Effort-Rewardimbalanceat Work:theeffectsofworkstress Onangerandcardiovascular Diseasesymptomsina Communitysample

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 16

Stress and Health

Stress and Health 21: 113–128 (2005)


Published online 3 March 2005 in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI: 10.1002/smi.1045

Effort–reward imbalance at
work: the effects of work stress
on anger and cardiovascular
disease symptoms in a
community sample
Luke A. Smith,1,*,† Angela Roman,1 Maureen F. Dollard,1 Anthony H. Winefield1 and
Johannes Siegrist2
1
Work and Stress Research Group, University of South Australia, Australia
2
University of Duesseldorf, Germany

Summary
Two studies of the Effort–Reward Imbalance (ERI) model of work stress with different end points
were undertaken in community samples from the Adelaide (Australia) metropolitan area. Study
1 examined the relationship between ERI at work and state anger. Study 2 extended the first
and examined the relationship between ERI, anger and cardiovascular disease (CVD) symptoms.
Hierarchical multiple regression confirmed a significant relationship between ERI and state anger
in Study 1 even after controlling for extraneous variables. Study 2 showed that ERI increases
CVD symptoms via the mediating variable of anger (an indirect effect). Furthermore, Study 2
revealed that people of lower income were more likely to experience higher anger. The results of
both studies have far ranging consequences for the emotional and physical health of individuals
who are experiencing an imbalance between their efforts and perceived rewards at work and those
with lower incomes may be more vulnerable. The wider public health implications of the rela-
tionship between work stress, and emotional and physical well being in the community are dis-
cussed. Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Key Words
effort–reward imbalance (ERI) model; work stress; state anger; trait anger; cardiovascular disease
(CVD)

Work stress—effort–reward imbalance occupational demands shifting from the physical


to the psychological and emotional domain. One
The nature of work has undergone immense of the most significant and alarming develop-
changes in the last few decades with the focus of ments is an increase in psychosocial risk factors
or work related stress (de Jonge, Bosma, Peter, &
Siegrist, 2000; van Vegchel, de Jonge, Meijer, &
* Correspondence to: Luke Smith, 73 Maple Avenue, Hamers, 2001). One theory of work stress and
Rostrevor 5073, SA, Australia. Tel: (08) 8365 4575; well-being is the ‘Effort–Reward Imbalance’
(08) 8372 2178; Mobile: 0408 085 016. (ERI) model. In the ERI model, a person’s occu-

E-mail: luke@alzheimerssa.asn.au pation is regarded as a vehicle for the fulfilment
Received 30 April 2004
Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Revised 6 December 2004
Accepted 15 December 2004
L. Smith et al.

of vital emotional and motivational needs. The approval are said to have ‘overcommitment’.
model was developed in order to account for the This personal pattern of coping, which is similar
increasing rates of cardiovascular disease (CVD) to the Type A Behaviour Pattern, is characterized
and related health problems of workers in many by excessive striving driven by a strong desire for
countries. The model developed by medical approval and esteem (de Jonge et al., 2000;
sociologist Johannes Siegrist assumes that adverse Tsutsumi, Ishitake, Peter, Siegrist, & Matoba,
health effects can be produced by a lack of reci- 2001). People who are high in overcommitment
procity between efforts and rewards at work will find an imbalance between their efforts and
(Siegrist, 1996). rewards particularly stressful and are consequently
More specifically, the model proposes that at a high risk of developing health problems
effort at work is part of a socially organized (Cooper, 1998; Kuper, Singh-Manoux, Siegrist, &
exchange process. In the workplace perceived Marmot, 2002; van Vegchel et al., 2001).
effort spent is viewed in terms of psychological High rates of ERI have been found in occupa-
and physical demands. The effort of employees is tions that deal with person-based interactions.
maintained by occupational rewards, which are In other words, ERI has been found to be more
provided by society. Societal rewards are distrib- prevalent in service occupations and professions
uted to an employee via three transmitter systems. (Bakker, Killmer, Siegrist, & Schaufeli, 2000;
These systems include money, esteem and career Kutash & Schlesinger, 1980). Kuper et al. (2002)
opportunities/job security. state that people of a lower socio-economic status
Siegrist (1996) argues that ERI has negative (SES) are also at a greater risk of suffering from
mental and physical outcomes for an individual. negative health consequences resulting from a
He suggests that an imbalance of effort and stressful work environment. This is attributed to
reward in employment, specifically high effort the fact that low SES people may not have the
associated with low reward, can be stressful to material resources, social support and skills that
the individual as such an imbalance violates a enable them to overcome stress related problems.
basic norm of social exchange, i.e. reciprocity. Despite the stress and unfulfilling conditions
A study conducted by Calnan, Wainwright, and produced by ERI, workers under such conditions
Almond (2000) found that key components of the do not always modify their work behaviour to
ERI model were significant predictors of work combat the imbalance, as one might expect.
stress and job satisfaction in employees of general Rather, under certain conditions, individuals suf-
medical practices. Other examples of a stressful fering from a mismatch between effort expended
imbalance include having a demanding and at work and rewards received continue their
unstable job or achieving great accomplishments working behaviour, thus maintaining the high
in the absence of possible promotional pathways cost/low gain conditions. This could be due to a
(de Jonge et al., 2000; Peter, Geibler, & Siegrist, lack of alternative options in the work domain,
1998). A lack of reciprocity between efforts and such that the prospect of being laid off, demoted
rewards may cause a state of emotional distress or unemployed is seen as worse than the cost of
that can lead to strain reactions like poor health high effort with insufficient rewards (Peter &
and psychopathological problems (van Vegchel et Siegrist, 1997; Siegrist, 1996, 1998).
al., 2001).
Research demonstrates that stress affects
people in different ways. The ERI model accounts ERI—emotional health/anger
for this by emphasizing the importance of certain
individual attributes. The demands or workload One of the most important and neglected areas
of a job is described as ‘extrinsic effort’ (Bosma of organizational psychology is an understanding
et al., 1998; Cooper, 1998; Peter et al., 1998). of the causes, characteristics, and consequences
The coping characteristics of a person are of emotions in the workplace (Brief & Weiss,
described as ‘intrinsic effort’ and are based on a 2002; Fitness, 2000). Components of the ERI
need for control. Intrinsic effort influences the model of work stress have been linked with emo-
motivation of an employee in demanding situa- tional strain/distress and psychological outcomes
tions. It is developed over a long-term period in such as depression, anxiety, psychiatric disorder
individuals and may therefore mediate the effects and emotional burnout (Bakker et al., 2000;
of stress. People with high intrinsic effort who Bennett, Lowe, Matthews, Dourali, & Tattersal,
inflate their efforts in order to gain esteem and 2001; Stansfeld, Fuhrer, Shipley, & Marmot,

114 Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Stress and Health 21: 113–128 (2005)
Effort-reward imbalance at work

1999; Tsutsumi, Kayaba, Theorell, & Siegrist, affected by employees who express their anger
2001). through poor work performance, threats, harass-
A popular debate in the literature is the role of ment, acts of sabotage against their employers,
negative affectivity in the work stress process. and physical attacks (Allcorn, 1994).
The ‘reverse causation’ theory argues that people Anger is often referred to in the literature as a
with high negative affectivity such as anger, are strong negative emotional state that has many
cynical towards their work environment because functions, which include energizing behaviour,
they tend to focus on the negative aspects of their the expression of negative affect towards others,
world. However, other researchers believe that defending against ego threat and a vehicle for
the work environment itself can increase the active coping. The main consequence of anger
negative affectivity or trait emotions of indi- is the disruption of behaviour through agitation
viduals. Consequently, controlling for negative and cognitive interference (Allcorn, 1994;
affectivity could produce an underestimation of Diamond; 1982; Douglas & Martinko, 2001).
the influence of work stressors on strain (Dollard The emotional state of anger can vary from mild
& Winefield, 1998). In the current study both of annoyance to extreme rage (Diamond, 1982;
these hypotheses were investigated. Fehr & Russell, 1994). The literature on anger
Research shows that anger is a frequently expe- generally discusses two types of anger. ‘State
rienced emotion. A community survey by Siegel anger’ is defined as an immediate emotional-
(1986) showed that over 75 per cent of those physiological response to a particular event.
sampled had been angry within 1 week. Some Whereas state anger refers to an immediate
researchers suggest that most people will experi- response, trait anger involves a predisposition to
ence annoyance and potential anger at least 10 experience state anger over time and events. Trait
times a day (Bensimon, 1997). The workplace has anger reflects a tendency to be more easily
been identified as one of the most frustrating envi- angered, respond with a higher intensity of anger
ronments that people are faced with and as a when provoked and to cope less well with anger
result, it is likely many people experience anger eliciting events. It is often seen as a stable per-
at work, regardless of whether or not this feeling sonality trait (Deffenbacher et al., 1996; Douglas
is expressed (Allcorn, 1994; Bensimon, 1997; & Martinko, 2001).
Fitness, 2000). It is estimated that approximately
one quarter of all employees in the U.S.A. feel
chronically angry at work (Johnson & Indvik, ERI—physical health
2001). The causes of anger in the workplace
range from immediate influences such as per- The most common physical health outcome that
ceived treatment at work to wider factors such as has been linked to ERI concerns the cardiovascu-
the changing structure of the nature of work lar system (Caltabiano & Sarafino, 2002;
(Bensimon, 1997; Diamond, 1982; Fitness, 2000; Kivimaki et al., 2002; Kuper et al., 2002). Mea-
Neuman & Baron, 1998). sures of CVD, including coronary heart disease
Chen and Spector (1992) conducted a study (CHD), coronary artery disease (CAD) and
that examined the relationship between aggres- studies focussing on symptoms of these diseases
sion, withdrawal, theft, and substance abuse. A are common in the stress-health literature. Two
sample of 400 employees from various occupa- separate studies by Bosma et al. (1998) and Kuper
tions was measured on five work stressors: role et al. (2002) analysed samples from the Whitehall
ambiguity, role conflict, interpersonal conflict, II study. Both studies found that a high ratio of
situational constraints, and workload. The state efforts to rewards was a significant risk factor for
anger scale, taken from the State-Trait Personal- CHD. The indicators of CHD in this study were
ity Inventory (STPI) (Spielberger et al., 1979), and angina pectoris and doctor-diagnosed ischemia.
three items related to feelings of stress were Even though several mechanisms have been doc-
applied. The researchers found that job stress was umented in experimental studies, a unified psy-
correlated with state anger, and that anger was chophysiological basis linking work stress with
related to aggressive behaviour. The authors sug- CVD is still lacking (Heslop, Davey-Smith,
gested that work stressors may create feelings of Metcalfe, Macleod, & Hart, 2002; Stansfeld &
frustration in employees that can lead to feelings Marmot, 2002).
of anger and acts of aggression. Research has The causal link or mechanism between ERI
shown employee morale and productivity are and specific health outcomes has been commonly

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Stress and Health 21: 113–128 (2005) 115
L. Smith et al.

hypothesized in two main theories. The first is Study 1: State anger and ERI at work
that ERI has a direct biological effect on the
development of CVD and this is supported by Previous research has demonstrated that state
epidemiological evidence. A review of cross- anger is an emotion that is experienced in the
sectional studies shows that ERI leads to sym- workplace with increasing frequency. Although a
pathetic nervous system (SNS) arousal, which is number of possible links between anger and ERI
associated with high blood pressure, increased have been discussed in the literature (Diamond,
risk of myocardial infarction, increased choles- 1982) no reported research has examined the
terol and higher heart rate (Cohen, Kessler, & relationship empirically. Factors such as demo-
Underwood-Gordon, 1995; Peter, Siegrist, graphic variables, personal disposition variables,
Hallqvist, Reuterwall, & Theorell, 2002; Vrijkotte, and life satisfaction were also of interest in the
Doornen, & de Geus, 1999). The second theory present study as they were assumed to affect the
suggests an indirect mechanism for the effect of relationship between the variables of interest and
ERI on CVD symptoms. Stress resulting from ERI state anger. This model is summarized in Figure
can lead to the uptake of smoking, substance 1.
abuse, bad dietary habits and restrict compliance Study 1 aimed to determine whether a rela-
to medical regimes (Kuper et al., 2002; Siegman tionship existed between reported ERI and
& Dembrowski, 1989). reported state anger in a community sample of
The third theory is that emotion, in particular adults in paid employment in Adelaide, South
anger, plays an important role in the development Australia and tested the following hypothesis:
of CVD (Deffenbacher et al., 1996; Kop, 1999;
Mona, Fitzmaurice, Kubzansky, Rimm, & Hypothesis 1: ERI would be positively
Kawachi, 2003; VanderVoort & Ragland, 1996; related to state anger, and this relationship
Wenneberg & Schneider, 1997). Diamond (1982) would be evident after controlling for life
undertook a critical analysis of the literature con- satisfaction, trait anger, overcommitment
cerning the role of anger in hypertension and and the demographic variables; age, sex, job
CHD. He concluded that anger and hostility industry, length of service, and job category.
appeared to play an important role in the devel-
opment of hypertension, although the mechanism
for the relationship was not identified. On the Method
whole, the research review provided support for
the proposal that ‘emotion’ is relevant to under- Participants. Data were collected from 102
standing the psychophysiological mechanisms participants from three metropolitan community
associated with CVD. More recently Gallacher, areas of Adelaide, South Australia (response rate
Yarnell, Sweetnam, Elwood, and Stansfeld (1999) of 81 per cent). The participants were selected
argue that numerous studies have shown that from three different areas of SES.
anger is related to CVD. They conclude that there
is an increased risk of CVD from 1.5 to 2.6 for Procedure. Byrne and Reinhart (1989) suggest
high levels of anger compared to low levels of that studies of CVD should not be restricted to
anger.
Wenneberg and Schneider (1997) utilized
state/trait and anger expression scales to in-
Demographics
vestigate potential risks for CHD. They found
collagen-induced platelet aggregation was posi-
tively correlated with both trait and state anger. Personal Disposition State Anger
Platelet aggregation is regarded as a precipitant
of acute coronary events and a risk factor for
CHD. Similar to ERI, anger can directly cause an Life Satisfaction
increase of CVD symptoms through physiologi-
cal arousal such as heart rate, or indirectly via Effort-Reward
Imbalance
psychosocial factors such as diet (Cohen et al.,
1995; Hemingway, 1995; Marmot & Wilkinson,
1999). Figure 1. ERI to state anger model.

116 Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Stress and Health 21: 113–128 (2005)
Effort-reward imbalance at work

samples of middle-aged, middle-class males in trait anger scale (alpha = 0.86) instructed par-
professional or managerial occupations. There- ticipants to respond to statements such as ‘I am
fore, suburbs for distribution of questionnaires quick tempered’ with the general frequency with
were randomly selected from a list of Local Gov- which the statements were true on a four-point
ernment Area (LGA) values in metropolitan scale. The state anger scale (alpha = 0.85) con-
Adelaide (Real Estate Institute of South Australia, tained 10 items relating to how the participant
2002), ensuring that each of the three suburbs was feeling at the time of completing the ques-
were high, medium and low SES (based on prop- tionnaire, and thus contained items such as ‘I am
erty value). This was achieved to maximize the furious’. The response format was a Likert scale
range of types of occupations and SES levels. Five ranging from almost never (1) to almost always
streets were randomly selected and approached in (4) for the trait anger scale, and not at all (1), to
each suburb via a doorknock, which occurred very much so (4) for the state anger scale.
during the evening (6:00–8:00 p.m.) on week-
nights for 4 weeks. Upon approaching a house, Life satisfaction. The satisfaction with life
the researcher introduced herself, explained the scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985)
nature of the study, and requested that the indi- followed a Likert-scale format. It consisted of five
vidual complete the questionnaire. Upon agree- statements such as ‘In most ways my life is close
ment to participate, individuals were presented to ideal’ (item 1), and subjects were required to
with a letter outlining the nature of the study, indicate their level of agreement with each state-
participation requirements, and further contacts ment. The response structure contained seven
regarding queries. Recruits were asked to com- responses, including strongly disagree (scored as
plete the questionnaire and seal it in the envelope 1), disagree, slightly disagree, neither agree nor
provided for collection by the researcher the next disagree, slightly agree, agree, and strongly agree
day. (scored as 7). Addition of the scores on each item
revealed a single life satisfaction score of between
Measures—ERI. ERI was measured with the 5 and 35 for each participant. Reliability analy-
23-item ERI scale (Siegrist, 1996). Extrinsic effort sis revealed an alpha coefficient of 0.86 for this
(alpha = 0.75) was measured by six items. scale.
Reward (alpha = 0.85) was measured by 11 items,
which was further broken down into status, Job satisfaction. Job satisfaction was measured
esteem and security rewards. Participants were by a single item taken from the Job Satisfaction
asked to indicate whether they agreed or dis- Scale (Warr, Cook, & Wall, 1979). The item
agreed with statements, such as ‘I have constant consisted of the question ‘Taking everything into
time pressure due to a heavy work load’ for consideration how do you feel about your job?’
extrinsic effort and ‘I receive the respect I deserve Participants were required to indicate how satis-
from my colleagues’ for reward. Participants fied they were with their job on a seven point
who agreed with these statements were further scale, ranging from extremely dissatisfied (scored
required to indicate the level of distress they felt as 1), to extremely satisfied (scored as 7).
as a result of the circumstances addressed in the Research has shown that a single item measure is
item. Responses were scored on a five-point scale; an accurate representation of overall job satisfac-
0 points allocated to a response of disagree and tion (Wanous, Reichers, & Hudy, 1997).
5 allocated to a response of ‘I am very distressed’.
Overcommitment (alpha = 0.75) was measured Demographic information. The questionnaire
by six items, such as ‘I get easily overwhelmed by asked for information regarding age and sex.
time pressures at work’. Participants were asked Furthermore, participants were asked to indicate
to respond to the Likert-type format by indicat- job sector (public or private sector), the number
ing their level of agreement with each statement of years they had been employed at that job,
on a four-point scale, ranging from strongly dis- and which of five categories described their job
agree to strongly agree. best (manager/administrator, professional/semi-
professional, tradesperson, clerical/sales/service,
Anger. Both the trait and state anger scales labourer). Categories were obtained from the
were taken from the STPI (Spielberger et al., nine categories of the Australian Bureau of
1979). The trait anger scales consisted of 10 items Statistics (1997) and modified to create the five
regarding the disposition of the individual. The categories in the present study.

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Stress and Health 21: 113–128 (2005) 117
L. Smith et al.

Table I. Means (M), standard deviations (SD), ranges, and reliability


coefficients for continuous variables.
Variable N M SD Range Alpha
Age 102 38.57 11.83 18–64
Years at job 101 6.59 8.26 0–42
ERI 97 0.52 0.29 0.2–2.34
Overcommitment 101 13.44 3.11 6–22 0.75
Trait anger 97 16.58 4.46 10–32 0.86
State anger 100 11.22 2.59 10–26 0.85
Life satisfaction 101 23.26 6.67 8–35 0.86
Job satisfaction 101 4.98 1.22 1–7

Results Table II. Frequencies and percentages for categorical


variables.
Demographics of participants. The study con- Variable N Percentage
sisted of participants ranging in ages from 18 to
64 (mean, M = 39 years; standard deviation SD Gender
= 12 years). The sample was relatively even in Male 52 51.0
terms of sex (Males = 52, Females = 50). Descrip- Female 50 49.0
Job sector
tive statistics of the sample are summarized in
Private 63 61.8
Tables I and II. Public 37 36.2
Missing 2 2.0
Correlations between variables. Table III Job category
demonstrates the correlations between variables Manager/administrator 18 17.6
of interest employed in the study. Correlations Professional/semi-professional 51 50.0
coefficients were defined as small when r = 0.20, Tradesperson 10 9.8
as medium when r = 0.30, and as large when Clerical/sales/service 17 16.7
Labourer 6 5.9
r = 0.50 (Cohen, 1992).

Correlations with state anger. As anticipated,


ERI correlated positively with state anger,
showing a medium size correlation, r (93) = 0.41,
p < 0.01. Trait anger was also found to correlate Discussion
with state anger, however this relationship
was small, r (95) = 0.26, p < 0.01. A small The results supported the hypothesis that ERI is
negative relationship was demonstrated between associated with state anger. Of all the variables,
state anger and life satisfaction, r (97) = -0.25, ERI shows the largest positive correlation with
p < 0.05. state anger. Furthermore, results of a hierarchical
regression analysis demonstrated that after con-
Hierarchical regression analyses. A hierarchi- trolling for factors, such as demographics, over-
cal multiple regression analysis was conducted to commitment, trait anger, and life satisfaction, ERI
determine whether a relationship existed between made an independent contribution to levels of
ERI and state anger (hypothesis 1), after con- state anger. This finding suggests that individuals
trolling for possible confounding variables. The who suffer from an imbalance of effort and
results of the regression analysis are presented in reward at work, such that they contribute greater
Table IV. efforts to their work than they receive in reward,
As Table IV shows, the only independent vari- have the propensity to feel angrier than those who
ables that were significant predictors of state are not experiencing ERI. Further, levels of anger
anger after controlling for other variables, were in the community may have their origins in stress-
ERI and trait anger. ful conditions at work.

118 Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Stress and Health 21: 113–128 (2005)
Table III. Intercorrelations of variables.
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13

1. Age —

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.


2. Sex -0.11 —
3. Job sector -0.24* -0.09 —
4. Length of service 0.51** -0.28** -0.12 —
5. Manager/administrator† 0.20* 0.01 0.09 0.14 —
6. Professional/semi-professional† 0.20* -0.12 -0.15 0.15 -0.46** —
7. Tradesperson† -0.29** -0.19 0.12 -0.11 -0.15 -0.33** —
8. Clerical/sales/service† -0.14 0.30** -0.01 -0.22* -0.21* -0.45** -0.15 —
9. Labourer† -0.17 0.01 0.02 -0.08 -0.12 -0.25* -0.08 -0.11 —
10. Overcommitment 0.15 -0.07 0.03 0.06 0.15 0.09 -0.09 -0.24* 0.05 —
11. Trait anger -0.30** -0.02 0.08 -0.13 -0.04 -0.25* 0.24* 0.07 0.18 0.01 —
12. Life satisfaction 0.00 0.06 0.04 0.07 0.05 0.08 -0.13 0.09 -0.23* -0.08 -0.25* —
13. ERI -0.02 -0.17 0.16 0.08 -0.03 -0.05 0.03 -0.09 0.25* 0.27** 0.09 -0.42** —
14. State anger -0.13 -0.09 0.11 0.07 0.01 -0.14 -0.00 0.07 0.17 0.17 0.26** -0.25* 0.41**

** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05. Sex: 1 = male, 2 = female; job sector: 1 = public, 2 = private.

Variables dummy coded from job category.

Stress and Health 21: 113–128 (2005)


Effort-reward imbalance at work

119
L. Smith et al.

Table IV. Hierarchical regression analysis predicting state anger.


Source DR2 df B (each step) B (full model) t
Step 1
Demographics 0.099 8,82
Age -0.189 -0.186 -1.421
Sex -0.114 -0.058 -0.552
Job sector 0.081 0.006 0.061
Years in job 0.212 0.181 1.514
Manager 0.059 0.046 0.430
Tradesperson -0.035 -0.084 -0.749
Clerical 0.144 0.186 1.692
Labourer 0.172 0.027 0.250
Step 2
Personal disposition 0.085* 2,80
Overcommitment 0.222* 0.134 1.260
Trait anger 0.210 0.182 1.743*
Step 3
Life satisfaction 0.049* 1,79 -0.242* -0.131 -1.148
Step 4
ERI 0.055* 1,78 0.283* 0.283* 2.466*
Full model: R2 = 0.288, Adjusted R2 = 0.179, F(12,78) = 2.63
* p < 0.05, t values derived from final model.

Demographics
CVD
Age Anger In Symptoms
Sex Trait Angina
Average Yearly Salary Intermittent-
Clerical/sales/service Anger
Claudication
Anger Out Myocardial-
Infarction
Effort Reward Blood Pressure
Referral
Imbalance

Figure 2. Stress/trait model.

Study 2: Trait anger, ERI at work and CVD Study 2 two models were tested. In the first
model, ERI was assumed to influence trait anger
Previous research has shown that ERI is related (Dollard & Winefield, 1998). Consequently, trait
to negative emotional outcomes, such as anger anger was assumed to influence an individual’s
as well as physical outcomes, such as CVD and chance of having CVD symptoms either directly
musculoskeletal problems. Furthermore, the way or through the anger expression variables of
individuals express anger has also been found to anger in or anger out. Clerical/sales/service was
influence their chance of showing CVD symptoms included in the model because it has been found
with most research highlighting the role of anger to be the occupational group with the highest
suppression (anger in). Study 2 was based on the prevalence of ERI (Kutash & Schlesinger, 1980).
model in Study 1 with a few alterations. The vari- It was decided not to incorporate overcommit-
able of state anger was replaced by trait anger, ment as a variable in the model because of its lack
given that trait anger (how you have been feeling of a significant relationship with anger in Study
generally) may be a more relevant assessment of 1. The model is summarized in Figure 2.
emotional reaction to the chronic stressor of ERI An alternative theory is that trait anger influ-
(personal communication by Professor Charles ences an individual’s perception of ERI at work.
Spielberger to Luke Smith, April 14, 2003). In This theory of ‘reverse causation’ was also

120 Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Stress and Health 21: 113–128 (2005)
Effort-reward imbalance at work

explored in the study through a second model item scale, which was used in Study 1. In the
that swapped the places of trait anger and ERI in current study participants were not asked to agree
the Stress/Trait Model. This model was called the or disagree with the statement and then indicate
Reverse Causation Model (Cohen et al., 1995; their level of distress but instead to indicate on a
Dollard & Winefield, 1998). four-point scale the extent to which they agree or
Study 2 aimed to determine if a relationship disagree with the statement. Replacing the ‘dis-
exists between ERI, trait anger and CVD symp- tressed’ component of the scale with a four-point
toms in a community sample of adults in paid agree/disagree scale was hypothesized to reduce
employment in Adelaide, South Australia. the affective evaluative aspect of the scale and
problems associated with common method vari-
The study aimed to test the following ance and conceptual overlap with affective out-
hypotheses in the Stress/Trait Model: comes (Dollard & de Jonge, 2003). Furthermore,
(1) Trait anger would be positively asso- in Study 1 participants encountered many prob-
ciated with CVD symptoms. lems with the ERI scale including confusion with
(2) Trait anger would mediate the rela- the use of the rating scale.
tionship between ERI and CVD Responses on the modified scale were scored on
symptoms. a four-point scale ranging from ‘strongly disagree’
(3) Anger in (anger suppression) and (1) to ‘strongly agree’ (4). Extrinsic effort (alpha
anger out (overt expression) would = 0.74) was measured by six items and reward
mediate the relationship between trait (alpha = 0.80) was measured by 11 items.
anger and CVD symptoms.
Trait anger. The same measure was used as in
The study aimed to test the following
Study 1 (alpha = 0.83).
hypotheses in the Reverse Causation Model:
(4) ERI would be positively associated
Anger expression. The anger expression vari-
with CVD symptoms.
ables of anger in (suppression), anger out (overt
(5) ERI would mediate the relationship
expression) and anger control were assessed
between trait anger CVD symptoms
using Spielberger’s anger expression (AX) scale
in the Reverse Causation Model.
(Spielberger et al., 1986). All 24 items were pre-
(6) Anger in (anger suppression) and
sented under a general headline of ‘When angry
anger out (overt expression) would
or furious I . . .’ and participants were asked to
mediate the relationship between ERI
respond to how often they generally reacted or
and CVD symptoms.
behaved in the manner described in the state-
ments. Anger in (alpha = 0.79) was measured by
Method six items involving statements such as ‘I control
my anger’. The scale was scored with 1 point for
Participants. Data were collected from 118 ‘almost never’ through to 4 points for ‘almost
participants from three socio-economic metro- always’.
politan areas of Adelaide, South Australia Anger out or anger expression was measured
(response rate 73 per cent). The participants were by six items from the AX scale (alpha = 0.76).
selected from three different areas of SES. Each item contained a statement such as ‘I argue
with others’ and a response format identical to
Procedure. The procedure used in this study the one used in the anger in scale.
was similar to the procedure used in Study 1,
except that the targeted suburbs for question- CVD symptoms. According to Siegrist (per-
naire distribution were randomly selected from sonal communication, March 11, 2003) the
Adelaide Metropolitan areas from the latest London School of Hygiene Questionnaire on
Australian Bureaus of Statistics census of popu- Chest Pain is still the most valid self-report
lation and housing (Cretteden, 2001). Three measure for CVD symptoms. This questionnaire
suburbs were taken from each of a high-income, measured the CVD symptoms of angina, possible
medium-income and low-income household. myocardial infarction and intermittent claudica-
tion (Rose, McCartney, & Ried, 1977). Angina
Measures—ERI. This was measured by a mod- (Part A) and intermittent claudication (Part C)
ified version of Siegrist’s (1996) original ERI 23- were measured using self-report checklists involv-

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Stress and Health 21: 113–128 (2005) 121
L. Smith et al.

ing questions with a ‘yes or no’ to questions such myocardial infarction, high blood pressure and a
as ‘have you ever had pain or discomfort in your referral to a doctor would be given a score of 3
chest?’ Other questions related to certain charac- out of 5 for CVD symptoms.
teristics of a symptom, i.e. the response of 10
minutes or less to ‘how much time does it take Demographic information. The questionnaire
until the pain goes away?’ Part B measured pos- asked participants for the same demographic
sible myocardial infarction or possible ‘heart information as in Study 1. Average yearly salary
attack’ and instructed participants who had expe- was included as a SES indicator because of its link
rienced chest pain to mark its position with an X. with work stress vulnerability and negative health
Throughout the questionnaire small alterations consequences (Kuper et al., 2002).
were made to the original questions and some
questions removed in order to improve the relia-
bility of the questionnaire. Results
Many researchers argue that the use of a
questionnaire as an indicator of CVD can be Demographics of participants. The study con-
problematic (Bosma et al., 1998: Ramsay, sisted of participants ranging in ages 19 to 72 (M
McDermott, & Bray, 2001). Therefore in order = 39 years, SD = 13 years). The sample involved
to improve and increase the data on CVD symp- 69 males (58.5 per cent) and 49 females (41.5 per
toms in the current study, two additional ques- cent). The descriptive statistics for the study are
tions were added in Part D after the sections from summarized in Tables V and VI.
the London School of Hygiene Questionnaire.
Question A concerned blood pressure and asked Statistical analysis. Alterations were made to
‘In the past two years have you had your blood the data to meet the basic assumptions of the sta-
pressure taken by a health professional or used tistical analyses. To improve the power of average
a self-administered machine?’ Participants who yearly salary and ERI, missing data were con-
indicated that they had high blood pressure, verted into the mean of their original variables. A
which is a known symptom and risk factor for Mahalanobis Analysis revealed that two cases
CVD, were assigned a score of 1. Question B violated the critical limit for multivariate outliers
asked, ‘In the past two years have you consulted and were consequently excluded from the path
a health professional with regard to chest pain, analyses.
heart attack, or any other cardiovascular related
health problems?’ This addition is similar to Path analyses. Path analysis is a form of
those made in other studies that have utilized Structural Equation Modelling and enables the
the London School of Hygiene Questionnaire examination of direct and indirect associations
on Chest Pain (Bullpitt et al., 1990; Yarnell, between one or more independent variables and
Sweetnam, Baker, & Bainton, 1988). Participants one or more dependent variables. A direct asso-
who answered ‘yes’ to this question were allo- ciation concerns the path coefficient between two
cated a score of 1. The ‘CVD symptoms’ variable variables, while an indirect association connects
was computed by adding the number of CVD one variable with another variable through the
symptoms that any one participant had out of a significant paths of other variables. It differs from
score of five. For example, a person with possible other linear equation models in that mediated

Table V. Means (M), Standard deviations (SD), ranges, and reliability


coefficients for continuous variables.
Variable N M SD Range Alpha
Age 118 38.64 13.30 19–72
Months at job 117 110.00 124.62 1–516
Salary 113 $50,333 $29,214 $5,200–$150,000
ERI 104 1.01 0.32 0.46–2.75
Trait anger 113 17.73 4.84 10–34 0.83
Anger in 116 16.03 4.06 8–28 0.79
Anger out 118 13.65 3.53 8–24 0.76

122 Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Stress and Health 21: 113–128 (2005)
Effort-reward imbalance at work

pathways can be examined, however the direction theory, regarding causality within the path model.
of these relationships cannot be established. A path diagram may involve two kinds of vari-
Instead, the one-headed arrows between variables ables: exogenous and endogenous. Exogenous
represent predetermined hypotheses taken from variables on the left hand side of the diagram
have no incoming arrows and the model does not
try to explain them. Endogenous variables do
Table VI. Frequencies and percentages for categorical have incoming arrows and can be either inter-
variables. vening or dependent variables. Arrows pointing
Variable N Percentage directly to an endogenous variable without a vari-
able preceding it is indicating the variance of the
Sex residual (unexplained variance). Path coefficients
Male 69 58.5 (standardized regression coefficients taken from a
Female 49 41.5
Job sector
multiple regression) are used to show the direct
Private 75 63.6 effect of an independent variable on a dependent
Public 42 35.6 variable in path model. When the model has two
Missing 1 0.8 or more causal variables, path coefficients are
Job category standardized partial regression coefficients, with
Manager/administrator 29 24.5 previous variables controlled for with the use of
Professional/semi-Professional 56 47.5 standardized data (Klem, 1995).
Tradesperson 8 6.8
Clerical/sales/service 21 17.8
Labourer 4 3.4
Correlations. The correlations for all of the
Angina variables used in the path analyses are shown in
Angina 8 6.8 Table VII. The only variable that was significantly
No Angina 110 93.2 correlated with CVD symptoms was trait anger
Possible myocardial infarction r(109) = 0.23, p < 0.05. Furthermore, ERI was
Possible myocardial infarction 23 19.5 associated only with trait anger r(109) = 0.22,
No myocardial infarction 95 80.5 p < 0.05.
Intermittent claudication
Possible intermittent claudication 21 17.8
No intermittent claudication 97 82.2
Fit of models. To determine the fit for the
Blood pressure Stress/Trait Model presented in Figure 2, the
Not measured 25 21.6 implied correlations (sum of direct effects, indirect
Low 13 11.5 effects, spurious effects and the unanalysed effects)
Normal 65 55.4 were calculated for all pairs of variables. By com-
High 13 11.5 paring the implied correlations with the observed
Referral to doctor correlations, it is possible to determine how well
No 100 84.7 the model reproduced the original correlations.
Yes 16 13.6
Missing 2 1.7
The average of the absolute differences
between the observed and implied correlations is

Table VII. Correlations for all variables in the path analyses.


1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. Age
2. Sex -0.21*
3. Salary 0.26** -0.37**
4. Clerical/sales/service† -0.06 0.24** -0.34**
5. ERI 0.05 -0.02 -0.01 -0.16
6. Trait anger -0.30** 0.07 -0.29** 0.14 0.22*
7. Anger out -0.23* 0.02 -0.18 0.13 0.16 0.68**
8. Anger in -0.22* 0.09 -0.23* 0.13 0.13 0.47** 0.24**
9. CVD symptoms 0.08 0.01 -0.14 0.09 0.15 0.23* 0.13 0.14
* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01. Sex: 1 = male, 2 = female.

Dummy coded from job category.

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Stress and Health 21: 113–128 (2005) 123
L. Smith et al.

the absolute residual. According to Klem (1995), The variables of ERI, salary and clerical/sales/
the absolute residual of 0.007 for the model indi- service significantly accounted for 10 per cent of
cates a good fit between the original correlations the variance in trait anger. All of the standardized
and those in the predicted model. Another model path coefficients between variables are presented
that was based on the Reverse Causation theory in Figure 3.
was tested and the absolute residual indicated a
good fit with the data.
Discussion
The path model for ERI and CVD symptoms.
The Stress/Trait Model and the Reverse Causa- It can be concluded that the Stress/Trait Model
tion Model were both tested using path analyses. proposed in Figure 2 represented the relationship
The Reverse Causation Model was not supported between ERI and CVD symptoms in this study
by the data. However, the Stress/Trait Model better than the Reverse Causation Model. The
presented in Figure 2, showed significant as- path analysis for the model showed that trait
sociations between variables as predicted by the anger had a direct positive association with CVD
theory. Therefore, it was concluded that the symptoms, supporting hypothesis 1. Further-
Stress/Trait Model best represented the relation- more, ERI had an indirect association with CVD
ship between ERI and CVD symptoms from the symptoms via trait anger. Therefore, the results
data. The absolute residuals revealed that the dis- support the Stress/Trait theory that people who
tance between implied correlations and the orig- have high trait anger as a result of a perceived
inal correlations was the largest in age and sex. imbalance between their efforts and rewards at
Therefore, these variables were taken out of the work are more likely to show CVD symptoms.
path model. The model is presented in Figure 3. However, the results did not support the hypoth-
The effects of anger expression on CVD symp- esis that the anger expression variables of anger
toms were assessed by the regression of CVD in and anger out would mediate the relationship
symptoms on two predictor variables (anger in between trait anger and CVD symptoms. Trait
and anger out). The results indicated that the two anger alone mediated the relationship between
predictors did not significantly account for the ERI and CVD symptoms, as predicted by hypoth-
variance in CVD symptoms R2 = 0.04, F(2, 109) esis 2. Salary also had a significant negative
= 2.0, p > 0.05. association with trait anger and an indirect
The effects of trait anger (predictor variable) association with CVD symptoms.
were then regressed on anger in, anger out and
CVD symptoms. The results indicated that trait
anger accounted for 19 per cent of the variance General discussion
in anger, 33.3 per cent of the variance in anger
out and 6 per cent of the variance in CVD The aim of Study 1 was to investigate the rela-
symptoms. tionship between stress caused by ERI at work

.90** Anger In .81*


ERI
.19*
.44** .12

.23*
Ave Yearly –.21* Trait Anger CVD
Salary Symptoms

.58**
.06 .13
Anger Out .97
Clerical/Sales/
.67**
Service

Figure 3. The path diagram for the stress/trait model. Note: *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.

124 Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Stress and Health 21: 113–128 (2005)
Effort-reward imbalance at work

and state anger, while controlling for other pos- symptoms. These conclusions are consistent with
sible confounding variables. In Study 2, trait the central concept of the ERI theory, which states
anger was not used as a control but as a predic- that a lack of reciprocity between efforts and
tor variable with the additional aim of analysing rewards at work may cause a state of emotional
the relationship between ERI and CVD symp- distress that can lead to psychopathological prob-
toms. lems (van Vegchel et al., 2001). Furthermore, this
The results of Study 1 found that ERI was sig- finding supports the conclusion of Gallacher et al.
nificantly related to state anger after controlling (1999) that numerous studies have reinforced
for confounding variables. In Study 2 ERI was the belief that anger is related to heart disease.
significantly associated with anger. The results However, the results did not support the hypoth-
also revealed that ERI was not directly associated esis that the anger expression variables of anger
with CVD symptoms, however a significant indi- in and anger out would mediate the relationship
rect relationship was found between ERI and between trait anger and CVD symptoms.
CVD symptoms via trait anger. This implies that The SES indicator variable ‘average salary’ had
trait anger is a potential mediator between ERI a significant negative association with trait anger
and CVD symptoms. Furthermore, salary was and an indirect association with CVD symptoms
found to be associated with trait anger directly in the model. The results indicated that a person
and indirectly with CVD symptoms. with a lower average yearly salary might have an
increased chance of experiencing high trait anger
and CVD symptoms. These results also support
ERI and anger other research linking low SES to CVD (Kuper
et al., 2002).
Both Study 1 and Study 2 aimed to examine the
relationship between ERI and anger. Study 1
found a positive association between state anger Practical implications
and ERI. This implies that people who experience
ERI at work were generally angrier during the Study 2 highlights a possible process involving
time of the data collection. However, Study 2 trait anger, through which ERI may increase an
found a positive association between ERI and individual’s chance of experiencing CVD symp-
trait anger. This result suggests that people who toms. The finding that ERI is related to CVD
experience an imbalance between their efforts symptoms is consistent with the findings of
and rewards at work may develop an ingrained numerous other studies (Bosma et al., 1998;
tendency to experience higher levels of anger over Kuper et al., 2002) and has implications at an
time. A link between the two studies is possible individual, community and organizational level.
considering that trait anger predisposes a person CVD is recorded as one of the most common
to interpret events with higher anger and gener- causes of death in Western society. Hypotheti-
ally experience more state anger. Despite the lack cally, reducing the prevalence of ERI at work may
of research examining the specific relationship improve the emotional and cardiovascular health
between ERI and anger, the results of the current of many individuals. Reducing CVD symptoms
study were consistent with the work of Chen and in employees may also have positive benefits for
Spector (1992), who also found a relationship organizations. Healthier employees have been
between work stressors and anger. found to be more efficient at work, take fewer
sick days and give longer service to an organiza-
tion (Kasl, 1996; Peter & Siegrist, 1997).
Implications for the model Many studies have found that high levels of
anger can be harmful to the health of an individ-
An analysis of the ‘Stress/Trait Model’ found that ual and the function of an organization. A worker
trait anger had a direct positive association with who interprets events with increased anger and
CVD symptoms and that ERI had an indirect generally experiences more anger will generally
association with CVD symptoms via trait anger. not work at an optimum level and will show more
These results imply that perceived high efforts to aggression to work mates, which is a common
low rewards at work may increase trait anger in organizational problem (Allcorn, 1994; Vander-
individuals through chronic stress, which in turn Voort & Ragland, 1996). Organizations need to
increases their chances of experiencing CVD be aware that a lack of rewards provided to

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Stress and Health 21: 113–128 (2005) 125
L. Smith et al.

employees can cause negative emotional and phys- Prineas, 1982; Siegrist, personal communication,
ical health. Reducing ERI at work may involve March 11, 2003).
increasing rewards for employees not just through Finally the sample sizes were limited, which
salary or status, but through esteem and recogni- may have influenced the power of both studies.
tion and job security, or tackling demands directly. However, a high response rate of 81 per cent for
On a final note, the discovery of an association Study 1 and 73 per cent for Study 2, as well as
between average yearly salary, trait anger and the sampling of a heterogeneous community popu-
CVD symptoms deserves some discussion. The lation enhanced the generalizability of the study.
link between low SES and CVD is well docu- Furthermore, both studies showed a very similar
mented (Kuper et al., 2002) but not between low job category pattern and public/private sector
SES and anger. People of lower SES (as measured ratio indicating that the population employment
by salary) may be more vulnerable to the experi- demographics are consistent across studies, and
ence of higher anger because of their circum- they were also representative of the Adelaide met-
stances, which in turn could lead to CVD ropolitan community.
symptoms.

Methodological limitations Research directions

The study had a number of limitations. Cross- Although Study 1 and 2 contained possible limi-
sectional designs were utilized for convenience tations, the results present an interesting picture of
but are unable to determine causality. The rela- the relationship between work stress, anger and
tionships and order of variables presented in CVD symptoms. Future studies could build on
the Stress/Trait Model are based on theory. The current findings to give an even greater picture of
model was judged a better fit of the relationship the wide-ranging effects of ERI on public health.
between ERI and CVD symptoms than the Through improvements in sample size, more direct
Reverse Causation theory from significant find- measures of CVD symptoms and the utilization of
ings in the results. For a concrete conclusion to a longitudinal design, more reliable conclusions
made concerning the order and causal influence relating to causality could be obtained.
of variables in the relationship between ERI, Future directions for research could include dif-
anger and CVD symptoms a longitudinal study ferent measures of physical and emotional health.
with numerous time waves would be preferable. Other measures of physical symptoms may
Another limitation is that unlike most other involve musculoskeletal symptoms, sleep related
studies researching ERI and CVD symptoms, the problems or immune related measures or blood
current study used a self-report measure rather pressure. Finally, it would be a positive step to use
than physiological measures of CVD symptoms. the information from this study and others to
Self-report measures of CVD symptoms are redesign jobs to decrease ERI to offset the poten-
generally inferior clinical judgement. Self-report tially damaging consequences of anger to the indi-
measures of CVD symptoms are at risk of vidual and to the organization. Interventions in
over-reporting or under-reporting prevalence this area could enhance the emotional and physi-
rates of symptoms. However, the aim of this cal health of workers and productivity of
research was to examine associations between organizations.
variables and some caveats must be drawn
around interpreting evidence of prevalence rates
Acknowledgement
from the data. Unfortunately the current study
could only feasibly employ self-report measures We would like to thank Professor Charles Spielberger
of CVD symptoms because of restrictions on from the University of South Florida for his valuable
time, finance and resources. Although inferior to assistance.
objective measures of CVD, the London School
of Hygiene Questionnaire on Chest Pain is still
References
the most valid self-report measure of CVD symp-
toms (Bosma et al., 1998; Heyden et al., 1971; Allcorn, S. (1994). Anger in the workplace: Understanding the
Lundmen, Liljefors, Cederlöf & Friberg, 1971; causes of aggression and violence. Westport, CT: Quorum
Ramsay et al., 2001; Rose, Blackburn, Gillum, & Books.

126 Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Stress and Health 21: 113–128 (2005)
Effort-reward imbalance at work

Australian Bureau of Statistics (1997). Australian Standard tional officers. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology,
Classification of Occupation (ASCO) (2nd ed., Data file). 3, 243–264.
Available from Australian Bureau of Statistics Website: Douglas, S.C., & Martinko, M.J. (2001). Exploring the role
http://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/abs@.nsf/ of individual differences in the prediction of workplace
Bakker, A.B., Killmer, C.H., Siegrist, J., & Schaufeli, W.B. aggression. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 547–559.
(2000). Effort–reward imbalance and burnout among Fehr, B., & Russell, J.A. (1994). Fuzzy concepts in a fuzzy
nurses. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 31, 884–892. hierarchy: Varieties of anger. Journal of Personality and
Bennett, P., Lowe, R., Matthews, V., Dourali, M., & Tattersal, Social Psychology, 67, 186–205.
A. (2001). Stress in nurses: Coping, managerial support and Fitness, J. (2000). Anger in the workplace: An emotion script
work demand. Stress and Health, 17, 55–63. approach to anger episodes between workers and their
Bensimon, H.F. (1997). What to do about anger in the work- superiors, co-workers and subordinates. Journal of Orga-
place. Training and Development, 51, 28–32. nizational Behavior, 21, 147–162.
Bosma, H., & Peter, R., et al. (1998). Two alternative job Gallacher, J.E.J., Yarnell, J.W.G., Sweetnam, P.M., Elwood,
stress models and the risk of coronary heart disease. P.C., & Stansfeld, S.A. (1999). Anger and incident heart
American Journal of Public Health, 88, 68–75. disease in the Caerphilly study. Psychosomatic Medicine,
Brief, A.P., & Weiss, H.M. (2002). Organizational behavior: 61, 446–453.
Affect in the workplace. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, Hemingway, H. (1995). Psychosocial factors in the aetiology
279–307. and prognosis of coronary heart disease: Systematic review
Bullpitt, C.J., Shipley, M.J., Demirovic, J., Ebi-Krydton, K.L., of prospective cohort studies. British Medical Journal, 318,
Markowe, H.L., & Rose, G. (1990). Predicting death from 1460–1468.
coronary heart disease using a questionnaire. International Heslop, P., Davey-Smith, G., Metcalfe, C., Macleod, J., &
Journal of Epidemiology, 19, 899–904. Hart, C., (2002). Change in job satisfaction, and its
Byrne, D.G., & Reinhart, M.I. (1989). Occupation, Type A association with self-reported stress, cardiovascular risk
behavior and self-reported angina pectoris. Journal of Psy- factors and mortality. Social Science and Medicine, 54,
chosomatic Research, 33, 609–619. 1589–1599.
Calnan, M., Wainwright, D., & Almond, S. (2000). Job strain, Heyden, S., Bartel, A.G., Tabesh, E., Cassel, J.C., Tyroler,
effort–reward imbalance and mental distress: A study of H.A., Cornoni, J.C., & Hames, C.G. (1971). Angina pec-
occupations in general medical practice. Work and Stress, toris and the Rose Questionnaire. Archives of Internal
14, 297–311. Medicine, 128, 961–964.
Caltabiano, M.L., & Sarafino, E.P. (2002). Health psychol- Johnson, P.R., & Indvik, J. (2001). Slings and arrows of rude-
ogy: Biosocial interactions, an Australian perspective. ness: Incivility in the workplace. Journal of Management
Brisbane: John Wiley & Sons Australia. Development, 20, 705–713.
Chen, P.Y., & Spector, P.E. (1992). Relationships of work Kasl, S.V. (1996). The influence of work environment on
stressors with aggression, withdrawal, theft and substance cardiovascular health: A historical, conceptual, and meth-
use: An exploratory study. Journal of Occupational and odological perspective. Journal of Occupational Health
Organizational Psychology, 65, 177–185. Psychology, 1, 42–56.
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, Kivimaki, M., Leino-Arjas, P., Luukkonen, R., Riihimaki, H.,
112, 155–159. Vahtera, J., & Kirjonen, J. (2002). Work stress and risk of
Cohen, S., Kessler, R.C., & Underwood-Gordon, L. (1995). cardiovascular mortality: Prospective cohort study of indus-
Measuring stress: A guide for health and social scientists. trial employees. British Medical Journal, 325, 857–861.
New York: Oxford University Press. Klem, L. (1995). Path analysis. In L.G. Grimm, & P.R.
Cooper, C.L. (1998). Theories of organisational stress. New Yarnold (Eds), Reading and understanding multivariate
York: Oxford University Press. statistics. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Cretteden, I. (2001). Adelaide—a social atlas: 2001 census of Association.
population and housing. Canberra: Australian Bureau of Kop, W.J. (1999). Chronic and acute psychological risk
Statistics. factors for clinical manifestations of coronary artery
de Jonge, J., Bosma, H., Peter, R., & Siegrist, J. (2000). Job disease. Psychosomatic Medicine, 61, 476–487.
strain, effort–reward imbalance and employee well-being: Kuper, H., Singh-Manoux, A., Siegrist, J., & Marmot, M.
A large scale cross-sectional study. Social Science and Medi- (2002). When reciprocity fails: Effort–reward imbalance in
cine, 50, 1317–1327. relation to coronary heart disease and health functioning
Deffenbacher, J.L., Oetting, E.R., Thwaites, G.A., Lynch, R.S., within the Whitehall II study. Occupational and Environ-
Baker, D.A., & Stark, R.S., et al. (1996). State-trait anger mental Medicine, 59, 777–785.
theory and the utility of the Trait Anger Scale. Journal of Kutash, I.L., & Schlesinger, L.B. (1980). Handbook on stress
Counseling Psychology, 43, 131–148. and anxiety. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass Publishers.
Diamond, E.L. (1982). The role of anger and hostility in Lundman, T., Liljefors, I., Cederlöf, R., & Friberg, L. (1971).
essential hypertension and coronary heart disease. Psycho- The validity of the questionnaire diagnosis. Archives of
logical Bulletin, 92, 410–433. Environmental Health, 22, 597–599.
Diener, E., Emmons, R.A., Larsen, R.J., & Griffin, S. (1985). Marmot, M., & Wilkinson, R.G. (1999). Social determinants
The Satisfaction With Life Scale. Journal of Personality of health. New York: Oxford University Press.
Assessment, 49, 71–75. Mona, P., Fitzmaurice, G., Kubzansky, L.D., Rimm, E.B., &
Dollard, M.F., & de Jonge, J. (2003). Measurement and Kawachi, I. (2003). Anger expression and risk of stroke and
methodological issues in work stress research. In M.F. coronary heart disease among male health professionals.
Dollard, A.H. Winefield, & H.R. Winefield (Eds), Occupa- Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 100–110.
tional stress in the service professions. London: Taylor & Neuman, J.H., & Baron, R.A. (1998). Workplace violence
Francis. and workplace aggression: Evidence concerning specific
Dollard, M.F., & Winefield, A.H. (1998). A test of the forms, potential causes, and preferred targets. Journal of
demand-control/support model of work stress in correc- Management, 24, 391–420.

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Stress and Health 21: 113–128 (2005) 127
L. Smith et al.

Peter, R., & Siegrist, J. (1997). Chronic work stress, sickness Spielberger, C.D., Johnson, E.H., Russell, S.F., Crane, R.J.,
absence, and hypertension in middle managers: General Jacobs, G.A., & Worden T.J. (1986). The experience and
or specific sociological explanations? Social Science and expression of anger: Construction and validation of an
Medicine, 45, 1111–1120. anger expression scale. In M.A. Chesney, & R.H. Rosen-
Peter, R., Geibler, H., & Siegrist, J. (1998). Associations of man (Eds), Anger and hostility in cardiovascular and behav-
effort–reward imbalance at work and reported symptoms ioral disorders. New York: Hemisphere.
in different groups of male and female public transport Stansfeld, S., & Marmot, M. (2002). Stress and the heart.
workers. Stress Medicine, 14, 175–182. London: BMJ Publishing Books.
Peter, R., Siegrist, J., Hallqvist, J., Reuterwall, C., & Theorell, Stansfeld, S.A., Fuhrer, R., Shipley, M.J., & Marmot, M.G.
T. (2002). Psychosocial work environment and myocardial (1999). Work characteristics predict psychiatric disorder:
infarction: Improving risk estimation by combining two Prospective results from the Whitehall II study. Journal of
complementary job stress models in the SHEEP Study. Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 56, 302–307.
Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health, 56, 294– Tsutsumi, A., Ishitake, T., Peter, R., Siegrist, J., & Matoba, T.
300. (2001). The Japanese version of the effort–reward imbal-
Ramsay, J.M., McDermott, M.R., & Bray, C. (2001) Com- ance questionnaire: A study in dental technicians. Work and
ponents of the Anger-Hostility Complex and the symptom Stress, 15, 86–96.
reporting in patients with coronary artery disease: A multi- Tsutsumi, A., Kayaba, K., Theorell, T., & Siegrist, J. (2001).
measure study. Journal of Health Psychology, 6, 713–729. Association between job stress and depression among
Real Estate Institute of South Australia (2002). Quarterly Japanese employees threatened by job loss in a comparison
Property Statistics (Data file). Available from Real Estate between two complementary job-stress models. Scandina-
Institute of South Australia Website: http://www.resia. vian Journal of Work, Environment and Health, 27,
com.au/news_propnews.htm. 146–153.
Rose, G.P., McCartney, P., & Ried, D.D. (1977). Self- Van Vegchel, N., de Jonge, J., Meijer, T., & Hamers, J.P.H.
administration of a questionnaire on chest pain and inter- (2001). Different effort constructs and effort–reward imbal-
mittent claudication. British Journal of Preventive and ance: Effects of employee well being in ancillary health care
Social Medicine, 31, 42–48. workers. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 34, 128–137.
Rose, G.A., Blackburn, H., Gillum, R.F., & Prineas, R.J. VanderVoort, D.J., & Ragland, D.R. (1996). Expressed and
(1982). Cardiovascular survey methods (2nd ed.). Geneva: suppressed anger and health problems among transit
World Health Organization. workers. Current Psychology, 15, 179–194.
Siegel, J.M. (1986). The Multidimensional Anger Inventory. Vrijkotte, T.G.M., van Doornen, L.J.P., & de Geus, E.J.C.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 191– (1999). Work stress and metabolic and hemostatic risk
200. factors. Psychosomatic Medicine, 61, 796–805.
Siegman, A.W., & Dembrowski, T.M. (1989). In search of Wanous, J.P., Reichers, A.E., & Hudy, M.J. (1997). Overall
coronary prone behavior: Beyond type A. Hillsdale NJ: job satisfaction: How good are single item measures?
Lawrence Erlbraum Associates, Publishers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 247–252.
Siegrist, J. (1996). Adverse health effects of high-effort/ Warr, P. B., Cook, J. D., & Wall, T. D. (1979). Scales for the
low-reward conditions. Journal of Occupational Health measurement of some work attitudes and aspects of psy-
Psychology, 1, 27–41. chological well-being. Journal of Occupational Psychology,
Siegrist, J. (1998). Reciprocity in basic social exchange and 52, 129–148.
health: Can we reconcile person-based with population- Wenneberg, S.R., & Schneider, R.H. (1997). Anger expression
based psychosomatic research? Journal of Psychosomatic correlates with platelet aggregation. Behavioral Medicine,
Research, 45, 99–105. 22, 174–178.
Spielberger, C.D., Jacobs, G., Crane, R., Russell, S., Yarnell, J.W., Sweetnam, P.M., Baker, I.A., & Bainton, D.
Westberry, L., & Barder, L., et al. (1979). Preliminary (1988). Diagnosis of past history of myocardial infarction
manual for the State-Trait Personality Inventory. Tampa, in epidemiological studies: An alternative based on the
FL: University of South Florida, Human Resources Caerphilly and Speedwell surveys. Journal of Epidemiology
Institute. and Community Health, 42, 116–120.

128 Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Stress and Health 21: 113–128 (2005)

You might also like