Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

Negotiation

t
A Newsletter from

os
Harvard Business School Publishing
and the Program on Negotiation
Decision-Making and Communication Strategies That Deliver Results at Harvard Law School

ARTICLE REPRINT NO. N0604C

rP
Is Your Counterpart Satisfied?

yo
op BY MAURICE E. SCHWEITZER
tC
No
Do

This document is authorized for use only by Julia Minson at Harvard Univ. - Kennedy School of Government until
November 2014. Copying or posting is an infringement of copyright. Permissions@hbsp.harvard.edu or
617.783.7860.
t
os
rP
yo
op
tC
No
Do

For a complete list of Harvard Business For reprint and subscription information For customized and quantity orders of reprints:
School Publishing newsletters: for Negotiation: Call 617-783-7626 Fax 617-783-7658
http://newsletters.harvardbusinessonline.org Call 800-988-0866 or 617-783-7500
For permission to copy or republish:
This document is authorized for http://negotiation.harvardbusinessonline.org
use only by Julia Minson at Harvard Univ. - Kennedy School of Government until
Call 617-783-7587
November 2014. Copying or posting is an infringement of copyright. Permissions@hbsp.harvard.edu or
617.783.7860.
ACROSS THE TABLE

t
Is Your Counterpart Satisfied?

os
Your counterpart’s satisfaction level can swing a deal to either a loss or a large profit.
BY MAURICE E. SCHWEITZER

O NE OF MY MBA STUDENTS recently told me a story that deal. Even a lengthy contract cannot cover every possible

rP
shows how important it is to ensure that your negotia- contingency, and the costs of enforcement are high.
tion counterpart is satisfied. Two customers made pur- Second, if your counterpart is satisfied with the deal, she
chases at my student’s family’s appliance store. One is also more likely to seek you out again and recommend
customer bought several appliances and negotiated a steep you for future business. The more satisfied she is, the more
discount for his business—so steep, in fact, that the store cooperatively she will approach future negotiations. Con-
barely made a profit on the sale. Nonetheless, the customer versely, a dissatisfied counterpart is likely to try to “even
left the store feeling dissatisfied, believing he should have the score” during the next round of talks.

yo
received an even better deal for purchasing so many appli- You might think that negotiator satisfaction is closely
ances at once. Not long after, the customer began to com- related to getting a great deal. A better deal should be more
plain about the items he’d bought, insisting on service and pleasing than an inferior one—right? Not necessarily. After
replacements. As a result, the store lost money on a cus- all, the quality of a deal can be difficult to assess. When
tomer who felt his large volume of business had created a issues are difficult to value or forecast, such as the future
windfall for the store. profitability of a newly purchased company, making steeper
Soon after this customer made his purchases, a second concessions to your counterpart may not directly increase
op
customer bought a single item and received a very small her satisfaction. In fact, as I will explain, making steep con-
discount. From this deal, the appliance store made a large cessions can actually decrease the other side’s satisfaction.
profit, but the customer left the store quite satisfied.
Because he had made a small purchase, he didn’t expect a Managing your counterpart’s satisfaction
large discount and thus he was happy with the discount he As the following points will demonstrate, ensuring that
received. This customer went on to recommend the store your counterpart is satisfied with a particular deal requires
tC

to family members and friends, and ultimately generated a you to manage several aspects of the negotiation process,
great deal of follow-up business. including his outcome expectations, his perceptions of
This story illustrates two important principles of nego- your outcome, the comparisons he makes with others, and
tiator satisfaction. First, your counterpart’s satisfaction his overall negotiation experience itself.
level can swing a deal to either a loss or a large profit. Sec-
ond, your counterpart’s level of satisfaction is a function of 1. Outcome expectations.
his perceptions of the deal—perceptions that may or may Prior to and during a negotiation, people develop
No

not be closely related to reality. expectations about the type of deal they will receive. Work
Making a fellow negotiator satisfied is more complicated by business-school professors Richard Oliver and Bruce
than merely offering a more favorable deal. The actions you Barry of Vanderbilt University and Sundar Balakrishnan of
take prior to, during, and following a negotiation can pro- the University of Washington demonstrates that negotia-
foundly influence your counterpart’s satisfaction. Here I tors automatically compare their actual outcome with the
will look at psychological factors that influence how nego- outcome they expected prior to negotiating. As a result of
tiators perceive outcomes as well as the important role of this process, two negotiators with the exact same outcome
the negotiation process in determining satisfaction. can feel very differently about their deal. For example, con-
sider two car buyers who both purchased the same model
Do

Why satisfaction matters car for $30,000. The buyer who expected to pay $29,000
Successful negotiators work hard to ensure that when they will be dissatisfied with this deal, while the buyer who
and their counterpart leave a negotiation, both sides feel expected to pay $31,000 will be quite pleased.
satisfied with the agreement. Why should you care whether Skilled negotiators manage expectations prior to and
the other side is pleased with the deal or not? First, because during a negotiation. Some managers do this instinctively.
satisfied negotiators are more likely to uphold the terms of a For example, in the month prior to salary negotiations

Copyright © 2006 This document


by Harvard is authorized
Business School for useAllonly
Publishing Corporation. rightsby Julia Minson at Harvard Univ. - Kennedy School of Government until
reserved. 3
November 2014. Copying or posting is an infringement of copyright. Permissions@hbsp.harvard.edu or
617.783.7860.
Satisfying Your Counterpart (continued)
with employees, managers may broadcast the message that wage and benefit concessions but also about how manage-
this has been a difficult year for the company. After having ment is making out.

t
their expectations lowered, some employees may be satis- This is a lesson that Donald Carty, former CEO of

os
fied to receive even a small cost-of-living raise. American Airlines, learned the hard way in 2003. The air-
Your reaction to an opening offer can also influence line was struggling with an uncertain financial future, and
your counterpart’s expectations. By reacting with a sur- the management team asked employee unions to cut their
prised look, a laugh, or a flinch, you can lower your coun- benefits by $10 billion over six years. After protracted
terpart’s expectations about the feasible bargaining zone. negotiations, the unions finally agreed to wage cuts that
Conversely, by appearing very cooperative or particularly ranged from 15.6% to 23%.

rP
eager for agreement, you may raise your counterpart’s The unions perceived that this deal was in their best
expectations. interest—until they learned about the special deal that
One common negotiation mistake is to escalate expecta- American Airlines executives had worked out for them-
tions by making a steep concession that could lead the other selves. Six top executives at the airline, including Carty,
side to expect another. Imagine that you’re bidding on a had arranged to earn large bonuses (twice their salary) if
house that has been on the market for they stayed at the company through
some time at a high list price of Be modest about your 2005; the company also set up a special

yo
$390,000. You like the house but start pension fund for 45 executives in the
with a low offer: $300,000. In response, gains, and commend event that American Airlines filed for
the seller offers a slight reduction from bankruptcy. These perks were dis-
your counterpart for
the list price: $385,000. Hoping to closed in Securities and Exchange
bridge the gap, you make an offer close his hard bargaining. Commission filings made public one
to your bottom line: $340,000. The day after the unions agreed to the wage
seller may misinterpret this move and believe that you can concessions. Outraged, the unions’ leadership rescinded
op
easily make another $40,000 jump. Rather than quickly the deal. One week later, Carty was forced to resign.
agreeing to your offer, the seller might escalate her expecta- Social psychologists George Loewenstein at Carnegie
tions regarding likely outcomes. Mellon University, Leigh Thompson at Northwestern Uni-
A related mistake is to agree to your counterpart’s versity, and Max Bazerman at Harvard University have
demands too quickly. Adam Galinsky and Victoria Medvec demonstrated that negotiator satisfaction is affected by
of Northwestern University, Vanessa Seiden of Chicago- social utility—the comparisons that people make between
tC

based Ruda Cohen and Associates, and Peter Kim of the their outcome and their counterpart’s outcome. The
University of Southern California studied reactions to ini- American Airlines workers were willing to agree to wage
tial offers in a negotiation. They found that negotiators concessions, but they wanted their sacrifice to be shared
whose initial offers were immediately accepted were less companywide.
satisfied with their agreement than were negotiators whose In negotiation, we form perceptions about how much
offers were accepted after a delay—even if the former value both sides claim. As the initial American Airlines
group reached better final outcomes than the latter group. deal illustrates, these perceptions can be mistaken. The
No

Those whose initial offers were immediately accepted were unions were unaware of the type of deal executives would
more likely to think about how they could have attained a be getting until management was forced to disclose their
better outcome than were negotiators whose offers were benefits. The lesson? As car salespeople have learned, be
accepted after a delay. modest about your gains from a deal, and commend your
As these results suggest, you can actually make your counterpart for his hard bargaining.
counterpart less satisfied by agreeing too quickly. In fact,
by delaying agreement and even asking for additional con- 3. Social comparisons.
cessions, you may be able to make your counterpart more My own work with Yale psychologist Nathan Novemsky
satisfied with a deal. identifies social comparisons as another critical factor in
Do

guiding negotiator satisfaction. Not only do negotiators


2. Perceptions of your outcome. compare their profit from a deal with the profit they imag-
Just as negotiators evaluate how good a deal is for them- ine their counterpart earned, but they also compare their
selves, they also assess how good a deal is for their counter- profit with the profits of other negotiators who were in a
part. For example, when negotiating a labor agreement, similar situation. For example, a car buyer is likely not only
unions often care not only about how much they gain in to assess how much the dealership made off of him but also

4 Negotiation Aprilis2006
This document authorized for use only by Julia Minson at Harvard Univ. - Kennedy School of Government until
November 2014. Copying or posting is an infringement of copyright. Permissions@hbsp.harvard.edu or
617.783.7860.
Satisfying Your Counterpart (continued)
to compare his price with the deal his neighbor got. If the Jerald Greenberg of Ohio State University has found that
buyer’s neighbor bought the same model car for a higher we attach a great deal of importance not only to our out-

t
price, he is likely to be more satisfied with his purchase comes but also to how we achieved these outcomes. In par-

os
than if his neighbor got a better deal. ticular, people feel more satisfied when a negotiation
Social comparisons can drastically skew our percep- involves procedures that they perceive to be fair—even
tions of a particular outcome. Unfortunately, our ability to when the outcome is unfavorable for them.
make comprehensive and accurate social comparisons is To guide these perceptions, give your negotiation
limited. We typically compare our own outcomes with counterpart a voice in the decision process. Even when
those who are close to us—neighbors, coworkers, and fam- you are in a position of power, be sure to acknowledge

rP
ily members. In addition, individuals or organizations your counterpart’s perspective and invite him to express
sometimes actively limit our access to information that his views, to suggest alternatives, and to react to initial
could improve the accuracy of our social comparisons; for proposals.
example, many companies discourage employees from dis- You can also enhance perceptions of fairness after an
closing their salaries to one another. Similarly, your outcome has been reached by providing detailed explana-
brother-in-law may choose to tell only you about the tions for unappealing actions or outcomes. These explana-
stocks he purchased that increased in value. tions, though time-consuming in the short run, can help

yo
As a negotiator, you need to recognize the limitations your counterpart develop a much more favorable view of
you face in developing a complete and accurate social com- the process—and ultimately save you substantial time and
parison set. In addition, you should seek to guide the com- effort later. !
parisons that your counterpart selects. When engaged in
labor negotiations, for instance, a management team might Maurice E. Schweitzer is an associate professor at the
highlight recent labor contracts in which other unions Wharton School at the University of Pennsylvania.
received less advantageous terms than the union desires. His research focuses on issues related to trust,
op
deception, and emotions in negotiations. He can
be reached at negotiation@hbsp.harvard.edu.
4. Negotiation experience.
Research on procedural justice by social psychologist
tC
No
Do

Negotiation
This document is authorized for use only by Julia Minson at Harvard Univ. - Kennedy School of Government untilApril 2006 5
November 2014. Copying or posting is an infringement of copyright. Permissions@hbsp.harvard.edu or
617.783.7860.

You might also like