Intentional Behavior: Martin Fishbein

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

a0005

Intentional Behavior
Martin Fishbein
Annenberg School for Communication, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA

1. Introduction meta-analysis A statistical procedure to quantify and sum-


2. Defining Intentions marize results from a number of different studies.
3. Intentions as Predictors of Behavior self-efficacy One’s perception that he or she can perform a
4. Moderators of the Intention–Behavior Relation given behavior, even in the face of various obstacles.
5. An Integrated Theoretical Model subjective norm One’s perception of what important others
6. Determinants of Behavior think he or she should (or should not) do.
7. Determinants of Intention
8. Determinants of Attitudes, Norms, and Self-Efficacy
9. The Role of Distal Variables Although it has been argued that many behaviors are
10. Conclusion performed automatically, without reflection on or
Further Reading
awareness of intention, it would be hard to argue that
most behaviors of interest to an applied psychologist
are either automatic or habitual. In fact, most people
GLOSSARY can quickly and easily report the likelihood that they
will or will not perform given behaviors, and when
attitude One’s overall feeling of favorableness or unfavor- these measures of intention (or self-prediction) are
ableness with respect to a given object or action. assessed appropriately, they are highly related to
behavior A directly observable action performed with re- actual behavioral performance.
spect to some target, in some context, at some point in
time.
behavioral categories Broad categories of behavior (e.g.,
prejudice, exercising, dieting) that are inferred from the 1. DEFINING BEHAVIOR s0005

observation of one or more behaviors.


behavioral intention A readiness to engage in a particular Although behavior is often taken as a given, it is im-
behavior. portant to distinguish among behaviors, behavioral
behavioral norm One’s perception of whether important
categories, and goals. All too often, investigators fail
others are (or are not) performing a given behavior.
to distinguish between behaviors per se and occur-
correspondence The principle that measures of behavioral
determinants and behavior involve exactly the same ac- rences that may be the outcome of those behaviors.
tion, target, context, and time elements. For example, weight loss and success (or failure) on an
goals Desired conditions, occurrences, or states that may be exam have often been used as behavioral criteria.
outcomes of performing one or more behaviors (e.g., los- Unfortunately, neither of these is a behavior. Success
ing weight, staying healthy, getting an A on an exam). on an exam is a possible outcome of specific actions

Encyclopedia of Applied Psychology, 329 #2004 Elsevier Inc.


VOLUME 2 All rights reserved.
330 Intentional Behavior

such as attending lectures, reading books, memorizing their local supermarket [context] during the past 2
materials, and even copying answers from another weeks [time]).
person’s test paper. Another problem is that investiga-
tors often treat inferences from behaviors as if they
themselves were behaviors. For example, aggression,
discrimination, dieting, and exercising have often been 2. DEFINING INTENTIONS s0010

viewed as behavioral criteria. Unfortunately, none of


these is a directly observable behavior. Instead, these Intentions can be conceptualized as a readiness to en-
are broad behavioral categories that are inferred from gage in a particular behavior. This readiness to act can
the observation of one or more behaviors. That is, be assessed by asking people to indicate the extent to
although it is possible to observe specific actions that which they agree (or disagree) with statements such as
are assumed to be instances of a general class or cate- ‘‘I will engage in the behavior,’’ ‘‘I intend to engage in
gory of behavior, the category per se cannot be directly the behavior,’’ ‘‘I expect to engage in the behavior,’’
observed. Thus, knowing that someone was ‘‘exercis- ‘‘I am willing to engage in the behavior,’’ and ‘‘I will try
ing,’’ for example, might reveal very little about the to engage in the behavior.’’ A number of investigators,
specific ‘‘exercise’’ behaviors that the person did (or however, have proposed that some of these items assess
did not) perform. distinct constructs. For example, it has been suggested
In contrast to goals (or outcomes) and behavioral that it is important to distinguish between intention
categories, behaviors are directly observable actions. and behavioral expectation. More specifically, it was
Note, however, that the direct observation of any be- hypothesized that behavioral expectations are better
havior always occurs in a given context at a given predictors of behavior than are behavioral intentions
point in time. This implies that both context and because the former are more likely to take into account
time should be included in the definition of a behav- possible impediments to performance of the behavior.
ior. Moreover, because most actions are directed at Thus, items such as ‘‘I intend to . . .,’’ ‘‘I will try to . . .,’’
some object or target, a behavioral definition should and ‘‘I plan to . . .’’ have been used to assess intentions
involve four elements: the action (e.g., enlisting, buy- and items such as ‘‘I expect to . . .’’ and ‘‘I will . . .’’ to
ing, using), the target (e.g., the army, cigarettes, con- assess behavioral expectations. Available evidence to
doms), the context (e.g., after graduating high school, date suggests that there is little to be gained by this
at the supermarket, for vaginal sex with one’s spouse), proposed distinction. For example, a meta-analysis of a
and the time (e.g., yesterday, during the past month). broad set of behaviors found no difference in the
Clearly, a change in any one of the elements changes predictive validity of expectations and intentions.
the behavior under consideration. For example, enlist- Thus, this article uses the term ‘‘behavioral intention’’
ing in the army is a different behavior from enlisting in to refer to measures that assess one’s readiness to en-
the navy (a change in target), using a condom for gage in a specific behavior. In addition, it should
vaginal sex with one’s spouse is a different behavior be noted that people may also form intentions to en-
from using a condom for vaginal sex with a new part- gage in a behavioral category (e.g., to exercise) or to
ner (a change in context), and asking someone pursue some goal (e.g., to lose weight).
whether he or she purchased a car during the past
3 months is assessing a different behavior from asking
whether someone purchased a car during the past
2 years. 3. INTENTIONS AS PREDICTORS s0015

Although all four elements should be considered in OF BEHAVIOR


arriving at one’s behavioral criterion, the level of specif-
icity or generality used to define each element should Empirical research over the past three decades or so
be determined by the substantive questions being has led to the recognition that behaviors can be pre-
asked. Thus, a behavior may be assessed at a fairly dicted with considerable accuracy by appropriately
general level (e.g., people could be asked whether assessing intentions to engage in the behaviors under
they have ever [time] purchased [action] cigarettes consideration. For a measure of intention to correlate
[targets] [context unspecified]) or at a more specific closely with behavior, the intention measure must in-
level (e.g., people could be asked whether they had volve exactly the same elements as does the behavior
purchased [action] Marlboro Light 100s [target] from itself.
Intentional Behavior 331
s0020
3.1. The Principle or exercise), but the behavioral criterion is a measure
of Correspondence of whether they have or have not performed one or
more specific behaviors that are assumed to be a good
The requirement that measures of intention and behav- indicant of the category. With respect to goal attain-
ior involve exactly the same action, target, context, and ment, it should be clear that achieving a goal often
time elements is known as the principle of correspon- requires more than a behavioral performance on the
dence or compatibility. The principle of correspon- part of an individual. Achieving a goal often depends
dence is sometimes misunderstood to mean that on the decisions or actions of other people or on the
intentions and behaviors should always be defined occurrence of certain events. To put this somewhat
and measured at a very specific level. In reality, the differently, goal (or outcome) attainment is often not
principle simply suggests that the two variables— completely under an individual’s control, and the less
intention and behavior—should be measured at equiv- control one has over attaining some goal, the lower the
alent levels of generality or specificity. If, for whatever intention–goal attainment relation. In the same sense,
reason, investigators are interested in a very specific one could argue that the less a given behavior is under
behavior performed in a particular context and at a one’s control, the lower the intention–behavior rela-
given point in time, the intention should be assessed tionship. Fortunately, as pointed out earlier, most
at that same level of specificity. However, investigators human behaviors (and certainly most that are of inter-
might not be interested in a particular context or time, est to an applied psychologist) are under volitional
and they can generalize across these elements in their control. Nevertheless, people do not always act in
measures of behavior and in their measures of inten- accordance with their intentions.
tion. To the extent that the assessments of intention
and behavior comply with the principle of correspon-
dence, they should correlate highly with each other.
Many studies have substantiated the predictive
4. MODERATORS OF THE s0025

validity of behavioral intentions. When measured INTENTION–BEHAVIOR RELATION


appropriately, behavioral intentions account for an ap-
preciable proportion of variance in actual behavior. Perhaps the two major factors that prevent one from
Meta-analyses covering diverse behavioral domains acting on his or her intentions are (a) a lack of neces-
have reported mean intention–behavior correlations sary skills and abilities and (b) the presence of envi-
ranging from .45 to .62. Studies in specific behavioral ronmental constraints (or the absence of environmental
domains, such as condom use and exercise, have pro- facilitators). Other factors may also account for
duced similar results, with intention–behavior correla- observed low intention–behavior relations. As dis-
tions ranging from .44 to .56. A meta-analysis of these cussed previously, low correlations may be due to a
and other meta-analyses reported an overall correla- lack of correspondence between the measures of inten-
tion of .53 between intention and behavior. tion and behavior. In addition, intentions may change
Thus, there is considerable evidence that the perfor- over time; the longer the time interval between the
mance or nonperformance of a given behavior is deter- assessment of intention and the assessment of behav-
mined primarily by the strength of a person’s intention ior, the greater the likelihood that the intention will
to perform (or to not perform) that behavior, where change. This implies that the longer the interval be-
intention is defined as the subjective likelihood that tween the assessment of intention and the observa-
one will perform (or try to perform) the behavior in tion of behavior, the lower the intention–behavior
question. In contrast to the strong relationship be- correlation.
tween behavioral intention and behavior, however,
intentions to reach goals and intentions to engage in
behavioral categories might not provide very good 5. AN INTEGRATED THEORETICAL s0030

prediction of goal attainment or the performance of a MODEL


specific behavior within the behavioral category. In the
latter case, the problem is largely methodological and Although there are many theories of behavioral predic-
in many ways is nothing more than a case of failure to tion and behavior change in the literature (e.g., the
obtain correspondence. That is, people are asked their theory of planned behavior, the theory of subjective
intentions to engage in a class of behaviors (e.g., to diet culture and interpersonal relations, the transtheoretical
332 Intentional Behavior

model of behavior change, the information/motivation/ efficacy or personal agency that involves the individ-
behavioral skills model, the health belief model, social ual’s perception that he or she can perform the behav-
cognitive theory, the theory of reasoned action), a care- ior in question under a variety of difficult or
ful consideration of these theories suggests that there are challenging circumstances. These and other consid-
only a limited number of variables that need to be erations suggest the following integrative model of
considered in predicting and understanding any given behavioral prediction (Fig. 1).
behavior.

s0035
5.1. Attitudes, Perceived Norms, 6. DETERMINANTS OF BEHAVIOR s0040

and Self-Efficacy In Fig. 1, it can be seen that any given behavior is most
In general, one can identify three factors that may likely to occur if one has a strong intention to perform
directly influence an individual’s intentions and behav- the behavior, if one has the necessary skills and abil-
iors: (a) the individual’s attitude toward performing ities required to perform the behavior, and if there are
the behavior (i.e., the person’s overall positive or no environmental constraints preventing behavioral
negative feelings of favorableness or unfavorableness performance. Indeed, if one has made a strong commit-
with respect to performing the behavior) that is based ment (or has formed a strong intention) to perform a
on the person’s beliefs that performing the behavior given behavior, if one has the necessary skills and
will lead to various positive or negative consequences abilities to perform the behavior, and if there are no
(or outcomes), (b) perceived normative pressure that environmental constraints to prevent the performance
includes the perception that the individual’s impor- of that behavior, the probability is very high that the
tant others think that one should (or should not) behavior will be performed.
perform the behavior in question as well as the per- In some populations or cultures, the behavior might
ception that these important others are (or are not) not be performed because people have not yet formed
themselves performing the behavior, and (c) self- intentions to perform the behavior. In other populations,

Distal
Variables
Past behavior
Demographics Behavioral
& culture beliefs & Environmental
outcome Attitudes constraints
Attitudes evaluations
toward
targets
(stereotypes &
stigma)
Normative
Personality, beliefs &
Norms Intention
moods, & motivation to Behavior
emotions comply

Other
individual Control
difference beliefs & Skills &
Self-efficacy
variables perceived abilities
(perceived risk) power

Intervention
exposure
Media
exposure

f0005 FIGURE 1 An integrative model of behavioral prediction.


Intentional Behavior 333

the problem might be a lack of skills and/or the presence performing) the behavior. Finally, the more one per-
of environmental constraints. In still other populations, ceives that he or she can (i.e., has the necessary skills
more than one of these factors may be relevant. Clearly, and abilities to) perform the behavior even in the face of
if people have formed intentions but are not acting on specific barriers or obstacles, the stronger one’s self-
them, behavioral performance will depend on enhancing efficacy will be with respect to performing the behavior.
their skills and abilities or on removing (or learning to It is at this level of underlying beliefs that the sub-
overcome) environmental constraints. stantive uniqueness of each behavior comes into play.
Clearly, a change in any one element in the behavioral
definition will usually lead to very different beliefs about
the consequences of performing that behavior, about the
s0045
7. DETERMINANTS OF INTENTION
expectations of relevant others, and about the barriers
that may impede behavioral performance and, thus,
On the other hand, if strong intentions to perform the
will usually lead to very different attitudes, subjective
behavior in question have not been formed, the model
norms, and perceptions of self-efficacy and intentions.
suggests that there are three primary determinants of
For example, the barriers to, and the outcomes (or
intention: the attitude toward performing the behavior,
consequences) of, always using a condom for vaginal
perceived norms concerning performance of the behav-
sex with an occasional partner may be very different
ior, and one’s self-efficacy with respect to performing the
from those associated with always using a condom for
behavior. It is important to recognize that the relative
vaginal sex with one’s spouse. Yet it is these specific
importance of these three psychosocial variables as deter-
beliefs that must be considered if one wishes to fully
minants of intention will also depend on both the behav-
understand intentions and behavior. Although an inves-
ior and the population being considered. For example,
tigator can sit in her or his office and develop measures
one behavior may be determined primarily by attitudinal
of attitudes, perceived norms, and self-efficacy, the
considerations, whereas another behavior may be influ-
investigator cannot know what a given population (or
enced primarily by feelings of self-efficacy. Similarly, a
a given person) believes about performing a given
behavior that is attitudinally driven in one population or
behavior. Thus, one must go to members of that pop-
culture may be normatively driven in another population
ulation to identify salient outcome, normative, and eff-
or culture. Thus, to understand intentional behavior, it is
icacy beliefs. To put this somewhat differently, one must
important to first determine the degree to which that
understand the behavior from the perspective of the
intention is under attitudinal, normative, or self-efficacy
population that one is considering.
control in the population in question.

9. THE ROLE OF DISTAL VARIABLES s0055

s0050
8. DETERMINANTS OF ATTITUDES,
NORMS, AND SELF-EFFICACY Finally, Fig. 1 also shows the role played by more
traditional demographic, economic, personality, attitu-
The model in Fig. 1 also recognizes that attitudes, dinal, and other individual difference variables (e.g.,
perceived norms, and self-efficacy are themselves func- perceived risk, sensation seeking). Although it is clear
tions of underlying beliefs—about the outcomes of that these types of variables may often be important
performing the behavior in question, about the norma- determinants of a given behavior, the model suggests
tive proscriptions and/or behaviors of specific refer- that these types of variables play primarily an indirect
ents, and about specific barriers to behavioral role in influencing behavior. That is, these ‘‘back-
performance.Thus, the more one believes that per- ground’’ factors may or may not influence the behav-
forming the behavior in question will lead to ‘‘good’’ ioral, normative, or self-efficacy beliefs underlying
outcomes and prevent ‘‘bad’’ outcomes, the more favor- attitudes, norms, or self-efficacy. For example, al-
able one’s attitude toward performing the behavior though men and women may hold different beliefs
should be. Similarly, the more one believes that spe- about performing some behaviors, they may hold very
cific others are performing the behavior and/or the similar beliefs with respect to performing other behav-
more these others think that one should (or should iors. Similarly, rich and poor, old and young, those
not) perform the behavior in question, the more social from developing countries and those from developed
pressure one will feel with respect to performing (or not countries, those who plan to go to college and those
334 Intentional Behavior

who do not, those with favorable attitudes toward law See Also the Following Articles
enforcement and those with unfavorable attitudes
Self-Control
toward law enforcement, those who are happy and
those who are sad or angry, those who have used drugs
and those who have not—all may hold different attitu- Further Reading
dinal, normative, or self-efficacy beliefs with respect to
one behavior but may hold similar beliefs with respect to Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational
another behavior. In other words, there is no necessary Behavior & Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211.
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude–behavior rela-
relation between these ‘‘distal’’ or ‘‘background’’ variables
tions: A theoretical analysis and review of empirical
and any given behavior. Nevertheless, distal variables,
research. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 888–918.
such as cultural and personality differences, moods and Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control.
emotions, and differences in a wide range of values, New York: Freeman.
should be reflected in the underlying belief structure. Becker, M. H. (1974). The health belief model and per-
sonal health behavior. Health Education Monographs,
2, 324–508.
s0060
10. CONCLUSION Fishbein, M. (2000). The role of theory in HIV prevention.
AIDS Care, 12, 273–278.
In general, most human behavior is intentional; that is, Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention,
people typically perform behaviors that they intend (or and behavior: An introduction to theory and research.
Reading, MA: Addison–Wesley.
plan) to perform, and they do not perform behaviors
Fishbein, M., Triandis, H. C., Kanfer, F. H., Becker, M.,
they do not intend to perform. In addition, they usually
Middlestadt, S. E., & Eichler, A. (2001). Factors influen-
intend to perform (or not perform) a behavior for one cing behavior and behavior change. In A. Baum, & T. A.
or more of three reasons: they think that performing Revenson (Eds.), Handbook of health psychology. Mahwah,
the behavior is a ‘‘good’’ thing to do, they feel strong NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
social pressure to perform the behavior, or they believe Fisher, J. D., & Fisher, W. A. (1992). Changing AIDS-risk
that they have the necessary skills and abilities to per- behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 455–474.
form the behavior. These attitudes, perceived norms Petraitis, J., Flay, B. R., & Miller, T. Q. (1995). Reviewing
(or social pressure), and feelings of efficacy are them- theories of adolescent substance use: Organizing pieces in
selves determined by underlying beliefs—about the the puzzle. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 67–86.
expected outcomes of performing the behavior, about Prochaska, J. O., & DiClemente, C. C. (1992). Stages of
change in the modification of problem behaviors.
the expectations and behaviors of others, and about
Progress in Behavior Modification, 28, 184–218.
their ability to overcome specific barriers that could
Sheeran, P. (2002). Intention–behavior relations: A concep-
impede behavioral performance. The full understand- tual and empirical review. In W. Stroebe, & M. Hewstone
ing of the performance or nonperformance of a given (Eds.), European review of social psychology (Vol. 12,
behavior ultimately rests on understanding which of pp. 1–36). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
these underlying beliefs is (are) responsible for the Triandis, H. C. (1972). The analysis of subjective culture.
performance or nonperformance of that behavior. New York: John Wiley.

You might also like