Rzezak Et Al - 2016 - Decision Making

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Epilepsy & Behavior 60 (2016) 158–164

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Epilepsy & Behavior

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/yebeh

Decision-making in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy: Delay


gratification ability is not impaired in patients with
hippocampal sclerosis
Patricia Rzezak b,c,d,⁎, Ellen Marise Lima c, Fabricio Pereira b, Ana Carolina Gargaro e, Erica Coimbra e,
Silvia de Vincentiis a,b,c,d, Tonicarlo Rodrigues Velasco e, João Pereira Leite e,
Geraldo F. Busatto b,c, Kette D. Valente a,b,c,d
a
Laboratory of Clinical Neurophysiology,Psychiatry Department,University of São Paulo (USP) School of Medicine,São Paulo, SP, Brazil
b
Laboratory of Neuroimaging in Psychiatry (LIM 21),University of São Paulo (USP) School of Medicine,São Paulo, SP, Brazil
c
Group for the Study of Cognitive and Psychiatric Disorders in Epilepsy — Clinics Hospital,University of Sao Paulo (USP),Brazil
d
Center for Interdisciplinary Research on Applied Neurosciences (NAPNA),University of Sao Paulo (USP),Brazil
e
Ribeirao Preto School of Medicine,Department of Neurosciences and Behavior,University of Sao Paulo (USP),Brazil

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Background: Decision-making abilities have rarely been examined in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy related
Received 26 August 2015 to hippocampal sclerosis (TLE-HS). We aimed to investigate the ability to delay gratification, a decision-making
Revised 18 April 2016 subdomain, in patients with intractable TLE-HS and to verify the association of delay gratification performance
Accepted 19 April 2016 and cool executive function tests.
Available online 18 May 2016
Methods: We evaluated 27 patients with TLE-HS (mean age: 35.46 [±13.31] years; 7 males) and their cognitive
performance was compared with that of 27 age- and gender-matched healthy controls (mean age: 35.33
Keywords:
Decision-making
[±12.05] years; 7 males), without epilepsy and psychiatric disorders. Patients were assessed using the delay
Epilepsy discounting task (DDT) and tests of attention, shifting, inhibitory control, and concept formation. Results were
Temporal lobe correlated with clinical epilepsy variables such as age of onset, epilepsy duration, AED use, history of status epi-
Delay discounting lepticus, febrile seizures, and the presence of generalized seizures. Statistical analysis was performed using one-
Executive functions way ANCOVA with years of education as a confounding factor.
Hippocampal sclerosis Results: Patients and controls demonstrated similar performance on DDT, showing similar discount rate (p =
0.935) and probability rate (p = 0.585). Delay gratification was not related to cool executive function tests
(Digit Span, Stroop Color Test, Trail Making Test, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, and Connors' CPT). History of status
epilepticus, presence of generalized seizures and higher seizure frequency, age at onset, and epilepsy duration
had a significant impact on DDT.
Conclusion: Patients with intractable TLE-HS showed unimpaired delay gratification abilities, being able to accept
a higher delay and a lower amount of chance for receiving a higher reward in the future. Clinical variables related
to the epilepsy severity impacted the performance on delay gratification. Impairment on cool aspects of executive
function was unrelated to this decision-making domain.
© 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction aspects of EF related to planning, cognitive flexibility, and inhibition —


the cool domains of EF. The so-called cool EF are often associated with
It is well established that patients with temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) lateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) and are elicited by relatively abstract,
may present deficits in executive functions (EF). Executive functions is decontextualized tasks [2–4].
an umbrella term for several cognitive subfunctions, including working Hot domains of EF are defined as those observed in emotionally and
memory, inhibitory control, decision-making, and task-switching [1]. At motivationally significant situations since they involve meaningful, self-
present, most studies in epilepsy have focused on the most traditional relevant rewards or punishments [5]. One of the most studied hot EF
abilities across neurological and psychiatric disorders is decision-
making. The ventromedial PFC is frequently associated with this ability
⁎ Corresponding author at: University of São Paulo, Laboratory of Clinical
Neurophysiology, Department of Psychiatry, R. Dr. Ovídio Pires de Campos, 785 - Caixa
[6]. In addition, limbic structures, such as the amygdala, are assumed to
Postal 3671, CEP 01060-970 São Paulo, SP, Brazil. be essential for the generation of an automatic emotional state, which is
E-mail address: patriciarzezak@gmail.com (P. Rzezak). implicated in response to a gain/loss in the context of decision-making

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2016.04.042
1525-5050/© 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
P. Rzezak et al. / Epilepsy & Behavior 60 (2016) 158–164 159

[7]. Emerging evidence suggests that damage to medial temporal lobes cool EF. Based on the hypothesis that delay gratification may be more
impairs performance on decision-making tasks when choice is influ- related to the affective system, but not to the reflective system, we
enced by representations of previous experiences [8,9]. It is reasonable predicted that patients with TLE-HS would show similar performance
to believe that medial temporal lobe-based memory functions may be when compared with healthy subjects in a delay gratification task, and
implicated in the decision-making processes. Nevertheless, these find- that the cool EF performance would not be correlated to delay
ings are controversial as some studies demonstrated that patients discounting performance.
with amnesia with lesions to the medial temporal lobes had a normal
performance on a decision-making paradigm [10]. 2. Methods
The investigation of decision-making abilities in patients with TLE
with lesions located in mesial temporal structures, such as TLE caused 2.1. Participants
by hippocampal sclerosis (TLE-HS), may corroborate the importance
of these structures to this subdomain of hot EF. Although there might Patients with TLE-HS were followed in the Outpatient Epilepsy Clinic
be an overlap between brain regions that participate in decision- in Clinics' Hospital — University of São Paulo. All subjects signed an in-
making and those related to TLE-HS, this cognitive domain has seldom formed consent form approved by the local Ethics Committee. Patients
been investigated in these patients. and controls were enrolled in a protocol that included neurological,
It is relevant to have in mind that decision-making is not a unidi- psychiatric, and neuropsychological evaluations. In addition, patients
mensional construct, and the type of decision-making ability can vary underwent a neurophysiological (including electroencephalogram
according to the precision and predictability of the knowledge of the [EEG] and video-EEG) and neuroimaging study with 3 Tesla magnetic
possible outcomes of a decision (i.e., decision under ambiguity [“implic- resonance imaging (MRI — Intera Achieva, Philips). Epilepsy clinical
it”] and under risk [“explicit”] conditions) or the tendency to discount information was obtained from the patient and a relevant other in an
future rewards (i.e., delay discounting). Dual-process models of interview, close to the time of the neuropsychological assessment.
decision-making suggest that risk-taking decisions can be made by Patients and controls were interviewed by a psychiatrist, using a
affective or cognitive controlled systems. The first one is automatic, structured clinical interview, the Structured Clinical Interview for
effortless, fast, and emotional while the second is deliberative, con- Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR)
trolled, slow, neutral, and reflective [11–13]. Different brain regions Axis I Disorders (SCID-I/P) [22] for the assessment of the presence of
are involved in each of these types of decision-making. The neural cor- any psychiatric disorders. After this evaluation, patients and controls
relates underlying the affective system are thought to be the amygdala with major psychiatric disorders (major mood disorders, generalized
and the striatum. On the other hand, the ventromedial PFC, dorsolateral anxiety disorder, conversive/dissociative disorders, or psychosis) were
PFC, the anterior cingulate, and the hippocampus are thought to be excluded.
implicated in the reflective system [14]. Thus, one may assume that Patients with prior history of neurosurgery (including epilepsy
according to the type of decision-making paradigm, and the system surgery), drug intoxication, previous or current history of substance
required to perform this task, different brain regions may be activated. abuse, lack of adherence to treatment, and IQ scores lower than 70
The reflective system is assumed to have a more pronounced (obtained from Block Design and Vocabulary subtests of the Wechsler
relationship with cool EF. Brand et al. [15] suggested that cool EF are Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd Edition — WAIS-III) were not included in
important in risk-taking decisions because they are crucial for the cate- the current study. Finally, patients with other lesions, such as dual pa-
gorization of information and options, the implementation of strategies, thology, previous history of stroke, or any other neurological disorder
and the integration of feedback. were excluded from this study.
Delay discounting is a type of decision-making characterized by a For each selected patient with TLE-HS, an age- and gender-matched
depreciation of the value of a long-term reward with an overvaluation control participant was included. In order to match participants by age,
of a short-term recompense. Therefore, the value of a reward is time- we considered an age difference between subjects of no more than five
related, considering the time that this reward takes to be delivered. years.
Delay discounting is widely used as a measure of impulsiveness. In pa-
tients with impulse control impairment, higher rates of delay 2.1.1. Patients with TLE-HS
discounting are documented since these subjects prefer to sacrifice Forty-three patients with TLE were enrolled after neurological and
long-term greater rewards in favor of smaller rewards that are available psychiatric evaluation. Four patients with TLE were excluded because
immediately [16]. of lack of confirmation of hippocampal sclerosis in 3.0 T MRI, one for in-
Emerging evidence suggests that patients with TLE have worse per- complete neuropsychological assessment, and one with an IQ lower
formance on decision-making tasks in which consequences and their than 70.
probabilities are “implicit” (decision-making under ambiguity or Our final sample consisted of 27 patients with TLE-HS who were
feedback-based decision-making) [17]. In this context, the decision candidates with refractory epilepsy. All patients were evaluated before
maker has to initially figure out the options' qualities by processing the surgical procedure. These patients had an unequivocal diagnosis of
feedback of previous decisions [18–21]. On the other hand, patients TLE-HS according to MRI. The epileptogenic zone was determined by
with TLE usually show similar performance compared with controls in EEG and long-term inpatient video-EEG for surgical purposes.
tasks of decision-making when information is “explicit” about the po- This group was composed of twenty females and seven males.
tential consequences of different options and their subsequent probabil- Participants had a mean age of 35.46 years old (SD: 13.31, ranging
ities [18,19,21]. Therefore, there is some evidence that these patients from 16 to 58 years old), average length of education of 10.44 years
may show impairments in some decision-making domains and not in (SD: 3.02, ranging from 4 to 16 years), and showed mean estimated IQ
others [18–21]. To the best of our knowledge, delay gratification abilities (based on Vocabulary and Block Design subtests of WAIS-III) of 93.19
have not been investigated in patients with TLE-HS. Moreover, it is still (SD: 13.05, ranging from 74 to 129). Eight patients (29.6%) had a history
not clear whether a delay discounting paradigm, with no feedback of of status epilepticus, ten patients (38.1%) had a history of febrile sei-
the consequence of a decision, may be influenced by cool EF impair- zures, and three patients (12.5%) were seizure-free at the time of clinical
ments in patients with epilepsy. The delay discounting paradigm does evaluation. Of the remaining patients, seizure semiologies were as fol-
not offer an immediate reward or punishment related to the decisions, lows: dyscognitive seizures (seven patients); generalized tonic-clonic
and does not require updating previous choices before deciding be- seizures GTC (one patient); autonomic and dyscognitive seizures (five
tween immediate and delayed gratifications. Thus, we assume that it patients); autonomic, dyscognitive and GTC (five patients); autonomic
is more related to the affective system and may not be influenced by and GTC (four patients); and dyscognitive and GTC (one patient).
160 P. Rzezak et al. / Epilepsy & Behavior 60 (2016) 158–164

Mean age at epilepsy onset was 13.52 years (SD: 9.34; ranging from 1 to 5. Connors' CPT [35] — vigilance and inhibitory control abilities.
40 years). Mean duration of epilepsy was 22.52 years (SD: 13.19; rang- Numbers of omission and commission errors were used for this test.
ing from 3 to 54 years). Seven patients were on monotherapy, and eigh-
teen were on polytherapy. Clinical data were not available for two
patients. 2.3. Data analysis

2.1.2. Group of healthy controls Demographical variables, such as age, years of education, and IQ
Twenty-seven healthy volunteers with no history of neurological or scores, were compared between groups using Student t-tests. In relation
psychiatric disorders were also evaluated. Twenty subjects (61.5%) to our primary outcome, for each individual, a hyperbolic discount rate
were female, and seven were male, with a mean age of 35.33 years (k, amount of time that the subject accepted to delay his/her gratifica-
(SD: 12.05, ranging from 18 to 55 years). Average length of education tion for receiving a higher reward) and probability rate (h, amount of
was 13.22 years (SD: 3.07, ranging from 8 to 23 years), and mean esti- chance that the subject agreed to gamble for receiving a higher reward)
mated IQ was 111.00 (SD: 14.97, ranging from 86 to 138). were estimated from their choice data in the DDT. Because the k and h
parameters were not normally distributed (Kolmogorov–Smirnov
2.2. Procedures Z = 1.57; p = 0.015 and Z = 1.23; p = 0.096, for k and h scores in
the patient group, respectively), the distributions of these parameters
2.2.1. Delay discounting task (DDT) were normalized by using the natural log transformation. This proce-
Participants were evaluated with a computerized delay discounting dure was previously used by others in similar studies using this task
task (DDT). This task consisted of a series of choices between two possi- [17]. To compare performance on DDT and other executive function
bilities: 1) to earn a certain amount of money immediately or 2) to re- tests, we applied analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), using years of formal
ceive R$ 1000 after a defined period of time. The amount of money education as a covariate. Our patient group had fewer years of formal
varied among twenty alternatives (1; 5; 10; 20; 40; 60; 80; 100; 150; education than controls (t(53) = −3.30; p = 0.002). Education attain-
300; 450; 600; 750; 900; 920; 940; 960; 980 or 990 Brazilian Reais) ment might have an impact in executive function as previously demon-
whereas the period of time had seven delay options (1 day, 1 month, strated by Cutler et al. [36]. For this reason, education was considered as
6 months, 1 year, 5 years, 10 years, or 50 years). Choices were made a covariate. We used Pearson's correlation coefficient to identify associ-
within a set of pairwise combinations (amount of money and time ations between DDT scores and results of executive functions tests in
delay variables) that totaled 140 trials. patients with TLE-HS. In addition, we explored associations between
Participants were also evaluated under the Probability Discounting the task parameters (log(k) and log(h)), and TLE-HS clinical variables
Test (PDT). This task is very similar to the previous one, but here, all (i.e., age at onset, epilepsy duration, number of AEDs, presence of gener-
trials reward the participant immediately. Subjects have to choose be- alized seizures, seizure frequency, history of status epilepticus, and his-
tween earning for sure an amount of money or taking a probability of tory of febrile seizures) in patients using a series of Student t-tests for
gaining R$ 1000. The variable “amount of money” varied identically to categorical variables and Pearson's correlation analysis for continuous
the DDT whereas there were seven alternatives for the “probabilistic variables. All reported probability values are two-tailed with the signif-
variable” (5%, 10%, 30%, 50%, 70%, 90%, and 95%). Pairwise permutations icance set at p = 0.05. A tendency towards significance was adopted
(amount of money and probabilistic with both variables) also summed when the p-value was between 0.050 and 0.100. A conservative thresh-
140 trials. old of p = 0.001 was used for correlation analysis in order to decrease
The delay discounting task was translated to Portuguese, and the the chance of error due to multiple comparisons. The Stata 12.0 was
task was implemented according to Rachlin et al. [23]. All data collection used for the statistical analyses.
and management software were executed in Matlab Version r2012a
(MathWorks, Natick, Mass). The ranges of delays and cash values tested 3. Results
have been explored in previous studies [23,24]. The 280 trials of both
discounting tests were presented in a single session in a random 3.1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the sample
order. The elapsed time to make each choice was not limited, and partic-
ipants were not instructed as to how quickly they should respond. Patients with TLE-HS and healthy controls had similar age [t(53) =
1.67; p = 0.102]. On the other hand, patients with TLE-HS had fewer
2.2.2. Executive function tests years of formal education [t(53) = −3.30; p = 0.002] and had lower es-
The following neuropsychological evaluation was designed to timated IQ scores [t(53) = − 4.61; p b 0.001] than healthy subjects
investigate cool EF of patients with TLE-HS. This protocol was based (Table 1).
on previously published literature in patients with TLE [25–29].
3.2. Performance on delay discounting task
1. Digit Span Forward and Backward [30] — auditory attention (Digit
Forward) and working memory abilities (Digit Backward). The raw No significant differences between patients with TLE-HS and con-
scores of Digit Forward and Backward were used separately. trols were observed either in the discounting rate (Log (k); F(1,52) =
2. Stroop Color Test [31] — selective attention, mental flexibility, and in- 0.01; p = 0.935) or in the probability rate (Log (h); F(1,52) = 0.31;
hibitory control. Only the score (total time) of the last presentation p = 0.583) of the delay discounting task (Table 2). To further explore
(Stroop III), which requires higher inhibitory control, was included this lack of differences between groups, the same analysis was done
in the analyses, as previously done by others [32,33].
3. Trail Making Test [32] — complex visual scanning, visual search speed,
visual attention, mental flexibility, and task-switching. An index score Table 1
Sociodemographic description of the two samples.
was obtained by discounting the time to complete Part A from Part B.
In that way, an actual value of task-switching is obtained [32,33]. TLE-HS group mean Control group mean t p
4. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test [34] — abstract behavior, set-shifting, (SD) (SD)
response inhibition, and mental flexibility. Here, we considered the Age 35.46 (13.31) 35.33 (12.05) 0.04 0.971
number of categories achieved, the number of perseverative Education 10.44 (3.02) 13.22 (3.07) −3.30 0.002⁎
responses, number of nonperseverative errors, and number of fail- IQ 93.19 (13.05) 111.00 (14.97) −4.61 b0.001⁎

ures to maintain the set. ⁎ Statistical significance at p b 0.05.


P. Rzezak et al. / Epilepsy & Behavior 60 (2016) 158–164 161

Table 2 3.5. Impact of clinical epilepsy variables on the delay gratification ability
Between-group comparisons in delay gratification and cool executive function tests, using
education as a covariate.
Significant impact of epilepsy variables in DDT scores were observed
TLE-HS group mean Control group mean F p for the following variables: presence of generalized seizures (yes[mean]
(SD) (SD) versus no[mean] on Log DDT probability: −0.17 versus 0.35; t = −2.51;
Log (k) −1.70 (5.91) −1.76 (4.25) 0.00 0.935 p = 0.023), seizure frequency (frequent[mean] versus
Log (h) 1.20 (4.24) 1.44 (3.40) 0.31 0.583 unfrequent[mean] on DDT probability: 0.02 versus 0.07; t = − 2.09;
DDT 0.05 (0.07) 0.03 (0.02) 0.94 0.338
p = 0.057); history of status epilepticus (yes[mean] versus no[mean]
PDT 2.06 (2.30) 1.90 (1.78) 0.01 0.934
Digit Forward 6.58 (2.23) 8.11 (2.08) 2.02 0.162 on Log DDT probability: − 0.12 versus 0.25; t = − 1.89; p = 0.076),
Digit Backward 4.29 (2.18) 6.22 (2.49) 4.34 0.043⁎ and seizure control (yes[mean] versus no[mean] on Log DDT delay:
Stroop Test 38.17 (14.69) 32.23 (42.18) 0.01 0.954 −2.34 versus − 1.68; t = −1.75; p = 0.096). Longer duration of epilep-
TMT 120.96 (107.62) 33.41 (20.90) 8.58 0.005⁎ sy was related to Log of DDT (r = 0.42; p = 0.066) and to DDT probabil-
WCST cat 2.71 (1.85) 2.88 (1.56) 0.13 0.719
WCST PR 19.17 (17.07) 8.85 (8.50) 2.37 0.130
ity (r = 0.46; p = 0.042). Earlier age of onset was related to DDT delay
WCST NPE 14.50 (9.30) 13.22 (7.31) 0.09 0.771 scores (r = 0.041; p = 0.067).
WCST FS 0.54 (0.78) 0.38 (0.70) 0.36 0.554
CPT om 16.10 (19.97) 3.07 (4.66) 6.25 0.016⁎ 4. Discussion
CPT com 14.25 (6.06) 9.19 (5.85) 8.21 0.006⁎

Legend: Log (d): discount rate; Log (h): probabilistic rate; TMT: Trail Making Test; WCST: In the present study, we demonstrated that patients with TLE-HS
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; WCST cat: number of categories achieved; WCST PR: number have similar performance on an “explicit” decision-making task com-
of perseverative responses; WCST FS: number of failures to maintain set; CPT: Connors'
Continuous Performance Test; CPT om: number of errors of omission; CPT com: number
pared with healthy subjects with neither neurological nor psychiatric
of errors of commission. conditions. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to inves-
⁎ Statistical significance at p b 0.05. tigate delay gratification abilities in an etiological homogeneous group
of patients with TLE determined by HS. Despite the lack of hot EF impair-
ments, our patients showed the already described cool EF deficits. As a
without the normalization of DDT. Differences between groups secondary goal, we also demonstrated that cool EF performance is not
remained nonsignificant. related to delay gratification abilities, but some clinical epilepsy
variables may negatively impact the ability to postpone an immediate
reward considering a greater recompense in the future.
3.3. Performance in cool executive function tests Our work is in agreement with previous studies reporting that pa-
tients with TLE have preserved decision-making abilities under risk con-
Patients with TLE-HS showed worse performance on the Digit Back- ditions [18,19,21]. Different tests were used in these studies, and
ward (F(1,52) = 4.24; p = 0.043) and Trail Making Test (F(1,52) = although they were all designed to measure “explicit” decision-making,
8.58; p = 0.005), and made a higher number of omission (F(1,52) = they require different aspects of risk-taking behaviors. Delazer et al. [19]
6.25; p = 0.016) and commission errors (F(1,52) = 3.42; p = 0.006) and Bonatti et al. [21] applied the Probability-Associated Gambling Task
in the Connors' Continuous Performance Test. These findings show a (PAG). In this task, subjects have to choose between a safe small amount
more pronounced impairment in concentration, inhibitory control, of loss/win prize or to take the risk and gamble for a higher gain/loss.
task-switching, mental flexibility, and working memory. No other The chance of winning by gamble is available. Labudda et al. [18], on
significant differences were observed in cool executive function tests the other hand, used the Game of Dice Task (GDT). In this paradigm,
(Table 2). subjects must guess which number will be thrown before a die is rolled.
They can choose between different options associated with a predefined
gain/loss probability, and the rules of the game are explicit. Both tasks
3.4. Correlation between delay gratification scores and cool EF tests deliver a feedback after each trial, and the subject has the possibility
to adjust an ongoing behavior considering the amount of money that
No significant correlation was demonstrated between either the he gained/lost and the probability of a later negative reward.
discounting rate or the probability rate with the cool executive function The DDT is a relatively different task. It is a monetary delay gratifica-
tests in the group of patients with TLE-HS (Table 3). Similar results were tion task that measures individual's preferences between smaller-but-
obtained after replicating this analysis with nonnormalized DDT scores. immediate rewards versus larger-but-postponed or smaller-but-with a
greater probability of success rewards. Subjects are instructed to re-
spond quickly, and no feedback regarding the success of his/her re-
Table 3
Correlation between delay gratification and cool executive function tests in TLE-HS group. sponses is given. It provides an estimate for individual discount and
probability rates, reflecting how rapidly a reward loses subjective
Log (d) Log (h)
value as a function of how long a person must wait to receive it or the
r p r p chance of receiving it [37]. In real life, this ability is related to resisting
Digit Forward 0.08 0.701 0.09 0.675 temptation in favor of long-term goals, which is an important aspect
Digit Backward −0.06 0.768 0.07 0.757 of social competence and economic gain.
Stroop Test −0.11 0.596 −0.05 0.817 Impairment on delay gratification is usually related to impulsive-
TMT 0.14 0.505 −0.19 0.379
ness, and has been widely documented in patients with impulsive con-
WCST cat 0.08 0.704 0.05 0.834
WCST PR 0.29 0.180 0.03 0.880 trol impairment, such as patients with bipolar disorder [38], substance
WCST NPE −0.23 0.284 0.18 0.428 abuse [39], pathological gambling [40], and ADHD [41].
WCST FS 0.32 0.140 0.36 0.102 Differently than GDT and PAG, in DDT, feedback of responses and
CPT om −0.13 0.582 0.05 0.844
amount of money gained/lost are not provided, and subjects do not
CPT com −0.47 0.033 −0.27 0.232
have the opportunity to adjust their behavior accordingly. In the context
Legend: Log (d): discount rate; Log (h): probabilistic rate; TMT: Trail Making Test; WCST: of the dual-system theory of decision-making, it is reasonable to
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; WCST cat: number of categories achieved; WCST PR: number
of perseverative responses; WCST FS: number of failures to maintain set; CPT: Connors'
suppose that the emotional feedback route is more involved with DDT
Continuous Performance Test; CPT om: number of errors of omission; CPT com: number while the cognitive feedback route is more so used in GDT and PAG. Re-
of errors of commission. sponses in DDT tend to be automatic, effortless, fast, emotional, and
162 P. Rzezak et al. / Epilepsy & Behavior 60 (2016) 158–164

associative while in the other tasks, subjects use information related to had a worse performance on DDT. These results may point to the pres-
feedback (e.g., amount of money won or lost and what the new overall ence of clinical subgroups with distinct degrees of severity in all aspects,
capital is) in controlled cognitive processes such as rethinking strategies as already demonstrated in TLE and other syndromes [25,27,57]. The
and probabilities [13,42,43]. analysis of a larger sample is indeed necessary in order to establish pre-
Labudda et al. [18] suggested that patients with TLE show good per- dictors of worse performance on cognitive aspects as well as to establish
formance in decision-making under “explicit” conditions because they more suitable interventions for distinct groups.
can use information about the contingencies of the task to generate, The importance of mesial temporal lobe damage to decision-making
monitor, and modify advantageous decision-making strategies. We has been proposed in studies with patients with amnesia. Emerging
added to the current literature by demonstrating that patients with evidence suggests that damage to the mesial temporal lobes impairs
TLE showed similar performance when compared with healthy subjects performance on decision-making tasks under ambiguity (e.g., Iowa
not only on tasks that require mostly the reflective system, but also on Gambling Task) [8,9,58], suggesting that memory processes play a role
those tasks requiring the impulsive system of decision-making under in shaping decisions. Nevertheless, previous research established that
risk. hippocampal damage does not affect delay discounting performance,
Currently, epilepsy is seen as a connectivity disorder. In this context, as patients with amnesia with damage to mesial temporal lobes dis-
there is a consensus that neuropsychological studies need to address count the value of delayed rewards similarly to healthy subjects
more than memory and language impairments. Patients with TLE-HS [10,56]. Our findings of similar DDT performance in patients with TLE-
have cognitive deficits that are often related to other brain areas besides HS, with lesions to the mesial temporal structures, corroborate the
the temporal lobes [26–28,44]. Our study is in agreement with previous lack of an association between damage to hippocampus and
data [4,28,44–46] that demonstrated that adult patients with TLE-HS surrounding tissue and delay of gratification.
show pronounced impairment in some domains of EF represented by Some strengths of the present study must be emphasized. First, our
inhibition, shifting, mental flexibility, and working memory — cool EF. patient group was composed of patients with TLE with a well-defined
Brand et al. [15] suggest that cool EF are important for decision- etiology (restricted to mesial temporal lobe structures), with no current
making under risk because they guide the categorization, development, major psychiatric disorders, and inclusion of presurgical patients solely.
and application of strategies, and are necessary for the integration of Secondly, our experimental and control groups were closely matched by
feedback. Indeed, several studies demonstrated that patients with gender and age. There is evidence that gender may influence decision-
executive dysfunction (measured with similar tests as the ones used making abilities in individuals with no current or past neurological or
in the present study — WCST, Tower of London/Hanoi, Trail Making psychiatric conditions. Although this finding is generally observed
Test, Color Word Interference Test) choose more high-risk options when subjects are under ambiguous risk conditions [59,60], the rela-
with negative consequences in the GDT, the PAG task, and in the Cam- tionship of gender and DDT performance has been less studied. Thus,
bridge Gambling Task [47–50]. The only study that specifically evaluat- we decided to be conservative in control group selection and minimize
ed the relationship of cool and hot EF in TLE demonstrated a correlation the chance of gender differences jeopardizing data interpretation.
between cool EF, such as set-shifting, cognitive flexibility, and categori- Besides, age has a well-studied impact in risk-taking abilities. It is sup-
zation with GDT [15]. According to these authors, an impaired mental posed to be low in childhood, increase during adolescence and young
flexibility and inhibition underlies a worse decision-making behavior adulthood, and decrease again in adults [12]. As our patient sample
[51]. was composed of subjects with different age ranges, subtle differences
In our study, cool EF impairments were not related to hot executive in DDT performance could be related to the above-mentioned develop-
performance. One possible explanation for our different results relies mental trajectory of decision-making; thus, matching patients with
on the dual-process theory of decision-making. Based on this, some controls by age range was implemented.
studies demonstrated that deliberative decision-making usually relies Some limitations of this study should also be considered. Firstly, dif-
on cool EF whereas decision-making tasks related to affective processes ferences in intellectual functioning between patients and controls were
have been shown to be relatively independent of cool EF [52]. not included in the data analysis. Some studies demonstrated the im-
Delay gratification is related to the ability to postpone an immediate pact of intelligence on decision-making abilities in people without neu-
reward in favor of long-term goals. This ability is usually related to rological or psychiatric disorders [61]. In our analysis, we had to choose
cognitive control processes [53]. A key component of cognitive control between the covariation for IQ scores or years of education, as these two
is the ability to suppress or override competing attentional and behav- factors are highly intercorrelated. As lower IQ scores are an underlying
ioral responses (inhibitory control) [54,55]. In this scenario, someone feature of epilepsy [62,63], but lower educational attainment is not,
could expect a positive correlation between DDT scores, Stroop Color we chose to correct for the latter. The work of Davis et al. [64], demon-
Test, and Conners' CPT commission errors. On the other hand, a number strated a highly significant effect of education on the IGT. As the level of
of strategies, including making choices based on intuition (i.e., a “gut education increased, performance to improve more rapidly and reached
feeling”) or consideration of economic factors (i.e., interest, inflation) a higher eventual positive score. Therefore, education should be used as
were reported by subjects evaluated with a delay discounting paradigm a stratification or matching variable in case–control research, using de-
[56]. It is reasonable to believe that these strategies rely on different cision-making tests, as stated by the authors. An exploratory analysis
cognitive processes, with the second being more related to cool EF. In without correcting for education attainment did not show different re-
an “intuition–strategy” scenario, vigilance and concentration may be sults in DDT performance, but showed differences in the cool EF com-
more required than cool EF, which could explain our findings. Neverthe- parison between groups. Future studies that investigate the impact of
less, CPT omission errors were not related to DDT scores, which speak intelligence on delay gratification abilities in patients with TLE-HS
against the possible explanation that vigilance impairments could influ- with different IQ ranges should help to further explore this relationship.
ence DDT scores in patients with TLE-HS. In order to corroborate this as- Another aspect that could not be explored in this study is reaction time
sumption, future studies should investigate the strategy used by differences between groups. Faster response times can be an indication
patients with TLE-HS to solve DDT. of a more automatic response. Due to time constraints and considering
It is also relevant to discuss the impact of some clinical aspects of that patients usually have a slower reaction time, all pairs of decisions
epilepsy severity and worse performance on delay gratification. were read aloud by the examiner, and patients' responses were marked
Although, we evaluated an etiologically homogeneous group of patients by her. Studies using a shorter version of DDT, with less pairs of choices,
with TLE-HS, there were within-group differences considering seizure considering reaction times are warranted to further explore this possi-
control and use of AEDs. Patients with early onset epilepsy and worse ble strategy that might be used by patients with TLE. Finally, although
seizure severity (seizure frequency and occurrence of GTC seizures) we examined a homogeneous group of patients with very restrictive
P. Rzezak et al. / Epilepsy & Behavior 60 (2016) 158–164 163

inclusion and exclusion criteria, we analyzed a small sample size. One J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 2009;35:709–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0014983.
may pose the question about the impact of some AEDs on impulsive- [13] Schiebener J, Brand M. Decision making under objective risk conditions-a review of
ness. Indeed, this may represent another limitation. However, merely cognitive and emotional correlates, strategies, feedback processing, and external in-
three of our patients were using topiramate, an AED that can decrease fluences. Neuropsychol Rev 2015;25:171–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11065-
015-9285-x.
impulsive behavior and therefore potentially have an impact on the [14] Bechara A. Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a
finding of a lack of an association between TLE-HS and “explicit” neurocognitive perspective. Nat Neurosci 2005;8:1458–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.
decision-making impairment [65]. 1038/nn1584.
[15] Brand M, Labudda K, Markowitsch HJ. Neuropsychological correlates of decision-
Decision-making ability, a cognitive process that allows the individ- making in ambiguous and risky situations. Neural Netw 2006;19:1266–76. http://
ual to compare values between the immediate and delayed consump- dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2006.03.001.
tion of a determined commodity [56], was not impaired in patients [16] Tschernegg M, Pletzer B, Schwartenbeck P, Ludersdorfer P, Hoffmann U, Kronbichler
M. Impulsivity relates to striatal gray matter volumes in humans: evidence from a
with TLE-HS. These findings allow us to make some assumptions re-
delay discounting paradigm. Front Hum Neurosci 2015;9:384. http://dx.doi.org/10.
garding risk-taking behaviors of these patients. Despite other cognitive 3389/fnhum.2015.00384.
deficits, these patients are probably able to postpone an immediate [17] Sohn SY, Kang JI, Namkoong K, Kim SJ. Multidimensional measures of impulsivity in
reward in view of a greater reward in the future. In conclusion, our find- obsessive–compulsive disorder: cannot wait and stop. PLoS One 2014;9, e111739.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0111739.
ings corroborate previous data showing that patients with TLE-HS pres- [18] Labudda K, Frigge K, Horstmann S, Aengenendt J, Woermann FG, Ebner A, et al.
ent impairments on mental flexibility, inhibition, and complex attention Decision making in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychologia 2009;
subdomains, but have decision-making behavior under explicit 47:50–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.08.014.
[19] Delazer M, Zamarian L, Bonatti E, Kuchukhidze G, Koppelstätter F, Bodner T, et al.
situation spared, despite the presence of refractory epilepsy. Decision making under ambiguity and under risk in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy.
Neuropsychologia 2010;48:194–200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.
2009.08.025.
Acknowledgments [20] Delazer M, Zamarian L, Bonatti E, Walser N, Kuchukhidze G, Bodner T, et al. Decision
making under ambiguity in temporal lobe epilepsy: does the location of the under-
lying structural abnormality matter? Epilepsy Behav 2011;20:34–7. http://dx.doi.
Dr. Vincentiis received grants from Foundation for Research Support org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2010.11.006.
of the State of São Paulo (FAPESP 13/11361-4). Dr. Rzezak received a [21] Bonatti E, Kuchukhidze G, Zamarian L, Trinka E, Bodner T, Benke T, et al. Decision
grant from Foundation for Research Support of the State of São Paulo making in ambiguous and risky situations after unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy
surgery. Epilepsy Behav 2009;14:665–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2009.
(FAPESP 12/09025-3; 12/13065-0). Dr. Valente received grants from
02.015.
the Brazilian Research and Development Council (CNPQ 307262/ [22] First MB, Spitzer RL, Miriam G, Williams JBW. Structured clinical interview for DSM-
2011-1) and Foundation for Research Support of the State of São IV-TR Axis I disorders, research version, non-patient edition. New York, New York,
Paulo (FAPESP 13/11361-4; 12/09025-3; FAPESP 12/09025-3). Dr. USA: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 2002.
[23] Rachlin H, Raineri A, Cross D. Subjective probability and delay. J Exp Anal Behav
Busatto received grants from the Foundation for Research Support of 1991;55:233–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1991.55-233.
the State of São Paulo (FAPESP 12/09025-3 AND 2012/50329-6). Dr. [24] Petry NM, Casarella T. Excessive discounting of delayed rewards in substance
Leite received grants from the Brazilian Research and Development abusers with gambling problems. Drug Alcohol Depend 1999;56:25–32. http://dx.
doi.org/10.1016/S0376-8716(99)00010-1.
Council (CNPq 476250/2013-7 and 466995/2014). [25] Hermann B, Seidenberg M, Bell B, Rutecki P, Sheth R, Ruggles K, et al. The
neurodevelopmental impact of childhood-onset temporal lobe epilepsy on brain
Conflict of interest structure and function. Epilepsia 2002;43:1062–71.
[26] Oyegbile TO, Dow C, Jones J, Bell B, Rutecki P, Sheth R, et al. The nature and course of
neuropsychological morbidity in chronic temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology 2004;
The authors declare no conflicts of interest. 62:1736–42.
[27] Rzezak P, Fuentes D, Guimarães CA, Thome-Souza S, Kuczynski E, Li LM, et al. Frontal
lobe dysfunction in children with temporal lobe epilepsy. Pediatr Neurol 2007;37:
References 176–85.
[28] Igarashi K, Oguni H, Osawa M, Awaya Y, Kato M. Wisconsin card sorting test in chil-
[1] Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity dren with temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain Dev 2002;24:174–8.
and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal [29] Hermann BP, Seidenberg M, Bell B. The neurodevelopmental impact of childhood
Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol 2000;41:49–100. onset temporal lobe epilepsy on brain structure and function and the risk of progres-
[2] Hermann B, Seidenberg M, Lee E-J, Chan F, Rutecki P. Cognitive phenotypes in sive cognitive effects. Prog Brain Res 2002;135:429–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
temporal lobe epilepsy. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2007;13:12–20. S0079-6123(02)35040-4.
[3] Kim C-H, Lee S-A, Yoo H-J, Kang J-K, Lee J-K. Executive performance on the Wisconsin [30] Wechsler D. WAIS-III: Wechsler adult intelligence scale. San Antonio: Psychological
Card Sorting Test in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Eur Neurol 2007;57:39–46. Corporation; 1997.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000097009. [31] Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol 1935;18:
[4] Rzezak P, Fuentes D, Guimaraes CA, Thome-Souza S, Kuczynski E, Guerreiro M, et al. 643–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0054651.
Executive dysfunction in children and adolescents with temporal lobe epilepsy: is [32] Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Bigler ED, Tranel D. Neuropsychological assessment. New
the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test enough? Epilepsy Behav 2009;15:376–81. http:// York: Oxford University Press; 2012.
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2009.04.014. [33] Strauss E, Sherman E, Spreen O. A compendium of neuropsychological tests:
[5] Kerr A, Zelazo PD. Development of “hot” executive function: the children's gambling administration, norms, and commentary. New York: Oxford University Press; 2006.
task. Brain Cogn 2004;55:148–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00275-6. [34] Heaton R. The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test manual. Odessa: Psychological Assess-
[6] Bechara A, Van Der Linden M. Decision-making and impulse control after frontal ment Resources Inc.; 1981.
lobe injuries. Curr Opin Neurol 2005;18(6):734–9. [35] Conners CK, Staff MHS, Connelly V, Campbell S, MacLean M, Barnes J. Conners'
[7] Damasio AR, Tranel D, Damasio H. Somatic markers and the guidance of behavior: Continuous Performance Test II (CPT II V. 5). Multi-Health Syst Inc 2000;29:
theory and preliminary testing BT — frontal lobe function and dysfunction. Front 175–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2901_9.
Lobe Funct Dysfunct, 1991, p. 217–35. [36] Cutler DM, Lleras-Muney A. Understanding differences in health behaviors by educa-
[8] Gutbrod K, Krouzel C, Hofer H, Müri R, Perrig W, Ptak R. Decision-making in amne- tion. J Health Econ 2010;29:1–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jhealeco.2009.10.003.
sia: do advantageous decisions require conscious knowledge of previous behaviour- [37] Mischel W, Ayduk O, Berman MG, Casey BJ, Gotlib IH, Jonides J, et al. “Willpower”
al choices? Neuropsychologia 2006;44:1315–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. over the life span: decomposing self-regulation. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2011;6:
neuropsychologia.2006.01.014. 252–6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq081.
[9] Gupta R, Duff MC, Denburg NL, Cohen NJ, Bechara A, Tranel D. Declarative memory is [38] Strakowski SM, Fleck DE, DelBello MP, Adler CM, Shear PK, Kotwal R, et al. Impulsiv-
critical for sustained advantageous complex decision-making. Neuropsychologia ity across the course of bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord 2010;12:285–97. http://dx.
2009;47:1686–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.007. doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618.2010.00806.x.
[10] Kwan D, Craver CF, Green L, Myerson J, Gao F, Black SE, et al. Cueing the personal [39] Hulka LM, Vonmoos M, Preller KH, Baumgartner MR, Seifritz E, Gamma A, et al.
future to reduce discounting in intertemporal choice : is episodic prospection Changes in cocaine consumption are associated with fluctuations in self-reported
necessary? Hippocampus 2015;25(4):432–43. impulsivity and gambling decision-making. Psychol Med 2015;1–14. http://dx.doi.
[11] Epstein S, Pacini R, Denes-Raj V, Heier H. Individual differences in intuitive- org/10.1017/S0033291715001063.
experiential and analytical-rational thinking styles. J Pers Soc Psychol 1996;71: [40] Kräplin A, Dshemuchadse M, Behrendt S, Scherbaum S, Goschke T, Bühringer G.
390–405. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.390. Dysfunctional decision-making in pathological gambling: pattern specificity and
[12] Figner B, Mackinlay RJ, Wilkening F, Weber EU. Affective and deliberative pro- the role of impulsivity. Psychiatry Res 2014;215:675–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.
cesses in risky choice: age differences in risk taking in the Columbia Card Task. 1016/j.psychres.2013.12.041.
164 P. Rzezak et al. / Epilepsy & Behavior 60 (2016) 158–164

[41] Crunelle CL, Veltman DJ, Van Emmerik-van Oortmerssen K, Booij J, Van den Brink W. [54] Casey BJ, Thomas KM, Welsh TF, Badgaiyan RD, Eccard CH, Jennings JR, et al.
Impulsivity in adult ADHD patients with and without cocaine dependence. Drug Dissociation of response conflict, attentional selection, and expectancy with
Alcohol Depend 2013;129:18–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.09. functional magnetic resonance imaging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2000;97:
006. 8728–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.97.15.8728.
[42] Evans JSBT. Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. [55] Jonides J, Marshuetz C, Smith EE, Reuter-Lorenz PA, Koeppe RA, Hartley A. Age differ-
Annu Rev Psychol 2008;59:255–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59. ences in behavior and PET activation reveal differences in interference resolution in
103006.093629. verbal working memory. J Cogn Neurosci 2000;12:188–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.
[43] Denes-Raj V, Epstein S. Conflict between intuitive and rational processing: when 1162/089892900561823.
people behave against their better judgment. J Pers Soc Psychol 1994;66:819–29. [56] Palombo DJ, Keane MM, Verfaellie M. The medial temporal lobes are critical for
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.66.5.819. reward-based decision making under conditions that promote episodic future
[44] Hermann B, Seidenberg M. Executive system dysfunction in temporal lobe epilepsy: thinking. Hippocampus 2014;8:1–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hipo.22376.
effects of nociferous cortex versus hippocampal pathology. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol [57] Valente KD, Rzezak P, Moschetta SP, de Vincentiis S, Coan AC, Guerreiro CA. Delin-
1995;17:809–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01688639508402430. eating behavioral and cognitive phenotypes in juvenile myoclonic epilepsy: Are
[45] Corcoran R, Upton D. A role for the hippocampus in card sorting? Cortex 1993;29: we missing the forest for the trees? Epilepsy Behav. Jan 2016;54:95–9.
293–304. [58] Yee LTS, Warren DE, Voss JL, Duff MC, Tranel D, Cohen NJ. The hippocampus uses in-
[46] Rzezak P, Fuentes D, Guimarães CA, Thome-Souza S, Kuczynski E, LM L, et al. Frontal formation just encountered to guide efficient ongoing behavior. Hippocampus 2014;
lobe dysfunction in children with temporal lobe epilepsy. Pediatr Neurol 2007;37: 24:154–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hipo.22211.
176–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2007.05.009. [59] Bolla KI, Eldreth DA, Matochik JA, Cadet JL. Sex-related differences in a gambling task
[47] Delazer M, Sinz H, Zamarian L, Benke T. Decision-making with explicit and stable and its neurological correlates. Cereb Cortex 2004;14:1226–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.
rules in mild Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychologia 2007;45:1632–41. http://dx. 1093/cercor/bhh083.
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.01.006. [60] Lighthall NR, Sakaki M, Vasunilashorn S, Nga L, Somayajula S, Chen EY, et al. Gender
[48] Delazer M, Sinz H, Zamarian L, Stockner H, Seppi K, Wenning GK, et al. Decision differences in reward-related decision processing under stress. Soc Cogn Affect
making under risk and under ambiguity in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia Neurosci 2012;7:476–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr026.
2009;47:1901–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.034. [61] Donati MA, Panno A, Chiesi F, Primi C. A mediation model to explain decision making
[49] Drechsler R, Rizzo P, Steinhausen H-C. Decision-making on an explicit risk-taking under conditions of risk among adolescents: the role of fluid intelligence and prob-
task in preadolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Neural Transm abilistic reasoning. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2014;36(6):588–95. http://dx.doi.org/
2008;115:201–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00702-007-0814-5. 10.1080/13803395.2014.918091.
[50] Grant JE, Derbyshire K, Leppink E, Chamberlain SR. Suicidality in non-treatment [62] Hunter RM, Reilly C, Atkinson P, Das KB, Gillberg C, Chin RF, et al. The health,
seeking young adults with subsyndromal gambling disorder. Psychiatry Q 2014; education, and social care costs of school-aged children with active epilepsy: a
85(4):513–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11126-014-9312-8. population-based study. Epilepsia 2015;56:1056–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/epi.
[51] Brand M, Pawlikowski M, Labudda K, Laier C, Von Rothkirch N, Markowitsch HJ. 13015.
Do amnesic patients with Korsakoff's syndrome use feedback when making deci- [63] Park J, Yum MS, Choi HW, Kim EH, Kim HW, Ko TS. Determinants of intelligence in
sions under risky conditions? An experimental investigation with the Game of childhood-onset epilepsy: a single-center study. Epilepsy Behav 2013;29:166–71.
Dice Task with and without feedback. Brain Cogn 2009;69:279–90. http://dx.doi. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2013.07.010.
org/10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.003. [64] Davis C, Fox J, Patte K, Curtis C, Strimas R, Reid C, et al. Education level moderates
[52] Turnbull OH, Evans CEY, Bunce A, Carzolio B, O'connor J. Emotion-based learning and learning on two versions of the Iowa Gambling Task. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2008;
central executive resources: an investigation of intuition and the Iowa Gambling 14:1063–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617708081204.
Task. Brain Cogn 2005;57:244–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2004.08.053. [65] Fontenelle LF, Oostermeijer S, Harrison BJ, Pantelis C, Yücel M. Obsessive–
[53] Mitchell SH. The genetic basis of delay discounting and its genetic relationship to al- compulsive disorder, impulse control disorders and drug addiction: common fea-
cohol dependence. Behav Processes 2011;87:10–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. tures and potential treatments. Drugs 2011;71:827–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.2165/
beproc.2011.02.008. 11591790-000000000-00000.

You might also like