Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Rate of Habituation and Visual Memory in Infants

Author(s): Judy S. DeLoache


Source: Child Development, Vol. 47, No. 1 (Mar., 1976), pp. 145-154
Published by: Wiley on behalf of the Society for Research in Child Development
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1128293 .
Accessed: 17/07/2013 08:59

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Wiley and Society for Research in Child Development are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and
extend access to Child Development.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 152.3.102.242 on Wed, 17 Jul 2013 08:59:53 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Rate of Habituationand Visual Memory
in Infants

Judy S. DeLoache
University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign

DELOACHE, JUDY S. Rateof Habituationand VisualMemoryin Infants.CHLD DEVELOPMENT,


1976, 47, 145-154. This study investigated 17-week-old infants' response to discrepancy as a
function of rate of habituation. 36 subjects were repeatedly shown a pattern containing 4
geometric shapes until they reached a proportional criterion of habituation: each infant's fix-
ation time had to decrease by at least 50%. On the basis of 2 nonoverlapping distributions of
trials to criterion, fast and slow habituators were identified. Recovery of fixation time was
found for both fast and slow habituators when a discrepant stimulus was presented following
habituation. These data were interpreted as reflecting developmental differences in encoding.
After several interpolated trials with the new pattern, the infants' response to the original
habituation stimulus increased, suggesting that the interpolated trials had interfered with their
memory for the habituation stimulus.

The habituation paradigm has provided Gurk 1972), condition at birth, later IQ (Lewis
invaluable access to the phenomena of infant 1967), discrimination learning (Gelber 1972b),
perception and memory (Jeffrey & Cohen and complexity preference (Greenberg, O'Don-
1971). In visual attention studies infants are nell, & Crawford 1973).
repeatedly shown the same pattern, and typi-
cally their fixation time decreases over trials. Lewis (1967) and McCall (1971) have
This decline in attention (habituation) is gen-
speculated that fast habituators might be su-
erally thought to reflect the acquisition of an perior or more efficient information processors.
internal representation or memory model of the This view is based in part on several experi-
stimulus (Cohen 1973; Lewis 1967; McCall ments (McCall & Kagan 1970; McCall & Mel-
1971; Sokolov 1963). Presumably, the subject son 1969; Melson & McCall 1970) in which
compares each incoming stimulus to his mem- fast habituators recovered (i.e., increased their
ory model of past stimuli. When the two match, fixation time) when a discrepant stimulus was
attention is inhibited; if the present stimulus
is discrepant from the model, the subject re- presented, while slow habituators tended not
to respond differentially. These data have been
sponds positively to it (recovery).
interpreted as evidence that the slow habitu-
ators had poorer or less complete memory
The primary focus of this experiment con-
models; they did not discriminate between the
cerned individual differences in rate of habitua- familiar and novel stimuli because their mem-
tion. In any visual habituation study, some
ory model of the familiar one was inadequate.
subjects habituate very quickly while others
continue to look at the stimulus throughout the In these studies, each subject experienced
session. Rate of habituation has been shown to a fixed number of familiarization trials. Since
correlate positively with age (Lewis 1967; Mc- by definition the slow habituators did not
This study is based on a dissertation submitted to the University of Illinois at Urbana-
Champaign in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the doctoral degree. This research
was supported in part by grant HD-03858 from the National Institute of Child Health and
Human Development to Leslie B. Cohen. The author wishes to express appreciation to Dr.
Cohen for his helpful guidance and support, to Gerald L. Clore for his suggestions and criti-
cisms of the manuscript, and to Margaret Wetherford Rissman for preparation of the figures
and other assistance. The author is also at Illinois State Pediatric Institute, Chicago. Requests
for reprints should be addressed to the author at Children's Research Center, University of
Illinois, Champaign, Illinois 61820.
[Child Development, 1976, 47, 145-154. @ 1976 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
All rights reserved.]

This content downloaded from 152.3.102.242 on Wed, 17 Jul 2013 08:59:53 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
146 Child Development
habituate during those trials, the question re- having habituated, or even as being fast habit-
mains whether they would eventually habituate uators, without ever decreasing their looking
if given more experience with the standard. In time. Probably because the standard stimulus
other words, are slow habituators incapable of was extremely simple (an arrow in either a
storing stimulus information, or are they simply horizontal or vertical orientation), many sub-
slower to do so? If they do eventually process jects appear to have been at or near the 3-sec
and store the information, do they form as criterion even at the start of the experiment.
accurate and complete a model as fast habitu- In fact, only 17 of the 120 subjects in the ex-
ators? periment looked longer on their first than on
their last familiarization trial. Thus, the dif-
The present study was designed to answer ferentiation of fast and slow habituators was
these questions. To do so, it was essential that somewhat problematical.
all infants be allowed ample opportunity to
habituate. Therefore, instead of giving the sub- The proportional criterion used in the
jects a fixed number of trials, a proportional present study avoids such difficulties. No infant
criterion of habituation was used. Each infant could be judged to have habituated without
was presented with a pattern repeatedly until actually decreasing his response from its orig-
his or her fixation time decreased by one-half inal level. Furthermore, the relative amount of
or more of its original level. Thus, the infants decrement shown by all subjects was equiva-
could take any number of trials to reach cri- lent. Various criteria of habituation are dis-
terion, but they were equated for relative cussed in more detail in Cohen and Gelber
amount of habituation. Each subject was then (1975) and DeLoache (1973).
shown a stimulus that differed to some extent
from the standard. This procedure permitted This experiment also investigated transfer
a comparison of fast and slow habituators' re- of habituation. After reaching the criterion
sponse to discrepancy after both had habitu- specified above, the subjects received a second
ated to the standard. habituation series with a pattern that contained
half the elements in the original standard
An infant's response to discrepant stimuli (medium discrepancy) or a pattern made up
is generally considered a test of memory for of all new elements (high discrepancy). It was
the standard. Therefore, if slow habituators are expected that habituation would occur more
able eventually to process and store an ade- rapidly to the moderately discrepant stimulus,
quate model of the stimulus, they should show since the subject had already habituated to
as much recovery as fast habituators. On the part of it.
other hand, if fast habituators have basically
superior memorial processes, they could still be Delayed recognition was also tested by
expected to respond more positively to the re-presenting the original standard following
novel stimuli. the second habituation series. If a subject were
still habituated to the stimulus (i.e., if he re-
McCall, Hogarty, Hamilton, and Vincent membered it), his response to it should remain
(1973) also investigated this problem. They low. If, however, the intervening stimulation
exposed 12- and 18-week-old infants to a sim- or time interval had interfered with his mem-
ple standard stimulus until their fixation time ory trace, then his fixation time should recover
met an absolute criterion; an infant was con-
upon presentation of the original standard.
sidered to have habituated when he fixated the
pattern for 3 sec or less on two consecutive Method
trials (following the first five stimulus presenta-
tions). Both fast and slow habituators re-
Subjects.-The subjects in the final sample
sponded positively to subsequent discrepant were 36 infants, 18 male and 18 female, whose
stimuli, and the slow habituators actually
looked longer than the fast habituators. average age was 16 weeks, 6 days (range =
15 weeks, 3 days to 17 weeks, 6 days). Their
However, the fact that an absolute cri- names were obtained from local newspaper
terion of habituation was used makes inter- birth announcements. The parents were invited
pretation of these results difficult. The criterion by mail and telephone to participate in the
required that an infant fixate the stimulus for study and were paid $2.00 for their coopera-
3 sec or less. Consequently, infants with ini- tion. Only full-term infants with no history of
tially low fixation times could be classified as eye disorders were included.

This content downloaded from 152.3.102.242 on Wed, 17 Jul 2013 08:59:53 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Judy S. DeLoache 147
An additional 35 infants were tested but .92 (Cohen, Gelber, & Lazar 1971; Pancratz
were excluded from the study for crying (27) & Cohen 1970).
and failureto turn to the blinkinglight between
trials (six). One subject was eliminated be- Procedure.-The experimenterbriefly ex-
cause he had such low initial fixation times plained the nature of the study, and the mother
that it was impossible for him to reach the (or father) was told she (he) could terminate
one-half criterion.Another subject whose look- the experiment at any time. Then the mother
ing time for the firstthree trials was more than was seated with the infant sitting on her lap
18 standarddeviations longer than that of the facing the semicircular screen. The infant's
other infants was excluded. head was approximately24 inches away from
the screen. The experimenter informed the
Apparatus.-The mother sat with her in- mother that it was essential that she not influ-
fant on her lap facing a 4-ft-high semicircular ence her baby in any way during the presenta-
screen painted silver gray. The screen was tion of stimuli. To insure that the mother
mounted on 14-inch metal pipe legs. A blinking would not unintentionally affect her infant's
light was used to attract the subject'sattention behavior, she was instructed to keep her eyes
at the start of each trial. The light was a Dialco closed throughoutthe session so she could not
28-v bulb with translucentwhite cover % inch see the stimuli at the same time as the infant.
in diameter which was mounted on the right At the termination of the session, the mother
side of the screen, 24 inches from the right side was shown the blinking light and the patterns
and 15 inches from the bottom of the screen. her infant had seen.
When turned on, the light blinked at a rate of
0.3 sec on and 0.3 sec off. Each trial began with the light on the
subject'sright blinking on and off. The purpose
Visual patternswere provided by a carou- of the light was to controlwhere the infant was
sel slide projectorlocated in an adjacentcontrol looking when the pattern was turned on (see
room behind the infant. A shutter in front of Cohen 1973). As soon as the infant fixated the
the lens blocked the image until a pattern was light, the observer pressed a switch which
to be projected. Then the shutter opened, and turned off the light and presented the stimulus
the slide was projectedthrough a window onto on the opposite side of the panel. When the
a mirrorin the experimentalroom. The mirror infant began looking at the pattern, the ob-
reflected the image onto the left side of the server pressed another switch which she re-
screen. (Reflecting the stimulus off the mirror leased when the infant turned away. Releasing
was necessary since the mother was sitting be- the fixation-timeswitch turned off the slide and
tween the projectorand the screen.) The size turned on the blinking light to begin the next
of the projected image was 11 x 14 inches. trial.
The bottom edge of the pattern was located
14 inches up from the bottom edge of the Table 1 shows the design of the experi-
screen, and the right edge was 20 inches to ment. On the first two trials every subject was
the left of the light. shown an 8 X 8 black-and-whitecheckerboard
pattern as a warm-up stimulus to control for
A portable television camera on a tripod startle effects. Then for the first series of ha-
behind the screen recorded the infant's head bituation trials (Habituation I) each subject
and eye movements through a 2-inch hole in was shown one of two standard stimuli (A or
the center of the screen 12 inches from the B). StandardA was a pattern made up of four
bottom edge. An observerwatched the subject's geometric shapes each of a different color on
head and eyes on a TV monitorin the control a black background (see fig. 1). Standard B
room. She held one switch in each hand. contained four different shapes of different
When she judged the infant to be looking at colors.
the light she pressed one button; when she
The standard stimulus was repeatedly
judged the infant to be fixating the stimulus, presented until the subject's fixation time
she pressed the other button. The switches reached the proportionalcriterion level (one-
were connected to the programmingequipment half). A special apparatuscomputed the total
and to an Esterline-Angusevent recorder, all fixation time for the first three habituation
located in the control room. Previous studies trials and comparedit to the sum of every three
have demonstratedthe reliability of recording subsequent consecutive trials (including trials
fixations from the monitor with r's of .89 and 2, 3, and 4). If the new total was greater than

This content downloaded from 152.3.102.242 on Wed, 17 Jul 2013 08:59:53 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
148 Child Development
TABLE 1
EXPERIMENTALDESIGN

HabituationII
(to Criterion
or 8 Trials);
Recoveryto Delayed
Warm-up HabituationI Discrepancy Recognition Recovery
DiscrepancyLevela (2 Trials) (to Criterion) (1st 2 Trials) (2 Trials) (2 Trials)
High ................... 8X8 A B A 8X 8
8X8 B A B 8X8
Medium ................ 8X 8 A C A 8X 8
8X8 B C B 8X8
Zero ................... 8X 8 A A B 8X 8
8X8 B B A 8X8
a Half the males and half the females in each discrepancy condition experienced standard A, half B.

one-half of the first three trials, the slide pro- tion series, the subjects in the high- and
jector did not advance and the same slide was medium-discrepancy conditions were given two
shown again on the next trial. When the sum trials with their original standard (the stimu-
of any three consecutive trials was equal to lus to which the subject had originally been
or less than one-half the original total, then habituated).' The purpose of re-presenting the
the criterion computer caused the slide pro- original standard was to test for delayed recog-
jector to advance. nition.
On the next trial a stimulus was shown Finally, all subjects received two trials
which was of either high, medium, or zero with an 8 X 8 black-and-white checkerboard
discrepancy relative to the previous standard. to test for recovery to a relatively novel stimu-
In the high-discrepancy condition all the colors lus. This control was necessary to insure that
and shapes in the standard pattern were the infants were still generally attentive at the
changed (subjects who had previously seen end of the session (Jeffrey & Cohen 1971).
standard A were presented with B, and vice
versa). Subjects in the medium-discrepancy Results
condition received a partially changed stimulus,
C. This pattern contained two of the colored As shown in figure 2, the 36 subjects were
shapes from standard A and two from standard divided into fast and slow habituators on the
B. Thus, it was equally discrepant from both basis of number of trials to reach criterion in
standard stimuli. The zero-discrepancy group Habituation I. There appeared to be two
continued to receive the same pattern they distinct populations of subjects-19 infants
had seen previously. reached the one-half criterion in eight or fewer
These trials began the second habituation trials, and 17 subjects took 11 trials or longer.
The mean number of trials to criterion was
series (Habituation II). With the first presenta-
10.1.
tion of the novel stimulus, the criterion com-
puter reset and started figuring a new criterion The fixation-time data were transformed
based on the subject's fixation time for the first into base-10 logarithms prior to analysis. Un-
three trials with the new pattern. (For the weighted-means analyses of variance (Winer
zero-discrepancy group, the criterion computer 1962) were performed with the factors of sex
reset following the achievement of criterion (male, female), discrepancy level (high, me-
even though the infants continued to see the dium, zero), and rate of habituation (fast,
same pattern.) The stimulus was presented slow). Initial analyses showed no significant
repeatedly until the subject either reached the effects for the standard stimuli (A vs. B), and
new one-half criterion of habituation or had they were combined in subsequent analyses.
experienced eight trials with the new pattern. Habituation I.-To determine if any ini-
Following the end of the second habitua- tial differences in fixation time existed among
1 As shown in table 1, the zero-change group was shown a high-discrepancy stimulus on
the two trials following Habituation II to test for recovery. The infants' fixation time signifi-
cantly increased on these trials.

This content downloaded from 152.3.102.242 on Wed, 17 Jul 2013 08:59:53 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Judy S. DeLoache 149

Ui

mu
Fwo.1.-Experimentalstimuli.StimulusC was composedof two of the shapesfromstandardA and
two fromstandardB.

the three discrepancy levels, looking times longer fixationtimes than females. An analysis
during the first series of habituationtrials were of the number of trials to criterion (with the
analyzed. No significanteffects were found in factors of sex, discrepancylevel, and standard
an analysis of log mean fixation times for Ha- stimulus) also showed no significant effects.
bituation I or in an analysis of only the first
threehabituationtrials (the trialswhich formed Thus, these data indicate that the subjects did
not differ significantly (other than in the num-
the reference on which each subject'shabitua- ber of trials to reach criterion) prior to the test
tion criterion was based). In both analyses,
the only factor to approach significance was for recovery of fixationtime.
sex (p = .07), with males tending to have Recovery to discrepancy.--A repeated-

This content downloaded from 152.3.102.242 on Wed, 17 Jul 2013 08:59:53 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
150 Child Development

10
Z9
u
0 5
6
0 5

z 2 "

2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 22 24
in
FIG. 2.-Distribution of trials to criterion.Infantsreachingcriterion eight
or fewer trialswere
designatedas fast habituators,and those reachingcriterionin 11 or more trials were designatedslow
habituators.
habituators.

measures analysis of variance compared each To determine if fast and slow habituators
subject's fixation time on the last two trials of differedin amountof recoveryto the discrepant
Habituation I and the two recovery trials. As stimuli, a repeated-measuresanalysis of vari-
shown in figure 3, the infants' fixationtime in- ance compared the last two trials of Habitu-
creased to the discrepant stimuli, F(1,24) = ation I with the two recovery trials for only
38.75, p < .001. The predicted interaction of the high- and medium-discrepancyconditions.
=
discrepancy level x trials was significant, Significant recovery occurred, F (1,16)
F (2,24) = 5.97, p < .01. Inspection of the 51.60, p < .001, and there was no difference
means revealed that both the high- and in the amount of recovery shown by fast and
medium-discrepancygroups recovered, while slow habituators (the F value for the rate x
the zero-change group did not. Obviously, the trialsinteractionwas less than one). Thus, after
prediction that the high-change group would habituating to the same level as fast habitu-
show greater recovery than the medium group ators, slow habituatorsresponded just as posi-
was not supported. tively to the presentation of a new stimulus.
The zero-changeconditionprovided a con- Habituation II.-It was predicted that
trol for regressionto the mean following Habit- transfer from Habituation I would cause sub-
uation I, which could be expected to occur to jects in the medium-changeconditionto habitu-
the extent that subjects happened to reach ate more rapidly during Habituation II than
criterionthroughchance fluctuationsin fixation subjects in the high-discrepancycondition. The
time. A t test confirmed that these subjects predicted differencein numberof trialsto reach
did not increase significantly after attaining criterionin Habituation II for the high (M =
criterion. 7.33) and medium (M = 6.92) discrepancy

This content downloaded from 152.3.102.242 on Wed, 17 Jul 2013 08:59:53 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Judy S. DeLoache 151

.5

W MEDIUM
.4 HIGH
O

S .3 ZERO

O
.1

HABITUATION I RECOVERY
FIG. 3.-Recovery to three levels of discrepancy

subjects was not significant. However, the re- of Habituation II. Their looking time on the
stricted number of trials allowed may have last two trials was 98% as high as on the first
been responsible, since few subjects reached two trials.
criterion within the maximum eight trials.
Delayed recognition.-Following Habitu-
Even though not many infants reached the ation II, the subjects in the high- and medium-
50% criterion in Habituation II, an analysis discrepancy groups were given two trials with
comparing the log fixation times for the first their original standard stimulus from Habitua-
two and the last two trials of Habituation II tion I. A repeated-measures analysis of vari-
revealed a significant trials effect, F(1,24) = ance showed that the infants looked at the pat-
13.65, p < .01. The infants' fixation time de- tern longer on the recognition test than they
creased from the beginning to the end of Ha- had on the last two trials of Habituation I,
bituation II. The predicted difference between F(1,16) = 12.09, p < .01. The high-discrep-
amount of habituation for the high- and me- ancy subjects increased slightly but not sig-
dium-discrepancy conditions was not found. A nificantly more than the medium-discrepancy
significant effect was found for sex, F (1,24) = subjects did. Apparently, the habituation stim-
8.55, p < .01, with boys looking at the stimuli ulus was not recognized on the delayed test;
twice as long as girls. the response to it had recovered during the
several intervening trials.
The zero-change group showed no change
in fixation time from the beginning to the end Checkerboard trials.-Fixation times on

This content downloaded from 152.3.102.242 on Wed, 17 Jul 2013 08:59:53 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
152 Child Development
the checkerboard trials at the beginning and tion in what they see. Or the infants may
end of the session were compared to make cer- initially be attending to different aspects of
tain that the infants were still attentive at the the stimulus situation. Another alternative is
conclusion of the session. This procedure is that slow habituators process and store some in-
necessary to rule out the possibility that the ob- formation on every trial, but a smaller amount
served decrement in fixation time was caused by per trial than fast habituators. Further research
peripheral factors such as fatigue (Jeffrey & should attempt to determine which particular
Cohen 1971). A significant trials effect, aspects of encoding are performed differently
F(1,24) = 4.58, p < .05, showed that the by these two groups of infants.
infants actually looked longer at the checker-
boards at the end of the session than they had McCall et al. (1973) concluded that, for
at the beginning. This finding clearly rules infants, differences in developmental level ap-
out fatigue or adaptation as important factors pear more in encoding than in retrieval pro-
in the present study. There was also a signifi- cesses. Thus, if rate of habituation reflects cog-
cant but uninterpretable four-way interaction nitive development, as was suggested earlier,
of sex x discrepancy level x rate of habitua- one would expect the main difference between
tion x trials, F (2,24) = 3.67, p < .05. fast and slow habituators to appear in the pro-
cess of forming a memory model of a stimulus.
Discussion After development of a model, differences
should be minimal. Since just these results were
One of the most striking findings of the obtained in the present investigation, the data
present study was the distinctly bimodal dis- are consistent with the view that fast habitu-
tribution of number of trials to reach the cri- ators are more advanced infants. One test of
terion of habituation (fig. 2). One group of this analysis would be to compare long-term
infants (labeled fast habituators) took eight retention (instead of recovery to discrepancy)
or fewer trials to habituate, while a separate in fast and slow habituators. They should be
group (slow habituators) required 11 or more equally capable of recognizing a pattern to
trials to reach criterion. Thus, rate of habitua- which they had previously been habituated.
tion appears to identify two quite different
populations of infants, and the data reported The present finding of recovery to dis-
here provide some indication of the source of crepancy by slow as well as fast habituators is
the difference. consistent with the data of McCall et al.
(1973), who also used a criterion of habitu-
Slow habituators were not incapable of ation. In light of these experiments, it is not
performing the various aspects of habituation surprising that several previously cited studies
(information processing and storage, retrieval, which presented fixed numbers of trials (Mc-
and comparison). Given repeated exposure to Call & Kagan 1970; McCall & Melson 1969;
the pattern, they eventually habituated to it Melson & McCall 1970) did not find recovery
and subsequently recovered to a novel stimulus. by slow habituators. Those infants simply had
Furthermore, there was no difference in the not habituated by the end of the session. To
degree of recovery for fast and slow habitu- the extent that response habituation reflects the
ators. It appears that these two groups of formation of a memory model of the stimulus,
infants differed in how many exposures it took only after infants have habituated can they
for them to form an accurate model of the reasonably be expected to discriminate and
stimulus. However, once that model was respond to a novel stimulus.
formed well enough to inhibit further looking
at the pattern, they were equally capable of It should be noted that the present results
discriminating a new stimulus from the old one. also differ in some ways from those of McCall
et al. (1973), since they found differences in
Thus, fast and slow habituators probably degree of recovery for infants identified as fast
do not differ very much in the adequacy of and slow habituators. However, this discrep-
their memory model of the stimulus, or in their
ancy is probably due to the criteria employed
ability to retrieve the model or to compare new in the two studies.
stimuli to it. The source of variation in rate of
habituation seems rather to be in the functions The present investigation is the first to
of analyzing or encoding stimulus information. employ a proportional criterion of habituation.
A difference may exist in how quickly fast and The principal advantage of this type of cri-
slow habituators begin to process the informa- terion is that the investigator can equate the

This content downloaded from 152.3.102.242 on Wed, 17 Jul 2013 08:59:53 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Judy S. DeLoache 153
relative amount of habituation shown by all even though they continued to see the same
infants. In this study all subjects were required stimulus for eight more trials during Habitu-
to decrease their fixation time by the same ation II.
proportion (50%). Since the mean fixation
times of the fast and slow habituators did not The same criterion might or might not be
differ at the start or end of the habituation appropriate for a different situation with differ-
trials, the groups were equated for all variables ent stimuli, however. Whether a given propor-
tional criterion of habituation results in an
except the relevant one of rate of habituation.
asymptotically low response could depend on
The criterion used by McCall et al. (1973) the initial level. For example, if a stimulus
specified an absolute level of fixation time to elicits extremely long fixation times at the
be reached by all subjects. While this is an beginning of an experiment, the infants could
improvement over the previous practice of reach a 50% criterion of habituation and still
giving all subjects the same number of famil- be looking fairly long at the stimuli. Further
iarization trials, there are still several problems. experience with the stimulus might produce
One of the most serious is that the procedure even more response decrement. Thus, pilot
allows short lookers to be classified as fast testing of the experimental stimuli should be
habituators, even though their response may done to select the appropriate criterion.
not decrease at all. If the actual difference be-
tween infants classified as fast and slow habit- Another advantage of a proportional cri-
uators is not the rate at which their response terion of habituation is that the criterion itself
to a repeated stimulus decreases, but rather can be manipulated experimentally. For ex-
their general tendency to be relatively short or ample, the response to novelty by infants who
long lookers, then the response to discrepancy had decreased their fixation time by only 25%
of the two groups may be confounded with the could be compared to that by subjects who had
differences in their overall fixation times. Since reached a more stringent criterion. Such an
the slow habituators in that study seem to be analysis could provide important information
longer lookers, the differences reported for re- about the early stages of information pro-
covery to discrepancy may not be due to rate cessing and storage.
of habituation.
The present study also investigated de-
McCall et al. (1973) were not unaware of layed recognition, and it provides the first
this problem, for they reported that "to insure direct evidence of interference in a habituation
that the results were not a function of differ- paradigm. When the original (Habituation I)
ences in response style and 'initial values,' the standard stimulus was re-presented following
analyses were repeated using the response to several trials with a different pattern, it elicited
the standard immediately preceding the first significantly longer looking times that it had
discrepancy as a covariate ..." (p. 285). How- at the end of Habituation I. The intervening
ever, as the authors pointed out, the response stimulation apparently had interfered with the
to the last standard was one of the criterion subjects' memory for the original pattern.
trials and therefore could only be 3 sec or less.
Other infant habituation studies have not
Hence, all subjects were by definition respond-
ing at a very similar level and little variance reported interference effects (Caron & Caron
was possible. The appropriate covariate would 1969; Gelber 1972a; Martin 1973), even
have been initial fixation time or mean fixation when several different stimuli have been inter-
time for the noncriterion habituation trials. polated. Cohen & Gelber (1975) suggest
that the intervening stimulation may have to
A proportional criterion thus seems to be be processed and stored in memory before it
preferable for studying individual differences will disrupt an infant's recognition of a pre-
in rate of habituation. Another of its advantages viously stored stimulus. For young infants,
is that one can ensure that at the end of the several stimulus presentations are probably
habituation trials the infants have reached an necessary for a competing memory model to
asymptotically low response level (and that be formed. In the present experiment only one
model formation is complete). The 50% cri- pattern was repeatedly exposed during the
terion of habituation used in the present study interval between habituation and the recogni-
met this stipulation. After reaching the one- tion test. Such repetition should facilitate pro-
half criterion in Habituation I, the zero-change cessing and storing of the interpolated stimulus
group's fixation time did not decline further and thus increase the likelihood of interference.

This content downloaded from 152.3.102.242 on Wed, 17 Jul 2013 08:59:53 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
154 Child Development
In the one other study in which the interval was Jeffrey, W. E., & Cohen, L. B. Habituation in the
filled with a single stimulus (Pancratz & Cohen human infant. In H. Reese (Ed.), Advances
1970), infants subsequently failed to differen- in child development and behavior. Vol. 6.
tiate between the familiar and a novel pattern, New York: Academic Press, 1971.
suggesting that interference had occurred. It Lewis, M. Infant attention: response decrement
appears, then, that infants will recognize a as a measure of cognitive processes, or what's
pattern to which they were earlier habituated new Baby Jane? Paper presented at the
unless a competing memory model is subse- meeting of the Society for Research in Child
quently formed. For that to happen, the inter- Development, New York, 1967.
vening stimulus should be repeatedly pre- McCall, R. B. Attention in the infant: avenue to
sented, and it may also be essential that the the study of cognitive development. In D.
infant habituate to it. Walcher & D. Peters (Eds.), Early childhood:
the development of self-regulatory mecha-
References nisms. New York: Academic Press, 1971.
McCall, R. B.; Hogarty, P. S.; Hamilton, J. S.;
Caron, R. F., & Caron A. J. Degree of stimulus & Vincent, J. H. Habituation rate and the
complexity and habituation of visual fixation infant's response to visual discrepancies.Child
in infants. Psychonomic Science, 1969, 14, 78-
79. Development, 1973, 44, 280-287.
McCall, R. B., & Kagan, J. Individual differences
Cohen, L. B. A two-process model of infant visual in the infant's distribution of attention to
attention. Merrill-PalmerQuarterly, 1973, 19,
stimulus discrepancy. Developmental Psy-
157-180.
Cohen, L. B., & Gelber, E. R. Infant visual mem- chology, 1970, 2, 90-98.
McCall, R. B., & Melson, W. H. Attention in
ory. In L. Cohen & P. Salapatek (Eds.), Infant infants as a function of magnitude of discrep-
perception: from sensation to cognition: basic
visual processes. Vol. 1. New York: Academic ancy and habituation rate. Psychonomic Sci-
Press, 1975. ence, 1969, 17, 317-319.
Cohen, L. B.; Gelber, E. R.; & Lazar, M. S. Infant McGurk, H. Infant discrimination of orientation.
habituation and generalization to repeated Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
visual stimulation. Journal of Experimental 1972, 14, 151-164.
Child Psychology, 1971, 11, 379-389. Martin, R. M. Long-term effects of stimulus famil-
DeLoache, J. S. Infant visual memory. Unpublished iarization. Paper presented at the meeting of
doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois, the Society for Research in Child Develop-
Urbana-Champaign, 1973. ment, Philadelphia, 1973.
Gelber, E. R. The effect of time and intervening Melson, W. H., & McCall, R. B. Attentional re-
items on the recovery of an habituated re-
sponses of five-month girls to discrepant audi-
sponse. Unpublished master's thesis, Univer-
tory stimuli. Child Development, 1970, 41,
sity of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign,1972. (a) 1159-1171.
Gelber, E. R. Habituation, discriminationlearning,
and visual information processing in infants. Pancratz, N., & Cohen, L. B. Recovery of habitu-
ation in infants. Journalof ExperimentalChild
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University
of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, 1972. (b) Psychology, 1970, 9, 208-216.
Greenberg, D. J.; O'Donnell, W. J.; & Crawford, Sokolov, E. N. Perception and the conditioned re-
D. Complexitylevels, habituation, and individ- flex. New York: Macmillan, 1963.
ual differences in early infancy. Child De- Winer, B. J. Statistical principles in experimental
velopment, 1973, 44, 569-574. design. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1962.

This content downloaded from 152.3.102.242 on Wed, 17 Jul 2013 08:59:53 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like