Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

LSHSS

Tutorial

Tutorial: Speech Motor Chaining


Treatment for School-Age Children
With Speech Sound Disorders
Jonathan L. Preston,a Megan C. Leece,a and Jaclyn Stortoa

Purpose: Operationalized treatments for school-age the procedure. Data on fidelity of implementation and
children with speech sound disorders may result in more dose per session are presented. Clinical and research evidence
replicable and evidence-based interventions. This tutorial is provided to illustrate likely outcomes with the procedure.
describes Speech Motor Chaining (SMC) procedures, Results: SMC is a method that can result in successful
which are designed to build complex speech around core acquisition of target speech patterns and generalization to
movements by incorporating several principles of motor untrained words. Most clinicians can implement the procedure
learning. The procedures systematically manipulate factors with over 90% fidelity, and most children can achieve over
such as feedback type and frequency, practice variability, 200 trials per session.
and stimulus complexity based on the child’s performance. Conclusion: Clinicians and researchers can use or adapt
Method: The rationale and procedures for SMC are described. the operationally defined SMC procedures to incorporate
Examples are presented of how to design stimuli, deliver several principles of motor learning into treatment for
feedback, and adapt the approach. Free resources are school-age children with speech sound disorders.
provided to guide clinicians through implementation of Supplemental Material: https://osf.io/5jmf9/

S
peech sound disorder (SSD) involves impaired pro- McAllister Byun, 2015; Maas et al., 2008). However, it can
duction of speech sounds in relation to a person’s be a complex task to structure treatment in a manner that
age and language experience. In school-age children purposefully manipulates numerous aspects of practice and
with persisting SSD, motor-based treatments requiring re- feedback according to principles of motor learning. The
peated practice of speech targets are common (Hitchcock purpose of this tutorial is to provide a description of Speech
& McAllister Byun, 2015; Murray, McCabe, & Ballard, Motor Chaining (SMC), one type of structured motor-
2012; Shriberg & Kwiatkowski, 1982). Such drill-based based speech sound treatment utilized in both research and
approaches may be appropriate to address speech errors clinical practice with school-age children with SSD. The
in school-age children with SSD, including those with ar- tutorial presents an overview of the evidence base, the the-
ticulation disorders and childhood apraxia of speech. A oretical rationale, and a description of the procedures for
common basis in these subtypes of SSD is impairment at SMC so that clinicians and clinical researchers may use
some level of speech motor function (i.e., articulatory spec- and adapt the framework to fit a variety of treatment
ifications for a specific phoneme, or planning and pro- needs. Data on treatment fidelity and dose are also sum-
gramming of speech movement sequences). Importantly, marized, and a freely available data sheet with stimuli is
the application of motor learning theories to the treat- provided to guide clinicians to implement the procedure.
ment of motor-based SSDs suggests that attention to par-
ticular principles may be warranted (e.g., Hitchcock &
Research Evidence for SMC
SMC procedures have been applied in a number of
a
Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, case studies and single-subject experimental designs (Preston
Syracuse University, NY & Leaman, 2014; Preston, Leece, & Maas, 2016, 2017;
Correspondence to Jonathan Preston: jopresto@syr.edu Preston, Leece, McNamara, & Maas, 2017; Preston,
Editor-in-Chief: Shelley Gray Maas, Whittle, Leece, & McCabe, 2016; Preston et al.,
Editor: Ignatius Nip 2014; Sjolie, Leece, & Preston, 2016). SMC has been
Received July 19, 2018 used as part of treatment with children, adolescents, and
Revision received October 2, 2018
Accepted October 16, 2018 Disclosure: The authors have declared that no competing interests existed at the time
https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_LSHSS-18-0081 of publication.

Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 50 • 343–355 • July 2019 • Copyright © 2019 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 343
adults with residual speech sound errors; children with To offer an example of more homogeneous sample,
apraxia of speech; and adults with acquired apraxia of Figure 2 provides data on accuracy of /ɹ/ in untrained
speech. These procedures can be used both with and words for children whose treatment included only SMC
without biofeedback (e.g., Mental et al., 2016; Preston, with no biofeedback. Accuracy is averaged across six
Leece, & Maas, 2017; Preston et al., 2014; Vick, Mental, children of ages 10–16 years with residual speech errors
Carey, & Lee, 2017). Although the procedures were ini- affecting /ɹ/ who participated in seven 1-hr treatment ses-
tially developed to provide a standardized practice structure sions scheduled twice per week. The average data are from
as part of a series of studies that included biofeedback a single-subject design described by Preston, Leece, and
during sound production training, the SMC procedures Maas (2017). Although there was individual variation in
are used in each session during periods of practice with- response to the treatment, the data suggest that the average
out biofeedback as well. improvement on untreated items was approximately 33%
Figure 1 presents individual data from six different from pretreatment to immediately after the 7 hr of therapy.
case series and single-subject experimental designs that have Moreover, continued monitoring for 5 weeks after with-
included 39 children of ages 8–21 years who were treated drawal of SMC treatment suggested ongoing improvement,
for residual speech errors, primarily on /ɹ/ (Preston, Leece, which is evidence of motor learning. Although more than
et al., 2016; Preston, Leece, & Maas, 2017; Preston, Leece, 7 hr of therapy may be required, this provides evidence
McNamara, et al., 2017; Preston, Maas, et al., 2016; that residual speech errors can improve with this motor-
Preston et al., 2014; Sjolie et al., 2016). Most of these chil- based approach.
dren had received prior intervention that had not success-
fully remediated all of their sound errors. The mean
pretreatment accuracy for sounds selected for treatment Background and Theoretical Motivation for SMC
was 12.4% (SD = 14.5), and the mean posttreatment accu- Although the primary aim of this tutorial is to describe
racy was 50.2% (SD = 32.1). However, these studies vary the SMC procedures, we begin with a brief rationale. Per-
somewhat in the exact characteristics of the participants sisting SSD can often be characterized as involving incor-
(some had childhood apraxia of speech with numerous rect articulatory movements—that is, articulator positions
sound errors, some had residual /ɹ/ distortions only) and and transitions between positions, which result in erred
in the features of the treatment (i.e., the number of hours productions of speech sounds. Persisting SSD in school-age
of treatment ranged from 14 to 16, the distribution of ses- children can be the consequence of a history of speech
sions ranged from 1 to 7 weeks, and the amount of biofeed- delay, a history of normal speech sound development but
back varied from approximately 20% to 45% of the session). failure to achieve accurate production of one or two par-
ticular speech sounds, or a history of motor speech im-
pairment (e.g., Shriberg et al., 2010, 2017). Under the
Figure 1. Target sound accuracy on untreated words for 39 children assumption that the goals of accurate speech sound pro-
whose treatment included Speech Motor Chaining. Each line
represents a separate participant. duction involve an acoustic target and general movement
goals (e.g., Tourville & Guenther, 2011), the SMC proce-
dures involve training speech movement patterns with feed-
back about both the acoustic quality of speech sounds
and the articulatory actions. The procedures are intended
to address isolated speech sound errors and errors associ-
ated with symptoms of impaired transitions between sounds
and syllables. Therefore, the procedures have been imple-
mented both with children with residual speech sound errors
and children with persisting errors associated with child-
hood apraxia of speech.
Several principles derived from nonspeech motor
learning literature may be relevant to speech motor learn-
ing. Critically, these principles, primarily derived from a
schema-based motor learning theory, define a difference
between instructional parameters that best enhance skill
acquisition versus skill learning (Maas et al., 2008; Schmidt
& Lee, 2011). Skill acquisition refers to increased perfor-
mance during practice. In the context of speech therapy,
acquisition may reflect temporary improvement in speech
sound accuracy while practicing within a session. Conversely,
skill learning refers to relatively permanent changes, includ-
ing retention and generalization of the skill to untrained
tasks. Thus, some clients show acquisition of correct artic-
ulatory patterns (improved accuracy during sessions) but

344 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 50 • 343–355 • July 2019
Figure 2. Mean accuracy on word-level generalization probe for six children with /ɹ/ distortion. B = baseline
(pretreatment); P = posttreatment; 2M = 2 months follow-up. Error bars represent standard deviation.
Treatment occurred for seven sessions (dashed box).

fail to demonstrate learning (i.e., poor generalization or reten- by structured steps toward speech motor learning. There-
tion). In the context of therapy for SSD, early stages of fore, SMC feedback and practice procedures are designed
treatment may require initial emphasis on skill acquisition to initially facilitate acquisition of simple speech targets
to establish the core movements; however, a plan for sys- through conditions such as frequent and specific feedback
tematic transition to skill learning is essential. and constant practice. Based on the learner’s success, the
Maas et al. (2008) outlined several key aspects of treatment is dynamically adapted through five levels to ad-
practice and feedback, which are believed to influence skill dress increasingly complex stimuli with strategies that in-
acquisition and learning. For example, with respect to the clude less frequent and less specific feedback and more
complexity of targets, practice with simple targets (e.g., variable practice.
syllables) is expected to yield high performance during ac- Another guiding principle is the need to practice at
quisition, whereas practice on complex targets (e.g., sen- the optimal level of difficulty while offering the appropri-
tences) is more likely to facilitate skill learning. In addition, ate level of support (Guadagnoli & Lee, 2004; Hitchcock
constant practice (e.g., minimal variation in consonants, & McAllister Byun, 2015). The assumption is that, to max-
vowels, and prosodic content) should enable skill acquisi- imize motor learning, learners must be appropriately
tion, but variable practice (e.g., increasing stimulus set size challenged based on the task demands and the available
and varying prosody) should enhance learning. The prac- information (e.g., cues, feedback). Therefore, many tradi-
tice schedule may also play a role, such that blocked prac- tional hierarchies of articulation therapy require learners to
tice (e.g., many consecutive repetitions of the same target) achieve a predetermined accuracy within a session (e.g., 85%
leads to skill acquisition whereas random practice (e.g., accuracy on /ɹ/ at the syllable level) prior to advancing to
mixing up stimuli) facilitates motor learning. Moreover, a more complex task (e.g., /ɹ/ at the word level). However,
with respect to the feedback that is provided to the learner, the SMC procedures are designed to adapt more quickly as
knowledge of performance (KP) feedback (e.g., detailed the learner is successful, with changes in the task occurring
feedback about movements, such as “the sides of your ton- after every six trials. Thus, for children who are successful, it
gue were down too low for /s/”) may lead to skill acquisi- is possible to practice syllables, words, phrases, and sen-
tion, but less specific feedback or knowledge of results tences all within the course of approximately 2 min. For
(KR; e.g., information about correctness, such as “good /s/ children who are unsuccessful in their productions, practice
sound”) should facilitate motor learning. Skill acquisition can remain at the syllable level for an entire session. This
is expected to increase when frequent feedback is provided frequent decision making enables more flexibility than some
to the learner, but learning may be facilitated by less fre- traditional approaches in which modifications to linguistic
quent external feedback. Finally, immediate feedback is level (e.g., syllables vs. words) or clinician support (e.g.,
hypothesized to enhance skill acquisition, but a brief delay amount of feedback) occur only between sessions.
before feedback is provided may facilitate motor learning. Finally, the SMC procedures described herein involve
Several of these principles are embedded within SMC stimuli that are chained. The general purpose of chaining is
treatment. to quickly build to complex speech movements (i.e., words
Individuals with SSD may require scaffolded support and phrases) around previously trained smaller units (i.e.,
to achieve the goal of consistent production of articulatory sound sequences embedded in syllables). Chaining has been
movements that result in acoustically acceptable productions described for many years in the speech disorder literature
of phonemes. The support provided by clinicians should as a way to teach appropriate coarticulation between sounds,
change over time in order to optimize learning. The SMC to produce appropriate allophonic variation, and to main-
treatment described here involves an initial emphasis on tain syllabic integrity through the smooth transitioning
acquisition to elicit and establish speech sounds, followed among syllables (Chappell, 1973; Johnson & Hood, 1988;

Preston et al.: Speech Motor Chaining 345


Young, 1987). The SMC procedures described here are an sequences. For example, the target of onset /ɹ/ could be ad-
attempt to operationalize a form of chaining for the pur- dressed with four variants: two /ɹ/ singleton variants such
poses of clinical practice and clinical research (though as /ɹi/ and /ɹɑ/ and two /ɹ/ cluster variants such as /dɹ-/ and
other formalisms could be developed). The procedures in- /kɹ-/. Coda /k/ could be addressed with four variants such
volve the selection of a target sound in syllable position as singletons /ek/ and /ʊk/ and clusters /-kt/ and /-sk/. In each
(e.g., /k/ in onset or /s/ in coda). Sound sequences (CV, session, two different chains are chosen per variant, resulting
VC, or CC) are chosen, which represent the target sound in 16 chains per session (2 sounds × 4 variants × 2 chains).
in syllable position. The selection of a sound sequence as This choice of sound sequences as treatment targets (rather
the unit of training is motivated by the literature attesting than isolated sounds) demands intentional focus on a move-
to notion that the units of speech planning extend beyond ment pattern (such as a consonant transitioning into or out
individual phonemes (e.g., Bohland, Bullock, & Guenther, of various vowels), rather than simply production of a
2010; Redford, 2015; Tilsen, 2013). During practice, addi- phoneme. In selecting these sequences, a relative range of dif-
tional movements are gradually added before the target ficulty could be considered based on the client’s observed
sequence (backward chaining) or after the target sequence success during an assessment; for example, some variants
(forward chaining). For example, when training the target may include sequences that are likely to be facilitative, and
/k/ in onset, one representative sound sequence is /kl/. some variants may include sequences that are likely to be
Chaining for each sequence begins in a syllable (e.g., /kle/), more challenging. Chains are then built around the chosen
followed by a monosyllabic word (e.g., claim), a multisyl- variants.
labic word (e.g., proclaim or claiming), a short phrase (e.g., Phonetic context should be considered when selecting
proclaim the news, or He wasn’t claiming), and eventually a phoneme sequences that represent the target sound in word
self-generated sentence (e.g., using the word proclaim or position. If a client is minimally stimulable in one phonetic
claiming in a sentence). Practice at the higher levels of lin- context, it is important to try to practice that facilitative
guistic complexity only occurs when there is success at the context in at least one chain to allow the client to experience
previous level. Within each level, however, the feedback some success in a session. If the client is not stimulable in
provided to the learner addresses the target movement se- any context, syllables can be chosen, which theoretically
quence (/kl/). Thus, there is an intentional plan for building may be phonetically facilitative (e.g., /ɑɹ/ may be potentially
complexity around each stimulus that is chosen, as the facilitative for /ɹ/ because /ɑ/ promotes pharyngeal constric-
larger units (e.g., multisyllabic words) are designed to en- tion; /ts/ may be facilitative for /s/ due to the presence of a
compass the smaller units (e.g., monosyllabic words and preceding alveolar stop; /ʊk/ may be facilitative for /k/ due
syllables) to facilitate success. to the presence of the mid–high back vowel). When possible,
the chosen sound sequences should sample a variety of pho-
netic environments to encourage accurate production of
Assessment Considerations movement gestures across various coarticulatory contexts.
Thus, when selecting vowel contexts to be paired with a
and Treatment Planning consonant, nonadjacent vowels along the vowel quadrilat-
Treatment can be planned appropriately following a eral should be considered. For example, variants to address
thorough assessment. In addition to a standardized assess- /ɹ/ in onset may include /ɹi/ and /ɹɑ/, because of the high-
ment of speech sound production to verify the presence of a front and low-back vowels (vis-à-vis /ɹi/ and /ɹɪ/, which are
SSD, deep testing is particularly helpful when using SMC. closer on the vowel quadrilateral). Similarly, choosing con-
Deep testing involves assessing a speech sound across a va- sonant clusters that vary in place of articulation allows
riety of words sampling various phonetic contexts, including for practice across various coarticulatory environments
different word positions, stress patterns, and adjacent pho- (e.g., /dɹ-/ and /ʃɹ-/ may be preferred variants because they
nemes (Kent, 1982). Finding prosodic environments and differ in place of articulation, manner, and voicing of the
coarticulatory contexts in which the child is most successful first phoneme, vis-à-vis /dɹ/ and /tɹ/, which differ only in
can assist in selecting initial stimuli for SMC sessions. For voicing). Practicing such variation may facilitate acquisi-
example, for a child whose /ɹ/ in coda position is correct tion of contextually driven allophones (cf. Mielke, Baker,
only 10% of the time, noting that the correct production & Archangeli, 2016). Table 1 provides examples of target
occurs following a back vowel /ɑ/ may encourage the selec- sequence selection that encourages varied phonetic contexts.
tion of /ɑɹ/ as one target syllable to practice in treatment, Supplemental Material S1 provides further examples of
which may facilitate acquisition of /ɹ/ in other contexts. how chains are built around the selected variants.
Appropriate targets for SMC are typically sounds
that are below 50% accuracy in a particular syllable posi-
tion when tested at the word level. Typically, two broad Prepractice
targets are treated per child for each session (e.g., onset /ɹ/ Typically, SMC sessions are initially divided into two
and coda /k/). Following the selection of sounds in word phases: prepractice (elicitation) and structured practice using
position targets, four variants (i.e., sound sequences) are SMC. Later, a third phase, randomized practice, may be
initially chosen. Variants of the target are two-phoneme (e.g. added. Within the prepractice phase, the clinician helps the
CC, CV, VC) or occasionally three-phoneme (e.g., CCC) learner to identify a minimum reference for correctness

346 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 50 • 343–355 • July 2019
Table 1. Examples of target sound/position and four possible variants 4. phrases, which include two to five words and which
(sound sequences) for practice across a range of phonetic contexts.
contain the monosyllabic word or the multisyllabic
word; and
Target sound/position Variant 1 Variant 2 Variant 3 Variant 4
5. self-generated sentences, in which the child uses either
/ɹ/ onset /ɹɑ/ /ɹi/ /dɹ/ /kɹ/ the monosyllabic word or multisyllabic word in a
/s/ onset /si/ /so/ /sk/ /sn/
novel sentence.
/k/ rhyme /ɪk/ /æk/ /sk/ /kt/
/ʧ/ rhyme /iʧ/ /æʧ/ /ʌʧ/ /nʧ/ Advancing from one level of a chain to the next level
requires at least five of six correct productions in a block.
For example, five correct productions of the syllable /ɹɑ/
are needed to progress to the monosyllabic word rock.
such that the child becomes familiar with the distinction
Failure to achieve at least five correct productions at any
between correct and incorrect productions of the variants.
level results in moving to a new chain with a different vari-
Stimulability for the chosen variants is achieved with model-
ant of the target, returning to Level 1 (e.g., switching from
ing, shaping, and verbal and visual cues for articulator
a /ɹɑ/ chain to a /ɹi/ chain). In the current operationalized
placement and movement (e.g., Secord, Boyce, Donohue,
structure, a chain is mastered when a child successfully pro-
Fox, & Shine, 2007). For example, to address /ɹ/, shaping
gresses through all five levels of the chain on two separate
strategies may involve /l/→/ɹ/ or /ɑ/→/ɹ/. However, trials
occasions (i.e., at least five correct productions of a self-
within prepractice are not considered successful until the
generated sentence). Once the chain is mastered, a replace-
entire sound sequence is produced correctly (e.g., correct
ment chain with the same variant is used in the next ses-
production of the syllables /ɹi/ or /ɹɑ/ with smooth transitions
sion (e.g., replace rock-rocket-my rocket ship with a chain
between segments, not just isolated /ɹ/). Phonetic placement
such as rot-rotten-rotten food ).
for /ɹ/, for example, may consist of cues to elevate the ante-
The SMC structure enables the client to quickly
rior tongue, keep the dorsum low, elevate the sides of the
practice up to a level that is sufficiently challenging, similar
tongue, retract the tongue root, or keep the lips steady.
to the “challenge point” framework (Guadagnoli & Lee,
Visual cues to aid the prepractice stage may include images
2004; Hitchcock & McAllister Byun, 2015; Rvachew &
of vocal tract shape (e.g., http://www.seeingspeech.ac.uk/
Brosseau-Lapré, 2012). That is, clients work at a level of
ipachart/) or tongue–palate contact with electropalatography
difficulty in which some but not all attempted utterances
examples (e.g., McLeod & Singh, 2009). Within the pre-
are likely to be correct. If the task proves too hard, practice
practice phase, the clinician provides frequent cueing and
returns to the syllable level again in conditions that are more
immediate detailed feedback to elicit correct productions
acquisition focused (limited linguistic level/less complex,
from the client. In our approach, prepractice is continued
frequent and specific feedback) before building up to tar-
until the client achieves several correct productions of each
gets that are more learning focused (higher linguistic levels,
variant (at least three correct productions of each variant).
more variability, reduced feedback).
During the prepractice phase, the clinician can determine
the phonetic cues that are most valuable in helping the child
achieve correct productions, and such cues may be continued Feedback
during the feedback that is provided during structured Verbal feedback by the clinician on any trial during
practice with SMC. SMC consists of KR, KP, or no feedback. Table 3 pro-
vides examples of clinician responses for these types of
feedback.
SMC
KR refers to information only about the correctness
After a sufficient number of correct productions of of the speech sounds (phonetically correct or incorrect).
each variant are achieved during prepractice, practice with That is, the acoustic accuracy is judged as either being a
SMC begins. The SMC data sheet is designed to guide good (accurate) or poor (inaccurate) form of the target
clinicians through the implementation of the following prin- sounds. KR feedback is typically associated with motor
ciples. Target syllables, words, and phrases are elicited in learning because the clinician offers no specific information
blocks of six consecutive attempts, and practice starts at about why a production was correct or incorrect or what
the syllable level. Some example chains are shown in Table 2. movements the client should change (Maas et al., 2008).
Chains consist of five practice levels, which are defined by KP refers to feedback about the nature of the move-
complexity around the target variants: ment that was just performed. If a sound is produced in
1. syllables, which contain at least one consonant and error, the KP feedback provided by the clinician should
one vowel: CV, CC(V), VC, or (V)CC—these are the make reference to aspects of the phonetic placement, vocal
basic sound sequences (variants) representing the target; tract configuration, or movement sequencing that need to
change to achieve a correct production. If a target sound is
2. monosyllabic words, which begin or end with the syl- produced correctly, the KP feedback should highlight as-
lable and which contain both an onset and a coda; pects of the movement that resulted in correct production.
3. multisyllabic words, which include two or more syl- The KP feedback is therefore specific to the type of error
lables and which contain the monosyllabic word; produced or the movement that was achieved. The KP

Preston et al.: Speech Motor Chaining 347


Table 2. Examples of chains.

Sound/position Syllable Monosyllabic word Multisyllabic word Phrase Self-generated sentence

/ɹ/ onset /ɹɑ/ rot rotten rotten food ?


/ɹ/ rhyme /ɔɹ/ for before just before ?
/k/ onset /ki/ keep keeping keeping my money ?
/k/ rhyme /ʊk/ book bookshelf on the bookshelf ?
/s/ onset /sɪ/ sit sitting sitting down ?
/s/ rhyme /æst/ cast broadcast national broadcast ?
/ʧ/ onset /ʧæ/ champ champion world champion ?
/ʧ/ rhyme /ɪʧ/ witch sandwich eat a sandwich ?

Note. Observe that the target sequence (in bold) remains consistent throughout the chain.

feedback typically aids acquisition and is provided fre- fewer trials with feedback) and changes in the type of
quently in the early stages of treatment because the clini- feedback from primarily KP to primarily KR. The type
cian offers specific information to the client about the of feedback assigned to a trial is specified on the SMC
essential aspects needed to achieve the intended speech data sheet.
movements. In SMC, the KP feedback is provided on
four of six trials at the syllable level and on one of six trials
at the sentence level. Generally, when the KP feedback is SMC Data Sheet
provided, it is implicitly or explicitly accompanied by the A data sheet is available to guide users to implement
KR feedback as well. For example, “Good job [KR]! You the SMC procedures. A sample data sheet is available in
lifted the back of your tongue up [KP] when you started the Appendix. A fillable electronic copy in Microsoft Excel
/ki/.” In several previous studies, periods of treatment ses- is freely available at https://osf.io/5jmf9/, which can be used
sions have included verbal KP supplemented with visual with a computer or a tablet (see Supplemental Material S1).
KP in the form of visual biofeedback (e.g., Preston & Leece, A number of prepopulated chains are included in the file
2017; Preston, Leece, et al., 2016; Preston et al., 2014). (accessible as drop-down selection choices), although new
The amount and type of feedback at each level is chains can be added by users.
intended to change as client’s progress in SMC, with At the top of the data sheet, the target sound/word
successive reductions in the amount of feedback (i.e., position is entered along with four variants. Also, along

Table 3. Examples of knowledge of results and knowledge of performance feedback.

Feedback type Segments Positive feedback Negative feedback

Knowledge of results Any Correct. Not that time.


You got it. No.
Great. Didn’t get that one.
Excellent. Not that one.
Way to go. Not quite.
Knowledge of performance /ɹ/ Good, your tongue tip was raised. Not quite. Lift the front of your tongue
up a little.
Got it! Your sides were up. Not quite. Try to lift the sides up to touch
the back molars.
Yes, you got tongue root back into the throat. Not that time, try to pull the tongue root
back into the throat.
Good, you kept the back of your tongue down. No, I think you raised the tongue dorsum
too high.
Good, you kept the lips steady. Not quite. Your lips were sticking out
too much.
Knowledge of performance /k/ Awesome! You kept your tongue tip down. No. Both the tongue tip and the tongue
dorsum were up on that one.
Good, you raised the back of your tongue. Not quite. You raised the tip up, not the
dorsum.
Great job lifting the dorsum up. Didn’t get the dorsum up to the roof of
your mouth.
Knowledge of performance /s/ Got it. The sides of the tongue were touching Not quite. The middle and sides were even.
your teeth.
I like that you made a nice deep groove. Not that time. The groove wasn’t there.
Great job keeping the air moving the No. The air stopped and didn’t move through
whole time. the whole /s/ sound.

348 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 50 • 343–355 • July 2019
the top is a section to track the number of correct produc- example of portion of a session using SMC is provided in
tions during prepractice. Supplemental Material S2.
The remainder of the data sheet displays the list of
chains chosen for practice during the session. The client’s Increasing Difficulty of Chains
performance is assessed by the clinician after every six tri-
als to determine whether practice should build to a more Intentional modifications to the chains selected for
complex stimulus (moving to the right on the data sheet) treatment may influence task difficulty. Initially, chains are
or whether practice should switch to a chain for a different chosen which place the target sound sequence in a facilita-
variant (moving down on the data sheet). The criterion of tive context in word-initial or word-final position, and
five correct in a block of six is required to move to more chains include the target sound in a stressed syllable in mul-
complex targets in a chain. Thus, each trial should be tisyllabic words. However, over the course of treatment,
scored, and a sum should be computed after each block chains that are mastered (i.e., produced correctly through
of six trials. the sentence level on multiple occasions) need not be re-
The data sheet also provides information to guide peated but instead can be replaced by chains that may be
the nature and type of feedback that should be provided more challenging to the speech system. Three strategies
by the clinician. Within each block, feedback in the form for increasing the difficulty of chains are discussed below.
of KR, KP + KR, and no feedback trials are randomly The properties of the chains differ starting at the mono-
allocated to the six trials while maintaining the expected syllabic or multisyllabic word level. Examples of these
proportion (e.g., at the syllable level, one trial with no modifications are shown in Table 4.
feedback, one trial with KR feedback, and four trials
with KP + KR feedback, but at the sentence level, there Word-Medial Position
are three trials with no feedback, one trial with KR + KP When targeting a sound sequence in word-initial or
feedback, and two trials with KR feedback). word-final position, there is only one coarticulatory transi-
tion that must be practiced (i.e., transitioning into or out
of the target movement sequence, but not both, as in /kle/
Prosodic Variation at beginning of claiming). Positioning the target sound se-
quence in medial position of multisyllabic words creates a
Practice variability can be included in the form of potentially more challenging coarticulatory environment,
prosodic variation. This is intended to encourage practice requiring smooth transitions into and out of the target
of the target sound sequences in utterances that vary in movement pattern (e.g., /kle/ in proclaim). This change may
rate, intonation, or loudness. When prosodic variation is increase the demands on motor planning as the sequences
included, clients are cued to practice utterances slow, fast, are embedded in a larger stream of movements. Often, word-
loud, as a question, as a command, or as a statement. Al- medial chains are introduced with the addition of a bound
though this variation was found to have a minimal impact morpheme (prefix or suffix), although compound word
on speech sound learning in children with residual speech contexts (e.g., two-syllable spondees) can be used as well.
sound errors (Preston et al., 2014), recent research with
children with childhood apraxia of speech has suggested Lexical Stress
that this prosodic variation may aid generalization and To increase the difficulty of planning multisyllabic
retention in that population (Preston, Leece, McNamara, words, the target sequences can also be practiced in syl-
et al., 2017). Thus, prosodic variation may help children lables that do not carry primary stress. Often, targets in
learn to integrate articulatory movements with other param- syllables with primary stress (e.g., /ɹi/ in reading) are
eters of speech that are planned. more accurate because they are typically longer in dura-
tion than syllables that do not carry primary stress (e.g.,
/ɹi/ in realistic); thus, stressed syllables may reduce the
Self-Monitoring demands on motor programming. In unstressed syllables,
Another feature of SMC includes explicit require- however, the target sequences may be more challenging
ments for clients to self-rate the accuracy of their produc- to the client because there is less time to plan and exe-
tions. In our current version of SMC, each block of six cute movements and less transition time between articu-
trials requires self-rating on three trials. Before any feed- latory gestures.
back is given by the clinician, the child is asked to pro-
vide a self-evaluation of accuracy. Such procedures have Competing Sounds
been shown to facilitate generalization in speech sound To increase the difficulty of chains, competing sounds
learning (Koegel, Koegel, & Ingham, 1986; Ruscello & may be included. Competing sounds are those that share
Shelton, 1979). After the child self-evaluates, the clinician some articulatory gestures with the target (typically manner
may provide feedback by stating whether they agree or or place of articulation of consonants). Competing sounds
disagree with the child’s judgment. may also include the child’s typical error for a sound and
A sample chain including prosodic cues and self- may be present in the same word or in another word in a
monitoring is provided in the Appendix. In addition, a video phrase. Competing sounds challenge the speech planning

Preston et al.: Speech Motor Chaining 349


Table 4. Strategies to increase the difficulty of chains.

Multisyllabic word options


Monosyllabic “Basic” speech Medial Lexical Competing sounds
Variant word motor chain position stress or multiple targets

/ɑɹ/ tar guitar tarnish starvation rock-star


/ɑk/ lock unlock clockwise hemlock gridlock
/se/ sale sailor assailant wholesale sailboats

Note. Observe that the target sequence (in bold) remains consistent throughout the chain.

system by intentionally including articulatory gestures, Caveats


which may interfere with the intended sound sequences (cf.
SMC procedures are designed to facilitate a transition
Rogers & Storkel, 1998; Tilsen, 2013). For example, com-
from acquisition to motor learning. However, SMC may be
peting sounds for /ɹ/ in a target sound sequence may in-
supplemented with other clinical procedures. For example,
clude /l/ (which is also a liquid) or /w/ (which is also a
auditory perception and phonological awareness are relevant
semivowel and is a common substitute for /ɹ/). Thus,
skills that may be related to SSD in school-age children
words such as Larry and rewind would be target words
(e.g., Preston & Edwards, 2007; Shuster, 1998). Therefore,
with competing sounds for the target sequence /ɹi/. Com-
auditory perceptual training and phonological awareness
peting sounds for /k/ in a target sound sequence may in-
may also be targeted in a session to address multiple levels
clude /ɡ/ or /ŋ/ (which are also velars) or /t/ (which is a
of representation of speech (Preston & Leece, 2017; Preston,
common substitute for /k/). Thus, words such as took and
Leece, et al., 2016; Preston, Leece, McNamara, et al., 2017).
book bag include competing sounds for the target sound
Thus, a comprehensive treatment session may include SMC
sequence /ʊk/. The competing sounds strategy can be im-
supplemented with additional clinical procedures.
plemented at the monosyllabic word level; however, it is
In addition, the SMC procedures described here have
recommended that competing sounds be introduced when
been developed over time and should not be viewed as
the client can readily produce the target sound sequence
unchanging. For example, the randomized practice, self-
in words that do not have a competing sound. Compet-
monitoring, and prosodic variations that are included in
ing sounds could also include repetition of the target
the current iteration of SMC were not all part of the proce-
sound sequence in different syllables or words, as in took
dure when it was initially developed. Because a number
the book.
of parameters have been operationalized, they can con-
tinue to be empirically tested. For example, variations in
Randomized Practice the number of items per block, frequency of feedback,
SMC procedures may set the foundation to transi- and amount of self-monitoring could all be varied and
tion into more complex tasks. For example, the blocked tested to maximize efficiency.
practice in SMC may improve acquisition, but randomiza- Finally, it should be noted that, like all current treat-
tion may aid learning. Therefore, in our recent iteration of ments, SMC may not be appropriate for all children. For
SMC (e.g., Preston & Leece, 2017; Sjolie et al., 2016), a example, we expect that many 3- to 4-year-olds, or older
period of randomized practice has been included at the end children with significant intellectual disabilities, may not
of sessions once a child is capable of progressing through respond well to a highly structured drill-based treatment
prepractice quickly (i.e., with zero or one error on syllables). (though this speculation remains to be empirically tested).
In the current version of practice sessions, randomized Furthermore, as was evident in Figure 1, many but not all
practice is included in the last 5–8 min of a session using children show signs of generalization with this treatment.
the highest items on each chain that were produced cor- To date, we have observed acquisition (correct productions
rectly during SMC (i.e., items with at least five of six trials of target sound patterns within treatment sessions) for
correct). Based on the client’s performance during SMC nearly every child, but those gains in treatment sessions
earlier in the session, randomized practice items may range do not ubiquitously result in increased performance on
from syllables through sentences. The varied stimulus generalization measures. This may be a function of an in-
items from the chains practiced during the session can then sufficient number of hours of treatment or a need for a dif-
be shuffled such that the child is unaware of which item is ferent type of treatment. Therefore, continued refinement
coming next (cf. Skelton & Hagopian, 2014). To further of the procedure remains warranted.
incorporate motor learning principles, prosodic variation
is also cued during randomized practice (e.g., say the word
read loudly; say the sentence I found a book as a question), Implementation of SMC Procedures
and delayed KR feedback is provided on only 50% of trials In addition to the research implementation described
during randomized practice. earlier, SMC has been used by a number of certified

350 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 50 • 343–355 • July 2019
speech-language pathologists and student clinicians in the fidelity on the feedback structure can be achieved even
Gebbie Speech-Language Clinic at Syracuse University. for student clinicians.
The procedures have been used with children ages 6 years
through adults with various SSD profiles, including resid- Dose
ual speech errors and childhood apraxia of speech. Although
the primary implementation has been during individual Studies have shown that a relatively high rate of
therapy sessions, the procedures have also been adapted practice trials in speech therapy can be beneficial for learn-
for group therapy (cf. Skelton & Richard, 2016). For ex- ing (e.g., Edeal & Gildersleeve-Neumann, 2011). Figure 3
ample, children in the group may practice a chain one at a shows the distribution of the average number of SMC
time (from syllable level through the highest level to practice trials per participant across 36 participants of
which he or she can progress). To encourage generalization ages 7–24 years reported in six studies (Preston & Leece,
and attention to others’ speech, children in the group can 2017; Preston, Leece, et al., 2016; Preston, Leece, & Maas,
be taught to attend to others’ productions and to provide 2017; Preston, Leece, McNamara, et al., 2017; Preston,
KR feedback to other members of the group (cf. Johnston Maas, et al., 2016; Sjolie et al., 2016). Each participant’s
& Johnston, 1972). Reinforcers such as a turn-taking game mean number of trials per session is based on 14–20 treat-
can be completed with one child while another child be- ment sessions that included 40–52 min of time devoted to
gins a new chain. speech practice. One quarter of the participants (nine of
Typically, in a clinic setting, sessions are scheduled the 36) practiced an average of less than 100 trials per ses-
one to two times per week for an hour, although other service sion, primarily because they required several sessions to
delivery options are feasible. For example, a more frequent achieve stimulability and therefore had numerous sessions
treatment schedule (e.g., daily sessions) can be considered, with zero trials in SMC practice. That is, a lower dose is
particularly in the early stages of acquisition (cf. Preston & generally due to the time spent achieving several successful
Leece, 2017). SMC may also be a viable approach for productions in prepractice before moving to SMC (rather
implementing short individualized drill-based sessions in than a function of inefficient SMC). Typically, participants
school settings (cf. Sacks, Flipsen, & Neils-Strunjas, 2013). attempt fewer trials in early sessions (when more time is
spent in prepractice and less time is spent in SMC) and in-
creasingly more trials in later sessions (when less time is spent
Fidelity in prepractice and more time is spent in SMC). Overall,
Procedures that are most likely to be adopted clini- these participants averaged 218 trials per session (SD = 131),
cally are those in which high levels of fidelity can be and the data suggest SMC may be viewed as yielding a
achieved. Data from studies that have included five certi- high dose for most participants.
fied speech-language pathologists and two graduate stu-
dents are listed in Table 5. The clinicians for whom
Performance During Acquisition
fidelity data are reported engaged in a one-on-one train-
ing with the first author, with total training lasting approx- Versus Generalization
imately 3 hr. This training session consisted of review of The data collected during SMC are reflective of the
the SMC data sheet, a manual, cueing and feedback acquisition process, but not the generalization process.
strategies, and audio files of sessions. The fidelity data in Therefore, generalization data on untreated items, rather
Table 5 represent the proportion of trials in which the treat- than success during SMC practice trials, should serve as
ing clinician provided feedback as specified by the proce- a primary outcome when monitoring progress. Our practice
dures. The mean proportion of trials in which adequate to monitor generalization takes approximately 3 min to
feedback was provided exceeds 80% for all clinicians, and administer and includes probing accuracy of the target sound
it exceeds 90% for all but one clinician. This suggests in word position on a list of 25 untreated monosyllabic

Table 5. Fidelity on feedback structure.

Percent of trials in which prescribed


feedback was provided
Clinician No. of sessions M (SD) Range

SLP 1 23 98.5 (2.2) 84–100


SLP 2 15 97.0 (1.8) 93–99.4
SLP 3 12 81.9 (19.2) 41.8–94.5
SLP 4 8 98.0 (1.4) 95.6–100
SLP 5 4 99.1 (0.5) 98.2–99.5
SLP Student 1 8 96.3 (2.7) 91.7–98.7
SLP Student 2 4 95.1 (6.6) 86.9–95.24

Note. SLP = speech-language pathologist.

Preston et al.: Speech Motor Chaining 351


Figure 3. Boxplot representing mean number of Speech Motor generalization probes containing untrained words. Moni-
Chaining (SMC) trials per session for 36 participants with speech
sound disorders. Each data point represents mean number of
toring on these probes continued weekly for 5 weeks after
trials per session across 14–20 sessions. withdrawal of SMC treatment.
The bars in Figure 4 show within-session performance
for Ryan during the seven treatment sessions. In the first
session, he did not reach the criterion to advance to SMC,
and he, therefore, remained in prepractice (i.e., sessions
consisted of strategies such as shaping and phonetic place-
ment cues to elicit /ɹ/ in syllables). However, in Sessions 2
and 3, he progressed from prepractice to SMC where he
was successful in achieving correct productions only at the
syllable level. At Session 4, he began to achieve successful
productions in monosyllabic words; in Session 5, he pro-
duced chains through multisyllabic words and phrases, and
in Sessions 6 and 7, he progressed to chains with sentences.
It can be seen in Figure 4 that the total number of trials
increased greatly as he spent less time in prepractice, be-
came increasingly accurate, and required less feedback
and instruction. Furthermore, SMC resulted in a high rate
of practice with over 350 productions per hour during
Sessions 5–7.
Figure 4 also shows his performance on the generali-
zation probe (solid line) before, during, and after treat-
ment (as rated by three listeners who were blind to when
the recordings were collected). He was perceived to be
below 10% accurate before treatment, and the week after
and multisyllabic words. Items are administered by hav- his SMC sessions ended, he was 66% accurate on /ɹ/ on-
ing children read a list of words (or name pictures if reading sets. A final follow-up session 2 months later revealed
skills significantly interfere with the process). Essentially, word-level performance over 90%.
any word that is not on the generalization list would be eli- The acquisition data during SMC treatment and the
gible to select for training in SMC. In several previous generalization data in Figure 4 provide complementary
studies, generalization probes have been administered after perspectives on his performance. That is, it was only after
approximately every two sessions. However, the require- seven sessions of treatment that he began to show signs
ments of frequent monitoring in an experimental design of generalization to untreated words. It may be that a par-
may not be the same as those required clinically; there- ticular threshold of accuracy or a cumulative effect of
fore, as generalization is a gradual process, probes may be many correct trials within a session is required before gen-
useful for monitoring progress after approximately every eralization is observed.
three to four sessions. We recommend administering probes
at the beginning of a treatment session rather than at the
end, as accuracy at the beginning of the session will reflect Conclusion
not only generalization but also retention from previous ses- SMC is an operationalized form of speech practice,
sions. As performance reaches approximately 80% or higher which is designed to dynamically transition from an em-
on untreated words, probing accuracy in 10–15 untrained phasis on principles of acquisition to an emphasis on prin-
sentences may replace word-level probes. Accuracy above ciples of motor learning. Speech movement patterns are
80% on sentence-level stimuli may be sufficient to discon- gradually expanded, such that larger speech units (e.g.,
tinue SMC and to then shift focus to treatments that focus multisyllabic words) incorporate the same general move-
on conversational monitoring. ment patterns that were produced successfully in smaller
speech units (e.g., monosyllabic words, which were also
practiced in syllables). Moreover, as motoric and linguistic
Case Example complexity expands, variability of practice is added and
Ryan (pseudonym) was a 10-year-old with normal feedback type and frequency are manipulated. The proce-
language and hearing. He participated in seven 1-hr treatment dures involve a rapid rate of practice with the potential for
sessions to address production of /ɹ/ in onsets. Prior to frequent changes to practice conditions. However, the pro-
treatment, he was not stimulable for /ɹ/ in any contexts. cedures can be delivered with high fidelity using a freely
His treated variants included /ɹæ/ and /ɹi/, with chains such available data sheet, allowing for replicable implementa-
as rap, rapid, rapid decision and read, redeem, redeeming tion. Furthermore, the theoretically motivated framework
quality. In addition to his within-session performance, allows for systematic study of the key elements (an ongo-
progress was tracked every other session on word-level ing process), and modifications of the approach for clinical

352 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 50 • 343–355 • July 2019
Figure 4. Acquisition data (bars) and generalization data (line) for a 10-year-old with /ɹ/ distortion. Generalization probe
data were collected before, during, and after seven sessions of treatment with Speech Motor Chaining (SMC).

and research endeavors are quite feasible. SMC is therefore Johnston, J. M., & Johnston, G. T. (1972). Modification of conso-
one approach that may be considered for treatment of nant speech-sound articulation in young children. Journal of
SSD in school-age children. Applied Behavior Analysis, 5(3), 233–246.
Kent, R. D. (1982). Contextual facilitation of correct sound pro-
duction. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools,
Acknowledgments 13(2), 66–76. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461.1302.66
Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., & Ingham, J. C. (1986). Program-
The authors thank Patricia McCabe for feedback on early ming rapid generalization of correct articulation through self-
stages of the development of this approach. Research reported monitoring procedures. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders,
in this publication was supported by the National Institute on 51(1), 24–32.
Deafness and Other Communication Disorders under Awards Maas, E., Robin, D. A., Austermann Hula, S. N., Freedman, S. E.,
R15DC016426 and R03DC012152 (J. Preston, PI). The content is Wulf, G., Ballard, K. J., & Schmidt, R. A. (2008). Principles of
solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily motor learning in treatment of motor speech disorders. American
represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 17(3), 277–298. https://
doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2008/025)
McLeod, S., & Singh, S. (2009). Seeing speech. San Diego, CA:
References Plural.
Bohland, J. W., Bullock, D., & Guenther, F. H. (2010). Neural Mental, R., Foye, M., Lee, G., Schreiber, N., Barnes, A., & Vick, J.
representations and mechanisms for the performance of simple (2016). Opti-Speech: A newly developed biofeedback software
speech sequences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(7), for persistent speech sound disorders. Technical Clinical session
1504–1529. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21306 presented at the Annual Convention of the American Speech-
Chappell, G. E. (1973). Childhood verbal apraxia and its treat- Language-Hearing Association, Philadelphia, PA.
ment. The Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 38(3), Mielke, J., Baker, A., & Archangeli, D. (2016). Individual-level
362–368. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.3803.362 contact limits phonological complexity: Evidence from
Edeal, D. M., & Gildersleeve-Neumann, C. E. (2011). The impor- bunched and retroflex /ɹ/. Language, 92(1), 101–140.
tance of production frequency in therapy for childhood apraxia Murray, E., McCabe, P., & Ballard, K. J. (2012). A comparison
of speech. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, of two treatments for childhood apraxia of speech: Methods
20(2), 95–110. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2011/09-0005) and treatment protocol for a parallel group randomised control
Guadagnoli, M. A., & Lee, T. D. (2004). Challenge point: A trial. BMC Pediatrics, 12(112). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-
framework for conceptualizing the effects of various practice 2431-12-112
conditions in motor learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, Preston, J. L., & Edwards, M. L. (2007). Phonological processing
36(2), 212–224. https://doi.org/10.3200/JMBR.36.2.212-224 skills of adolescents with residual speech sound errors. Language,
Hitchcock, E. R., & McAllister Byun, T. (2015). Enhancing gener- Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 38(4), 297–308.
alisation in biofeedback intervention using the challenge point https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2007/032)
framework: A case study. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, Preston, J. L., & Leaman, M. (2014). Ultrasound visual feedback
29(1), 59–75. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2014.956232 for acquired apraxia of speech: A case report. Aphasiology,
Johnson, H. P., & Hood, S. B. (1988). Teaching chaining to unin- 28(3), 278–295. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2013.852901
telligible children: How to deal with open syllables. Language, Preston, J. L., & Leece, M. C. (2017). Intensive treatment for per-
Speech, and Hearing Services in the Schools, 19(2), 211–220. sisting rhotic distortions: A case series. American Journal of

Preston et al.: Speech Motor Chaining 353


Speech-Language Pathology, 26(4), 1066–1079. https://doi.org/ Secord, W. A., Boyce, S. E., Donohue, J. S., Fox, R. A., & Shine,
10.1044/2017_AJSLP-16-0232 R. E. (2007). Eliciting sounds: Techniques and strategies for
Preston, J. L., Leece, M. C., & Maas, E. (2016). Intensive treat- clinicians (2nd ed.). Clifton Park, NY: Thomson Delmar
ment with ultrasound visual feedback for speech sound errors Learning.
in childhood apraxia. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10(440). Shriberg, L. D., Fourakis, M., Hall, S. D., Karlsson, H. B.,
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00440 Lohmeier, H. L., McSweeny, J. L., . . . Wilson, D. L. (2010).
Preston, J. L., Leece, M. C., & Maas, E. (2017). Motor-based Extensions to the Speech Disorders Classification System
treatment with and without ultrasound feedback for residual (SDCS). Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 24(10), 795–824.
speech-sound errors. International Journal of Language & Com- https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2010.503006
munication Disorders, 52(1), 80–94. https://doi.org/10.1111/ Shriberg, L. D., & Kwiatkowski, J. (1982). Phonological disorders. 2.
1460-6984.12259 A conceptual framework for management. Journal of Speech
Preston, J. L., Leece, M. C., McNamara, K., & Maas, E. (2017). and Hearing Disorders, 47(3), 242–256.
Variable practice to enhance speech learning in ultrasound Shriberg, L. D., Strand, E. A., Fourakis, M., Jakielski, K. J.,
biofeedback treatment for childhood apraxia of speech: A Hall, S. D., Karlsson, H. B., . . . Wilson, D. L. (2017). A diag-
single case experimental study. American Journal of Speech- nostic marker to discriminate childhood apraxia of speech
Language Pathology, 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1044/2017_AJSLP- from speech delay: I. Development and description of the pause
16-0155 marker. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research.
Preston, J. L., Maas, E., Whittle, J., Leece, M. C., & McCabe, P. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-S-15-0296
(2016). Limited acquisition and generalisation of rhotics with Shuster, L. I. (1998). The perception of correctly and incorrectly
ultrasound visual feedback in childhood apraxia. Clinical Linguis- produced /r/. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
tics & Phonetics, 30(3–5), 363–381. https://doi.org/10.3109/ 41, 941–950.
02699206.2015.1052563 Sjolie, G. M., Leece, M. C., & Preston, J. L. (2016). Acquisition,
Preston, J. L., McCabe, P., Rivera-Campos, A., Whittle, J. L., retention, and generalization of rhotics with and without ultra-
Landry, E., & Maas, E. (2014). Ultrasound visual feedback sound visual feedback. Journal of Communication Disorders,
treatment and practice variability for residual speech sound 64, 62–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2016.10.003
errors. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, Skelton, S. L., & Hagopian, A. L. (2014). Using randomized vari-
57(6), 2102–2115. https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_JSLHR-S- able practice in the treatment of childhood apraxia of speech.
14-0031 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 23(4), 599–611.
Redford, M. A. (2015). Unifying speech and language in a devel- https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_AJSLP-12-0169
opmentally sensitive model of production. Journal of Phonetics, Skelton, S. L., & Richard, J. T. (2016). Application of a motor
52, 141–152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2015.06.006 learning treatment for speech sound disorders in small groups.
Rogers, M. A., & Storkel, H. L. (1998). Reprogramming phono- Perceptual and Motor Skills, 122(3), 840–854. https://doi.org/
logically similar utterances: The role of phonetic features in 10.1177/0031512516647693
pre-motor encoding. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Tilsen, S. (2013). A dynamical model of hierarchical selection and
Research, 41(2), 258–274. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4102.258 coordination in speech planning. PLOS ONE, 8(4), e62800.
Ruscello, D. M., & Shelton, R. L. (1979). Planning and self-assessment https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0062800
in articulatory training. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disor- Tourville, J. A., & Guenther, F. H. (2011). The DIVA model: A
ders, 44(4), 504–512. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.4404.504 neural theory of speech acquisition and production. Language
Rvachew, S., & Brosseau-Lapré, F. (2012). Developmental phono- and Cognitive Processes, 26, 37–41. https://doi.org/10.1080/
logical disorders: Foundations of clinical practice. San Diego, 01690960903498424
CA: Plural. Vick, J., Mental, R., Carey, H., & Lee, G. (2017). Seeing is treat-
Sacks, S., Flipsen, P., & Neils-Strunjas, J. (2013). Effectiveness of ing: 3D electromagnetic midsagittal articulography (EMA)
systematic articulation training program accessing computers visual biofeedback for the remediation of residual speech errors.
(SATPAC) approach to remediate dentalized and interdental The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 141(5, Pt. 2),
/s,z/: A preliminary study. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 117(2), 3451–4086.
559–577. https://doi.org/10.2466/24.10.PMS.117x21z2 Young, E. C. (1987). The effects of treatment on consonant cluster
Schmidt, R. A., & Lee, T. D. (2011). Motor control and learning: and weak syllable reduction processes in misarticulating chil-
A behavioral emphasis (5th ed.). Champaign, IL: Human dren. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 18(1),
Kinetics. 23–33. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461.1801.23

354 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 50 • 343–355 • July 2019
Appendix
Speech Motor Chaining Data Sheet

Preston et al.: Speech Motor Chaining 355

You might also like