Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Patient Education and Counseling 58 (2005) 288–295

www.elsevier.com/locate/pateducou

The impact of an intervention in intercultural communication on


doctor–patient interaction in The Netherlands
Barbara C. Schouten a,*, Ludwien Meeuwesen a, Hans A.M. Harmsen b
a
Interdisciplinary Social Science Department, Utrecht University, P.O. Box 80.140, 3508 TC Utrecht, The Netherlands
b
Department of Health Policy and Management, Erasmus MC, University Medical Centre Rotterdam, The Netherlands

Received 24 February 2005; received in revised form 11 May 2005; accepted 4 June 2005

Abstract

Objective: Findings of scarcely available studies indicate that there are substantial gaps in intercultural doctor–patient communication. In
order to improve intercultural communication in medical practice in The Netherlands, an educational intervention was developed. The aim of
the present study was to examine the effects of this intervention on doctor–patient communication.
Methods: Participants (general practitioners: n = 38; patients: n = 124) were assigned at random to an intervention or a control group. GPs in
the intervention group received 2.5 days training on intercultural communication. Patients in the intervention group were exposed to a
videotaped instruction in the waiting room, right before the consultation. Data were collected through videotapes of visits of ethnic minority
patients to their GP and home interviews with the patients after their medical visit. Communication behaviour was assessed using the Roter
interaction analysis system (RIAS). Interview length was assessed as well.
Results: The length of the medical encounter increased significantly after having received the intervention. Total number of GP utterances
increased significantly too. When comparing relative frequencies on affective and instrumental verbal behaviour of both patients and doctors,
no significant changes could be detected.
Conclusion: It is concluded that there seems to be some change in doctor–patient interaction, but RIAS may not be suitable to detect subtle
changes in the medical communication process. It is recommended to use other analysis methods to assess cultural differences in medical
communication.
Practice implications: Knowledge about possible antecedents of gaps in intercultural medical communication should be increased in order to
be able to design effective interventions for intercultural doctor–patient communication.
# 2005 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Doctor–patient communication; Intervention study; Intercultural communication

1. Introduction in many Western countries. For example, in The Netherlands


more than 18% of the population is from other ethnic origin
Patients’ ethnic and cultural background is an often nowadays (Statistics Netherlands, 2004). Findings of
overlooked but nonetheless important variable within scarcely available studies indicate that there are indeed
doctor–patient communication studies. This topic has been variations and gaps in the communication process between
seldom investigated in such studies [1], despite the well- patients and doctors, due to ethnic and cultural background
recognized fact that one’s culture has a profound effect on variables [7–11]. For example, in two recent Dutch studies
the way people communicate [2–6]. Incorporating patients’ on communication differences between medical consulta-
cultural and ethnic background as a factor impacting on tions with ethnic minority patients and with native-born
doctor–patient communication is becoming increasingly patients [7,11], it was found that consultations with migrant
important though, because of the growing cultural diversity patients are shorter, that there is less social talk and mutual
involvement, and that doctors spend more time trying to
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +31 30 2537786; fax: +31 30 2534733. build a relationship with them compared to Dutch patients.
E-mail address: b.c.schouten@fss.uu.nl (B.C. Schouten). Moreover, the doctor–patient interaction in intercultural

0738-3991/$ – see front matter # 2005 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.pec.2005.06.005
B.C. Schouten et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 58 (2005) 288–295 289

consultations is rated as less friendly and warm than the practitioners, the second part towards patients belonging to
doctor–patient interaction in intra-cultural consultations. different ethnic groups, who were visiting their general
These communication gaps between doctors and patients practitioner. Former research found that this educational
can give rise to a wide range of health-related problems, intervention significantly increased the mutual understand-
having detrimental effects on the quality of care ethnic ing between GPs and non-Western patients [29]. As it is
minority patients receive. Indeed, several studies have likely that this finding can at least partly be attributed to
shown that there is more misunderstanding, less compliance improved doctor–patient communication, the aim of the
and less satisfaction in intercultural medical consultations present study was to examine the combined effects of this
compared to intra-cultural medical consultations [12–15]. two-part intervention on patients’ and GPs’ communicative
Difficulties in intercultural communication are for the behaviour. Based on previous literature on gaps in doctor–
greater part caused by language barriers and differences in patient communication within intercultural consultations, it
cultural values regarding communication and health [16– was hypothesised that if participants have more knowledge
22]. Differences in communication behaviour across about cultural differences, this will be reflected in an
cultures often are explained in terms of diverging cultural increase in both affective communicative behaviour (e.g.
values, such as individualism–collectivism (i.e. the extent to more social talk, more expressions of agreement) of GPs and
which personal goals or group norms prevail within a migrant patients as well as longer consultation length.
culture), high-context versus low-context cultures (the
extent to which cultures pay attention to situational cues
within conversations) and independent self-construals 2. Method
versus interdependent self-construals (the extent to which
the self is seen as independent from groups) [3,4,23,24]. 2.1. Study design and samples
Research shows that variations on these dimensions
influence various aspects of individuals’ communication A total of 170 general practitioners in Rotterdam, The
behaviour, such as the directness of the communication, the Netherlands, with at least 25% patients from a non-Western
use of silence in communicative acts, openness in country of origin were approached by mail and by one repeat
conversations and so on [3,4,6,25]. It is obvious that this request by telephone to participate in the study. Of the GPs,
communicative diversity across cultures can easily lead to 38 agreed to participate, 29 male and 9 female. Participants
communication problems and misunderstandings between were assigned at random to an intervention (n = 19) or a
members of different cultures. Within health care, different control group (n = 19). The intervention group received an
views on health and illness across cultures further educational intervention on intercultural communication
complicate matters. According to Kleinman [26], discre- given over a 2.5-day period (see below). Measurements of
pancies between patients’ and doctors’ perceptions about the GPs’ communicative behaviour took place at baseline, and
patient’s illness, which he labels ‘explanatory models’, within 1 and 6 months after the intervention. A randomised
create gaps in doctor–patient communication which in turn controlled trial study design was thus used to evaluate the
lead to misunderstanding and lack of agreement about effects of doctors’ exposure to an educational intervention
diagnosis and treatment. For effective communication to on intercultural communication in general practice. Com-
take place, doctor and patient should exchange each other’s munication data were collected by means of videotapes of
views on the patient’s illness during the consultation, migrant patient visits (n = 124) to assess doctors’ commu-
thereby uncovering and solving discrepancies in each other’s nication skills. 28% of consultations took place at baseline,
explanatory models [27]. 38% of consultations took place at 1 month after doctors’
In sum, due to linguistic and cultural barriers, differences intervention and 34% of consultations took place at 6
between doctors’ and patients’ cultural and/or ethnic months after doctors’ intervention.
background may easily result in communication gaps Patients were included in the study if they belonged to an
between the two. As a consequence of these gaps, ethnic minority group (mostly of Turkish, Moroccan and
compliance, satisfaction and mutual understanding are Surinamese origin) and were older than 17. When these
likely to be poorer in intercultural medical consultations criteria were met, patients visiting one of the 38 GPs for a
compared to intra-cultural medical consultations. In order to consultation in February, May and November 2000 were
improve intercultural communication in medical practice in asked to participate. Patients were approached in the waiting
The Netherlands, an educational intervention was developed room by a research assistant, who explained the procedure
by Harmsen [12]. The content of the intervention was based and general aim of the study. When patients agreed to
on Pinto’s theory about intercultural communication participate, they signed consent forms indicating their
difficulties [28], which states that these problems can be willingness to participate and their understanding of the
traced back to a lack of knowledge about cultural differences procedures and purpose of the study. Approximately half of
in communication behaviour and a lack of skills to bridge these patients (n = 65) received an intervention on inter-
the gap between these cultural differences. The first and cultural doctor–patient communication (see below); these
main part of the intervention was directed towards general were all patients of GPs in the intervention group. The other
290 B.C. Schouten et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 58 (2005) 288–295

patients (n = 59) did not receive an intervention; these were (good, moderate, poor). Because of the high correlations
patients of GPs in the control group. Comparisons between between these three measures, patients’ self-reports are
these two patient groups were made to evaluate the effects of reported and used in further analyses.
the intervention on patients’ communicative behaviour. 4. Patients’ religiosity. This was assessed by asking patients
if they are religious, and if so, whether they are actively
2.2. Description of intervention practicing their religion. The answers led to a classifica-
tion into three groups: (a) religious, actively practicing;
As mentioned, the educational intervention contained a (b) religious, not actively practicing, (c) not religious.
combined offering of a main part to the GPs and a small part 5. Patients’ sociodemographics, including age, gender and
to the patients. The GPs in the intervention group received a educational level.
2.5-day training on intercultural communication. The
training was based on Pinto’s ‘three step method’ [28]. In The dependent variables in this study were interview
the first step, GPs reflected on their own culturally based length, number of utterances, and instrumental and
norms, views and communication style; next, GPs’ affective verbal behaviour of both GPs and patients. Verbal
sensitivity and knowledge about differences in these norms, behaviour of both was based on videotapes of doctor–
views and communication styles between members of patient consultations, and was measured using Roter’s
different cultures were enhanced; finally, GPs were trained interaction analysis system (RIAS; [31]), an extensively
in skills to solve possible gaps in intercultural communica- used instrument in the field of patient-provider commu-
tion. Two weeks later, GPs were given the opportunity to nication. The instrument’s unit of analysis is the utterance,
discuss problems experienced in their practice concerning that is, the smallest meaningful string of words. The RIAS
this issue during the last training session. consists of several mutually exclusive and exhaustive code
Additionally, the patients in the intervention group categories representing independent communication ele-
were exposed to a 12 min communication instruction on ments, some focusing on affective communicative beha-
videotape in the waiting room, immediately before the viour, some focusing on instrumental communicative
consultation. The instruction was given in their preferred behaviour. All verbal (affective and instrumental) beha-
language (Moroccan-Arabic, Moroccan-Berber, Turkish and viour of GPs and patients is thus merged into a number of
Dutch). The main message was to illustrate to patients the mutually exclusive categories. In this study, we made use
advantages of direct communication and to instruct them to of the following seven affective categories: social beha-
freely express any misunderstandings and disagreements. viour, agreement, paraphrasing, showing concern, reassur-
This was done by showing them two examples of a ance, reflection, and disagreement. To assess the
consultation, one with indirect communication leading to instrumental, task-focused verbal behaviour, the following
misunderstandings between patient and doctor, and one with nine categories were used: giving directions, asking for
direct communication leading to mutual understanding. clarification, asking questions about medical/therapeutical
issues, asking questions about lifestyle issues and feelings,
2.3. Data collection and description of study variables counselling on medical/therapeutical issues, counselling
on lifestyle issues and feelings, and other. The consulta-
Data for this study were derived from videotaped tions were rated by two trained observers using a
observations and home interviews with the patients, carried computerised rating method, CAMERA [32]. Interrater
out 3–8 days after their visit to the GP. For purposes of the reliability was assessed by means of interclass correlations
present study, the following independent variables were for each category and is given in three categories: good
included: (ICC > 0.60), doubtful (0.40 < ICC < 0.60), and poor
(ICC < 0.40).
1. Patients’ ethnic background. To assess ethnic back-
ground, the patients were categorised into an ethnic 2.4. Data analysis
minority group in accordance with their own and their
parents’ country of birth [30]. Most of the migrants were Data were first processed by means of descriptive
from Turkey, Morocco, Surinam and the Antilles. analyses (frequencies, means, etc.). To investigate whether
2. Patients’ orientation. Patients were further divided into a there were differences in patient characteristics between the
Western and a non-Western group, based on their own intervention and control groups, t-and x2-tests were
and their parents’ country of birth. Patients of west- performed. t-Tests were carried out to test for differences
European, American, Canadian and Australian back- in patients’ communication behaviour between the control
ground were considered Western, patients from other group and the intervention group, and MANOVA’s were
countries were considered non-Western. performed to look for possible interaction effects between
3. Patients’ language proficiency. Language proficiency the intervention and patient characteristics. To test whether
was assessed by means of three methods: patients’ self- the communicative behaviour of doctors in the intervention
reports, interviewer’s assessment and GPs’ assessment group improved over time in contrast with the commu-
B.C. Schouten et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 58 (2005) 288–295 291

nicative behaviour of doctors in the control group, ANOVA’s Table 2


with measurement time as repeated measure were performed Number of GPs’ and patients’ utterances in control and intervention groups
for each of the communication variables. Control group Intervention group p
(n = 59) (n = 65)
GP utterances 82.2 (S.D.: 28.4) 109.2 (S.D.: 59.3) .001
Patient utterances 70.8 (S.D.: 32.3) 74.6 (S.D.: 47.3) .607
3. Results

3.1. Patient sample


length of control group: 7.6 min, S.D.: 3.3; p = .01). Table 2
Patient characteristics are described in Table 1, separately shows the number of utterances of both GPs and patients
for the intervention and control groups. There are in the control and intervention conditions. The total number
significantly more male patients in the intervention group of utterances of GPs and patients were higher in the
compared with the control group. With regard to the intervention condition compared with the control condition.
remaining patient characteristics, no significant differences With regard to GP utterances, the difference reached
emerged between the control group and the intervention significance (t( 3.2); p = .001).
group. Most migrant patients were Turkish or Surinamese,
or belonged to the ‘other ethnicity’ category (e.g. Cape- 3.2.2. Patients’ communicative behaviour
Verdian, Eastern European migrants, etc.). Table 3 shows the percentages of utterances for each
category made by patients for both the control group and the
3.2. Consultation characteristics intervention group. Patients’ utterances mostly concerned
giving information about their medical condition and
3.2.1. Interview length therapeutic regimen (39%), followed by showing agreement
Mean duration of the medical interview was 8.8 min (26%). There was also some information giving about
(S.D.: 4.8). The interviews in the intervention condition lifestyle and psychosocial issues (11%) and social behaviour
were significantly longer than the interviews in the control (7%). With regard to patients’ verbal behaviour no
condition (both at 1 month after the intervention and at 6 significant differences were found between the intervention
months after the intervention) (mean consultation length of group and the control group on the relative frequencies of the
intervention group: 9.8 min, S.D.: 5.6; mean consultation various RIAS categories.

Table 1
Patient sample
Control group (n = 59) Intervention group (n = 65) Total (n = 124)
Mean age 39.0 (S.D.: 11.9) 40.7 (S.D.: 13.3) 39.9 (S.D.: 12.6)
Sex* (%)
Male 30.5 49.2 40.3
Female 69.5 50.8 59.7
Education (%)
Lower 40.4 41.0 40.7
Medium 25.0 31.1 28.3
Higher 34.6 27.9 31.0
Ethnicity (%)
Surinamese/Antillean 28.8 21.6 25.0
Turkish 23.7 36.9 30.6
Moroccan 6.8 9.2 8.1
Other 40.7 32.3 36.3
Orientation (%)
Western 22.0 13.8 17.7
Non-Western 78.0 86.2 82.3
Proficiency in Dutch (%)
Good 47.5 40.6 43.9
Average 27.1 34.4 30.9
Poor 25.4 25.0 25.2
Religion (%)
Yes, practicing 62.1 67.7 65.0
Yes, not practicing 20.7 23.1 22.0
No religion 17.2 9.2 13.0
*
Significant difference between intervention group and control group on this variable ( p < .05).
292 B.C. Schouten et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 58 (2005) 288–295

Table 3
Patients’ communicative behaviour in control and intervention groups
Behaviour category Control group Intervention group p Total (n = 124) ICC
(n = 59) % (S.D.) (n = 65) % (S.D.) % (S.D.)
Affective behaviour
Social behaviour 7 (5) 6(5) ns 7 (5) G
Agreement 25 (11) 27 (11) ns 26 (11) D
Paraphrase 2 (3) 2 (3) ns 2 (3) G
Showing concern 1 (3) 2 (3) ns 1 (3) G
Reassurance 0 (1) 0 (1) ns 0 (1) D
Disagreement 0(1) 0(1) ns 0 (1) P
Reflect feelings 0 (1) 0 (1) ns 0 (1) G
Total 35 (12) 37 (12) ns 36 (12)

Instrumental behaviour
Giving directions 1 (1) 1 (1) ns 1(1) D
Asking clarification 1 (2) 1 (2) ns 1(1) G
Asking questions
Medical/therapeutical 4 (4) 4 (4) ns 4 (4) G
Lifestyle/psychosocial 1 (4) 0(1) ns 1 (3) P
Giving information
Medical/therapeutical 40 (15) 38 (13) ns 39 (14) G
Lifestyle/psychosocial 12 (13) 11 (11) ns 11 (12) D
Counsels
Medical/therapeutical 0 (0) 0 (0) ns 0 (0) P
Lifestyle/psychosocial 0 (0) 0 (0) ns 0 (0) D
Other utterances 6 (6) 8 (8) 0.08 7 (7) G
Total 65 (12) 63 (12) ns 64 (12)
ICC: intraclass correlation; G: ICC > 0.60; D: 0.40 < ICC < 0.60; P: ICC < 0.40.

Although no main effects of the intervention were found, communication measures as dependent variable.1 No
a number of significant interaction effects emerged between significant effects of the intervention over time on doctors’
some patient characteristics (i.e. educational level and communicative behaviour were found.
cultural orientation) and having received the intervention or
not. With regard to education, the intervention had a
significant positive effect on the percentage of utterances in
the ‘other’ category of RIAS for patients with medium and 4. Discussion and conclusion
higher educational levels, but not for patients with lower
educational level (F = 4.4; p = .01). Furthermore, while no 4.1. Impact of the educational intervention on doctor–
main effect of intervention on giving information on patient interaction
medical/therapeutical issues emerged, we found an inter-
action effect between the intervention and having Western/ In this article we assessed the effects of an educational
non-Western orientation. Patients in the intervention group intervention aimed at improving doctors’ and patients’
gave significantly more medical/therapeutical information communicative behaviour during intercultural medical
compared to patients in the control group when they were encounters. Results show that total interview length
Western-oriented, but not when they were non-Western increased after the intervention by more than 2 min,
oriented (F(4.7); p = .03). suggesting that changes in the communication process did
take place. Also, a significant increase emerged in doctors’
3.2.3. Doctors’ communicative behaviour utterances but not in patients’ utterances. Hence, the
Table 4 summarises the percentages of utterances for increase in interview length seems to be attributable to
each category made by doctors for both the control group doctors talking significantly more to their patients, but less
and the intervention group at the three different measure- so the other way around. Whether the increased talking time
ment times. Doctors’ utterances mostly concerned giving by GPs can be interpreted as a favourable outcome is hard to
medical/therapeutical information, followed by showing tell, because no differences could be detected before and
agreement. There was also some paraphrasing, giving after the intervention on the relative frequencies of the
directions and asking questions about medical/therapeutical 1
ANOVA’s with repeated measures could not based on all consultations,
issues. To test whether the intervention improved doctors’ because as this analysis technique is a within-subject design, it requires data
communicative behaviour a series of ANOVA’s with on all measurement times. As this was not always the case, the lower
repeated measures were carried out, with each of the number of consultations (n = 88) is due to missing data.
B.C. Schouten et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 58 (2005) 288–295 293

Table 4
Doctors’ verbal behaviour in control and intervention groups
Baseline One month after intervention Six months after intervention ICC
Control Intervention Control Intervention Control Intervention
(N = 15) (N = 12) (N = 13) (N = 15) (N = 16) (N = 17)
% S.D. % S.D. % S.D. % S.D. % S.D. % S.D.
Social behaviour 7 2 7 4 8 3 6 3 7 3 6 6 G
Agreement 18 6 20 9 18 7 19 6 17 4 23 9 G
Paraphrase 10 4 9 5 9 5 11 3 8 4 9 4 G
Reflect feelings 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 2 2 1 1 P
Showing concern 1 1 1 1 0 1 0 1 0 1 0 1 G
Reassurance 3 2 3 3 1 1 2 2 2 1 2 1 G
Disagreement 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 P
Total 40 5 41 9 37 8 39 5 36 6 41 10

Giving directions 10 6 8 4 8 4 9 4 8 3 9 5 G
Asking clarification 4 3 3 2 4 3 3 2 4 3 3 2 D
Asking questions medical/therapeutical 9 3 7 3 8 4 9 3 10 4 8 5 G
Asking questions lifestyle/psychosocial 3 3 4 2 3 2 4 2 3 2 3 2 G
Giving information medical/therapeutical 24 9 23 8 23 10 22 7 24 8 22 11 D
Giving information lifestyle/psychosocial 1 2 1 2 4 5 1 1 3 2 2 3 P
Counsels medical/therapeutical 4 3 5 3 5 2 6 3 5 3 4 2 G
Counsels lifestyle/psychosocial 0 1 2 3 1 1 1 1 1 2 1 1 G
Other utterances 5 4 6 5 7 6 6 5 6 5 6 4 G
Total 60 5 59 9 63 8 61 5 64 6 59 10
ICC: intraclass correlation; G: ICC > 0.60; D: 0.40 < ICC < 0.60; P: ICC < 0.40.

various RIAS categories. That is, all categories of talk were [33], in which patients were extensively trained and guided
increased by the same amount, leaving us an unknown in before their medical visit to become more assertive with
terms of which factors this increased encounter length can be their physician.
ascribed to. Still, when combining the fact that there was
better mutual understanding in the intervention group 4.2. Study limitations and prescription for future
compared to the control group [29] with the fact that research
previous research has shown that intercultural consultations
are shorter than intra-cultural consultations, we are inclined An important limitation of this study is that especially in
to conclude that this increased encounter length is probably the GP group, the sample size was low, which may have led
for the better. to power problems of the statistical tests. For instance, as can
Additionally, no main effects of the intervention on be seen from Table 3, there are some communication
affective and instrumental verbal behaviour (as assessed by measures that improved over time in the GPs’ intervention
RIAS) of patients could be detected. However, two group, such as showing agreement and asking for clarifica-
significant interaction effects did emerge, suggesting that tion; these might have been statistically significant with a
the intervention might only have had an effect on specific larger sample size. However, in this kind of research it is
subgroups of migrant patients. It was found that for both very hard to find enough participants, which makes a
higher educated migrant patients and Western-oriented replication study with a larger, more representative sample
migrant patients, the intervention has led to increased unlikely to achieve. Nonetheless, in order to enhance the
percentages in the ‘other’ category of RIAS and in giving generalisability of the results, this study should be replicated
medical/therapeutical information, respectively. So, as far as with other samples and intervention techniques, such as the
RIAS results emerged, it suggests that only Western- one described above.
oriented and/or higher educated patients seem to have A second limitation of this study is the way the
benefited from the intervention. These results point to the observational data were analysed, that is, with frequency
possibility that the mere exposure to videotapes in possibly countings of behavioural units. Although it is beyond dispute
noisy and crowded waiting rooms may not be enough of an that scoring techniques such as the RIAS have their own merit,
intervention to reach all migrant patients, and that only as they provide us with information about the relational
higher educated, Western migrants profited from these aspects of interactions, they fail to present us with details on
videotaped instructions. It would therefore be worthwhile to the dynamics and specific content of doctor–patient interac-
educate these harder-to-reach migrant groups through more tion. With regard to this study, it is plausible to assume that the
extensive interventions, such as the one carried out by Roter intervention did have an effect on doctors’ and patients’
294 B.C. Schouten et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 58 (2005) 288–295

communicative behaviour, as both interview length and GPs’ intercultural communication difficulties in general practice.
utterances significantly increased. The RIAS however may That is, more knowledge is needed about which aspects of
not be suitable to detect more subtle changes in commu- patients’ and doctors’ beliefs, values, knowledge systems
nication patterns. Therefore, we are currently investigating and behaviour play a role in these communication gaps, and
the same data with other observational techniques (i.e. why. For example, doctors in this study were trained to
sequence analysis and discourse analysis), to see to which become more aware of the existence of cultural differences
changes in communicative behaviour this increased encoun- in health views and in strategies to solve gaps in views and
ter length can be attributed to. These techniques also enable us communication styles. While there is no doubt about the
to research the exact content of the ‘other’ category of RIAS, value of such an approach (physicians who are culturally
as this is one of the few categories that increased slightly for sensitive report being more capable of adequate intercultural
all patients, substantially so in the case of higher educated communication and are less anxious during such encounters
migrant patients. [39]), it is questionable that this will suffice in overcoming
Finally, it should be noted that the intraclass correlations all communication gaps between people with different
of a notable number of RIAS categories were low, indicating cultural or ethnic background. For instance, it is also
that various verbal aspects of doctor–patient communication possible that communication problems are not primarily the
were scored unreliable. More empirical research is thus result of lack of knowledge about cultural differences but of
needed on assessing doctor–patient communication reliably linguistic barriers or time management problems during the
within an intercultural context, preferably by making use of consultation. Consequently, research aimed at clarifying
observers belonging to the same ethnic groups as the patients which aspects of the intercultural medical communication
under study. process should be the target of behavioural change, would
greatly enhance our possibilities to design effective
4.3. Practice implications interventions to improve intercultural doctor–patient com-
munication.
To overcome cultural barriers to communication in health
care a number of strategies have been proposed which aim to
increase the cultural competency and sensitivity of health care References
practitioners and the health care system. For example, Brach
and Fraser [34] identified nine major cultural competence [1] Kiesler DJ, Auerbach SM. Integrating measurement of control and
affiliation in studies of physician–patient interaction: the interpersonal
techniques, including the use of interpreter services, circumplex. Soc Sci Med 2003;5:1707–22.
increasing the number of minority staff, training health care [2] Gudykunst WB, Shapiro RB. Communication in everyday interper-
practitioners and coordination with other parties involved sonal and intergroup encounters. Int J Intercult Rel 1996;20:19–45.
such as traditional healers and family members. A technique [3] Gudykunst WB, Matsumoto Y, Ting-Toomey S, Nishida T, Kim K,
to improve intercultural/interethnic doctor–patient commu- Heyman S. The influence of cultural individualism–collectivism, self
construals, and individual values on communication styles across
nication that has received considerable attention are educa- cultures. Hum Commun Res 1996;22:510–43.
tional interventions aimed at increasing doctors’ (and [4] Gudykunst WB. Cultural variability in communication. Commun Res
sometimes patients’) cultural competence [34–37]. Such 1997;24:327–48.
interventions are designed to improve or alter physician’s [5] Brislin R. Understanding culture’s influence on behavior. San Diego:
Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, 1993.
attitudes, knowledge and skills in the context of intercultural
[6] Holtgraves T. Styles of language use: individual and cultural varia-
communication. The idea behind such interventions is that bility in conversational indirectness. J Pers Soc Psychol 1997;73:
difficulties in communicating with migrant patients arise out 624–37.
of a lack of knowledge about the influence of culture on health [7] Meeuwesen L, Harmsen H, Bernsen R, Bruijnzeels M. Differences in
and a deficiency in communicative skills to reach some kind medical communication between ethnic-discordant and ethnic-con-
of mutual understanding with these patients. Enhancing these cordant consultations, Soc Sci Med, submitted for publication.
[8] Cooper-Patrick L, Gallo JJ, Gonzales JJ, Vu HT, Powe NR, Nelson C,
skills and knowledge will help achieve a more adequate Ford DE. Race, gender, and partnership in the patient–physician
negotiating process in which explanatory models can be relationship. J Am Med Assoc 1999;282:583–9.
exchanged and mutually desirable goals reached. Up to now, [9] Cooper LA, Roter DL, Johnson RL, Ford DE, Steinwachs DM,
the empirical evidence for the effectiveness of such Powe NR. Patient-centered communication, ratings of care, and
concordance of patient and physician race. Ann Int Med 2003;139:
interventions is weak. In a review of the empirical literature
907–15.
on the effectiveness of five different types of interventions to [10] Harmsen JAM, Bruijnzeels MA, van der Wouden JC, Bohnen AM.
enhance cultural competence in health care systems, Communicatie tussen huisarts en allochtone ouder [GP consultations
Anderson et al. [38] concluded that empirical evidence is with ethnic minority children. Communication between parents and
insufficient to determine the effectiveness of such interven- the general practitioner]. Huisarts en Wetenschap 1999;42:211–5.
tions, and that more research is needed to arrive at valid [11] van Wieringen JCM, Harmsen JAM, Bruijnzeels MA. Intercultural
communication in general practice. Eur J Public Health 2002;12:63–8.
conclusions. [12] Harmsen JAM. When cultures meet in general practice: improvement
We would like to add that it is necessary to gain more in intercultural communication evaluated. Ph.D. Thesis, Rotterdam:
knowledge about the underlying (cultural) mechanisms of Erasmus University; 2003.
B.C. Schouten et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 58 (2005) 288–295 295

[13] Laveist TA, Nuru-Jeter A. Is doctor–patient race concordance asso- [26] Kleinman A. Patients and healers in the context of culture. London:
ciated with greater satisfaction with care? J Health Soc Behav 2002; University of California Press, 1980.
43:296–306. [27] Kleinman A. The cultural meanings and social uses of illness. J Fam
[14] Gerits YC, Uitenbroek DG, Dijkshoorn H, Verhoeff AP. De commu- Pract 1983;16:539–45.
nicatie tussen huisarts en Turkse en Marokkaanse patiënten nader [28] Pinto D. Intercultural communication: a three-step method for dealing
bekeken [A study about communication between GP and Turkish and with differences. Leuven: Garant, 2000.
Moroccan patients]. Tijdschrift voor Gezondheidswetenschappen [29] Harmsen JAM, Bernsen RMD, Meeuwesen L, Thomas S, Dorrenboom
2001;79:15–20. G, Pinto D, Bruijnzeels MA. The effect of educational intervention on
[15] Stone MA, Patel H, Panja KK, Barnett DB, Mayberry JF. Reasons for intercultural communication: results of a randomised trial. Br J Gen
non-compliance with screening for infection with Helicobacter pylori, Pract 2005;343–50.
in a multi-ethnic community in Leicester, UK. Public Health 1998; [30] ISEO. Beter meten [Measuring better]. Rotterdam: Instituut voor
112:153–6. Sociologisch Economisch Onderzoek, 1987.
[16] Nierkens V, Krumeich A, de Ridder R, van Dongen M. The future of [31] Roter DL. The Roter method of interaction process analysis: RIAS
intercultural mediation in Belgium. Pat Educ Counsel 2002;46:253–9. manual. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University, 1993.
[17] Scharwächter W. Op weg naar ‘interculturele kennisdeling’ [Towards [32] Vlugt van der MJ, Kruk MR, van Erp AMM, Geuze RH. CAMERA: a
intercultural sharing of knowledge]. Tijdschrift gezondheidsvoorlicht- system for fast and reliable acquisition of multiple ethological records.
ing 2002;22–3. Behav Res Meth Instrum Comput 1992;24:147–9.
[18] Helman CG. Culture, health and illness. London: Arnold, 2001. [33] Roter DL. Patient participation in the patient–provider interaction: the
[19] Brown CM, Segal R. The effects of health and treatment perceptions effects of patient question asking on the quality of interaction,
on the use of prescribed medication and home remedies among satisfaction and compliance. Health Educ Monogr 1977;281–315.
African American and white American hypertensives. Soc Sci Med [34] Brach C, Fraser I. Can cultural competency reduce racial and ethnic
1996;43:903–17. disparities? A review and conceptual model. Med Care Res Rev
[20] Lillie-Blanton M, Brodie M, Rowland D, Altman D, McIntosh M. 2000;57:181–217.
Race, ethnicity, and the health care system: public perceptions and [35] Carrillo JE, Green AR, Betancourt JR. Cross-cultural primary care: a
experiences. Med Care Res Rev 2000;57:218–35. patient based approach. Ann Int Med 1999;130:829–34.
[21] Naish J, Brown J, Denton B. Intercultural consultations: investigation of [36] Kagawa-Singer M, Kassim-Lakha S. A strategy to reduce cross-
factors that deter non-English speaking women from attending their cultural miscommunication and increase the likelihood of improving
general practitioner for cervical screening. Br Med J 1994;309:1126–8. health outcomes. Acad Med 2003;78:577–87.
[22] Patel S. Intercultural consultations; language is not the only barrier. Br [37] McNaughton Dunn A. Cultural competence and the primary care
Med J 1995;310:194. provider. J Pediat Health Care 2002;16:105–11.
[23] Hofstede G. Culture’s consequences. Thousand Oaks: Sage, 2001. [38] Anderson LM, Scrimshaw SC, Fullilove MT, Fielding JE, Normand J.
[24] Markus HR, Kitayama S. Culture and the self: implications for Culturally competent healthcare systems: a systematic review. Am J
cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol Rev 1991;98:224–53. Prev Med 2003;24:68–79.
[25] D’Andrade R. Some propositions about the relations between culture [39] Ulrey KL, Amason P. Intercultural communication between patients
and human cognition. In: Stigler JW, Shweder RA., editors. Cultural and health care providers: an exploration of intercultural communica-
psychology, essays on comparative human development. New York: tion effectiveness, cultural sensitivity, stress, and anxiety. Health
Cambridge University Press, 1990. Commun 2001;13:449–63.

You might also like