mattlar C-E., Raitasalo R., Putkonen A-R., Hyypp~i
6. Vingerhoets A. J. J. M. and van Heck G. Gender, M. T., Englund Ch., Helenius H. and Knuts L-R. The
coping and psychosomatic symptoms. Psychol. Med. prevalence of depression in a random sample of Finns,
20, 125, 1990. and the association of depression with various cognitive
7. Folkman S., Lazarus R. S., Dunkel-Schetter C., functions. Abstracts of the International Epidemiological
DeLongis A. and Gruen R. J. Dynamics of a stressful Association 3/1 Scientific Meeting, Helsinki, Finland,
encounter: cognitive appraisal, coping, and encounter 8-13 August 1987. International Epidemiological
outcomes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 50, 992, 1986. Association, Helsinki, 1987.
8. Pearlin L. J. and Schooler C. The structure of coping. 30. Brown G. and Harris T. Social Origins of Depression.
J. Hlth Soc. Behav. 19, 2, 1978. Tavistock Publications, Cambridge, 1984.
9. Menaghan E. Measuring coping effectiveness: a panel 31. Kobasa, S. Stressful life events, personality, and health:
analysis of marital problems and coping efforts. J. Hlth an inquiry into hardiness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 37, 1,
Soc. Behav. 23, 220, 1982. 1979.
10. Billings A. G. and Moos R. H. Coping, stress, and 32. Stanton A. L., Danoff-Burg S., Cameron C. L. and Ellis
social resources among adults with unipolar depression. A. P. Coping through emotional approach: problems
J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 46, 877, 1984. of conceptualization and confounding. J. Pets. Soc.
11. Scheirer M. F., Weintraub J. K. and Carver C. S. Psychol. 66, 350, 1994.
Coping with stress: divergent strategies of optimists and 33. Ross C. E. and Mirowsky J. Explaining the social
pessimists. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51, 1257, 1986. patterns of depression: control and problem-solving--
12. Jorgensen R. S. and Dusek J. B. Adolescent adjustment or support and talking? J. Hlth Soc. Behav. 30, 206,
and coping strategies. J. Pers. 58, 503, 1990. 1990.
13. Folkman S. and Lazarus R. An analysis of coping in a 34. Greeno C. G. and Wing R. R. Stress-induced eating.
middle-aged community sample. J. Hlth Soc. Behav. 21, Psychol. Bull, 115, 444, 1994.
219, 1980. 35. Cramer P. Children's use of defence mechanisms in
14. Stone A. A. and Neale J. M. New measure of daily reaction to displeasure caused by others. J. Pers. 51, 78,
coping: development and preliminary results. J. Pers. 1983.
Soc. Psychol. 46, 892, 1984. 36. Levit D. Gender differences in ego defenses in
15. Nolen-Hoeksema S. Sex Differences in Depression. adolescence: sex roles as one way to understand the
Stanford University Press, Stanford, 1990. differences. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 61,992, 1991.
16. D'Arcy C. and Siddique C. M. Psychological distress 37. Rohde P., Lewinsohn P. M., Tilson M. and Seeley J. R.
among Canadian adolescents. Psychol. Med. 14, 615, Dimensionality of coping and its relation to depression.
1984. J, Pers. Soc. Psychol. 58, 449, 1990.

You might also like