Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

Journal of Abnormal Psychology Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

2000, Vol. 109, No. 2, 335-340 0021-843X/00/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0021-843X.109.2.335

The Importance of Callous-Unemotional Traits for Extending the Concept


of Psychopathy to Children

Christopher T. Barry, Paul J. Frick, Tammy M. DeShazo, Monique G. McCoy, Mesha Ellis,
and Bryan R. Loney
University of Alabama

This study focused on the use of callous-unemotional (CU) traits to identify a subgroup of children with
both attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and a conduct problem diagnosis (oppositional
defiant disorder [ODD] or conduct disorder [CD] who show characteristics similar to adults with
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

psychopathy. In a clinic-referred sample of children aged 6 to 13 years (N = 154), those with diagnoses
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

of both ADHD and ODD/CD were divided on the basis of teacher ratings of CU traits. Children high on
these traits showed features typically associated with psychopathy, such as a lack of fearfulness and a
reward-dominant response style. Furthermore, children with CU traits seemed less distressed by their
behavior problems. These findings are consistent with research on adults showing that impulsivity and
antisocial behavior alone are insufficient to document persons who fit the construct of psychopathy.

The concept of psychopathy has proven to be quite important for a number of neuropsychological correlates (e.g., poor passive
understanding antisocial behavior in adults (Cleckley, 1976). For avoidance learning, cortical underarousal, deficits in executive
example, psychopathic traits have been shown to predict a partic- functions) that make this group of children seem similar to adults
ularly severe and violent pattern of antisocial behavior that has with psychopathy. In support of this theory, Lynam (1998) re-
been resistant to many intervention approaches (Hart, Kropp, & ported that children from a community sample of 12- and 13-year-
Hare, 1988; Serin, 1993; Serin, Peters, & Barbaree, 1990). Fur- old boys who were elevated on teacher ratings of both conduct
thermore, the causes of antisocial behavior in individuals with problems and ADHD symptoms tended to exhibit a number of
psychopathic traits may be different from the causes of antisocial characteristics that have been associated with psychopathy (e.g.,
behavior in nonpsychopathic individuals (Hare, Hart, & Harpur, higher self-reported rates and variety of delinquency, poor passive
1991; Lykken, 1995). As a result of this research, there has been avoidance learning). Such characteristics were not associated with
a growing interest in extending the concept of psychopathy to either conduct problems alone or ADHD symptoms alone.
children and adolescents to identify important causal factors early A second line of research that has attempted to extend the
in the development of these traits so that intervention programs can concept of psychopathy to children comes from the work of Frick
be developed to intervene when these traits are hopefully more and colleagues (Frick, 1998; Frick, Barry, & Bodin, in press). This
malleable (Frick, 1998). line of research attempts to identify childhood precursors to psy-
One important and influential effort to extend the concept of chopathy by focusing specifically on the callous and unemotional
psychopathy to children was made by Lynam (1996) in his review (CU) traits (e.g., lack of guilt, absence of empathy, shallow and
of research on children who manifest the problems of inattention, constricted emotions) that have been hallmarks of conceptualiza-
impulsivity, and overactivity often associated with a diagnosis of tions of adult psychopathy (Cleckley, 1976). In support of this
attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) combined with method, children with conduct problems who also exhibit high
the severe conduct problems often associated with diagnoses of levels of CU traits display a greater severity and variety of anti-
oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) and conduct disorder (CD; social behavior and have more early contact with the police than
American Psychiatric Association, 1994). In his review, Lynam do children who exhibit conduct problems without CU traits
(1996) provided evidence that children with both ODD/CD and (Christian, Frick, Hill, Tyler, & Frazer, 1997). Furthermore, only
ADHD show a particularly severe form of antisocial behavior and conduct-problem children with these traits show the reward-
dominant response style that characterizes the avoidance learning
deficits found in psychopathic adults (O'Brien & Frick, 1996), and
Christopher T. Barry, Paul J. Frick, Tammy M. DeShazo, Monique G. only these children who show the preference for thrill and adven-
McCoy, Mesha Ellis, and Bryan R. Loney, Department of Psychology, ture seeking activities (i.e., fearlessness; Frick, O'Brien, Wootton,
University of Alabama, Tuscaloosa. & McBurnett, 1994) that have been critical to many theories of
Paul J. Frick is now at the Department of Psychology, University of psychopathic behavior (Lykken, 1995). Finally, and also consistent
New Orleans.
with causal theories of psychopathic behavior, conduct-problem
Work on this article was supported by Grant R29 MH55654-02 from the
National Institute of Mental Health.
children with CU traits are less likely to show intellectual deficits,
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Paul J. especially verbal deficits, than other children with conduct prob-
Frick, the Department of Psychology, University of New Orleans, 2001 lems (Loney, Frick, Ellis, & McCoy, 1998), and their conduct
Geology and Psychology Building, New Orleans, Louisiana 70148. Elec- problems are less strongly associated with dysfunctional parenting
tronic mail may be sent to pfrick@uno.edu. practices (Wootton, Frick, Shelton, & Silverthorn, 1997), further
335
336 SHORT REPORTS

supporting the possibility of different causal mechanisms under- (DISC 2.3; Shaffer, Fisher, Piacentini, Schwab-Stone, & Wicks, 1992) was
lying their conduct problem behavior (Lykken, 1995). administered to each child's teacher to assess for the presence of all
Given the promising findings from both of these lines of re- symptoms included in the revised third edition of the Diagnostic and
search, it is likely that an integration of these findings could be Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III-R; American Psychiatric
very beneficial for advancing our knowledge of psychopathic traits Association, 1987) criteria for ADHD, ODD, and CD.' Each child over 8
in children. For example, it is possible that, consistent with Ly- years old and all parents also were interviewed separately using the
nam's (1996) model, childhood precursors to psychopathy may be DISC-2.3 to assess for the symptoms of DSM-III-R anxiety disorders (i.e.,
found in children who show both ADHD and conduct problems. overanxious disorder, social phobia, simple phobia, and separation anxiety
disorder). Overall, 38% of the sample was over 8 years old, and this
However, consistent with Prick's (1998) model, it is possible that
percentage was fairly evenly distributed across clinic groups of interest,
the children within this group who also show high levels of CU
ranging from 31% of the ADHD-only group to 50% of the ADHD-and-
traits show many of the characteristics typically associated with
ODD/CD high-CU group, ^(3, N = 152) = 2.32, ns. A combination of
psychopathy. This possibility would be consistent with findings in
parent and child reports, when both informants were available, was used
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

adult samples. Specifically, studies in prison samples consistently


for analyses, on the basis of the recommendations of Piacentini, Cohen, and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

have shown that most inmates meet criteria for antisocial person- Cohen (1992). The DISC-2.3 interviews were conducted by trained grad-
ality disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) or score uate students or a licensed clinical psychologist and have been shown to
high on the Impulsive/Deviant Lifestyle dimension (i.e., Factor 2) provide highly reliable symptom estimates (Loney et al., 1998).
of the Psychopathy Checklist—Revised (PCL-R; Hare et al., Psychopathy Screening Device. We used the Psychopathy Screening
1990). Both of these constructs capture behaviors related to poor Device (PSD; Frick & Hare, in press) to assess CU traits in children. The
impulse control and a chronic pattern of antisocial and criminal PSD is a 20-item questionnaire designed to assess characteristics associ-
behavior that make them analogous to children with both ADHD ated with psychopathy, using the items from the PCL-R (Hare, 1991) to
and ODD/CD. However, in adult forensic samples, the subgroup of define its content (see Frick et al., 1994, and Frick et al., in press). Items
impulsive and antisocial individuals who also show CU traits on the PSD are scored on a 3-point scale ranging from 0 (not at all true)
exhibits an especially severe and violent pattern of antisocial to 2 (definitely true). Similar to the methodology used to diagnose ADHD,
behavior and shows a unique pattern of emotional deficits that ODD, and CD, only teachers' ratings were used to form groups on the basis
have traditionally been associated with psychopathy (Hare et al., of high-CU scores. The CU scale from the PSD was based on a factor
1991). analysis of clinic-referred children (Frick et al., 1994) and contains 6 items
In this study, we provide initial tests of this integration by (e.g., "Feels bad or guilty when he/she does something wrong," "Is con-
dividing clinic-referred children who have both an ADHD diag- cerned about the feelings of others"). The coefficient alpha for these 6
items based on teacher report in this clinic sample was .68.
nosis and a conduct-problem diagnosis (e.g., either ODD or CD)
When psychopathic traits are measured with the PCL-R in adult forensic
into those high and low on CU traits. Using methodology similar
samples, a cut-off score of 30 is established as having good predictive
to Lynam's (1998), we based diagnoses and the measurement of
validity and usually designates between 20% and 40% of forensic popu-
CU traits solely on teacher reports so that there was no overlap
lations (Hare et al., 1991). Unfortunately, no similar cut-off score has been
between the method of group formation and the measurement of
well established for the childhood PSD (see Wootton et al., 1997, for a
dependent variables. We then compared these groups on several rationale for considering psychopathy as a taxon rather than as a continu-
indices that theoretically are related to psychopathy and that are ous personality dimension). For the purposes of group formation in this
similar to those used by Lynam (1998). These measures include a study, we used the cut-off score of 7 out of a possible score of 12, which
measure of a reward-dominant response style, a measure of visual- designates approximately the upper quartile of the clinic sample and which
spatial reasoning, a measure of a child's preference for thrill and
adventure seeking activities, and a measure of anxiety. On the
1
basis of our model, we predicted that only those children with Data collection began before the publication of the fourth edition of the
ADHD and ODD/CD who also showed CU traits would show Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV; Amer-
characteristics that were consistent with the research on psycho- ican Psychiatric Association, 1994), which led to our use of DSM-lll-R
pathic adults. criteria to define ADHD, ODD, and CD. However, it is likely that our
results will be applicable to DSM-IV criteria for several reasons. First,
there is extensive overlap between DSM-III-R and DSM-IV in their
Method operationalizations of ODD and CD, with over 90% of the children
diagnosed by one criterion set being diagnosed by the other (Lahey,
Participants
Applegate, Barkley, et al., 1994). Second, the DSM-IV field trials indicated
The sample consisted of 154 children between the ages of 6 and 13 years that most children who met DSM-III-R criteria for CD but not the more
(M = 8.36, SD = 1.81) who were consecutive referrals to a university- stringent DSM-IV criteria for CD exhibited sufficient conduct problems to
based outpatient diagnostic and referral service. This service provides meet criteria for ODD. Therefore, the combined diagnostic category used
comprehensive psychological evaluations to children with severe emo- in our study likely captured the variations in severity that produced dis-
tional, behavioral, or learning problems, who are then referred to appro- crepancies between the two versions of the DSM. Third, the more dramatic
priate mental health agencies for intervention. The sample was 78% Cau- change in the ADHD criteria from a single symptom list combining
casian and 22% African American and approximately 78% male. More inattentive, impulsive, and overactive behavior in DSM-III-R to a dual
details on this sample are provided elsewhere (Loney et al., 1998). symptom list dividing inattention—disorganization and impulsivity-
overactivity in DSM-IV was largely done to capture children with only
inattention and disorganization. The DSM—III-R category seemed to cap-
Measures
ture children with severe impulsive and overactive behaviors quite well
Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children. The National Institute of (Lahey, Applegate, McBurnett, et al., 1994), and it was this aspect of
Mental Health Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children, Version 2.3 ADHD that seemed most critical to our study focus.
SHORT REPORTS 337

falls at the 90th percentile of a community sample of elementary-school- levels. Within 2 weeks of the evaluation, the child's primary teacher was
aged children (Frick et al., in press). contacted over the phone and administered the teacher version of the
Block Design. Scores on the Block Design subtest of the Wechsler DISC-2.3 by the same interviewer who had administered it to the child and
Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R; Wechsler, 1974) or the to his or her parent. In addition, the teacher version of the PSD was mailed
third edition of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-HI; to each child's teacher along with a self-addressed stamped envelope.
Wechsler, 1991)2 were used as a measure of visual-spatial reasoning. This
subtest has been shown to differentiate adult inmates high on psychopathic
traits from other inmates (Smith, Arnett, & Newman, 1992). Results
Thrill and Adventure Seeking. We used the Thrill and Adventure
Seeking subscale (TAS) of the Sensation Seeking Scale for Children On the basis of the teacher version of the DISC-2.3, we initially
(SSS-C; Russo et al., 1993) to measure participants' preferences for novel formed three groups of children. The first group (n = 68), a clinic
and dangerous activities. On this scale, a child chooses between a pair of control group, consisted of children who were referred for emo-
statements to indicate which is more true of him or her. One statement tional, behavioral, or learning problems but did not meet the
(e.g., "I enjoy the feeling of riding my bike fast down a big hill") describes criteria, according to their teachers, for either ADHD, ODD, or
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

sensation seeking behaviors, whereas the other statement describes a pref- CD. Another group of children (n = 58) met the criteria for ADHD
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

erence against sensation seeking behaviors (e.g., "Riding my bike fast but not for ODD/CD, and a third group (n - 28) met the criteria
down a hill is scary for me"). Although we administered the entire SSS-C for both ADHD and ODD/CD, according to teacher report. Con-
to the clinic sample, we used only the TAS scale in analyses, because this sistent with past research using clinic-referred samples of pread-
scale seems to reflect the aspect of sensation seeking that is most closely
olescent children (Abikoff & Klein, 1992), there were very few
related to the personality dimensions of fearlessness and harm avoidance
children (n = 3) who met criteria for ODD/CD but not for ADHD.
(Levenson, Kiehl, & Fitzpatrick, 1995; Watson & Clark, 1984) and it is
these aspects of anxiety that have been conceptually linked to psychopathy These children were excluded from further analyses. The most
(Lilienfeld, 1994). The coefficient alpha for this subscale in this sample common diagnosis in the clinic control children was an anxiety
was .76. Children who scored above the upper quartile of the sample (i.e., 9 disorder (50%), on the basis of either parent or child report on the
out of a possible 12) on the TAS were considered to show a clear DISC-2.3, although this diagnosis was also common in the
preference for thrill and adventure seeking activities (see Kagan, 1997, for ADHD-only (41%) and ADHD-and-ODD/CD (60%) groups as
a rationale for considering TAS as a taxon rather than as a continuous well, ^(2, N = 152) = 3.20, ns. The three diagnostic groups
personality dimension). differed on the proportion of children who were elevated on their
Reward-dominance computer task. We assessed a reward-dominant teacher's report of CU traits, with 57% of the ADHD-and-
response style using the paradigm employed in previous research with
ODD/CD group showing these elevations compared to 37% of the
adults (e.g., Newman, Patterson, & Kosson, 1987) and children (Daugherty
ADHD-only group and 9% of the clinic control group, x*(2, N =
& Quay, 1991). A more complete description of the task can be found
elsewhere (O'Brien & Frick, 1996; O'Brien, Frick, & Lyman, 1994). The 152) = 26.35, p < .001.
task consisted of four games, and participants were allowed to earn prizes This latter finding is consistent with Lynam's (1996) contention
on the basis of their scores across all four games. In each game, a stimulus that the ADHD-and-ODD/CD group would be the one most likely
(card, door, box, or fisherman) appeared on the screen and the child chose to contain those children who showed traits consistent with psy-
whether to press a key to view the other side of the stimulus or to press a chopathy. However, to test the main study hypothesis that only
key to stop the game. For each game, there was a successful outcome or an those children within this group who showed CU traits would
unsuccessful outcome on the other side of the stimulus. Each child began show characteristics typically associated with psychopathy, we
with 50 points, and a point was either added or taken away, depending on
divided the ADHD-and-ODD/CD group into those children with
the outcome of each trial. The proportion of successful outcomes across
elevations on the CU scale of the PSD (n = 16) and those who did
each successive 10 trials decreased from 90% to 0% over 100 trials. The
not show these elevations (n = 12). The demographic character-
total number of trials played was recorded and was used as a dependent
variable in the analyses. Studies have shown that adults with psychopathic istics of the four groups are presented in Table 1. The only
traits, once they establish a reward-oriented response set (e.g., high rates of demographic variable to differ across the four groups was full
reward trials early in the games), are more likely to continue playing scale intelligence, with the ADHD-and-ODD/CD low-CU group
despite the increasing ratio of punished to rewarded trials (Newman et al., scoring significantly lower, F(3, 149) = 2.6, p < .05, than the
1987).3 other groups on their measured intelligence. Because of this dif-
ference in overall intelligence, we repeated all subsequent analyses
controlling for intelligence, using it as either a covariate in an
Procedure analysis of covariance for continuous dependent variables or as a
All children in the study were referred to an outpatient diagnostic and predictor in a logistic regression equation for dichotomous depen-
referral service for a comprehensive psychological evaluation. Each child dent variables. Controlling for intelligence did not substantively
was accompanied to the evaluation by at least one custodial parent, which influence the results of any of the analyses.
in the vast majority of cases was his or her mother. Each child gave assent
and each parent gave informed consent for all of the information to be used
2
anonymously in research. Parents were told that the use of information for At the early stages of data collection, the WISC-R was used as the
research was strictly voluntary and would not affect the clinical services measure of intelligence (n = 68), with the remainder of the sample being
they received. After consent was obtained, a brief background interview tested with the newer WISC-HI.
3
with the parent was conducted, followed by the DISC-2.3 structured Although this response style has been variously labeled (e.g., response
interview. While the parent was completing these measures, the child was perseveration, response modulation) in research, depending on the hypoth-
administered either the WISC-R or the WISC-ffl, followed by the child esized cognitive process underlying it, for the purposes of this study we
version of the DISC-2.3, the reward-dominance computer task, and the used the purely descriptive label of the reward-dominant response style
SSS-C. The SSS-C was read to all children to equate for different reading (see O'Brien & Frick, 1996, for further discussion).
338 SHORT REPORTS

Table 1
Group Comparisons of Means for Demographic and Main Study Variables

ADHD and ADHD and


Clinic control ADHD only ODD/CD low ODD/CD high
Variable (« == 68) (n = 58) CU (n = 12) CU (n = 16)

Demographics
Age 8.53 (1.94) 8.03 (1.70) 8.47 (2.19) 8.81 (1.39)
Gender (% male) 70.60 86.20 75.00 81.30
Ethnicity (% African American) 19.10 15.50 41.70 37.50
Duncan's socioeconomic index 38.73 (26.67) 36.85 (24.00) 28.86 (18.74) 29.79 (19.31)
Full scale IQ* 95.22a(14.06) 93.65a (11.01) 84.42b (9.73) 93.38a(10.53)
Main study
Thrill and adventure seeking 25.00a 40.70a,b 20.00a,b 56.30b
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(% elevated)*
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Block design 8.91 (3.42) 7.72 (2.97) 7.08 (3.26) 7.88 (3.42)
Trials played on reward- 242.14a(85.60) 236.2 l a (81.37) 202.91a (98.16) 306.38b(63.48)
dominance task**
Anxiety symptoms* 3.41. (3.17) 3.41. (3.56) 6.33b (3.63) 4.25a (3.92)

Note. Means with different subscripts were significantly different in pairwise comparisons using Tukey's
procedure for continuous variables or pairwise chi-square tests for dichotomous variables. ADHD = attention
deficit/hyperactivity disorder; ODD = oppositional defiant disorder; CD = conduct disorder. Standard devia-
tions are in parentheses.
*p<.05. **p<.01.

We compared the four groups on the percentage of each they did not cross a diagnostic threshold and (b) the number of
group with elevations on the TAS, and the results of these children in the clinic control group who were elevated on these
comparisons are also shown in Table 1. There was a significant traits was quite small (n = 6). However, to begin exploring this
effect in the chi-square analyses, \2(2, N = 152) = 7.97, p < important issue, we conducted a series of 2 X 3 ANOVAs with
.05, with pairwise comparisons showing that only the ADHD- elevations on CU traits and diagnostic group status (clinic
and-ODD/CD high-CU group differed from the clinic control control, ADHD, and ADHD and CD) as the two factors. Several
group. In fact, the ADHD-and-ODD/CD low-CU group had the significant findings emerged from these analyses. First, on the
lowest percentage of individuals showing clear preferences for reward-dominance task, there was a significant interaction be-
thrill and adventure seeking activities. There was also a signif- tween CU traits and diagnostic status, F(l, 143) = 3.59, p <
icant group effect in a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) .05. This was due to the fact that children in the ADHD-and-
comparing the groups on the number of trials played on the ODD/CD high-CU group clearly played the most trials on this
computerized reward-dominance task, F(3, 149) = 4.10, p < task (M = 306.40, SD = 63.00) compared to children in any of
.01. As illustrated in Table 1, the ADHD-and-ODD/CD the other cells of the design (means ranging from 203.00 to
high-CU group played significantly more trials than any of the
244.00). However, the main effect for CU traits also approached
other groups on this computer task. One final difference that
significance, F(l, 143) = 3.56,^ < .06, suggesting that overall,
emerged between groups was the number of anxiety symptoms,
children high on CU traits tended to play more trials on this
on the basis of parent and child reports from the DISC-2.3
task, irrespective of their diagnostic group status. Second, when
interviews, F(3, 149) = 2.77, p < .05. Pairwise comparisons
the number of anxiety symptoms was used as a dependent
indicated that the ADHD-and-ODD/CD low-CU group exhib-
variable, there was a significant main effect for diagnostic
ited the highest rate of anxiety of any of the groups. When
status, F(2, 147) = 4.73, p < .01, and the interaction between
symptoms for the specific anxiety disorders were run sepa-
rately, the analyses using symptoms of overanxious disorder CU traits and diagnostic status approached significance, F(2,
and simple phobia as the dependent variables reached signifi- 147) = 2.76, p < .07. Inspection of the means suggested that
cance, suggesting that symptoms of these disorders primarily children with ADHD-and-ODD/CD had higher rates of anxiety
accounted for the overall effect of anxiety symptoms. The one symptoms, irrespective of the presence of CU traits. However,
comparison in which no significant effects emerged across this effect was moderated somewhat by the presence of CU
groups was on the Block Design subtest measuring visual- traits (see Table 1). Finally, the analyses using full scale intel-
spatial abilities. ligence as the dependent variable resulted in a significant in-
The results presented in Table 1 do not indicate whether high teraction between diagnostic status and elevations on CU traits,
rates of CU traits in the absence of ADHD and ODD/CD F(2, 146) = 3.37, p < .05, with no main effects approaching
diagnoses are associated with the dependent variables used in significance. This interaction was due to the much lower full
this study. Our clinic-referred sample was not the optimal one scale intelligence in the ADHD-and-ODD/CD group
with which to conduct this test because (a) all children were (M = 84.40, SD = 9.70) than in any of the other cells of the
referred for emotional, behavioral, or learning problems even if ANOVA design (means ranging from 90.00 to 96.00).
SHORT REPORTS 339

Discussion using a community sample were mixed in this regard. Lynam


reported that children with conduct problems without ADHD
Our findings support Lynam's (1996) contention that children symptoms scored equally high on a measure of psychopathic traits
who show symptoms of ADHD in combination with severe con- when compared to children high on both conduct problems and
duct problems are most likely to also show features associated with ADHD symptoms. However, the pure conduct-problem group did
psychopathy. However, also consistent with Prick's (1998) con- not show other characteristics that would make them seem analo-
ceptualization, it was only those children within this group who gous to adult psychopaths (e.g., a reward-dominant response
also showed high rates of CU traits who exhibited these features. style).
Only children high on CU traits showed a preference for thrill and Another limitation of our study was a reliance on teacher
adventure seeking activities (i.e., fearlessness) and were more report to assess ADHD, ODD/CD and CU traits, whereas it is
likely than other clinic-referred children to show a reward- typically recommended that researchers use multiple informants
dominant response style, both of which have been critical to many
to assess most forms of childhood psychopathology (Kamphaus
theories of psychopathy (Lykken, 1995). Furthermore, both of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

& Frick, 1996). We felt this method was justified to more


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

these behavioral patterns could reflect an underlying deficit in


closely approximate the methodology used by Lynam (1998)
behavior inhibition (Kagan, Reznick, & Snidman, 1987), which
has been linked to deficits in children's early development of guilt and to ensure that there was no overlap between the method of
and empathy (Kochanska, 1993). group formation and the measurement of dependent variables,
Despite having significant behavioral difficulties, the group high some of which were based on child self-report (i.e., thrill and
on CU traits did not show elevated levels of anxiety symptoms adventure seeking) or a combination of parent and child report
compared to other clinic groups, whereas those children with (i.e., anxiety symptoms). In addition, some of the groups used
ADHD and severe conduct problems who were low on CU traits in comparisons, especially the two groups with ADHD and
showed the highest levels. This finding is consistent with the view ODD/CD diagnoses, were moderate in size (n = 12 and n = 16,
that symptoms of DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, respectively), which could have limited our power to detect
1994) anxiety disorders are highly related to trait anxiety or to the group differences, especially in those comparisons using non-
negative affectivity that is often experienced because of the ex- parametric techniques. Therefore, our comparisons should be
pectation of negative events (Lilienfeld, 1994). Furthermore, our considered conservative tests of potential differences between
findings suggest that children high on CU traits, despite showing these subgroups of children. A final limitation, and one that is
high rates of conduct problems and the negative events often perhaps most crucial to this and other studies attempting to
associated with them (e.g., peer rejection, discipline confronta- define a childhood precursor to psychopathy (e.g., Lynam,
tions), are not as distressed by these behaviors as children with 1998), is the lack of a long-term follow up to determine whether
conduct problems who do not show these traits. This failure to be children, even those who show features typically associated
distressed, despite showing quite problematic behavior, is again with psychopathy, actually constitute the group most likely to
consistent with findings for adults with psychopathic traits, who show psychopathy as adults. Such prospective longitudinal
also tend to score lower on measures of negative affectivity than studies are critical for providing the most compelling evidence
antisocial adults without these traits (Hare et al., 1991). for which children are at most risk for showing psychopathic
The one finding that was not consistent with predictions was the traits as adults.
finding that a measure of nonverbal visual-spatial abilities did not Despite the lack of longitudinal data, studies such as this one
differ between the groups high and low on CU traits, as has been and the one by Lynam (1998) are critical for designating groups
the case in incarcerated adult samples (Smith et al., 1992). How- that may be at most risk for showing psychopathy in adults, so that
ever, it is consistent with the findings of Lynam (1998), who longitudinal studies can focus more closely on these groups. Fur-
reported that children with ADHD symptoms and severe conduct thermore, even before a conclusive link can be made, it is clear that
problems did not differ on these abilities from children with only
children with features associated with psychopathy constitute a
ADHD symptoms and only differed from normal control children.
group that is very severely impaired (Christian et al., 1997; Lynam,
In our study, there was no normal control group used in compar-
1998) and that shows characteristics that could reflect unique
isons, only a clinic control group. These findings suggest that
deficits in visual-spatial abilities do not consistently differentiate causal processes underlying its conduct problems (Frick, 1998). As
children with CU traits from other children with problems of a result, interventions that are more specifically targeted to the
impulse control or antisocial behavior. Our findings did suggest, unique characteristics of this group may be more effective than
however, that general intellectual deficits are specifically associ- those that are designed for all children with severe conduct prob-
ated with conduct problems in children who are not high on CU lems (see Frick, 1998, for a further discussion). Finally, the unique
traits (see also Loney et al., 1998). contribution of this study is to illustrate that the combination of
All of the interpretations made from this study need to be made problems of impulse control and antisocial behavior are not suf-
in light of several limitations. First, by using a clinic-referred ficient to capture the construct of psychopathy in children, which
sample, we were not able to define a group with only an ODD or is consistent with the findings in adults (e.g., Hare et al., 1991).
CD diagnosis. Therefore, we were not able to directly test whether Instead, it appears to be critical to also consider the presence of CU
this group would show a high level of CU traits or whether those traits, which have been the hallmark of most clinical descriptions
with CU traits and conduct problems but without the problems of of psychopathy (Cleckley, 1976; Hare, 1994) but have been ig-
impulsivity associated with ADHD would show characteristics nored in most assessments of antisocial individuals across the life
consistent with psychopathic adults. The results of Lynam (1998) span (Frick et al., in press; Hare et al., 1991).
340 SHORT REPORTS

References attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents.


American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 1673-1685.
Abikoff, H., & Klein, R. G. (1992). Attention-deficit hyperactivity and Levenson, M. R., Kiehl, K. A., & Fitzpatrick, C. M. (1995). Assessing
conduct disorder: Comorbidity and implications for treatment. Journal psychopathic attributes in a noninstitutionalized population. Journal of
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 881-892. Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 151-158.
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical man- Lilienfeld, S. O. (1994). Conceptual problems in the assessment of psy-
ual of mental disorders (3rd ed., revised). Washington, DC: Author. chopathy. Clinical Psychology Review, 14, 17-38.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical man- Loney, B. R., Frick, P. J., Ellis, M., & McCoy, M. G. (1998). Intelligence,
ual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. psychopathy, and antisocial behavior. Journal of Psychopathology and
Christian, R. E., Frick, P. J., Hill, N. L., Tyler, L., & Frazer, D. R. (1997). Behavioral Assessment, 20, 231-247.
Psychopathy and conduct problems in children: II. Implications for Lykken, D. T. (1995). The antisocial personalities. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
subtyping children with conduct problems. Journal of the American Lynam, D. R. (1996). Early identification of chronic offenders: Who is the
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 233-241. fledgling psychopath? Psychological Bulletin, 120, 209-234.
Cleckley, H. (1976). The mask of sanity (5th ed.). St. Louis, MO: Mosby. Lynam, D. R. (1998). Early identification of the fledgling psychopath:
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Daugherty, T. K., & Quay, H. C. (1991). Response perseveration and Locating the psychopathic child in the current nomenclature. Journal of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

delayed responding in childhood behavior disorders. Journal of Child Abnormal Psychology, 107, 566-575.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 453-461. Newman, J. P., Patterson, C. M., & Kosson, D. S. (1987). Response
Frick, P. J. (1998). Conduct disorders and severe antisocial behavior. New perseveration in psychopaths. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96,
York: Plenum. 145-148.
Frick, P. J., Barry, C. T., & Bodin, S. D. (in press). Applying the concept O'Brien, B. S., & Frick, P. J. (1996). Reward dominance: Associations
of psychopathy to children: Implications for assessment of antisocial with anxiety, conduct problems, and psychopathy in children. Journal of
children and adolescents. In C. B. Gacono (Ed.), The clinical and Abnormal Child Psychology, 24, 223-239.
forensic assessment of psychopathy: A practitioner's guide. O'Brien, B. S., Frick, P. J., & Lynam, R. D. (1994). Reward dominance
Frick, P. J., & Hare, R. D. (in press). The psychopathy screening device. among children with disruptive behavior disorders. Journal of Psycho-
Toronto: Multi-Health Systems. pathology and Behavioral Assessment, 16, 131-145.
Frick, P. J., O'Brien, B. S., Wootton, J., & McBurnett, K. (1994). Psy- Piacentini, J. C., Cohen, P., & Cohen, C. (1992). Combining discrepant
chopathy and conduct problems in children. Journal of Abnormal Psy- diagnostic information from multiple sources: Are complex algorithms
chology, 103, 700-707. better than simple ones? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 20,
Hare, R. D. (1991). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised. Toronto: 51-63.
Multi-Health Systems. Russo, M. F., Stokes, G. S., Lahey, B. B., Christ, M. A. G., McBurnett, K.,
Hare, R. D. (1994). Without conscience: The disturbing world of psycho- Loeber, R., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., & Green, S. M. (1993). A sensation-
paths among us. New York: Simon & Schuster. seeking scale for children: Further refinement and psychometric devel-
Hare, R. D., Harpur, T. J., Hakstian, A. R., Forth, A. E., Hart, S. D., & opment. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 15,
Newman, J. P. (1990). The Revised Psychopathy Checklist: Descriptive 69-86.
statistics, reliability, and factor structure. Psychological Assessment, 2, Serin, R. C. (1993). Diagnosis of psychopathology with and without an
338-341. interview. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 49, 367-372.
Hare, R. D., Hart, S. D., & Harpur, T. J. (1991). Psychopathy and the Serin, R. C., Peters, R. D., & Barbaree, H. V. (1990). Predictors of
DSM-IV criteria for the antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Ab- psychopathy and release outcome in a criminal population. Psycholog-
normal Psychology, 100, 391-398. ical Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 2,
Hart, S. D., Kropp, P. R., & Hare, R. D. (1988). Performance of male 419-422.
psychopaths following conditional release from prison. Journal of Con- Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., Piacentini, J. C., Schwab-Stone, M., & Wicks, J.
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 227-232. (1992). National Institute of Mental Health Diagnostic Interview Sched-
Kagan, J. (1997). Temperament and the reactions to unfamiliarity. Child ule for Children: Version 2.3. New York: Columbia University.
Development, 68, 139-143. Smith, S. S., Arnett, P. A., & Newman, J. P. (1992). Neuropsychological
Kagan, J., Reznick, S., & Snidman, N. (1987). The physiology and psy- differentiation of psychopathic and nonpsychopathic criminal offenders.
chology of behavioral inhibition in children. Child Development, 58, Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 1233-1243.
1459-1473. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1984). Negative affectivity: The disposition to
Kamphaus, R. W., & Frick, P. J. (1996). Clinical assessment of child and experience aversive emotional states. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219-
adolescent personality and behavior. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. 235.
Kochanska, G. (1993). Toward a synthesis of parental socialization and Wechsler, D. (1974). The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (2nd
child temperament in early development of conscience. Child Develop- ed.). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.
ment, 64, 325-347. Wechsler, D. (1991). The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (3rd
Lahey, B. B., Applegate, B., Barkley, R. A., Garfinkel, B., McBurnett, K., ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Kerdyk, L., Greenhill, L., Hynd, G. W., Frick, P. J., Newcorn, J., Wootton, J. M., Frick, P. J., Shelton, K. K., & Silverthorn, P. (1997).
Biederman, J., Ollendick, T., Hart, E. L., Perez, D., Waldman, I., & Ineffective parenting and childhood conduct problems: The moderating
Shaffer, D. (1994). DSM-IV field trials for oppositional defiant disorder role of callous—unemotional traits. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
and conduct disorder in children and adolescents. American Journal of Psychology, 65, 301-308.
Psychiatry, 151, 1163-1171.
Lahey, B. B., Applegate, B., McBurnett, K., Biederman, J., Greenhill, L.,
Hynd, G. W., Barkley, R. A., Newcorn, J., Jensen, P., Richters, J., Received March 26, 1999
Garfinkel, B., Kerdyck, L., Frick, P. J., Ollendick, T., Perez, D., Hart, Revision received June 29, 1999
E. L., Waldman, L, & Shaffer, D. (1994). DSM-IV field trials for Accepted June 30, 1999 •

You might also like