Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research,

Practice, and Policy


Understanding the Relationship Between Social Support
and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder/Posttraumatic Growth
Among Adolescents After Ya’an Earthquake: The Role of
Emotion Regulation
Xiao Zhou, Xinchun Wu, and Rui Zhen
Online First Publication, October 13, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/tra0000213

CITATION
Zhou, X., Wu, X., & Zhen, R. (2016, October 13). Understanding the Relationship Between Social
Support and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder/Posttraumatic Growth Among Adolescents After
Ya’an Earthquake: The Role of Emotion Regulation. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research,
Practice, and Policy. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/tra0000213
Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy © 2016 American Psychological Association
2016, Vol. 8, No. 6, 000 1942-9681/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/tra0000213

Understanding the Relationship Between Social Support and Posttraumatic


Stress Disorder/Posttraumatic Growth Among Adolescents After Ya’an
Earthquake: The Role of Emotion Regulation

Xiao Zhou Xinchun Wu and Rui Zhen


Beijing Normal University and Tel Aviv University Beijing Normal University

Objective: Posttraumatic distress disorder (PTSD) and posttraumatic growth (PTG) may coexist in
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

trauma survivors, but there are mixed relationships between PTSD and PTG. To elucidate their
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

relationship and constructs, it is necessary to examine simultaneously predictive factors, and to compare
their determining factors. The aim of this study was to increasing our understanding the relationship
between PTSD and PTG by examining simultaneously the role of social support and emotion regulation
in PTSD and PTG among adolescents after the earthquake. Methods: Six months after the Ya’an
earthquake, 315 middle school students in Lushan county were assessed using measures of trauma
exposure, social support, and emotion regulation, as well as Child PTSD Symptom Scale and Posttrau-
matic Growth Inventory. Results: Social support had significant direct association with PTG but not with
PTSD, but social support had a negative indirect prediction on PTSD and a positive indirect prediction
on PTG through cognitive reappraisal. Social support, through expressive suppression, had a significant
and indirect prediction on PTSD, but a nonsignificant indirect prediction on PTG. Conclusions: This
study indicate that the predictive mechanism of PTSD and PTG were different and further suggest that
PTSD and PTG are separate, independent dimensions of psychological experiences following adversity.

Keywords: social support, cognitive reappraisal, expressive suppression, PTSD, PTG

It is documented that both posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is, some studies found a positive relation between the outcomes
and posttraumatic growth (PTG) are common and representative (e.g., Wu, Xu, & Sui, 2016), meaning that individual with in-
posttraumatic reactions (e.g., García Martinez, Reyes Reyes, & creased PTSD reported increase PTG. Other studies indicated a
Cova Solar, 2014; Pan et al., 2015; Ying, Wu, Lin, & Chen, 2013). negative relationship between PTSD and PTG (e.g., Johnson et al.,
The former can be considered a pathological outcome of trauma 2007), which suggested that PTG and PTSD can be conceptualized
that involves intrusive symptoms, avoidance, and hyper-arousal as two ends of the same continuum. Even some studies fail to find
(e.g., Foa, Johnson, Feeny, & Treadwell, 2001), and the latter is a significant relation between these two posttraumatic reactions
conceptualized as positive changes following trauma, including (e.g., Salsman, Segerstrom, Brechting, Carlson, & Andrykowski,
perceived changes in the self, sense of relationship with others, and 2009), suggesting PTSD and PTG may be two independent con-
life philosophy (Tedeschi & Calhoun, 1996). Importantly, PTSD structs. These inconsistent findings disturbed the understanding of
and PTG may coexist in trauma survivors (e.g., Tedeschi & Cal- constructs of PTSD and PTG. To elucidate their relationship and
houn, 1996), which shed light on the discussion the relationship constructs, Dekel, Mandl, and Solomon (2011) suggested that it is
between PTSD and PTG (e.g., Zhou, Wu, & Chen, 2015a). necessary to examine simultaneously predictive factors and to
Nevertheless, the relationship between PTSD and PTG remains compare their determining factors (e.g., García, Cova, Rincón, &
recently mixed (e.g., Shakespeare-Finch & Lurie-Beck, 2014); that Vázquez, 2015). The aim of this study was to examine simultane-
ously and compare possible factors and predictive mechanism of
PTSD and PTG among adolescents after the earthquake.
Social support is considered to be one of the most important
Xiao Zhou, Beijing Key Laboratory of Applied Experimental Psychol- predictors of posttraumatic reactions (e.g., Ozer, Best, Lipsey, &
ogy, School of Psychology, Beijing Normal University and I-Core Re- Weiss, 2003). Several theories have been proposed to explain the
search Center for Mass Trauma, Bob Shapell School of Social Work, Tel role of social support in posttraumatic outcomes. One representa-
Aviv University; Xinchun Wu and Rui Zhen, Beijing Key Laboratory of tive theory is the additive and direct model of social support, which
Applied Experimental Psychology, School of Psychology, Beijing Normal suggests that social support plays an independent role in alleviat-
University. ing stress reactions regardless of the effects of stressful events
This study was supported by the Major Project of Beijing Social Science
(Cohen & Syme, 1985). For example, social support can directly
Fund, China (grant: 15ZDA11). We sincerely appreciate the students and
teachers who voluntarily devoted time and interest to our research. influence adjustment through changes in health-promoting behav-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xinchun ior. Based on this model, studies indicated that social support is a
Wu, School of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, No. 19 Xinjiekou- protective factor for PTSD but promote PTG (e.g., Jia, Ying, Zhou,
wai Street, Beijing 100875, China. E-mail: xcwu@bnu.edu.cn Wu, & Lin, 2015; Yu et al., 2014). Nevertheless, the mechanism

1
2 ZHOU, WU, AND ZHEN

underlying the association between social support and PTSD/PTG Gross and John (2003) gave several potential explanations for this:
in the same study still was not evaluated. Thus, this study will First, suppression can limit exposure to negative emotions, and
further examine simultaneously the underlying mechanism of so- these emotions may thus continue to linger and accumulate unre-
cial support to PTSD and PTG. solved. Second, using expressive suppression to regulate emotions
An assumption of the relationship between social support and may consume too many cognitive resources, which will easily
posttraumatic reactions argues that social support exerts a positive cause burnout and result in further negative feelings. Third, sup-
effect in presence of stress by protecting people from the negative pression creates in the individual a sense of incongruence, or
effects of such stress (Cohen & Syme, 1985).For example, social discrepancy, between inner experience and outer expression (e.g.,
support can help people to redefine and reduce the potential for Rogers, 1951), which may well lead to negative feelings about the
harm posed by negative situations, or can bolster the ability to cope self. Therefore, it is proposed that expressive suppression may be
with imposed demands, thereby changing negative appraisals of a risk factor for PTSD while discouraging PTG.
stress. In this assumption, social support may have an effect on Although several theoretical predictions have been advanced to
stressful reactions by influencing others’ psychological processes describe the relationship between emotion regulation and negative
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(e.g., Schwarzer & Knoll, 2007). Emotion regulation is important feelings and/or well-being, the predictive utility of these theories
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

among potential psychological processes underlying both PTSD focused on the relationship between emotion regulation and PTSD/
and PTG. PTG has not been evaluated. In addition, prior studies investigat-
Emotion regulation is conceptualized as processes through ing various emotion regulation strategies failed to control for the
which individuals modulate their emotions consciously and non- use of other strategies, which may exaggerate the probability of
consciously to appropriately respond to environmental demands. making a Type I error because of co-occurrence of distinct emo-
Social support can satisfy people’s relationship needs and help tional strategies. More importantly, the transition through adoles-
people access more social resources, and thus social support can cence is accompanied by physical, psychological, and social trans-
exert a significant effect on emotion regulation strategies (e.g., formations that elicit novel experiences of emotional arousal, and
Marroquín & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2015). perhaps greater susceptibility to traumatic events, and thus, ado-
Specifically, social support can provide traumatized individuals lescence is an opportune time to explore processes and correlates
with a supportive environment (e.g., Carpenter, Fowler, Maxwell, of emotion regulation (e.g., Silk, Steinberg, & Morris, 2003).
& Andersen, 2010), wherein individuals can experience the feel- Given this backdrop, the present study examined how social
ings of safety and belongings (e.g., Zhou et al., 2014a), and in turn support and emotion regulation might be associated with both
encourage and activate traumatized individuals to disclose their PTSD and PTG in adolescent survivors of the Ya’an earthquake
information, and share their emotions (e.g., Pietrzak, Johnson, and propose that emotion regulation (i.e., cognitive reappraisal and
Goldstein, Malley, & Southwick, 2009). In addition, social support expressive suppression) would mediate the relationship between
can improve interpersonal relationship and increase interpersonal social support and both PTSD and PTG.
intimacy (e.g., Marroquín, 2011), which will also increase the
likelihood of emotion sharing and cognitive disclosure (e.g., Rimé, Method
2009). Thus, some studies found that social support had a positive
effect on cognitive reappraisal (e.g., Valentiner, Holahan, & Moos, Participants and Procedures
1994), and a negative effect on expressive suppression (e.g., Sriv-
astava, Tamir, McGonigal, John, & Gross, 2009). Considering that This study was conducted 6 months after the Ya’an earthquake
cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression are two different occurred on April 20, 2013. The area we focus on was Lushan
emotional strategies in the process model of emotion regulation county in Sichuan province, which was most severely affected by
(Gross, 1998a), it is likely that social support can exert effects on the earthquake. We first contacted the local education authority
emotion regulation. and informed them of the aims and methods of investigation, and
It is noteworthy that cognitive reappraisal, more specifically, is indicated that we could provide psychological services if required.
a form of cognitive change that construes a potentially emotion- With the approval of the local education authority, we chose two
eliciting situation in a way that changes its emotional impact (e.g., middle schools in Lushan county. Under the help of the principals
Lazarus & Alfert, 1964), one that involves making active efforts to and psychology teachers of the two schools, we randomly selected
repair bad moods as well as generating benign or positive inter- three classes from Grade 1, and also three from Grade 2, respec-
pretations or perspectives on a stressful situation (e.g., Gross, tively, to participate (students in Grade 3 were not available due to
1998b). These strategies can be helpful for traumatic survivors in arduous academic burden). All students in selected classrooms
rebuilding their understanding of the world after trauma, reducing attended school on the assessment date. Three hundred fifteen
distress, and fostering beneficial psychological changes post- adolescent survivors were selected. The mean age of the adoles-
trauma. Thus, studies suggest that cognitive reappraisal protects cents at the time of measurement was 14.62 (SD ⫽ 1.80) years, and
people from emotional disorders, decreases the severity of PTSD the range was 12.0 to 18.0 years. Of the 315 participants, 167
(e.g., Boden, Bonn-Miller, Kashdan, Alvarez, & Gross, 2012), and (53.0%) were female and 148 (47.0%) were male. Proportions of
promotes PTG (e.g., Brans, Koval, Verduyn, Lim, & Kuppens, missing data for six participants reached about 50% for all the
2013). items of main measures in this study, such that these participants
In contrast, expressive suppression primarily modifies the be- were excluded from further analysis. Participants were thus 309 in
havioral aspects of emotion response tendency (e.g., Brans et al., this study.
2013), that is, the suppression of emotional expression, a risk This study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of
factor for negative psychological reactions (e.g., Gross, 2002). Beijing Normal University and was conducted according to the prin-
SOCIAL SUPPORT, EMOTION REGULATION, PTSD/PTG 3

cipals of the participating schools. In the selected classes, everyone to 7 (completely agree). The questionnaire has previously exhib-
who attended school on the date of the survey was recruited to ited good reliability and validity (Wang et al., 2007). In this study,
participate. There were no exclusion criteria. Compensation was not internal reliability of the modified inventory was good (␣ ⫽ .77),
provided. The purpose of the study and the voluntary nature of the and the fit indices from a confirmatory factor analysis based on
students’ participation were highlighted before the survey, and written two dimensions were excellent (␹2/df ⫽ 2.06, CFI ⫽ 0.95, TLI ⫽
informed consent was obtained from school principals and classroom 0.94, RMSEA ⫽ 0.040).
teachers. In China, research projects that are approved by local edu- PTG. PTG was measured using a modified version of the
cation authorities and the school administrators and that are deemed to Posttraumatic Growth Inventory (Zhou et al., 2014a). The original
provide a service to the students do not require parental consent. The Posttraumatic Growth Inventory was developed by Tedeschi and
researchers administered the questionnaire packets in a classroom Calhoun (1996) and consists of the following five subscales:
setting without the presence of the teachers. The participants were personal strength, new possibilities, relating to others, appreciation
initially asked to provide demographic information, including gender of life, and spiritual change. Each of the 21 items is scored on a
and age. They were then asked to complete the remaining measures 6-point scale that ranges from 0 (no change) to 5 (very great
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

assessed traumatic exposure, social support, cognitive emotional reg- degree of change). The PTGI has good internal consistency and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ulation, PTG, and PTSD. After the questionnaire packets were com- good construct, convergent, and discriminate validities. The re-
pleted, the participants were told that school psychologists or teachers vised PTGI includes the following three subscales for a total of 22
were available to provide any psychological or counseling services items: Perceived changes in self, a changed sense of relationships
that they might need. with others, and a changed philosophy of life. The modified
inventory exhibited good reliability and construct validity for a
Measures sample of adolescent survivors of the Wenchuan earthquake (Zhou
et al., 2014a, 2015a). The score range of this questionnaire was
Trauma exposure. The trauma exposure questionnaire devel- 0 –110. In this study, the internal reliability of the modified inven-
oped by Wu, Zhang, Lin, and Zang (2013) was adopted to measure tory was good (␣ ⫽ .93), and the fit indices from a confirmatory
the severity of adolescent survivors’ traumatic experiences. This factor analysis based on three subscales were acceptable (␹2/df ⫽
questionnaire consists of 18 items and requires participants to 2.36, CFI ⫽ 0.91, TLI ⫽ 0.90, RMSEA ⫽ 0.067).
indicate whether they have directly seen or indirectly heard about PTSD. PTSD symptom levels were assessed using the Child
the death, injury or entrapment of parents, friends, teachers or PTSD Symptom Scale (Foa et al., 2001) as applied to the Chinese
others. Each of the items is rated on a 3-point scale ranging from population (Ying et al., 2013). This measure is a 17-item self-
1 (did not experience the situations above) to 3 (see myself). The report scale that was designed to assess the occurrence and fre-
score range of this questionnaire was 18 –54. The questionnaire has quency of DSM–IV–TR PTSD symptoms in relation to the most
previously exhibited good reliability in adolescent survivors of the distressing event experienced by an individual. In the present
Wenchuan earthquake (e.g., Wu et al., 2013). In this study, the
study, all of the items were translated into Chinese, and the
internal reliability of the questionnaire was again good (␣ ⫽ .72).
respondents rated the frequencies of the symptoms during the
Social support. This questionnaire is a 20-item instrument
previous 2 weeks on 4-point Likert scales that ranged from 0 (not
designed to measure the following forms of perceived social
at all/only once) to 3 (almost always or more times a week). The
support (Zou, 1999): Emotive support (i.e., Helping me to resolve
subscale scores range from 0 to 15 for intrusion symptoms, 0 to 21
troubles), instrumental support (i.e., Giving me some suggestion
for avoidance symptoms and 0 to 15 for hyper-arousal symptoms.
on solving problems), companion (i.e., Accompanying me to do
An overall severity score is generated by summing the scores for
something interesting), affirmative evaluation (i.e., Respecting my
the three symptom types. This scale exhibited good reliability and
ideas), and intimacy (i.e., I always tell my secrets to this person).
construct validity for a sample of adolescent survivors of the
All the items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale that ranges from
Wenchuan earthquake (e.g., Ying et al., 2013). The score range of
0 (completely disagree) to 4 (completely agree). The score range of
this scale was 0 –51. In this sample, the scale demonstrated good
this questionnaire was 0 – 80. The questionnaire has previously
internal consistency (␣ ⫽ .87), and the fit indices from a confir-
exhibited good reliability and validity in adolescent survivors of
matory factor analysis based on three subscales were acceptable
the Wenchuan earthquake (e.g., Zhou et al., 2014a). In this study,
(␹2/df ⫽ 2.26, CFI ⫽ 0.90, TLI ⫽ 0.89, RMSEA ⫽ 0.064).
the internal reliability of the modified inventory was good (␣ ⫽
.94), and the fit indices from a confirmatory factor analysis based
on five dimensions were acceptable (␹2/df ⫽ 2.61, comparative fit Data Analysis Strategies
index [CFI] ⫽ 0.93, Tucker-Lewis index [TLI] ⫽ 0.91, root mean
square error of approximation [RMSEA] ⫽ 0.073). Descriptive analyses were conducted for all of the measures
Emotion regulation strategies. Emotion regulation strategies administered. Pearson correlations were calculated to examine the
were assessed using this questionnaire (Chinese revised version; associations between the major variables.
Wang, Liu, Li, & Du, 2007). This questionnaire was consisted of The statistical analyses were conducted using Mplus 6.0 soft-
10 items, wherein six items were designed to measure cognitive ware (Muthén & Muthén, 2010). We first conducted an analysis of
reappraisal (score range was 6 – 42; i.e., When confronting with missing data in variables and found that missing data across all
negative emotions, I can change the way I think) and four were items were less than 1.9%. To assess whether the missing data
designed to measure expressive suppression (score range was pattern was random, we conducted analyses using Little’s missing
4 –28; i.e., I did not disclose my emotions). All the items are rated completely at random test. The analysis revealed that the data were
on a 7-point Likert scale that ranges from 1 (completely disagree) missing at random), ␹2(42) ⫽ 47.17, p ⫽ .269. We therefore
4 ZHOU, WU, AND ZHEN

adopted the robust maximum likelihood estimator (MLR) method survivor (Tedeschi & Calhoun, 1996). We also inserted emotion
are less biased and more reliable. regulation (i.e., cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression)
In addition, we examine the distribution of scale scores and between social support and PTSD/PTG and added one related path
found that skewness and kurtosis are ⫺0.18 and 0.13 for expres- between cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression to establish
sive suppression, ⫺0.31 and 0.45 for cognitive reappraisal, ⫺0.37 an indirect effects model. In addition, the traumatic exposure will be
and ⫺0.33 for social support, ⫺0.51 and 0.23 for PTG, and 0.76 controlled in this model (see Figure 1). Moreover, to test the signif-
and 0.47 for PTSD. Moreover, to examine the multivariate nor- icance of this indirect effect, we conducted bias-corrected bootstrap
mality of measures, Mardia’s multivariate normality tests were tests with a 95% confidence interval.
also conducted. The result showed that Mardia coefficient was
6.81 (CR ⫽ 7.16), which indicated that the data in this study did Results
not belong to multivariate normal distribution. Thus, we used
MLR in the model analysis according to Wang, Wang, and Jiang
Descriptive Statistics and Correlations Among
(2011) suggestion. To evaluate model fit, we used chi-square
Main Measures
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

values, the CFI, the TLI, the RMSEA, and the standardized root
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

mean residual (SRMR). A nonsignificant chi-square indicates Descriptive statistics and the correlations among the main measures
good model-data fit. The general cutoffs for accepting a model are are shown in Table 1. The mean levels of trauma exposure, social
equal to or greater than 0.90 for the CFI and TLI, and less than support, cognitive reappraisal, expressive suppression, PTSD, and
0.08 for the SRMR and RMSEA (e.g., Wen, Hau, & Marsh, 2004). PTG were 1.15, 2.40, 4.76, 3.84, 0.76, and 2.80, respectively. Pear-
We built structural equation models (SEM) to examine the roles son’s correlations were calculated. These analyses yielded significant
of social support and emotion regulation in PTSD and PTG. Here, correlations between all of main variables except for the relationship
the PTSD latent variable was evaluated according to CPSS sub- between social support and PTSD. More importantly, we obtained a
scale scores for intrusion, avoidance and hyper-arousal (Foa et al., nonsignificant relation between PTSD and PTG.
2001) whereas the PTG latent variable was evaluated in terms of
perceived changes in the self, a changed sense of relationships with
Analysis of the Indirect Effect
others, and a changed philosophy of life(Zhou et al., 2014a).
Next, we applied an SEM approach to assess structural paths from Correlation analysis found that social support was nonsignifi-
social support to PTSD and PTG, such that PTSD and PTG were cantly associated with PTSD, but according to Wen and Ye (2014),
assumed to be related because of their observed coexistence in trauma the indirect effect of social support on PTSD via a third variable

Hyper-arousal
Avoidance
Intrusion

0.78***
***
0.70 0.86***
CR
0.01 -0.17*
***
PTSD
0.22
TE *
0.14
0.22**
0.12*
0.12 *
0.25*** 0.07
0.32***
***
0.34
SS 0.18**
-0.14* PTG
ES 0.07
*** *** ***
0.91 0.84 0.71
0.91*** 0.80***
0.88*** 0.84*** 0.89***
Affirmative evaluation
Instrumental support

Philosophy changes
Emotive support

Relation changes
Companion

Self changes
Intimacy

Figure 1. Indirect effects model. Note: TE ⫽ Traumatic exposure; SS ⫽ Social support; CR ⫽ Cognitive
reappraisal; ES ⫽ Expressive suppression. ⴱ p ⬍ 0.05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ 0.01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ 0.001.
SOCIAL SUPPORT, EMOTION REGULATION, PTSD/PTG 5

Table 1
Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations Between Traumatic Exposure Severity, Cognitive
Reappraisal, Expressive Suppression, PTSD, and PTG

M ⫾ SD 1 2 3 4 5 6

1. Traumatic exposure 1.15 ⫾ 0.19 1.00


2. Social support 2.40 ⫾ 0.78 0.05 1.00
3. Cognitive reappraisal 4.76 ⫾ 0.97 0.05 0.26ⴱⴱ 1.00
4. Experssive suppression 3.84 ⫾ 1.20 0.10† ⫺0.12ⴱ 0.20ⴱⴱ 1.00
5. PTSD 0.76 ⫾ 0.46 0.20ⴱⴱ ⫺0.01 ⫺0.11ⴱ 0.22ⴱⴱ 1.00
6. PTG 2.80 ⫾ 0.93 0.15ⴱⴱ 0.26ⴱⴱ 0.33ⴱⴱ 0.10† 0.07 1.00
ⴱ ⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ

p ⬍ 0.086. p ⬍ 0.05. p ⬍ 0.01. p ⬍ 0.001.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

could be also examined as the “suppression effect” might exist. Salsman et al. (2009) studies, but not paralleled to others studies (e.g.,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Suppression effect refers to a situation where the magnitude of the Johnson et al., 2007). A potential explanation might be attributed to
relation between an independent variable and a dependent variable the difference of measuring instrument on PTSD and PTG, and
becomes larger when a third variable is included (MacKinnon, participants as well as their traumatic experiences. More importantly,
Krull, & Lockwood, 2000). Thus, we still examined the indirect we found that the level of adolescent’s PTSD was relatively low in
effect of social support on PTSD via emotion regulation in this this study. This may make it difficult to arouse traumatized individ-
study (e.g., cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression) to uals’ motion to activate them to engage in cognitive thoughts on
assess the potential suppression effect of emotion regulation. In traumatic clues (e.g., Meyerson, Grant, Carter, & Kilmer, 2011),
addition, social support and PTG was significantly correlated, and hence, it is hard for PTSD to promote growth. Therefore, we found a
the roles of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression be- nonsignificant relation between PTSD and PTG, although adolescents
tween social support and PTG were also examined. Taken to- had a high level of PTG.
gether, we assumed that social support had indirect effect on both In addition, consistent with previous studies (e.g., Jia et al., 2015;
PTSD and PTG via emotion regulation. Yu et al., 2014) and the direct model of social support (Cohen &
In addition, regarding for the coexistence of PTSD and PTG, Syme, 1985), we found that social support had a positive and direct
then a correlation path between PTSD and PTG was added. Fur- prediction on PTG, Here, social support appeared to provide some
thermore, we also controlled for the potential role of traumatic necessary emotional support for adolescent survivors, which can
exposure in structural equation model analyses, because of its enhance positive emotion, and in turn helps them to rethink self,
significant correlations with social support as well as PTSD/PTG. others and the world, and they then realize psychological growth (e.g.,
However, the direct path from social support to PTSD was not Zhou, Wu, Fu, & An, 2015b). In addition, social support can also
added because of the nonsignificant correlation between them. The improve one’s interpersonal relationship with others, and in turn
final indirect effects model was presented in Figure 1. The results increase the likelihood of emotion sharing and cognitive disclosure
of model analysis found that this indirect effects model exhibited (e.g., Rimé, 2009), and ultimately lead to the realization of PTG (e.g.,
acceptable fit indices, ␹2/df ⫽ 2.38, CFI ⫽ 0.95, TLI ⫽ 0.94, García, Cova, Rincón, Vázquez, & Páez, 2016).
RMSEA (90% CI) ⫽ 0.067 (0.054 –0.081), SRMR ⫽ 0.043. All However, inconsistent with the assumptions of the direct social
the paths were found significant except for the paths between support model, correlation analysis found a nonsignificant association
expressive suppression and PTG. PTSD and PTG were indepen- between social support and PTSD, which indicated that social support
dent. did not play a direct and significant role in PTSD. A possible expla-
Next, we conducted bias-corrected bootstrap tests with a 95% nation is that if constant social support from others is provided even
confidence interval to assess the significance of observed indirect after 6 months since the Ya’an earthquake, the traumatized adoles-
effects. Table 2 illustrates the results of these tests, which revealed
that the findings in Figure 1 were accepted, and indicated that social
support had a positive and direct prediction on PTG. Moreover, social Table 2
support had a negative indirect prediction on PTSD and a positive Bias-Corrected Bootstrap Tests of Mediating Effects
indirect prediction on PTG through cognitive reappraisal. Social sup-
port, through expressive suppression, had a significant and indirect 95% CI
prediction on PTSD, but nonsignificant indirect prediction on PTG. Paths Standardized ␤ (SE) Low High

Direct paths
Discussion SS - PTG 0.18 (0.07)ⴱⴱ 0.07 0.29
To our knowledge, this is the first study to date to simultaneously Indirect paths
SS - CR - PTSD ⫺0.05 (0.02)ⴱ ⫺0.09 ⫺0.02
examine the roles of social support and emotion regulation in influ- SS - CR - PTG 0.11 (0.03)ⴱⴱⴱ 0.06 0.15
encing PTSD and PTG, and to elucidate the relationship between SS - ES - PTSD ⫺0.03 (0.02)ⴱ ⫺0.06 ⫺0.001
PTSD and PTG by examining their differences in terms of influencing SS - ES - PTG ⫺0.01 (0.01) ⫺0.03 0.01
factors, after controlled traumatic exposure. The findings indicated a Note. SS ⫽ Social support; CR ⫽ Cognitive reappraisal; ES ⫽ Expres-
nonsignificant relation between PTSD and PTG in correlation analy- sive suppression.

sis and model analysis, which was consistent with the results from p ⬍ 0.05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ 0.01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ 0.001.
6 ZHOU, WU, AND ZHEN

cents in the environment that always protects traumatized adolescents of participants, our sample may be somewhat selective, which can
by others’ social support have few chances to learn to cope on their entail the risk of a reporting bias. Second, the study was conducted
own, and this will further damage their self-efficacy and enthusiasm with a sample of adolescent survivors after the Ya’an earthquake
in coping with trauma (e.g., Zhou, Wu, An, & Chen, 2014b), and in China, and generalizations to people with other traumatic ex-
thereby it is difficult for traumatized adolescents to relieve negative periences must be made with caution. Moreover, the study was a
psychological results, such as PTSD. cross-sectional study, and generalization of the results to other
Another potential explanation for the nonsignificant effect of social studies should be made with caution, future studies should further
support on PTSD is that “suppression effect” existed between them. examine these main measures from longitudinal perspective.
Specifically, the present study found that social support had an indi- Despite these limitations, this study contributes new knowledge to
rect and significant negative prediction on PTSD by increasing cog- previous theoretical and empirical studies on the relationships be-
nitive reappraisal. A previous study suggested that perceived high tween social support, emotion regulation, and PTSD/PTG. In partic-
social support can increase adolescents’ feelings of safety and belong- ular, this study further supports the assumption that pathways to
ing and can help adolescents to share their traumatic experiences PTSD and PTG are different (e.g., Chan, Ho, Tedeschi, & Leung,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

freely with supporters (e.g., Zhou et al., 2014a), which can increase 2011) and has demonstrated that PTSD and PTG have different
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

the probability of cognitive reappraisal on trauma and emotion. Fur- predictive factors as well as indirectly predictive process. In fact,
thermore, cognitive reappraisal represents a more effortful, deliberate Dekel et al. (2011) proposed that the existence of unique predictors of
cognitive process, and this process can serve as an adaptive coping PTSD and PTG would suggest that they were separate constructs.
strategy (e.g., Salsman et al., 2009), which helps to relieve the severity From this perspective, these results indicated that PTSD and PTG are
of PTSD. In addition, cognitive reappraisal provides an opportunity separate, independent dimensions of psychological experiences fol-
for producing the thoughts that concern the positive implications of a lowing adversity (e.g., Linley & Joseph, 2004).
traumatic experience (e.g.,Cann, Calhoun, Tedeschi, & Solomon, From an intervention and health-enhancement perspective, this
2010), and hence relieves the level of PTSD. Thereby, social support study also highlights important implications for adolescent survivors
could buffer the severity of PTSD via increasing traumatized adoles- of the Ya’an earthquake. The assessment for adaptation to the ado-
cents’ cognitive reappraisal. lescents with traumatic experience should incorporate both positive
An interesting finding of this study is that expressive suppression and negative indices of adjustment. Clinical efforts should focus on
also had a “suppression effect” between social support and PTSD. the improvement of cognitive reappraisal. For example, school psy-
Specifically, the finding found that social support also could relieve chologists or parents can encourage adolescents to think of the posi-
indirectly the severity of PTSD through decreasing expressive sup- tive aspect of posttraumatic world, and look positively on self, inter-
pression. Here, in a setting with high social support, people are more personal relationship, and the world after trauma. By this, adolescents
likely to expose their emotions to others and to vent negative feelings, can positively reframe the trauma reminders, and also disclosing their
and in turn the probability of expressive suppression is reduced. negative emotions to others will mitigate negative posttraumatic ef-
Generally, expressive suppression can lead to increased sympathetic fects and instead contribute to positive posttraumatic growth.
nervous system arousal (e.g., Harris, 2001) and impaired memory
(e.g., Richards, Butler, & Gross, 2003), which will elicit greater
depression and social anxiety, and more negative emotions in re- References
sponse to negative affective stimuli (e.g., Sperberg & Stabb, 1998). Boden, M. T., Bonn-Miller, M. O., Kashdan, T. B., Alvarez, J., & Gross,
Thus, social support has an indirect predictive role in PTSD by J. J. (2012). The interactive effects of emotional clarity and cognitive
reducing expressive suppression. reappraisal in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disor-
Nevertheless, our findings indicated that cognitive reappraisal, ders, 26, 233–238. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.11.007
but not expressive suppression, mediated the relationship between Brans, K., Koval, P., Verduyn, P., Lim, Y. L., & Kuppens, P. (2013). The
social support and PTG. Specifically, social support has indirect regulation of negative and positive affect in daily life. Emotion, 13,
and positive effect on PTG via increasing cognitive reappraisal but 926 –939. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032400
Cann, A., Calhoun, L. G., Tedeschi, R. G., & Solomon, D. T. (2010).
has nonsignificant indirect effect on PTG via decreasing emotion
Posttraumatic growth and depreciation as independent experiences and
suppression. Previous studies indicated that PTG could be acti-
predictors of well-being. Journal of Loss and Trauma, 15, 151–166.
vated by people’s cognitive activities, such as positive reappraisal http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15325020903375826
(e.g., Stockton, Hunt, & Joseph, 2011). Here, cognitive reappraisal Carpenter, K. M., Fowler, J. M., Maxwell, G. L., & Andersen, B. L. (2010).
can distract traumatized adolescents’ attention from negative as- Direct and buffering effects of social support among gynecologic cancer
pects of traumatic clues and keep their focus on the positive side survivors. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 39, 79 –90. http://dx.doi.org/
of posttraumatic world (e.g., Zhou, Wu, Fu, & An, 2015b). This 10.1007/s12160-010-9160-1
will then increase the likelihood for traumatized adolescents to Chan, M. W. C., Ho, S. M. Y., Tedeschi, R. G., & Leung, C. W. L. (2011).
restructure their understanding on self, interpersonal relationship, The valence of attentional bias and cancer-related rumination in post-
and the world following trauma, which promotes them to find the traumatic stress and posttraumatic growth among women with breast
meanings of posttraumatic world, and in turn their PTG realized cancer. Psycho-Oncology, 20, 544 –552. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pon
.1761
(e.g., Zhou et al., 2015b). Thus, when cognitive reappraisal and
Cohen, S., & Syme, S. L. (1985). Issues in the study and application of
expressive suppression were inserted in the relation between social social support. In S. Cohen & S. L. Syme (Eds.), Social support and
support and PTG simultaneously, the role of emotional process in health (Vol. 3, pp. 3–22). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
PTG was obscured by such cognitive process. Dekel, S., Mandl, C., & Solomon, Z. (2011). Shared and unique predictors
Several design and measurement limitations must be acknowl- of post-traumatic growth and distress. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
edged. First, because of the aim of our study and the characteristics 67, 241–252. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20747
SOCIAL SUPPORT, EMOTION REGULATION, PTSD/PTG 7

Foa, E. B., Johnson, K. M., Feeny, N. C., & Treadwell, K. R. H. (2001). Ozer, E. J., Best, S. R., Lipsey, T. L., & Weiss, D. S. (2003). Predictors of
The Child PTSD Symptom Scale: A preliminary examination of its posttraumatic stress disorder and symptoms in adults: A meta-analysis.
psychometric properties. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 30, Psychological Bulletin, 129, 52–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-
376 –384. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15374424JCCP3003_9 2909.129.1.52
García, F. E., Cova, F., Rincón, P., & Vázquez, C. (2015). Trauma or Pan, X., Liu, W., Deng, G., Liu, T., Yan, J., Tang, Y., . . . Xu, M. (2015).
growth after a natural disaster? The mediating role of rumination pro- Symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder, depression, and anxiety
cesses. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 6, 26557. http://dx among junior high school students in worst-hit areas 3 years after the
.doi.org/10.3402/ejpt.v6.26557 Wenchuan earthquake in China. Asia-Pacific Journal of Public Health,
García, F. E., Cova, F., Rincón, P., Vázquez, C., & Páez, D. (2016). 27, NP1985–NP1994. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1010539513488625
Coping, rumination and posttraumatic growth in people affected by an Pietrzak, R. H., Johnson, D. C., Goldstein, M. B., Malley, J. C., &
earthquake. Psicothema, 28, 59 – 65. Southwick, S. M. (2009). Psychological resilience and postdeployment
García Martinez, F. E., Reyes Reyes, A., & Cova Solar, F. (2014). Severity social support protect against traumatic stress and depressive symptoms
of trauma, optimism, posttraumatic growth and well-being in survivors in soldiers returning from Operations Enduring Freedom and Iraqi
of a natural disaster. Universitas Psychologica, 13, 575–584. Freedom. Depression and Anxiety, 26, 745–751. http://dx.doi.org/10
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Gross, J. J. (1998a). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: .1002/da.20558


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Richards, J. M., Butler, E. A., & Gross, J. J. (2003). Emotion regulation in
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 224 –237. http://dx romantic relationships: The cognitive consequences of concealing feel-
.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.224 ings. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 20, 599 – 620. http://
Gross, J. J. (1998b). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An inte- dx.doi.org/10.1177/02654075030205002
grative review. Review of General Psychology, 2, 271–299. http://dx.doi Rimé, B. (2009). Emotion elicits the social sharing of emotion: Theory and
.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271 empirical review. Emotion Review, 1, 60 – 85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social 1754073908097189
consequences. Psychophysiology, 39, 281–291. http://dx.doi.org/10 Rogers, C. R. (1951). Client-centered therapy: Its current practice, impli-
.1017/S0048577201393198 cations, and theory. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion Salsman, J. M., Segerstrom, S. C., Brechting, E. H., Carlson, C. R., &
regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well- Andrykowski, M. A. (2009). Posttraumatic growth and PTSD symptom-
being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348 –362. atology among colorectal cancer survivors: A three-month longitudinal
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348 examination of cognitive processing. Psycho-Oncology, 18, 30 – 41.
Harris, C. R. (2001). Cardiovascular responses of embarrassment and http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pon.1367
effects of emotional suppression in a social setting. Journal of Person- Schwarzer, R., & Knoll, N. (2007). Functional roles of social support
ality and Social Psychology, 81, 886 – 897. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ within the stress and coping process: A theoretical and empirical over-
0022-3514.81.5.886 view. International Journal of Psychology, 42, 243–252. http://dx.doi
Jia, X., Ying, L., Zhou, X., Wu, X., & Lin, C. (2015). The effects of .org/10.1080/00207590701396641
extraversion, social support on the posttraumatic stress disorder and Shakespeare-Finch, J., & Lurie-Beck, J. (2014). A meta-analytic clarifica-
posttraumatic growth of adolescent survivors of the Wenchuan earth- tion of the relationship between posttraumatic growth and symptoms of
quake. PLoS One, 10, e0121480. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone posttraumatic distress disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 28, 223–
.0121480 229. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.10.005
Johnson, R. J., Hobfoll, S. E., Hall, B. J., Canetti-Nisim, D., Galea, S., & Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. S. (2003). Adolescents’ emotion
Palmieri, P. A. (2007). Posttraumatic growth: Action and reaction. regulation in daily life: Links to depressive symptoms and problem
Applied Psychology, 56, 428 – 436. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1464- behavior. Child Development, 74, 1869 –1880. http://dx.doi.org/10
0597.2007.00296.x .1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00643.x
Lazarus, R. S., & Alfert, E. (1964). Short-circuiting of threat by experi- Sperberg, E. D., & Stabb, S. D. (1998). Depression in women as related to
mentally altering cognitive appraisal. Journal of Abnormal and Social anger and mutuality in relationships. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
Psychology, 69, 195–205. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0044635 22, 223–238. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1998.tb00152.x
Linley, P. A., & Joseph, S. (2004). Positive change following trauma and Srivastava, S., Tamir, M., McGonigal, K. M., John, O. P., & Gross, J. J.
adversity: A review. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 17, 11–21. http://dx (2009). The social costs of emotional suppression: A prospective study
.doi.org/10.1023/B:JOTS.0000014671.27856.7e of the transition to college. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
MacKinnon, D. P., Krull, J. L., & Lockwood, C. M. (2000). Equivalence ogy, 96, 883– 897. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014755
of the mediation, confounding and suppression effect. Prevention Sci- Stockton, H., Hunt, N., & Joseph, S. (2011). Cognitive processing, rumi-
ence, 1, 173–181. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1026595011371 nation, and posttraumatic growth. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 24,
Marroquín, B. (2011). Interpersonal emotion regulation as a mechanism of 85–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jts.20606
social support in depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 1276 – Tedeschi, R. G., & Calhoun, L. G. (1996). The Posttraumatic Growth
1290. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2011.09.005 Inventory: Measuring the positive legacy of trauma. Journal of Trau-
Marroquín, B., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2015). Emotion regulation and matic Stress, 9, 455– 471. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jts.2490090305
depressive symptoms: Close relationships as social context and influ- Valentiner, D. P., Holahan, C. J., & Moos, R. H. (1994). Social support,
ence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109, 836 – 855. appraisals of event controllability, and coping: An integrative model.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000034 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 1094 –1102. http://
Meyerson, D. A., Grant, K. E., Carter, J. S., & Kilmer, R. P. (2011). dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.66.6.1094
Posttraumatic growth among children and adolescents: A systematic Wang, J., Wang, X., & Jiang, B. (2011). Structural equation models:
review. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 949 –964. http://dx.doi.org/10 Methods and applications. Beijing, China: Higher Education Press.
.1016/j.cpr.2011.06.003 Wang, L., Liu, H., Li, Z., & Du, W. (2007). Reliability and validity of
Muthén, B. O., & Muthén, L. K. (2010). Mplus (version 6.00) [Computer Emotion Regulation Questionnaire Chinese Revised Version [Chinese].
software]. Los Angeles, CA: Author. China Journal of Health Psychology, 15, 503–505.
8 ZHOU, WU, AND ZHEN

Wen, Z., Hau, K., & Marsh, H. W. (2004). Structual equation model Zhou, X., Wu, X., An, Y., Chen, H., & Long, C. (2014a). Social support
testing: Cutoff criteria for goodness of fit indices and Chi-square test mediate the associations between gratitude and post-traumatic growth in
[Chinese]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 36, 186 –194. middle school students at three years after Wenchuan earthquake [Chi-
Wen, Z., & Ye, B. (2014). Analyses of mediating effects: The development nese]. Psychological Development and Education, 30, 68 –74.
of methods and models. Advances in Psychological Science, 22, 731– Zhou, X., Wu, X., An, Y., & Chen, J. (2014b). The roles of rumination and
745. social support in the associations between core belief challenge and
Wu, X., Zhang, Y., Lin, C., & Zang, W. (2013). The effect of disaster post-traumatic growth among adolescent survivors after the Wenchuan
exposure on PTSD of primary and secondary students: Mediating and earthquake [Chinese]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 46, 1509 –1520.
moderating effects [Chinese]. Psychological Development and Educa- Zhou, X., Wu, X., & Chen, J. (2015a). Longitudinal linkages between
posttraumatic stress disorder and posttraumatic growth in adolescent
tion, 29, 641– 648.
survivors following the Wenchuan earthquake in China: A three-wave,
Wu, Z., Xu, J., & Sui, Y. (2016). Posttraumatic stress disorder and
cross-lagged study. Psychiatry Research, 228, 107–111. http://dx.doi
posttraumatic growth coexistence and the risk factors in Wenchuan
.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.04.024
earthquake survivors. Psychiatry Research, 237, 49 –54. http://dx.doi
Zhou, X., Wu, X., Fu, F., & An, Y. (2015b). Core belief challenge and
.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.01.041
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

rumination as predictors of PTSD and PTG among adolescent survivors


Ying, L. H., Wu, X. C., Lin, C. D., & Chen, C. (2013). Prevalence and of the Wenchuan earthquake. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder and depressive symptoms Practice, and Policy, 7, 391–397. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/tra0000031
among child survivors 1 year following the Wenchuan earthquake in Zou, H. (1999). Social support networks and personal relationships in
China. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 22, 567–575. http:// middle school students [Chinese]. Journal of Beijing Normal University
dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00787-013-0400-3 (Social Sciences), 1, 34 – 42.
Yu, Y., Peng, L., Chen, L., Long, L., He, W., Li, M., & Wang, T. (2014).
Resilience and social support promote posttraumatic growth of women Received June 1, 2016
with infertility: The mediating role of positive coping. Psychiatry Re- Revision received August 2, 2016
search, 215, 401– 405. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.10.032 Accepted September 17, 2016 䡲

You might also like