How Does Green Advertising Skepticism On Social Media Affect Consumer Intention To Purchase Green Products?

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Received: 9 January 2019 Revised: 16 February 2020 Accepted: 24 February 2020

DOI: 10.1002/cb.1818

SPECIAL ISSUE ARTICLE

How does green advertising skepticism on social media affect


consumer intention to purchase green products?

Biao Luo1 | Ying Sun2 | Jie Shen3 | Lihui Xia2

1
School of Management, Hefei University of
Technology, Hefei, China Abstract
2
School of Management, University of Science Underpinned by the stimulus–organism–response model, this study explores the
and Technology of China, Hefei, China
underlying mechanism through which green advertising skepticism on social media
3
Shenzhen Audencia Business School,
Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, China affects consumer green purchase intention. It also investigates the boundary factors
that moderate this indirect relationship. Performing structural equation modeling on
Correspondence
Ying Sun, School of Management, University data collected from an online survey on Sina Weibo, this study finds that green adver-
of Science and Technology of China, tising skepticism on social media negatively affects green purchase intention through
No. 96, Jinzhai Road, Hefei, Anhui Province
230026, China. the mediation of perceived information utility. Moreover, interdependent self-
Email: suny11@mail.ustc.edu.cn construal positively moderates, and independent self-construal negatively moderates
this indirect relationship. These findings suggest that truthful and consumer-tailored
green advertising is crucial to green product marketing.

1 | HOW DOES GREEN ADVERTISING benefits. Such green advertisements may raise consumers' skepticism of
SKEPTICISM ON SOCIAL MEDIA AFFECT greenwashing (Baum, 2012; do Paço & Reis, 2012; Matthes &
CONSUMER INTENTION TO PURCHASE Wonneberger, 2014). It is argued that green advertising skepticism,
G RE E N P R O D U C T S ? defined as individuals' cynicism toward exaggerated or misleading green
advertising, widely exists, especially among green consumers
Green products, referring to the products themselves as well as their pro- (e.g., Atkinson & Kim, 2015; do Paço & Reis, 2012; Matthes &
duction processes, are resource (e.g., energy and water) saving, low pollut- Wonneberger, 2014; Shrum, McCarty, & Lowrey, 1995; Vermeir & Ver-
ing, low toxic, and recyclable (Sun, Wang, Gao, & Li, 2018). They are beke, 2006). While green advertising skepticism has received growing
better for the environment and human health (Schuitema & de Groot, research attention past research mainly focused on its antecedents, rather
2015; Weber, Baier, & Willers, 2015). Recent decades have witnessed a than its consequences. For example, Shrum et al. (1995) and Matthes and
growing interest in green products as pollution and climate change have Wonneberger (2014) explored and found the impact of green consumer-
become global social concerns (Zhang, Li, Cao, & Huang, 2018). There- ism on skepticism toward advertising. Research has begun to address
fore, enterprises can gain competitive advantages by introducing green whether consumers' intention to buy green products is undermined by
products, such as ecologically friendly products, to distinguish their prod- their skepticism toward green ads (e.g., Goh & Balaji, 2016; Wei, Chiang,
ucts from others (Epstein & Buhovac, 2014; Tan, 2011). Green advertising Kou, & Lee, 2017). Yet, how and when green advertising skepticism
has increasingly been used to promote green products, and its positive affects consumer green purchase intention remain largely understudied.
relationship with consumer green purchase intention has well been The current study addresses this long-standing issue in marketing
established in the literature (Ankit & Mayur, 2013; Schmuck, Matthes, & and advertising research regarding how and when green advertising
Naderer, 2018). It is suggested that green advertising builds a firm or skepticism affects consumer purchase intention. First, this study
product's green image by employing environmental claims to appeal to explores the direct effect of green advertising skepticism on consumer
green consumers (Benn, Dunphy, & Griffiths, 2014; Brettel, Reich, green purchase intention. Second, this study investigates the mediat-
Gavilanes, & Flatten, 2015; Segev, Fernandes, & Hong, 2016). ing role of perceived information utility in the relationship between
However, green advertisements often present confusing facts, jar- green advertising skepticism and consumer purchase intention. Per-
gon, and ambiguous messages. They often lack accurate information ceived information utility refers to the degree to which consumers
about the environmental attributes of products or exaggerate the perceive the information presented in advertisements as useful for

J Consumer Behav. 2020;1–11. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/cb © 2020 John Wiley & Sons Ltd 1
2 LUO ET AL.

helping them make the right purchasing decision (Matthes & skepticism. Taken together, this research advances the marketing and
Wonneberger, 2014). Our theorizing is underpinned by the stimulus– advertising literature. The theoretical framework and hypotheses are
organism–response (SOR) model (Mehrabian & Russell, 1974), which depicted in Figure 1.
posits that stimuli (i.e., external factors) affect internal states of an
individual (i.e., organism), which in turn influence the individual's atti-
tudes and behaviors (i.e., response). Stimuli can exert either a positive 2 | THEORETICAL BACKGROUND AND
or a negative effect. Green advertising skepticism will exert a negative HY P O T H E S E S D EV E L O P M EN T
effect on consumer perceptions of information utility. This study
focuses on skepticism toward green advertising on social media. 2.1 | Green advertising on social media and green
Advertising on social media is less regulated than on traditional media. purchase intention
The social attributes of social media create a more complicated envi-
ronment in which consumers assess green advertising (Brettel et al., Green advertising explicitly or implicitly promotes a green lifestyle
2015; Sin, Nor, & Al-Agaga, 2012). This increases the opportunity to and presents an image of corporate environmental responsibility
produce exaggerated and misleading green advertisements. (Banerjee, Gulas, & Iyer, 1995). With the rapid growth of the Internet,
The literature suggests that individual traits moderate the impact many enterprises choose to publish green advertisements on social
of organism on individual responses (e.g., Chang, Eckman, & Yan, media, including Facebook, microblogs, WeChat, wikis, and multime-
2011). Self-construal is a prominent individual trait that affects one's dia sharing sites, for example, YouTube. Social media is seen as an
attitudes and behavior toward others. Self-construal describes the effective platform for green advertising, as it allows for interpersonal
extent to which the self is defined independently of others or inter- interactions and networking that create an indirect, electronic form of
dependently in relation with others (Markus & Kitayama, 1991, 2010). communication that benefits from word-of-mouth (Brettel
People with high independent self-construal tend to pay attention to et al., 2015).
their own abilities, traits, preferences, and wishes. Individuals with Green advertising skepticism is a negative, context-specific, cog-
high interdependent self-construal have an intrinsic connectedness nitive component of consumer attitudes toward green products
with others and consider group goals over their own (for a detailed lu, & Ellen, 1998). Con-
(Manuel, Youn, & Yoon, 2014; Mohr, Erog
review on self-construal, see Cross, Hardin, & Swing, 2011). Indepen- sumers become skeptical of green ads that present misleading and
dent and interdependent construal can coexist in an individual and are exaggerated information about the green benefits or environmental
not mutually exclusive, although, they were initially considered exact or health attributes of products (e.g., Bickart & Ruth, 2012; do Paço &
opposites (Hu, Zhang, & Luo, 2016; Singelis & Sharkey, 1995). Inde- Reis, 2012). It is argued that whether consumers will consider buying
pendent self-construal and interdependent self-construal are regarded green products depends on the extent to which they have confidence
as two independent dimensions (Brewer & Gardner, 1996). Kim et al. in the green products (Goh & Balaji, 2016; Wei et al., 2017). When
(1996) proposed that individuals can simultaneously maintain their customers are skeptical of green advertising, they are more likely to
independent and interdependent construal as both high, both low, or attribute green advertisements to money-making or corporate image
as one high and one low. However, as the two are typically contrary improving motives (Elving, 2013; Leonidou and Skarmeas, 2015). Such
thoughts, emotions, and behaviors, an individual is less likely to score suspicion of a company's motive for green advertising leads to nega-
high in both at one time (Matsumoto, 1999). In accordance with the tive product judgment and lower purchase intention (Bickart & Ruth,
literature, we conceptualize that independent self-construal weakens 2012; do Paço & Reis, 2012; Morel & Pruyn, 2003). In general, if con-
the impact of perceived information utility on green purchase inten- sumers are skeptical of green ads due to their false and exaggerated
tion and that interdependent self-construal strengthens the impact. information about products' green benefits or environmental or health
We believe that this study makes important contributions to the attributes, they will intend not to buy the product. In line with Goh
advertising and marketing literature. The existing literature on green and Balaji (2016) and Wei et al. (2017), we develop the following
purchase focuses on how to promote green purchases. Likewise, the hypothesis:
positive effect of green advertising has received extensive academic
recognition (e.g., Ankit & Mayur, 2013; Zhu, 2013). Although recently Hypothesis 1 Green advertising skepticism on social media negatively
there has been growing research exploring antecedents of green affects consumer green purchase intention.
advertising skepticism, there has been little examination of its impact
on consumer behaviors or its underlying mechanism. This study con-
ceptualizes the mediation of perceived information utility in the rela- 2.2 | Green advertising skepticism on social media
tionship between green advertising skepticism and consumer green and perceived information utility
purchase intention as a SOR process. In doing so, this research
extends the SOR model. Moreover, exploring the contingent effects In an asymmetric information environment, consumers rely on envi-
of different self-construal on the relationship between perceived ronmental cues, such as green advertising, to prompt their internal
information utility and consumer green purchase intention helps cognition, and finally decide whether to purchase products or services
understand the different individual consequences of green advertising (Mikalef, Giannakos, & Pateli, 2013). The SOR model postulates that
LUO ET AL. 3

FIGURE 1 The conceptual model

environmental cues act as external stimuli that can affect an individ- consumers' cognitive responses to an advertisement will influence
ual's internal cognition, including their perceptual, physiological, feel- their purchase intention regardless of whether the product is at mar-
ing, and thinking activities (i.e., those of the organism) (Bagozzi, 1986; ket value (Batra & Ray, 1986). In a similar vein, it can be argued that
Mehrabian & Russell, 1974; Jacoby, 2002). Green advertising is an perceived information utility of green advertisements on social media
accessible piece of information used by marketers as a sales strategy is related to consumer green purchase intention, such that high infor-
and used by consumers to assess the quality or value of green prod- mation utility leads to a stronger purchase intention.
ucts (Bloom & Reve, 1990). Applying the SOR model, we argue that Due to the information asymmetry between manufacturers and
green advertisements on social media can be regarded as external consumers, consumers may lack information about green products
stimuli that affect viewers' perceived information utility of green (Matthes, Wonneberger, & Schmuck, 2014). Hence, they may strongly
advertising (i.e., internal cognition). As an external stimulus, green seek information about green products in order to participate in green
advertising is only effective when consumers perceive information consumption. The information provided by green advertisements is
utility to be high (Boulding & Kirmani, 1993). the primary source based on which consumers make decisions about
Green advertising meets the need of consumers for information whether they buy green products. When consumers perceive the
about product attributes and enables them to make better purchase information provided in green ads as useful, they will be more likely to
decisions. High information utility means that the information is per- be interested in and pay attention to green products. They will
ceived as truthful and useful. In contrast, low information utility browse, buy, and use more green products. Hence, the more the infor-
means that the information is perceived as untruthful, unhelpful, and mation in green advertising is perceived by consumers as useful, the
to be ignored (Wei et al., 2017). Consumer perceived information util- more likely they are to be exposed to green products. So, if the infor-
ity level will be lower if consumers are skeptical of green advertising mation provided by the green advertisement is helpful, and the infor-
due to the exaggerated, misleading and/or false information in adver- mation utility to consumers is high, the quality of green claims will be
tisements (Wei et al., 2017). On the contrary, consumer perceived positively evaluated (Matthes & Wonneberger, 2014). In contrast,
information utility level will be high if the information provided in when information utility is low, consumers will tend to consume fewer
advertisements is clear, reliable, and credible (Wei et al., 2017). There- green products (Wei et al., 2017). Organism in the SOR model repre-
fore, green advertising skepticism is negatively associated with con- sents cognitive states, which play a mediating role between stimuli
sumer perceived information utility. Hence, we develop the following and responses (Goi, Kalidas, & Zeeshan, 2014). In a similar vein, as
hypothesis: Hypothesis 2 proposes a direct relationship between green advertis-
ing skepticism and perceived information utility, bringing together the
Hypothesis 2 Green advertising skepticism on social media negatively above discussions materializes the following mediation relationship:
affects consumers' perceived information utility.
Hypothesis 3 Green advertising skepticism indirectly affects consumer
green purchase intention via the mediation of perceived informa-
2.3 | The mediation of perceived information tion utility.
utility

The second part of the SOR model suggests that internal cognition 2.4 | Moderating effects of self-construal
(i.e., organism) resulting from external stimuli influences individuals'
responses (Mehrabian & Russell, 1974). Perceived information utility The emergence of social media makes personal differentiation and
serves as organism, which has consumer purchasing consequences. interactivity more important for green advertising (Zhou, Zhang, Su, &
Supporting the SOR model, the advertising literature argues that Zhou, 2012; Liu et al, 2016; Lin et al, 2019). Individuals' interactions
4 LUO ET AL.

with others on social media influence their purchase intentions. The to buy green products, because they want to be different from others
concept of self-construal provides a conceptual basis for examining (Chen & Moosmayer, 2018). In other words, for individuals with highly
the degree to which consumers interact with others on social media independent self-construal, making decisions is a matter for them-
(Kim & Yun, 2007). Self-construal is a compilation of thoughts, emo- selves; in the decision-making process, they focus their attention on
tions, and behaviors in the context of the relationship and differences their own feelings. Thus, Hypothesis 5 is posited:
between the self and others (Markus & Kitayama, 1991, 2010). In the
context of social media, the self-construal of consumers has an impor- Hypothesis 5 Independent self-construal negatively moderates the rela-
tant impact on whether consumers choose to buy green products tionship between the consumers' perceived information utility of
(Hoffman & Novak, 2012). Prominent group members on social media green advertising on social media and green purchase intention,
may see themselves as concerning the group, and vice versa. Thus, sit- such that the relationship is weaker when the level of independent
uations that emphasize group relationships and social environments self-construal is high and stronger when the level is low.
may result in individuals having more collective and pro-
environmental considerations.
Interdependent self-construal emphasizes individuals' social con- 3 | RESEARCH METHODOLOGY
nections and group harmony. Individuals with high levels of
interdependent self-construal view themselves as part of the group. 3.1 | Sample and data collection
They strive to achieve collective goals and fulfill social obligations and
responsibilities, prioritizing these over individual goals (Singelis, 1994). With the rapid development of the Internet, recent decades have seen
Consumers with highly interdependent self-construal usually prioritize the explosive growth of green advertising on social media (Hensel &
the opinions of those who are important to them. Also, they may sac- Deis, 2010). China has the largest number of social media users in the
rifice their own interests to benefit their group. When consumers with world, far more than in any other country. For example, Sina Weibo
highly interdependent self-construal are suspicious of green advertis- has over 340 million monthly active users, and WeChat has a billion
ing, they may still be willing to buy green products if they feel socially monthly active users in China (Atkinson, 2018). So, China is an appro-
influenced by others to do so (Brewer & Gardner, 1996). When con- priate test field to conduct research on advertising on social media.
sumers with highly interdependent self-construal perceive higher An online survey was conducted from mid-May to mid-June 2018 on
information utility from green advertising, they are more likely to fos- Sina Weibo, China's largest social networking site, to collect the data
ter an environmental attitude favoring green products, as green pur- from Chinese social media users. In order to increase the response
chases will benefit the environment and other people. Such rate, we prompted our research on the Sina website and on the cam-
consumers hold this attitude even though they will pay more for pus in a Chinese in Hefei, Anhui Province. We explicitly explained the
green purchases. In other words, consumers with interdependent self- research objectives and indicated in the questionnaire that, in this
construal tend to consider their decisions as not only concerning study, green products are any product or its production process that
them, and they pay great attention to others’ feelings in the decision- saves energy and causes no or minimal harm to, or benefits, the envi-
making process. Thus, Hypothesis 4 is posited: ronment and human health. Examples of green products are recycled
products made of recycled material and phosphate-free laundry deter-
Hypothesis 4 Interdependent self-construal positively moderates the gent. The respondents were informed that their names and personal
relationship between consumers' perceived information utility of details would never be revealed and that there were no right answers
green advertising on social media and green purchase intention, or wrong answers. A total of 1,015 adult social media users
such that the relationship is stronger when the level of volunteered to participate in the online research in exchange for
interdependent self-construal is high and weaker when the level 50 yuan (approximately 7 USD). In total, 1,012 questionnaires were
is low. received. Of these surveys, 327 had missing values; these incomplete
questionnaires were removed. A t test was conducted to examine the
Independent self-construal consumers focus on being indepen- potential nonresponse bias; the results showed no significant pattern
dent and different from others (Singelis, 1994). Consumers with highly between demographic factors and nonresponses. Finally, 685 valid
independent self-construal usually seek autonomy, emphasize them- questionnaires were retained for further analysis. Table 1 shows the
selves, and focus on fulfilling their own needs and desires. They tend demographic profiles of the respondents.
to pay more attention to their own preferences and pleasant experi- Almost half of the respondents were male (44.38%). 69.78% of
ences, so they may be unwilling to sacrifice their interests in order to respondents were between the ages of 20 and 40. 31.82% of respon-
cater to social preferences. In addition, they are more likely to want to dents had a high school degree. 68.76% of the respondents had a
be special in a group and are less likely to respond to social pressure. monthly income between 2,000 and 8,000 yuan ($285 to $1,138).
When consumers with highly independent self-construal are suspi- According to statistics from the China Internet Network Information
cious of green advertising, they will not be willing to buy green prod- Center (CINIC) from August 2018, 52.70% of social media users are
ucts to benefit others. Even when such consumers perceive higher male; 68% of social media users are under the age of 40; and those
information utility from green advertising, they may still be reluctant with a high school degree account for around 30%. About 61% of
LUO ET AL. 5

TABLE 1 Demographic profiles of the respondents Self-construal was measured using the eight items from Singelis

Percentage (1994), which include four items for interdependent self-construal and
Demographics Frequency (%) four items for independent self-construal. A sample item for
Gender interdependent self-construal is, “I would like to maintain harmony in

1. Male 304 44.38 my team on social media.” A sample item for independent self-
construal is, “I want to be different from others on social media.”
2. Female 381 55.62
Green purchase intention was measured using four items adapted
Age
from Ajzen and Fishbein (1980) and Sun et al. (2018). A sample item
1. Below 20 23 3.36
is, “I would like to buy green products from now on.”
2. 21–30 218 31.82
3. 31–40 260 37.96
4. 41 and over 183 26.72 3.2.1 | Control variables
Education
1. Junior middle school or below 27 3.36 A range of demographic variables, including gender, age, household
2. High school degree 88 31.82 income, and education, have been found to be related to consumer
3. Bachelor's degree 350 37.96 green purchase intention (e.g., Sun et al., 2018). We therefore con-
4. Master's degree/doctorate 220 16.50 trolled for these variables in the substantial analyses. In addition, there
Household income is social pressure from consumers' immediate peers on social media

1. Less than ¥2,000 ($291) 41 5.99 for them to buy green products (Ajzen, 1991). Some consumers may
have more connections with environmentally friendly individuals
2. ¥2,000–¥4,000 ($291–$583) 118 17.23
(which also depend on their own level of green consumerism), so their
3. ¥4,000–¥8,000 ($583–$1,166) 182 26.57
social pressures may be greater than for those with less such connec-
4. ¥8,000–¥12,000 171 24.96
tions. Thus, perceived social pressure for a green lifestyle was also
($1,166–$1,748)
controlled for. Perceived social pressure was measured using four
5. Over ¥12,000 ($1,748) 173 25.26
items adapted from Chen and Tung (2014), including, for example,
Total 685 100
“Most people close to me suggest that I should buy green products.”
We also controlled for green consumerism, which refers to consumers'
care for the environment when they buy and consume products; it is
social media users have a monthly income between 2,000 and 8,000 believed to affect consumer green purchase intention (Mainieri,
yuan ($285–$1,138), and those earning between 2,000 and 5,000 Barnett, Valdero, Unipan, & Oskamp, 1997) and their tendency to be
yuan ($285–$711) form the largest group (36.7%). Comparing our skeptical of green advertisements (do Paço & Reis, 2012). Four items
sample with the total social media users from the CINIC's data, the adapted from Gilg, Barr, and Ford (2005) were used to measure green
t test did not indicate significant differences in gender (t = −1.33, consumerism. A sample item is, “I always purchase recycled products
p > .05), age (t = 1.16, p > .05), household income (t = 0.23, p > .05) in my daily life.”
nor education (t = −0.54, p > .05). Thus, the t tests indicate that the
composition of our sample is very similar to that of the CINIC. Hence,
our sample is likely to be representative of the netizens1 in China 4 | DATA ANALYSIS AND RESULTS
overall.
4.1 | Measurement reliability and validity

3.2 | Measures AMOS 22.0 and SPSS 19.0 software packages were used to analyze
the data in this research. The results from the confirmative factor
Existing measures were used for all study constructs. Slight modifica- analysis (CFA) show that the overall fit indices of the measurement
tions were made to suit the current research on green purchases and model including the normed χ 2(254) = 2.38, p < .001, Goodness-of-
green advertisements. A five-point Likert-type scale (1 = greatly dis- fit index (GFI) = 0.93, Comparative Fit Index (CFI) = 0.96, Tucker-
agree; 5 = greatly agree) was used for all measures. Lewis Index (TLI) = 0.95, Parsimony Normed Fit Index (PNFI) = 0.77,
Green advertising skepticism was measured using four items Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) = 0.05 were
adapted (i.e., we added “on social media” to each item) from Mohr acceptable, indicating that the model had satisfactory properties.
et al. (1998). A sample item is, “Most green advertisements on social The reliability, discriminant validity, and convergence validity of the
media are exaggerated or false.” structure were evaluated. As shown in Table 2, the values of
Perceived information utility was measured using four items from Cronbach's alphas ranged between .74 and .89, all being above the
Matthes and Wonneberger (2014). A sample item is, “Green advertise- required threshold of .70 (Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994). The values
ments on social media help me make green purchase decisions.” of composite reliability were between 0.85 and 0.90, all being above
6 LUO ET AL.

TABLE 2 Confirmatory factor analysis

Construct Items Standard loading Cronbach's alpha value Composite reliability AVE
Green advertising skepticism (GAS) GAS1 0.78 .78 0.85 0.60
GAS2 0.78
GAS3 0.74
GAS3 0.78
Information utility (IU) IU1 0.78 .80 0.87 0.63
IU2 0.83
IU3 0.80
IU4 0.76
Interdependent self-construal (ITSC) ITSC1 0.89 .84 0.90 0.76
ITSC2 0.87
ITSC3 0.86
Independent self-construal (ISC) ISC1 0.85 .89 0.89 0.72
ISC2 0.84
ISC3 0.86
Green purchase intention (GPI) GPI1 0.78 .85 0.90 0.69
GPI2 0.84
GPI3 0.86
GPI4 0.85
Green consumerism (GC) GC1 0.82 .81 0.87 0.64
GC2 0.72
GC3 0.82
GC4 0.83
Perceived social pressure (PSP) PSP1 0.82 .74 0.85 0.66
PSP2 0.87
PSP3 0.74

Note: AVE means average variance extracted.

T A B L E 3 Means, SD, and


Means SD GAS PIU ITSC ISC GPI GC PSP
correlations (n = 685)
GAS 2.61 0.54 0.77
PIU 2.99 0.48 −0.48*** 0.79
ITSC 4.14 0.66 −0.30*** 0.27*** 0.87
ISC 3.57 0.58 −0.22*** 0.27*** 0.57*** 0.85
GPI 3.99 0.60 −0.42*** 0.49*** 0.72*** 0.59*** 0.83
GC 4.03 0.57 −0.25*** 0.23*** 0.65*** 0.48*** 0.58*** 0.80
PSP 3.94 0.64 −0.22*** 0.20*** 0.49*** 0.44*** 0.56*** 0.47*** 0.81

Note: GAS, green advertising skepticism; GC, green consumerism; GPI, green purchase intention; ISC, independent self-construal; ITSC, interdependent
self-construal; PIU, perceived information utility; PSP, perceived social pressure. The square roots of the AVEs are the bold elements.
*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.

the required threshold of 0.70 (Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994). This the measurements had good convergent validity. The CFA results
means that the measurements had good internal consistency reli- for the measurements are shown in Table 2.
ability. The values of standard loadings were between 0.72 and The square roots of AVE values are shown on the diagonal of
0.89, well above the required threshold of 0.70 (Hair, Black, Babin, Table 3. Discriminant validity was measured by comparing the correla-
Anderson, & Tatham, 2006). The values of average variance tion of each pair of constructs with the square roots of AVE. The
extracted (AVE) were between 0.60 and 0.76, well above the results showed that the measurements had good discriminant validity,
required threshold of 0.5 (Fornell & Larcker, 1981). This means that as the square roots of AVE were all higher than the correlation of each
LUO ET AL. 7

pair of constructs (Paulraj, Lado, & Chen, 2008). Hence, the measure- (β = .26, t = 4.68, p < .001) had a significant positive effect on green pur-
ments had good convergent validity. The statistics including means, chase intention. Moreover, both perceived social pressure (β = 0.19,
SD, and correlations of the study variables, excluding demographic t = 5.23 p < .001) and interdependent self-construal (β = .27, t = 5.50,
variables, are shown in Table 3. p < .001) had positive effects on green purchase intention. As men-
Because single-source data were used in this study, it is important tioned earlier, individuals with highly interdependent self-construal con-
to examine whether common method variance (CMV) was a problem. sider interpersonal connections to be critical and are more likely to act
We adopted the approach suggested by Harman (1976), to let all mea- in accordance with other people's expectations when they perceive
suring items load on an additional factor. The results showed that a social pressure (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). Hence, we examined
single factor accounted for 32% of the variance, which is lower than whether independent self-construal and social pressure interactively
the required threshold of 50%, indicating that CMV was limited in our affected green purchase intention. The results show that there was sig-
study. nificant positive interaction between interdependent self-construal and
perceived social pressure (β = 0.10, t = 2.96, p < .01).
Hypothesis 1 suggests that green advertising skepticism is nega-
4.2 | Hypothesis testing tively related to consumer green purchase intention. The results
showed that there is no significant relationship between green adver-
We tested the research hypotheses by performing structural equation tising skepticism and consumer green purchase intention (β = −.06,
modeling. To reduce the possibility of multicollinearity, all study vari- t = −1.27, p > .05), which means that Hypothesis 1 was not
ables were mean-centered when they were used to analyze the data. supported.
We first compared the full mediation model (Model 1) with the partial Hypothesis 2 proposes that green advertising skepticism is nega-
mediation model by adding a path from green advertising skepticism tively related to consumer perceptions of information utility. The ana-
and green purchase intention (Model 2). The results show that there lytical results indicated that green advertising skepticism had a
are no significant differences between Model 1 (χ 2[128] = 2.72, significant negative impact on perceived information utility (β = −.60,
p < .001, CFI = 0.96, TLI = 0.95, IFI = 0.96, PNFI = 0.79, RMSEA = 0.05) t = −11.01, p < .001), which means that Hypothesis 2 received
and Model 2 (χ 2[127] = 2.73, p < .001, CFI = 0.96, TLI = 0.95, support.
IFI = 0.96, PNFI = 0.78, RMSEA = 0.05), which indicates that the par- Hypothesis 3 suggests that perceived information utility mediates
tial mediation does not improve the model. Next, we compared Model the relationship between green advertising skepticism and consumer
1 with the direct effect model (Model 3), in which green advertising green purchase intention. The analytical results showed that per-
skepticism directly affects green purchase intention. The results indi- ceived information utility had a significant positive impact on green
cate that Model 1 fitted the data better than Model 3 (χ 2[52] =6.92, purchase intention (β = .38, t = 8.00, p < .001). The indirect effect of
p < .001, CFI = 0.90, TLI = 0.88, IFI = 0.90, PNFI = 0.70, green advertising skepticism on green purchase intention through the
RMSEA = 0.09). Therefore, Model 1 was the preferred model. As our mediation of perceived information utility was examined through
CFA results show, the model with moderating variables was also decomposition tests using the bootstrapping technique. The boo-
acceptable (χ 2[282] = 3.74, p < .05, CFI = 0.91, TLI = 0.90, IFI = 0.91, tstrapping (bootstrapping 2,000 times) results showed that the indi-
PNFI = 0.87, RMSEA = 0.06). Collinearity tests showed that the vari- rect effect was significant, with a 95% confidence interval
ance inflation factors were all less than 10.0, indicating that (CI) excluding 0 (β = −.23, p = .001, 95% CI = [−0.31, −0.17]). There-
multicollinearity was unlikely to be a serious problem in this research. fore, Hypothesis 3 was supported.
Gender, age, household income, and education had no significant Hypothesis 4 indicates that interdependent self-construal posi-
influence on green purchase intention. As predicted, green consumerism tively moderates the relationship between perceived information

F I G U R E 2 The moderating effect of


interdependent self-construal on the
relationship between information utility and
intention to buy green products
8 LUO ET AL.

F I G U R E 3 The moderating effect of


independent self-construal on the
relationship between information utility and
intention to buy green products

utility and green purchase intention. The results showed that and such distortions can reduce information utility (Goh & Balaji,
interdependent self-construal (β = .27, t = 5.50, p < .001) and the 2016; Schmuck et al., 2018). This study discusses how green advertis-
interaction term “perceived information utility × interdependent self- ing skepticism on social media affects perceived information utility,
construal” (β = .15, t = 2.79, p < .01) had significant positive effects on which in turn affects consumer green purchase intention.
green purchase intention. These results indicate that interdependent The results show that green advertising skepticism has no direct
self-construal had a direct effect on green purchase intention as well impact on consumer green purchase intention. Rather, this study finds
as positively moderating the relationship between perceived informa- that green advertising skepticism indirectly affects consumer green
tion utility and green purchase intention. The moderation effect is purchase intention via the mediation of perceived information utility.
depicted in Figure 2, which shows the different strengths of the rela- The information provided in green advertisements may be considered
tionship between perceived information utility and green purchase unreliable if consumers are suspicious of the credibility of green
intention when interdependent self-construal level was one SD above advertisements. Consumers with a high level of skepticism toward
and below its mean. Thus, Hypothesis 4 was confirmed. green advertising tend to perceive a low level of information utility.
Hypothesis 5 denotes that independent self-construal negatively The information utility of green advertisements is the basic informa-
moderates the relationship between perceived information utility and tion with which consumers make their purchase decisions. As is
green purchase intention. The results showed that independent self- supported by this study, perceived low levels of information utility
construal had a significant positive effect on green purchase intention have a negative consequence for green product purchase intention.
(β = .14, t = 3.50, p < .001) and that the interaction term between When consumers perceive the information in green advertisements as
information utility and independent self-construal was negatively useful and helpful, they are more likely to buy the green products.
related to green purchase intention (β = −0.13, t = −2.14, p < .05). Moreover, the interaction between information utility and
These results indicated that independent self-construal had a direct interdependent self-construal significantly positively affects green
effect on green purchase intention and negatively moderated the rela- purchase intention. Specifically, for a given level of information utility,
tionship between perceived information utility and green purchase consumers with higher interdependent self-construal will have higher
intention. As shown in Figure 3, when independent self-construal was levels of green purchase intention. This result is in line with the early
one SD above its mean, the effect of perceived information utility was studies that argue that interdependent self-construal is associated
weaker than when independent self-construal was one SD below its with empathy (Markus & Kitayama, 1991), or that consumers with
mean. Therefore, Hypotheses 5 was supported. interdependent self-construal act according to social norms and seek
acceptance within the group (Wang & Juslin, 2009). Moreover, per-
ceived information utility and independent self-construal interactively
5 | DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION and negatively affect green purchase intention. Specifically, con-
sumers with higher independent self-construal are more likely to be
Social media is an effective platform for green advertisements different from others, act on their own preferences (Singelis, 1994),
because consumers can choose their own sustainable development and tend not to buy green products.
lifestyle groups through interacting with others (Minton, Lee, Orth, Thus, the findings of this study demonstrate that people's green
Kim, & Kahle, 2012). Consumers' purchase decisions are largely based purchase choices depend to some degree on their perceptions of their
on their perceptions of information utility from green advertising. self and relationships with others. People with highly interdependent
However, green advertisements may be misleading or exaggerated, self-construal are more likely to sacrifice their own interests in favor
LUO ET AL. 9

of group harmony (Duclos & Barasch, 2014). People with high their social media contacts, rather than by themselves. Advertisers
interdependent self-construal are more likely to buy green products and marketers should leverage the social attributes of social media
when they believe green advertisements offer useful information. In and demonstrate how their green products can be part of a shared
contrast, those with high independent self-construal feel a need to be connection between friends. They should also actively encourage con-
different from others by not consuming green products, even if they sumers to share green advertisements and interact with their group.
believe that green products are good for society. This would raise green awareness and increase green purchase inten-
Overall, this study contributes to the marketing and advertising tion among the group.
literature by addressing, firstly, the underlying mechanism through The current marketing strategy for green products generally
which green advertising skepticism influences consumer green pur- emphasizes benefits to the environment. This strategy should be
chase intention, and secondly, the boundary conditions under which directed more toward people with high independent self-construal as
the strength of this relationship changes according to the characteris- this group of people pay more attention to their own interests, prefer-
tics of an individual's self-construal. ences, and abilities. It can be suggested that advertisers and marketers
target highly independent self-construal consumers with green adver-
tisement content covering how the product will directly benefit the
5.1 | Practical implications consumer's own interests, such as their personal health, achievement,
pride, and quality of life.
This study has considerable practical implications. First, our findings
suggest that green advertising skepticism discourages green buying
via perceived information utility. Hence, false, misleading, and exag- 5.2 | Limitations and future research directions
gerated green advertising has serious consequences. Thus, it is imper-
ative to combat false, misleading, and exaggerated green advertising. There are several limitations to be addressed. First, this research
Significant measures should be taken at multiple levels. To develop argues that green advertising skepticism indirectly affects consumer
consumer confidence in green advertising and increase green product green purchase intention via perceived information utility. However,
consumption, the government should strengthen the legal regulations because we used cross-sectional data, the finding regarding this indi-
on green advertising on social media. Industry authorities should rein- rect relationship may not be causal. This is as a relationship the other
force their scrutinization of the green advertising of individual firms. way may also exist, that is, perceived information utility influencing
At the enterprise level, advertisers and marketers should avoid using green purchase intention via advertising skepticism. Also, we only
misleading and exaggerated green claims in advertising and should included one mediator, that is, perceived information utility, in our
bear the consequences should they publish false or otherwise decep- conceptual model. There may exist other mediators that link green
tive green advertisements. Authentic advertising reduces green skep- advertising skepticism and consumers' green purchase intention.
ticism, which helps increase the perceived information utility and Future research may explore alternative underlying mechanisms. Sec-
purchasing intention of consumers. Marketers and advertisers should ond, this study focused only on consumers' perceived skepticism
regularly conduct customer skepticism surveys and consider consumer toward green advertising and did not consider the impact of green
feedback in designing green advertising. Marketers and advertisers advertising frequency on consumers' green purchase intention. It is
should disclose complete information relating to their green products possible that the increase in consumer exposure to green advertise-
on social media and communicate with consumers about the benefits ments will affect consumers' green purchase decisions. It is worth-
of using such products. Consumers also need to be educated to dis- while for future research to examine or control for the influence of
cern genuine green products from products falsely claimed to be green advertising frequency when exploring the green advertising-
green. Correspondingly, they should also share green knowledge and green buying relationship. Third, eco-labeling may have an impact on
experience on social media to educate and inform others. Also, when consumer green purchase intention. Future studies should explore or
consumers are confronted with false green advertisements, they control for this impact. Finally, this research was conducted in China,
should exercise their rights and denounce this advertising in order to where the institutional environment significantly differs from other
protect the interests of the broader society (Schmuck et al., 2018). markets. We suggest that cross-cultural research be conducted to
Secondly, for a given level of information utility, consumers with increase the generalizability of the research findings.
higher interdependent self-construal will have higher levels of green
purchase intention. Marketers and advertisers therefore should
understand the importance of group relationships to different con- 5.3 | Conclusions
sumers in order to tailor green advertisements accordingly. People
with high interdependent self-construal tend to maintain harmonious Marketers and advertisers increasingly promote green products on
relationships with their communities, and such relationships may be social media. As social media is less regulated, green advertising often
more pronounced when they are under great social pressures. Mar- lacks reliable information about the environmental attributes of prod-
keters should therefore help foster close relationships among social ucts. Such green advertisements may raise consumer skepticism of
media users. These consumers may acquire green information through greenwashing. This study finds that green advertising skepticism
10 LUO ET AL.

affects consumers’ green purchase intention via the mediation of per- hedonic motivation in impulse buying behavior. The International
ceived information utility. Interdependent self-construal positively Review of Retail Distribution and Consumer Research, 21(3), 233–249.
Chen, M., & Zhang, C. (2016). Framing corruption in the Chinese govern-
moderates and independent self-construal negatively moderates this
ment: A comparison of frames between media, government, and Neti-
indirect relationship. The research findings suggest that truthful and zens. International Journal of Communication, 10, 20.
consumer-tailored green advertising is crucial to green product Chen, M. F., & Tung, P. J. (2014). Developing an extended theory of
marketing. planned behavior model to predict consumers' intention to visit green
hotels. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 36, 221–230.
Chen, Y., & Moosmayer, D. C. (2020). When guilt is not enough:
ORCID interdependent self-construal as moderator of the relationship
Ying Sun https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1446-7125 between guilt and ethical consumption in a confucian context. Journal
of Business Ethics, 161(1).
Cross, S. E., Hardin, E. E., & Swing, B. G. (2011). The what, how, why, and where
ENDNOTE
of self-construal. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15(2), 142–179.
1
The term “Netizens” is defined by Chen and Zhang (2016) and China do Paço, A. M. F., & Reis, R. (2012). Factors affecting skepticism toward
Internet Network Information Center as those who are 6 years old or green advertising. Journal of Advertising, 41(4), 147–155.
above and have used the Internet in the past 6 months. In this study, we Duclos, R., & Barasch, A. (2014). Prosocial behaviour in intergroup rela-
sampled netizens who were aged 18 and older. tions: How donor self-construal and recipient group-membership
shape generosity. Journal of Consumer Research, 41(1), 93–108.
Elving, W. J. L. (2013). Scepticism and corporate social responsibility com-
RE FE R ENC E S
munications: the influence of fit and reputation. Journal of marketing
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behaviour. Organizational Behaviour communications, 19(4), 277–292.
and Human Decision Processes, 50(2), 179–211. Epstein, M. J., & Buhovac, A. R. (2014). Making sustainability work: Best
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Predicting and understanding consumer practices in managing and measuring corporate social, environmental, and
behaviour: Attitude-behaviour correspondence. In Understanding Atti- economic impacts. San Francisco, CA: Berrett-Koehler.
tudes and Predicting Social Behaviour (pp. 148–172). Englewood Cliffs, Fornell, C., & Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models
NJ: Prentice Hall Publishers. with unobservable variables and measurement error. Journal of Mar-
Ankit, G., & Mayur, R. (2013). Green marketing: Impact of green advertising keting Research, 18(1), 39–50.
on consumer purchase intention. Advances in Management, 6(9), 14–17. Gilg, A., Barr, S., & Ford, N. (2005). Green consumption or sustainable life-
Atkinson, L., & Kim, Y. (2015). “I drink it anyway and I know I shouldn't”: styles? Identifying the sustainable consumer. Futures, 37(6), 481–504.
Understanding green consumers' positive evaluations of norm- Goh, S. K., & Balaji, M. S. (2016). Linking green skepticism to green pur-
violating non-green products and misleading green advertising. Envi- chase behaviour. Journal of Cleaner Production, 100(131), 629–638.
ronmental Communication, 9(1), 37–57. Goi, M. T., Kalidas, V., & Zeeshan, M. (2014). Comparison of stimulus–
Atkinson, S. (2018). WeChat hits one billion monthly users—Are you one organism–response framework between international and local
of them? BBC. Retrieved from https://www.bbc.co.uk/news/business- retailer. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 130, 461–468.
43283690. Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., Anderson, R. E., & Tatham, R. L.
Bagozzi, R. P. (1986). Principles of marketing management. Chicago, IL: Sci- (2006). Multivariate data analysis (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ:
ence Research Associates. Pearson Education Inc.
Banerjee, S., Gulas, C. S., & Iyer, E. (1995). Shades of green: A multi- Harman, H. H. (1976). Modern factor analysis. Chicago IL: University of Chi-
dimensional analysis of environmental advertising. Journal of Advertis- cago Press.
ing, 24(2), 21–31. Hensel, K., & Deis, M. H. (2010). Using social media to increase advertising
Batra, R., & Ray, L. M. (1986). Affective responses mediating acceptance and improve marketing. The Entrepreneurial Executive, 15, 87–97.
of advertising. Journal of Consumer Research, 13(2), 234–249. Hoffman, D. L., & Novak, T. P. (2012). Toward a deeper understanding of
Baum, L. M. (2012). It's not easy being green … or is it? A content analysis social media. Journal of Interactive Marketing, 2(26), 69–70.
of environmental claims in magazine advertisements from the United Hu, M., Zhang, M., & Luo, N. (2016). Understanding participation on video
States and United Kingdom. Environmental Communication: A Journal sharing communities: The role of self-construal and community inter-
of Nature and Culture, 6(4), 423–440. activity. Computers in Human Behavior, 62, 105–115.
Benn, S., Dunphy, D., & Griffiths, A. (2014). Organizational change for cor- Jacoby, J. (2002). Stimulus–organism–response reconsidered: An evolu-
porate sustainability. Oxford: Routledge. tionary step in modeling (consumer) behavior. Journal of Consumer Psy-
Bickart, B. A., & Ruth, J. A. (2012). Green eco-seals and advertising persua- chology, 12(1), 51–57.
sion. Journal of Advertising, 41(4), 51–67. Kim, K. H., & Yun, H. (2007). Cying for me, cying for us: Relational dialec-
Bloom, P. N., & Reve, T. (1990). Transmitting signals to consumers for tics in a Korean social network site. Journal of Computer-Mediated
competitive advantage. Business Horizons, 33(4), 58–67. Communication, 13(1), 298–318.
Boulding, W., & Kirmani, A. (1993). A consumer-side experimental exami- Kim, M. S., Hunter, J. E., Miyahara, A., Horvath, A. M., Bresnahan, M., &
nation of signaling theory: Do consumers perceive warranties as sig- Yoon, H. J. (1996). Individual-vs. culture-level dimensions of individu-
nals of quality? Journal of Consumer Research, 20(1), 111–123. alism and collectivism: Effects on preferred conversational styles. Com-
Brettel, M., Reich, J. C., Gavilanes, J. M., & Flatten, T. C. (2015). What munications Monographs, 63(1), 29–49.
drives advertising success on Facebook? An advertising-effectiveness Leonidou, C. N., & Skarmeas, D. (2015). Gray shades of green: causes and
model: Measuring the effects on sales of “likes” and other social- consequences of green skepticism. Journal of Business Ethics, 1–15.
network stimuli. Journal of Advertising Research, 55(2), 162–175. Lin, J., Luo, Z., Cheng, X., & Li, L. (2019). Understanding the interplay of
Brewer, M. B., & Gardner, W. (1996). Who is this “we”? Levels of collective social commerce affordances and swift Guanxi: An empirical study.
identity and self representations. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Information & Management, 56(2), 213–224.
chology, 71(1), 83–93. Liu, H., Chu, H., Huang, Q., & Chen, X. (2016). Enhancing the flow experi-
Chang, H. J., Eckman, M., & Yan, R. N. (2011). Application of the stimulus– ence of consumers in China through interpersonal interaction in social
organism–response model to the retail environment: The role of commerce. Computers in Human Behavior, 58, 306–314.
LUO ET AL. 11

Mainieri, T., Barnett, E. G., Valdero, T. R., Unipan, J. B., & Oskamp, S. Sun, Y., Wang, S., Gao, L., & Li, J. (2018). Unearthing the effects of person-
(1997). Green buying: The influence of environmental concern on con- ality traits on consumer's attitude and intention to buy green products.
sumer behavior. The Journal of Social Psychology, 137(2), 189–204. Natural Hazards, 93(1), 299–314.
Manuel, E., Youn, S., & Yoon, D. (2014). Functional matching effect in Tan, B. C. (2011). The roles of knowledge, threat, and PCE on green purchase
CRM: Moderating roles of perceived message quality and skepticism. behaviour. International Journal of Business and Management, 6(12), 14.
Journal of Marketing Communications, 20(6), 397–418. Vermeir, I., & Verbeke, W. (2006). Sustainable food consumption: Explor-
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for ing the consumer “attitude–behavioural intention” gap. Journal of Agri-
cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), cultural and Environmental Ethics, 19(2), 169–194.
224–253. Wang, L., & Juslin, H. (2009). The impact of Chinese culture on corporate
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (2010). Cultures and selves: A cycle of mutual social responsibility: The harmony approach. Journal of Business Ethics,
constitution. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(4), 420–430. 88(3), 433–451.
Matsumoto, D. (1999). Culture and self: An empirical assessment of Mar- Weber, T., Baier, K., & Willers, C. (2015). Sustainable (green) food and pur-
kus and Kitayama's theory of independent and interdependent self- chase intention—An analysis of influence factors. International Journal on
construals. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 2(3), 289–310. Advanced Science. Engineering and Information Technology, 5(4), 311–313.
Matthes, J., & Wonneberger, A. (2014). The skeptical green consumer Wei, C. F., Chiang, C. T., Kou, T. C., & Lee, B. C. (2017). Toward sustainable
revisited: Testing the relationship between green consumerism and livelihoods: Investigating the drivers of purchase behaviour for green
skepticism toward advertising. Journal of Advertising, 43(2), 115–127. products. Business Strategy and the Environment, 26(5), 626–639.
Matthes, J., Wonneberger, A., & Schmuck, D. (2014). Consumers' green Zhang, L., Li, D., Cao, C., & Huang, S. (2018). The influence of greenwash-
involvement and the persuasive effects of emotional versus functional ing perception on green purchasing intentions: The mediating role of
ads. Journal of Business Research, 67(9), 1885–1893. green word-of-mouth and moderating role of green concern. Journal
Mehrabian, A., & Russell, J. A. (1974). An approach to environmental psy- of Cleaner Production, 187, 740–750.
chology. Cambridge: M.I.T. Press. Zhou, Z., Zhang, Q., Su, C., & Zhou, N. (2012). How do brand communities
Mikalef, P., Giannakos, M., & Pateli, A. (2013). Shopping and word-of- generate brand relationships? Intermediate mechanisms. Journal of
mouth intentions on social media. Journal of Theoretical and Applied Business Research, 65(7), 890–895.
Electronic Commerce Research, 8(1), 17–34. Zhu, B. (2013). The impact of green advertising on consumer purchase inten-
Minton, E., Lee, C., Orth, U., Kim, C. H., & Kahle, L. (2012). Sustainable tion of green products. World Review of Business Research, 3(3), 72–78.
marketing and social media: A cross-country analysis of motives for
sustainable behaviours. Journal of Advertising, 41(4), 69–84.
Mohr, L. A., Erog lu, D., & Ellen, P. S. (1998). The development and testing
AUTHOR BIOGRAPHIES
of a measure of skepticism toward environmental claims in marketers'
communications. Journal of Consumer Affairs, 32(1), 30–55.
Morel, K. P., & Pruyn, A. T. H. (2003). Consumer skepticism toward new Biao Luo is a professor of School of Management, Hefei Univer-
products. European Advances in Consumer Research, 6, 351–358.
sity of Technology and his primary research interests are con-
Nunnally, J. C., & Bernstein, I. H. (1994). The assessment of reliability. Psy-
chometric Theory, 3(1), 248–292. sumer behavior and marketing.
Paulraj, A., Lado, A. A., & Chen, I. J. (2008). Inter-organizational communi-
Ying Sun is a PhD student of School of Management, University
cation as a relational competency: Antecedents and performance out-
comes in collaborative buyer–supplier relationships. Journal of of Science and Technology of China and her primary research
Operations Management, 26(1), 45–64. interests are green consumption and green marketing.
Schmuck, D., Matthes, J., & Naderer, B. (2018). Misleading consumers with
green advertising? An effect–reason–involvement account of green- Jie Shen is a professor of Management in Shenzhen Audencia
washing effects in environmental advertising. Journal of Advertising, 47 Business School (SABS), Shenzhen University, and his primary
(2), 127–145. research interests are green strategy management and green
Schuitema, G., & de Groot, J. I. (2015). Green consumerism: The influence
human resources management.
of product attributes and values on purchasing intentions. Journal of
Consumer Behaviour, 14(1), 57–69. Lihui Xia is a PhD student of School of Management, University
Segev, S., Fernandes, J., & Hong, C. (2016). Is your product really green? A
of Science and Technology of China and her primary research
content analysis to reassess green advertising. Journal of Advertising,
45(1), 85–93. interests are green strategy management and sustainable
Shrum, L. J., McCarty, J. A., & Lowrey, T. M. (1995). Buyer characteristics development.
of the green consumer and their implications for advertising strategy.
Journal of Advertising, 24(2), 71–82.
Sin, S. S., Nor, K. M., & Al-Agaga, A. M. (2012). Factors affecting Malaysian
young consumers’ online purchase intention in social media websites.
Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 40, 326–333. How to cite this article: Luo B, Sun Y, Shen J, Xia L. How does
Singelis, T. M. (1994). The measurement of independent and green advertising skepticism on social media affect consumer
interdependent self-construals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulle-
intention to purchase green products? J Consumer Behav.
tins, 20(5), 580–591.
Singelis, T. M., & Sharkey, W. F. (1995). Culture, self-construal, and embar- 2020;1–11. https://doi.org/10.1002/cb.1818
rassability. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26(6), 622–644.

You might also like